Influence of Mizo Language on Nasal and Oral Passage in English: a Nasometric Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Influence of Mizo Language on Nasal and Oral Passage in English: a Nasometric Study

INFLUENCE OF MIZO LANGUAGE ON NASAL AND ORAL PASSAGE IN ENGLISH: A NASOMETRIC STUDY Meenu Kalyani1, Ashok Kumar Sinha2, Himanshu Kumar3, Bibhu Prasad Hota4, Lorna Das5 1(Speech and language pathologist & Audiologist, Kolkata, India) 2(Director, AYJNIHH, Mumbai, India) 3(Lecturer (Speech & Hearing), AYJNIHH, ERC, Kolkata, India) 4(Lecturer (Speech & Hearing), AYJNIHH, ERC, Kolkata, India) 5(Post graduate trainee in Audiology and Speech Language Pathology, AYJNIHH, ERC, Kolkata, India)

ABSTRACT

Nasometer is computer based instrument which facilitates accurate signal

analysis, which yields “nasalance” score. The nasality measure is derived from the ratio

of acoustic energy output from the nasal and oral cavities of the speaker.

Most research reports on normative nasalance score of English language. There is

no report regarding nasalance score variance in the Mizo language speakers speaking

English. The study aims in establishing norms and measuring nasalance score in Mizo

speakers reading English passages.

A total of 30 subjects (15 males and 15 females) who are native speakers of Mizo

language who has exposure of English language since childhood were selected.

Nasometer II Model 6400 (Software version 2.6) of Key Elemetrics Corporation

was used.

1 Three standardized passages (Zoo passage, Rainbow passage and nasal sentences)

were used for the study.

The mean nasalance scores obtained for zoo, rainbow and nasal sentences in

female and male were 15.93 ± 3.15 SD, 35.60 ± 3.05 SD, 64.33 ± 3.26 SD and 18.26 ±

3.53 SD, 33.13 ± 1.68 SD, 63.20 ± 88 SD respectively.Standard norms shows significant

differences between male and female for Zoo and Rainbow Passages but not in Nasal

Sentences.

Keywords: Mizo Language, Nasometer, Nasalance score, Zoo Passages, Rainbow Passages

INTRODUCTION

Verbal communication is one of the most important models to communicate

between the human beings. The main elements of the verbal communication are hearing,

voice; language and speaking are mainly created through the harmonious functioning of

respiration, phonation, resonance, and articulatory muscles together. Resonance modifies

voice by increasing or decreasing the harmonics of voice. The resonator system has a

complex structure. Supraglottal air gaps act as resonators. These are complex air gaps

which are found in the tight area that goes through the larynx, the large opening of the

larynx, the wide cavity in the mouth and nasal cavity. Normal speech sound production

depends on the ability to rapidly couple and decouple the nasal cavity from the oral

cavity. Nasal speech sound require oral nasal coupling and oral sounds require oral-nasal

decoupling. The process of coupling and decoupling the oral and nasal cavities for speech

2 is called velopharyngeal valving. This valving is controlled by the elevation of the velum

and constriction of the pharyngeal walls.

Role of velopharyngeal mechanism in speech production

Velopharyngeal mechanism varies the degree of acoustic coupling between the

oral and nasal cavities. Inadequate closure of the velopharyngeal mechanism may result

in nasalized speech or the inability to impound air pressure within the oral cavity for the

production of consonants. Unvoiced consonants may become voiced, plosives become

snorts, and vowels exhibit an unmistakable nasal quality or twang. Inappropriate or

excessive velopharyngeal closure can result in the familiar “stuffy nose” quality.

Adequacy of velopharyngeal closure and appropriate timing of the valving action are two

important parameters of articulation. Closure is achieved by elevating and retracting the

soft palate and at the same time constricting the walls of the nasopharynx. The posterior

pharyngeal wall may move anteriorly to meet the soft palate in some individuals and may

be seen as a compensatory gesture in instances of a short palate (Zemlin, 1988).

Movement of velum is essentials for making the distinction between oral and nasal

sounds in speech.

Five muscles are involved in the velar functions which are levator veli

palatine, uvular muscles, tensor veli palatine, palatopharyngeous, and palatoglossus.

Elevation of velum is primarily contraction of the lavetor veli palatine muscles (Kent,

1997; Seikel et al 2000, Zemlin, 1998). Velopharyngeal closure is accomplished by the

coordinated movement of all of above structures.

