University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn UCHC Graduate School Masters Theses 2003 - University of Connecticut Health Center Graduate 2010 School

6-1-2007 Psychiatric and Neuroscience Unite in the Pursuit of Reformulated Schizophrenia Nosologies Karyn Groth

Follow this and additional works at: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/uchcgs_masters

Recommended Citation Groth, Karyn, "Psychiatric Epidemiology and Neuroscience Unite in the Pursuit of Reformulated Schizophrenia Nosologies" (2007). UCHC Graduate School Masters Theses 2003 - 2010. 139. https://opencommons.uconn.edu/uchcgs_masters/139 Psychiatric Epidemiology and Neuroscience Unite in the Pursuit of Reformulated Schizophrenia Nosologies

Karyn Groth

B.A., Quinnipiac College, 1999

A Thesis

Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

Master of Public Health

at the

University of Connecticut

2007 APPROV AL PAGE

Master of Public Health Thesis

Psychiatric Epidemiology and Neuroscience Unite in Pursuit of Reformulated Schizophrenia Nosology

Presented by Karyn Groth, B.A.

Major Advisor __----"-~.J--_~_'_ ..... i--_Jvv-....._-_ .. _&_"V_A_S._:..-______Jonathan Covault

Associate Advisor --~~~~~~~~------

Associate Advisor ---4~~--~-----= __~------n

University of Connecticut

2006

11 Acknowledgements Page

Thanks to Jonathan Covault for his willingness to mentor a public health student and for the time and effort involved in the process.

Thanks to Howard Tennen for graciously serving as a reader and associate advisor in my time of need.

Special thanks to Vince Calhoun for guiding me through the statistical theory and hands on data analysis over the past year of this project.

Special thanks to Godfrey Pearlson for providing me several years of interest and education in schizophrenia and the foundation for formulating my ideas about the benefits of collaboration between epidemiology and neuroscience.

111 Table of Contents

Introduction ......

Chapter 1: Epidemiology of Schizophrenia ...... 3

Chapter 2: Diagnostic Nosology...... 9

Chapter 3: Psychiatric Epidemiology of Neuroanatomy...... 13

Chapter 4: Evidence of Structural Differences and their Clinical Correlates...... 18

Chapter 5: Altemative Subtyping ...... 29

Chapter 6: Intercorrelations between Brain Volumes...... 34

Chapter 7: Aims and Hypotheses ...... 37

Chapter 8: Data Analysis Methods...... 40

Chapter 9: Results ...... 45

Chapter 10: Discussion ...... 51

Appendices...... 59

References...... 66

IV Ab breviati ons

basal ganglia (BG) cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) computerized axial tomography (CT) gray matter (GM) heteromodal association cortex (HAS e) independent component analysis (ICA) inferior parietal lobule (IPL) magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) planum temporale (PT) positron emission tomography (PET) region of interest (ROl) superior temporal gyrus (STG) voxcl-based morphometry (VBM) white matter (WM)

v Introduction:

Schizophrenia (SZ) remains one of the least understood mental disorders despite extensive study. Efforts to better characterize the illness have led to advancements in nosology and classification of sUbtypes through clinical observation and research. The clinical manifestations of the illness are hypothesized to originate as abnormalities in brain structure and function which are in turn hypothesized to be the expressions of gene variants. Since the classification system used for diagnosis is based on clinical representation rather than pathophysiological etiology, subtyping by modern diagnostic criteria (e.g., Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders) may be incorrect, or at least inexact, which may obscure attempts to accurately differentiate population groups. Given the complex heterogeneity of SZ, improved nosology may contribute substantially to proper diagnosis.

The principles of psychiatric epidemiology are particularly useful when attempting to reconceptualize the nosology of because they provide information necessary to significantly improve the validity of the diagnostic systems.

The clinical course of schizophrenia has been well documented but epidemiological study of pathophysiological markers is lacking. Advancements in vivo during the past few decades have allowed for extensive study of the brain both structurally and functionally and support the role of distributed aberrations of brain morphology in the etiology and expression of schizophrenia. Psychiatric epidemiology plays a pivotal role in improving our understanding of the brain variations detected in individuals with schizophrenia. Specifically, epidemiological study of pathophysiological traits in large, representative populations will determine the existence of and rates for phenotypic subtypes of . Simultaneous assessment of clinical symptomology and cognitive expression with these pathophysiological traits will allow for multifactorial patterns to emerge, thereby bridging the gap between pathophysiology and clinical manifestation. Thus, psychiatric epidemiology lies at the forefront of improving the nosology of SZ and the many efforts which employ this nosology.