3 Velopharyngeal closure pattern in female are in coronal pattern 19(35.85)%,

circular 15 (28.30)%, circular with passavant’s ridge 11 (20.75)%, and sagittal 8

(15.09)% where as in the male the frequency is with coronal pattern 14(38.89)%, circular

15 (41.67)%, circular with passavant’s ridge 3 (8.33)%, and in sagittal 4 (11.11)%.

Velopharyngeal closure occurs not only for speech, but also for other pneumatic

activities such as sucking, blowing, and whistling and non-pneumatic activities such as

gagging, swallowing, and vomiting. The position and degree of closure differ for all these

activities. The point of contact and degree of closure even vary different phonemes and

with different phonetic environments. (Moll, 1962; Shprintzen et al., 1975). Closure may

be completely for pneumatic activities but insufficient for speech or other pneumatic

activities (Shprintzen et al., 1975).

The term nasalance has been proposed by Fletcher and Frost, 1974 for a measure

of velopharyngeal closure during voiced speech in which nasally emitted acoustic energy

is compared to the orally emitted energy. Nasometer is developed by Samuel Fletcher,

Larry Adams, and Martin McCutcheon at the University of Alabama at Birmingham,

Biocommunications Department (1989). Since the introduction nasometer has been

utilized by a number of investigators in an attempt to determine its utility in the

assessment and treatment of patients at risk for manifesting velopharyngeal impairment.

The Nasometer has provided an easy, non-invasive method for assessing nasality

objectively.

Nasometer is computer based instrument employs microphones on other side of a

sound separator plate which rests on the upper lip. The nasality measure is derived from

4 the ratio of acoustic energy output from the nasal and oral cavities of the speaker. Using

an innovative input device consisting of a directional microphone mounted on either side

of an efficient sound separator plate the Nasometer headset facilitates accurate signal

analysis, which yields a “nasalance” score. The signal from each microphone is filtered

and digitized by custom electronic modules. The data are then processed by a computer

and accompanying software.

A numeric ratio of nasal acoustic energy to the sum of nasal plus oral acoustic

energy is calculated, multiplied by 100 and expressed as a “nasalance score” that is (N/

(N+O)) x 100 = Nasalance) and is displayed graphically on the host computer screen in

real time.

The output of this instrument provides the user with a score that reflects the

relative amount of nasal acoustic energy in a subject’s speech. Standardized nasometry

scores have been published in several languages such as English (Seaver et al., 1991, van

Doorn& Purcell, 1998); Flemish (Van Lierde et al., 2001); Thai (Prathanee et al., 2003).

The nasalance score is a valid correlate of perceived nasality (Fletcher, 1976), has a high

specificity (86%), a high sensitivity (87%), and a high overall all efficiency (87%)

(Dalston et al., 1993). Nasalance score has a limited implication for cross-country or

cross-language comparison because interpretation for identifying normal and abnormal

based on the cutoff scores. Therefore, it should be a supplementary but not a substitute

for clinical judgment (Vallino-Napoli and Montgomery 1997).

The Mizo language, or Mizo tawang, is spoken natively by the Mizo people in the

Mizoram state of India, Chin State in Burma, and the Chittagong Hill Tracts of

5 Bangladesh. The language is also known as Lushai, a colonial term, as the Lushei people

were the first to have external exposure. The Mizo language belongs to the Kukish

branch of the Tibeto-Burman family of languages. The numerous clans of the Mizo had

respective dialects, amongst which the Lushei (Lusei, by Mizo) dialect was most

common, and which subsequently became the Mizo language and the lingua franca of the

Kuki people due to its extensive and exclusive use by the Christian missionaries. Mizo

language has eight tones and intonations for each of the vowels /a/, /aw/, /e/, /i/ and /u/,

four of which are reduced tones and the other four long tones.

Tone systems have developed independently in many of the daughter languages

largely through simplifications in the set of possible syllable-final and syllable-initial

consonants. Typically, a distinction between voiceless and voiced initial consonants is

replaced by a distinction between high and low tone, while falling and rising tones

developed from syllable-final h, a glottal stop, which themselves often reflect earlier

consonants. In tone languages there are potential conflicts in the perception of lexical

tone and intonation, as both depend mainly on the differences in fundamental frequency

(F0) patterns.

It is well established that differences in nasalance scores occur among different

native languages. The frequency of phonemic distribution varies in different language.

The distribution of phonemes is different in different languages; standard passage for

each language should be developed. The corresponding normative nasalance scores

should be computed for each languages or regional dialect (Seaver et al., 1991) because

vowels are intentionally nasalized in some language (e.g., French) and some regional

dialect, for instances, English Phonetician often described the vowel in American English

6 dialects as more nasalized then the same vowel in queen’s English. For American English

(Flecther et al., 1989), Australian English (van Droon& Purcell, 1998), German (Heppt et

al., 1989), Castilian Spanish (Santosh Terron et al., 1991), Finnish (Happannen, 1991)

and Midwest Japanese (Tachimura& Mori, 2000) these normative nasalance score have

been computed.

AIM OF THE STUDY

Most research reports on normative nasalance score were made on the basis of

English language. There is no report regarding nasalance score variance in the Mizo

language speakers speaking English. From the review it is evident that nasal resonance

varies in speech sound of different language and in different dialects of same language. In

multilingual country like India where English is spoken as second or third language, the

nasalance score as measure from the zoo passage, rainbow passage and nasal sentences

might have to be interpreted differently. The study aims in establishing norms and

measuring nasalance score in Mizo speakers reading English passages.

METHODOLOGY

Participants

Native speakers of Mizo language with second language as English were selected

for the study. A total of 30 subjects comprising 15 male and 15 female were divided into

two groups. Group-I had 15 female native speakers of Mizo language and group-II had

15 male native speakers of Mizo language. The mean and standard deviation of the

7 subjects age was 21.3 and 1.79for male and for female 21.6 and 1.59 respectively. The

age ranges of all the subjects were between 18 to 30 years.

SUBJECT SELECTION CRITERIA

Inclusion criteria

All the subjects were native speaker of Mizo language, able to read English fluently and

within the age range of 18 to 30 years and willing to participate in the study.

Exclusion criteria

All the participants having any speech and language problem, perceived resonance

disorder, suffering from cold or other upper respiratory tract infections, and any hearing

problem were excluded from the study.

Instrumentation and test environment

Nasometer II Model 6400 (software version 2.6) of Key Elemetrics Corporation

was connected to a desktop computer model (HCL Pentium 4) was used for measurement

of mean nasalance in this study. Nasometer was housed in a quiet room which was

partially acoustically treated in the clinic of AYJNIHH, ERC, KOLKATA.

INSTRUCTIONS

Verbal instructions were given in English. The instruction was “Three texts will

appear on the screen. Read the text which appears on the screen exactly as it appears-‘Do

not repeat the text or add anything which does not appear on the screen’. Three trials will

be taken for each passage”. The subjects were instructed to start reading after the

8 recording icon was clicked. Nasalance measure was taken first for oral passage and, then

for oral nasal passage and then for nasal sentences. The subjects were instructed not to

repeat a syllable once spoken, and also not to add filters like /umm, or /aaa/ in between.

However, the subjects were allowed to pause in between reading, but to resume reading

from where they stopped.

READING STIMULI

For measurement of mean nasalance and in variance three standardized passage will be

used for the study.

1. The zoo passage (Fletcher, 1972)

2. The rainbow passage (Fairbanks, 1960)

3. A set of 5 nasal sentences (Fletcher, 1978)

TESTS

Three standardized passages for the measurements of nasalance were used. Zoo

passage given by Fletcher (1972) which excluded nasal consonants in English language

was used. Rainbow passage developed by Fairbanks (1960) containing 11.5% nasal

consonants in English language were used in the study. A set of five nasal sentences were

taken from the manual of nasometer II which contained 35% nasal consonants in English

were used. This was more than three times as many as would be expected in standard

American English sentences (Fletcher, 1978).

9 PROCEDURE

The nasometer II model 6400 was calibrated using the standard calibration

procedure provided by the manufacturer prior to data collection. At the beginning of each

data collection session, the nasometer was calibrated in accordance with manufacturer

instructions. The nasometer headpiece was then positioned such that the oral and nasal

microphones were at equivalent distances from the mouth and nose. The subject was

seated in front of a computer monitor and asked to read the Zoo passage, Rainbow

passage and Nasal sentences with normal loudness and at a normal rate of speech. The

proper placement of headset on a subject has been shown. All the subjects were given

instruction prior to the test.All subjects were given three trials. Average mean nasalance

scores were computed for statistical analysis. It took about 15 minutes to test each

subject.