In light of the inherently physiological etiology of the disorder, I assert that there is great potential to reassess the diagnostic criteria and current subtyping systems using biological paradigms. In the final chapters, I apply a novel technique for identifYing natural patterns of structural variation in patients and discuss the potential for such research to detect more homogeneous subgroups which could lead to more valid diagnostic criteria.

2 CHAPTER 1: Epidemiology of Schizophrenia

Psychiatric epidemiology is the study of rates of mental illness and associated

factors which impact onset, course and outcome. It relies on the descriptive and analytic principles of epidemiology to provide substantive information that can be utilized in a

variety of clinical, research, public health, and community settings. The first psychiatric

epidemiology study was a census of the "insane" in the 1840 (1). Rudimentary compared

to the field today, the census was the first effort to define rates of mental illness in the

population. As a scientific discipline, psychiatric epidemiology differs from standard

in its emphasis on epidemiological and biostatistical concepts in the

classification and nosology of psychopathology rather than the diagnosis and treatment of

individuals.

One of the first to strongly embody epidemiological principles in the study of

mental illness was Emil Kraepelin. As part of his extensive work to classify mental

disorders into different categories, Kraepelin, in 1893, was the first to draw a distinction

between what he termed dementia praecox and other psychotic illnesses. He focused on

patterns in cause, symptomology, course, final stage, and pathological anatomical

findings when defining this diagnostic classification (2). In 1911, Eugen Bleuler coined

the term' schizophrenia' in response to the misleading use of the term dementia. By this

time, interest in the clinical syndrome had taken root. Between the late 1800's and mid-

20th century, epidemiological study of schizophrenia flourished. European researchers

studied large populations for incidence and prevalence of mental disorders with special

attention to heredity (3). Their extensive studies resulted in information on population

incidence, prevalence, age at onset curve, age- and sex-specific morbid risk, and

3 morbidity risks for biological relatives of a schizophrenic proband (3) which remain consistent with current estimates. Americans focused on the social ecology, or correlates and risk factors, of psychosis (3). Demographic characteristics, innate factors that could be either predisposing to psychosis or protective from it, and environmental factors, such as nurturing style, were three major areas of interest (4). Although sl ight variations in the concept and classification of SZ occurred over the past century, the epidemiology of the illness has remained remarkably consistent (5).

Clinical Phenotype

Schizophrenia is characterized by a variety of symptoms that are traditionally organized into two categories. Positive (psychotic) symptoms represent aberrations of mental processing such that normal functions are distorted or in excess (1). They include hallucinations, delusions, disorganized speech/thought, and disorganized/catatonic behavior. Negative symptoms refer to a diminution or loss of normal function and include alogia, affective blunting, and avolition (1). Although not explicitly defined as symptoms in the diagnostic criteria, cognitive deficits are a major component of the illness that underlie the impairment in social and occupational function that is required for the diagnosis of SZ. Patients with SZ, on average, experience impairments in neurocognition in the order of 1.5 to 2 standard deviations below normal controls on tests of language, executive function, memory, attention, and motor processing (6, 7).

Cognitive deficits are the most disabling symptoms of SZ in terms of leading a normal life (8).

With the heterogeneity of the symptoms in SZ, several subtypes are used to further characterize patients with the illness (1). Paranoid subtype refers to patients

4 whose cardinal features are delusions and auditory hallucinations. These psychotic symptoms are often paranoid and persecutory in nature. Paranoid is the most common subtype. Disorganized subtype, also termed Hebephrenic, refers to patients whose behavior is characterized by incoherent speech, behaviors inappropriate to the situation, or other behaviors that indicate confusion. Catatonic subtype refers to patients who are socially and emotionally withdrawn and have marked psychomotor disturbance. The

Residual subtype categorizes those patients who are no longer actively psychotic but still experience symptoms. Unditferentiated subtype serves as a catchall for patients who do not conform to the previous four subtypes or to patients who display features of more than one subtype without a clear predominance for a single type.

Incidence and Prevalence

Lifetime prevalence of schizophrenia generally has been cited at one percent of

the population. The most recently published prevalence rate for non-affective psychosis

over the lifetime in the United States was 5.0 per 1000 population (9). Other current

studies which utilized representative community samples in both the United States and

Israel estimated lifetime and 6-month prevalence rates at 0.7% (10, 11). Differences in

reported rates over time are hypothesized to reflect changes in diagnostic criteria rather

than a changes in disease prevalence. These include changes in the criteria for diagnosis

of SZ or differences in the methods used to ascertain or identify patients (4, 5). Incidence

rates are available from 16 countries sampled for the World Health Organization (WHO)

Determinants of Outcome of Severe Mental Disorders study and a review of European

countries by Jablensky (12). Both sources cite incidences of schizophrenia at

approximately 0.2 per 1000 population (4). While some communities may demonstrate

5 substantially higher prevalence rates for indigenous reasons, schizophrenia appears in similar rates throughout the world (5, 13).