RESULTS

COMPARISON OF MEAN NASALANCE ACROSS THE PASSAGES

Mean nasalance for zoo passage, rainbow passage, and nasal sentences

All subjects were asked to read passage and scores of mean nasalance were analyzed.

Mean and standard deviation of mean nasalance for the Zoo passage, rainbow passage,

and nasal sentences were computed and are reported in Table 1 for group-I and group-II.

10 Table.1: Mean and standard deviation of mean nasalance score of group I and group II as

measured on Zoo Passage, rainbow passage and nasal sentences.

Passage Group- I (Female) Group – II (Male) Zoo passage Mean 15.93 18.26 SD 3.15 3.53 Rainbow passage Mean 35.60 33.13 SD 3.05 1.68 Nasal sentences Mean 64.33 63.20 SD 3.26 2.88

DIFFERENCES OF MEAN NASALANCE BETWEEN THE PASSAGES

Differences of mean nasalance between the Zoo Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow

Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Zoo Passage

The difference in mean and standard deviation of mean nasalance scores of group-I and

group-II subjects taken together were computed and reported in table 2 between Zoo

Passage and Nasal Sentences,Rainbow Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage

and Zoo Passage.

11 Table.2: Mean and standard deviation of mean nasalance score for Zoo Passage and Nasal

Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Zoo Passage

Passage N Mean Standard deviation Nasal sentences 30 63.76 3.08 Zoo Passage 30 17.10 3.49 Diff (1-2) 46.66 3.29 Nasal sentences 30 63.76 3.08 Rainbow passage 30 34.36 2.73 Diff (1-2) 29.40 2.91 Rainbow passage 30 34.36 2.73 Zoo passage 30 17.10 3.49 Diff (1-2) 17.26 3.13

The Degree of freedom and t-value of comparison of means for Zoo Passage and Nasal

Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Zoo Passage.

Table.3: comparison of means for Zoo Passage and Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage and

Nasal Sentences, Rainbow Passage and Zoo Passage.

Passages Variances Df t value Pr> /t/ ZooPassage Equal 58 54.84 <.0001 And Nasal Sentences Unequal 57.09 54.84 <.0001

Rainbow Passage Equal 58 39.08 <.0001 and Nasal Sentences Unequal 57.19 39.08 <.0001

Rainbow Passage Equal 58 21.30 <.0001 and Zoo Passage Unequal 57.19 21.30 <.0001

12 Nasalance scores along with Gender Differences

All the subjects were asked to read all three passages and the observations were

combined for female and male separately of normal Mizo speakers and mean and

standard deviation of female and male were found to be 38.622 and 38.200 respectively.

Table.4: Mean and Standard deviation of gender differences

Gender Observation Mean Standard Deviation Female 45 38.622 20.33

Male 45 38.200 19.09

Diff (1-2) 0.422 19.72

The degree of freedom and t-value of comparison of means of gender differences.

Table.5: Comparison of means of gender differences

Variances Df t-Value Pr>/t/ Equal 88 0.10 0.91

Unequal 87.65 0.10 0.91

13 Computation of t test

For examining the gender difference of the mean nasalance of the native speakers of

Mizo language, t test was computed and the summary of equality of mean nasalance has

been reported in Table 6

Table.6: Summary of equality of mean nasalance of Mizo speakers speaks English language as

second language.

t- test for Equality of means df Sig.(2tailed) Zoo Passage

Equal variance assumed 2.731 28 .011

Equal variance not assumed. 2.731 21.748 .012 Rainbow Passage

Equal variance assumed -1.909 28 .067

Equal variance not assumed -1.909 27.636 .067

Nasal Passage

Equal variance assumed 1.007 28 0.322

Equal variance not assumed 1.007 27.57 0.322

14 Table.7:.Mean and standard deviation of male, female, male + female and standard norms across the passages.

STANDARD MALE FEMALE MALE+FEM NORMS ALE PASSAGE MEAN MEAN MEAN MEAN S.D ZOO 18.26 15.93 17.1 11.25 5.63 PASSAGE

RAINBOW 33.13 35.60 34.36 31.47 6.65 PASSAGE NASAL 63.20 64.33 63.76 59.55 7.96 SENTENCES

t-value and degree of freedom of the Zoo Passage between male and female and male and

female together combined.

Table.8: t-value and degree of freedom of the Zoo Passage

t-Value df Observed Value Male 4.0857 8.48 1.860 Female 2.8192 7.92 1.860 Male+ Female 4.8375 33.33 1.697

t-value and degree of freedom of the Rainbow Passage between male and female and

male and female together combined.

Table.9: t-value and degree of freedom of the Rainbow Passage.

t-value df Observed Value Male 0.9373 5.03 2.015 Female 2.1851 5.50 2.015 Male+ Female 2.2020 22.36 1.717

15 t-value and degree of freedom of the Nasal Sentences between male and female and male

and female together combined.

Table.10: t-value and degree of freedom of the Nasal Sentences

t-Value df Observed Value Male 1.6700 3.68 2.353 Female 2.1522 3.76 2.132 Male+ Female 2.7018 15.44 1.750

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study is to develop to find out normative nasalance scores for

native Mizo language speaker speaking English as second language since school age for

different stimuli (Zoo Passage, Rainbow Passage and Nasal Sentences) as compared with

the normative data for the English speakers as measured by the Nasometer-II of Kay

Elemetrics Corporation. Gender differences in measures of mean nasalance were also

studied. Results of the statistical analysis were presented in the previous chapter.

Mean Nasalance of native speakers of Mizo

The mean nasalance scores and standard deviation of the native speakers (both

male and female) of the Mizo was obtained. As native speakers of Mizo were reading

English Passages containing no nasal sounds, 11.5% of nasal sounds and 35.5% of nasal

sounds in Zoo, Rainbow and Nasal Sentences, clearly shows significant differences in

nasalance score for Zoo Passage as compared to the normative data of the native English

speaker.

16 The t value of Rainbow Passage for female group, for male group and

for both combined male and female group shows significant differences in mean

nasalance scores as shown in the table 9.The t test of Nasal Sentences shows significant

differences for female group and for both combined male and female group. But there is

no significant differences in the male group.

Figure 1.Comparison of female and male nasalance scores on Zoo Passage,

Rainbow Passage, and Nasal Sentences

Figure 1.Shows passages on X-axis and nasal percentage on the y-axis and shows

significant nasal differences between female and male.

The main difference between nasal and oral vowels is due to the position of the

soft palate. During the articulation of nasal vowels, the soft palate is lowered down, so

that the air stream is free is free to pass not only through the buccal cavity but also

through the nasal cavities. Due to the coupling of the buccal and nasal cavities, the first

formant of all nasal vowels is slightly reduced in intensity. Generally speaking, the nasal

vowels are in the same articulatory position as the corresponding oral vowels (Ferguson

and Chowdhury, 1960).

Mizo language has eight tones and intonations for each of the vowels /a/, /aw/, /e/,

/i/ and /u/, four of which are reduced tones and the other four long tones. The vowel /o/

has only three tones, all of them of the reduced type; it has almost exactly the same sound

17 as the diphthong /oƱ/ found in American English. Mizo is a tonal language, in which

differences in pitch and pitch contour can change the meanings of words.

Most of the studies assert that higher nasalance in adult females can be attributed

to higher average pitch levels (Britto& Doyle, 1990) and the use of greater pitch

variability (Sulter& Peters, 1996).Female also use different intonation patterns (Elyan,

1988) and voice markers for resonance, loudness, and voice quality (Oates &Dacakis,

1997) in their speech.

The mechanism for velopharyngeal valving has been found to differ in male and

female. McKerns and BZoch (1970) investigated the mechanism for velopharyngeal

valving in 40 normal young adults (20 male & 20 female), using lateral cinefluorography,

and found significant differences between the genders. The basic orientation of velum to

pharynx in male can be described in terms of an acute angle and that of female more

approximately in terms of a right angle. The velar length is greater in male, the height of

elevation is greater, the contact is less, and the inferior point of contact is most usually

above palate plane. In females the above pattern is reversed.

Nasalance Scores: Gender Difference

There is no significant difference between the female and male

group as shown in the table 4 and 5. The t test for Equality of means of the Zoo Passages

and Rainbow Passage shows significant differences except for Nasal Sentences shown in

the table 6.

Study conducted by Dalston, (1991); Anderson, (1996); Neiman, and Gonzalez-

Landa, (1993); Seaver, Dalston, Leeper and Adams (1991); Thomas and Hixon, (1979);

18 and Seaver et al (1991) indicated the gender differences in the mean nasalance scores

with higher nasalance scores for female. Researchers have observed that female speakers

had significantly higher nasalance scores than male subjects on nasal sentences. This

could be due to the nasal airflow which is higher in females (Thomas and Hixon, 1979).

Also this may be due to the increased respiratory effort and increased nasal cross-section

area. One more explanation for the gender difference in nasalance scores was the

disparity might be due to the differences in nasal cross-section area and modal pitch.

Nasal cross-section area and F0 was evaluated using modification of theoretical hydraulic

principle and visipitch respectively. Seaver et al (1991) study found significant

differences in nasal cross-section areas and modal pitch between male and female

subjects.

There have been studies focused on sex difference in nasalance scores. Some

studies reported no sex differences in nasalance scores during reading of the Zoo passage

(Seaver et al., 1991; Leeper et al., 1992; Litzaw and Dalston, 1992; Mayo et al., 1996;

van Droon and Purcell, 1998), but others indicates higher nasalance scores among women

during reading of the nasal passage (Seaver et al., 1991; Vallino-Napoli and

Montgomery, 1997). That is although it is possible that sex differences in nasalance

scores are related to the test stimulus, it is not clear whether the findings obtained among

non-Japanese speakers are also valid for Japanese speakers which has different

phonological characteristics from Western Languages.

Gender related differences in nasalance values have been related to basic

anatomical structural and physiological differences between males and females. The

resonance of voice is influenced by the size, shape, and surface of the infraglottal and

19 supraglottall resonating structure and cavities (Shprintzen&Bardach, 1995). Various

studies have reported that the functioning of larynx and velopharynx is affected by a large

number of anatomical, physiological, and aerodynamical gender realted differences.

Physical size appears to be the predominant anatomical feature that differentiates male

and female larynges (Gooze’e et al., 1998 &Kahane, 1983).

Comparison on mean nasalance across the passages

Paired t-test was done to find out the differences in nasalance score between

Nasal sentences and Zoo Passages. The resulted depicted significant difference in mean

nasalancescores of Nasal Sentences and Zoo passage for all 30 subjects, Nasal Sentences

showed higher nasalance score as compared to Zoo passage as shown in the table 3.

The mean of nasal sentences and rainbow passages were calculated to find

out the mean nasalance. Significant differences were found between the nasal sentences

and Rainbow passage has been shown in the table 3. Thus the results indicate higher

nasalance values for Nasal Sentences.

The mean of Rainbow passage and Zoo Passage were calculated to

find out comparison of mean nasalance. Higher significant difference of nasalance for

Rainbow Passage as compared to the Zoo Passage as shown in the table 3.

CONCLUSION

The present study was taken up with the purpose of investigating the norms for

mean nasalance as measured by Zoo, Rainbow, and Nasal Sentences for the native

20 speakers of the Mizo language. The reported normative nasalance data will provide

important reference information for several clinicians who assess resonance disorders.

Speech pathologists can measure the effects of a specific therapy approach, and the

plastic surgeon can evaluate the effects of different nasal and pharyngeal surgical

techniques.

The normative scores can be used for assessment of different resonance disorders like:

-Cleft lip and Palate

-Motor Speech Disorder

-Hearing Impairment

-Functional Nasality Problems

-Singing Pedagogy

REFERENCES

1. Anderson, R. (1996). Nasometric Values for Normal Spanish-Speaking Females: A

Preliminary Report.The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 33(4), pp.333-336.

2. Britto, A. and Doyle, P. (1990). A Comparison of Habitual and Derived Optimal Voice

Fundamental Frequency Values in Normal Young Adult Speakers. J Speech Hear

Disord, 55(3), p.476.

21 3. Dalston, R., Neiman, G. and Gonzalez-Landa, G. (1993). Nasometric Sensitivity and

Specificity: A Cross-Dialect and Cross-Culture Study. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial

Journal, 30(3), pp.285-291.

4. Elyan, O. (1988). Sex differences in speech style. Womspeak, 4, pp.4-8.

5. Fairbanks, G. (1960). Voice and articulation drill book. New York: Harper & Row.

6. Fletcher, S. (1972). Contingencies for Bioelectronic Modification of Nasality. J Speech

Hear Disord, 37(3), p.329.

7. Fletcher, S., Sooudi, I. and Frost, S. (1974). Quantitative and graphic analysis of

prosthetic treatment for “nasalance” in speech. The Journal of Prosthetic Dentistry,

32(3), pp.284-291.

8. Fletcher, S., Adams, L. and McCutcheon, M. (1989). Cleft palate speech assessment

through oral nasal acoustic measures. Communicative disorders related to cleft lip and

palate., pp.246 -257.

9. Goozee, J., Murdoch, B., Theodors, D. and Thompson, E. (1998). The effect of age and

gender on laryngeal aerodynamics. . International Journal of Language &

Communication Disorders, 33, pp.221-238.

10. Haapanen, M. (1991). Nasalance Scores in Normal Finnish Speech. Folia Phoniatr

Logop, 43(4), pp.197-203.

11. Heppt, W., Westrich, M., Strate, B. and Mohring, L. (1991). A new concept of objective

analysis of nasality. Laryngo-Rhino-Otologie, 70(4), pp.208-213.

22 12. Kahane, J. (1983). Postnatal development and aging of the human larynx. . Seminar

Speech Langage, 4, pp.189-203.

13. Kent, R. (1997). The Speech Sciences. San Diego, London: Singular Publishing.

14. Lierde, K., Wuyts, F., Bodt, M. and Cauwenberge, P. (2001). Nasometric Values for

Normal Nasal Resonance in the Speech of Young Flemish Adults. The Cleft Palate-

Craniofacial Journal, 38(2), pp.112-118.

15. Litzaw, L. and Dalston, R. (1992). The effect of gender upon nasalance scores among

normal adult speakers. Journal of Communication Disorders, 25(1), pp.55-64.

16. Mayo, R. and Richardson, A. (1996). Acoustic and perceptual characteristics of the

voices of normal adult African-American and White females. Journal of speech, Hearing

and Language.

17. Mckerns, D. and Bzoch, K. (1970). Variations in velopharyngeal valving: The factor of

sex. Cleft Palate Journal, 7, pp.652-662.

18. Moll, K. (1962). Velopharyngeal Closure on Vowels. Journal of Speech Language and

Hearing Research, 5(1), p.30.

19. Oates, J. and Dacakis, G. (1997). Voice changes in transsexuals. Venereology, 10,

pp.178-187.

20. Prathanee, B., Thanaviratananich, S., Pongjunyakul, A. and Rengpatanakij, K. (2003).

Nasalance scores for speech in normal thai children. Scandinavian Journal of Plastic and

Reconstructive Surgery and Hand Surgery, 37(6), pp.351-355.

23 21. Seaver, E., Dalston, R., Leeper, H. and Adams, L. (1991). A study of nasometric values

for normal nasal resonance. Journal of speech, Hearing and resonance, 34, pp.715-721.

22. Seikel, J. and Drumright, D. (2000). Anatomy and Physiology for speech, language and

hearing. 3rd ed. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group, Inc.

23. Shprintzen, R., McCall, G., Skolnick, M. and Lencione, R. (1975). Selective movement

of the lateral aspects of lateral aspects of the pharyngeal walls during velopharyngeal

closure for speech, blowing, and whistling in normal. Cleft Palate Journal, 12, pp.51-58.

24. Tachimura, T., Mori, C., Hirata, S. and Wada, T. (2000). Nasalance Score Variation in

Normal Adult Japanese Speakers of Mid-West Japanese Dialect. The Cleft Palate-

Craniofacial Journal, 37(5), pp.463-467.

25. Thompson, A. and Hixon, T. (1979). Nasal airflow during normal speech

production. Cleft Palate Journal, 16, pp.412-420.

26. Vallino-Napoli, L. and Montgomery, A. (1997). Examination of the Standard Deviation

of Mean Nasalance Scores in Subjects with Cleft Palate: Implications for Clinical

Use. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 34(6), pp.512-519.

27. van Doorn, J. and Purcell, A. (1998). Nasalance Levels in the Speech of Normal

Australian Children.The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 35(4), pp.287-292.

28. Zemlin, W. (1988). Speech & hearing science. Anatomy and physiology. 3rd ed.

NewJersy: Englewood Cliffs.

24

Recommended publications