Demography

Onset of schizophrenia usually occurs in late teens and early twenties, with some severe cases presenting with earlier onset in the mid-teens. Onset is preceded by a prodromal phase which lasts, on average, 5 years (14) during which less severe traits of the disorder are present. Onset for women occurs approximately 5 years later than for men (13). Differences in prevalence rates between the sexes are cited by some to be non­ existent (15) while others claim a 2-fold increase in males (13). Differences in perceived rates may be attributed to sex differences in expression of the disease. Females tend to show less severe psychopathology which may reduce occasions that require clinical contact (16). For example, females tend to have better premorbid functioning, less disability, and higher percentage of remitting course (17) although symptomology is very similar (15). As discussed earlier, 5Z appears to occur at comparable rates in many cultures and races (5, 13), however racial differences in cultural norms or racial disparities in healthcare access can impact diagnosis and treatment (10).

Heredity/Genetics

Early studies of mental illness recognized the tendency for psychosis to run in families (3). Contemporary studies of heritability demonstrate significant increases in rates of the illness as the amount of shared genetic information increases. Compared to the baseline risk of 1 % in the general population, 1st degree relatives (siblings) have a 9% risk of developing the illness and children of parents who both have schizophrenia (full genetic loading) show a risk of

6 about 46% of developing the illness (18). Twin studies provide further support in that fraternal (dyzygotic) twins of affected individuals show a rate of risk similar to other siblings while identical (monozygotic) twins show a risk of almost 50% of developing the illness if the co-twin is affected (19). Adoption studies demonstrate that the disease is more than a function of environment.

Schizophrenia twins or offspring of schizophrenic parents adopted away from the schizophrenic environment still show increased risk while children adopted

into schizophrenia families do not show increased risk for the disease (20).

Alternatively, the influence of environment is reinforced by the fact that identical

twins show at most a 50% risk if the co-twin is affected. Specifically, geneticists

have calculated that approximately 70-80% of the liability for schizophrenia is

genetic while 20-30% is environmental (21-23). While 70-80% of the liability is

estimated to come from genetics, the 20-30% of liability attributed to

environmental factors may translate into a much greater that 20-30% influence on

expression. The fact that individuals with a genetic liability for the disease are at

significantly increased risk compared to the general population but still have at

most a 50% chance supports the theory that what is inherited is a predisposition to

developing schizophrenia (21).

Social Burden

Marked by debilitating mental function, people with schizophrenia have long

periods of illness, are unable to work, and have difficulty in sustaining family

relationships (21, 24) causing major strain for those who care for them (24) and society in

7 general (13, 25, 26). Although patients vary in course and outcome, the majority require psychiatric, community, or social services for the duration of their lives (27). For instance, a review of the literature on course and outcome suggested that "21 % to 57% of individuals achieve a relatively good outcome, whereas others continue to have significant impairment" (28). This dependence on others combines with early onset and poor outcome to become the 2nd ranked mental illness for the global burden of disease based on 2.3 million years of potential life lost due to premature mortality or due to loss of productivity resulting from disability (25). Given the level of required care and treatment services and the chronicity of the illness, it is not surprising that the direct costs of schizophrenia exceed those of unipolar depression (29). In 1990, the direct cost of treating individuals with schizophrenia totaled 17.3 billion, or about 53.2% of the total

expenditures for hospitalization, treatment and rehabilitation, and lost productivity of

32.5 billion (29).

8 CHAPTER 2: Diagnostic Nosology

Kraepelin postulated that psychiatric diseases are principally caused by biological and genetic disorders. He believed that the natural patterns of the clinical syndromes could provide a provisional measure of validity of the disease concept until final verification could be achieved by establishing brain pathology and etiology (2, 3). Both Kraepelin and Bleuler emphasized that schizophrenia was a disorder in which a morbid fragmentation of cognitive processes developed unusually early in life (30). The terms" dementia praecox" and" schizophrenia", chosen separately by these scientists, reiterated this conceptualization. The severity and variety of symptoms associated with schizophrenia were not considered to be the defining traits. Positive symptoms, for example, were recognized to occur in a number of other psychiatric disorders mitigating any specificity to schizophrenia. Rather, Kraepelin "saw the diversity of symptoms as unified by the underlying destruction of cognitive processes" (31). Kraepelin's systematic investigations of the symptoms, course, outcome and correlates of schizophrenia were aimed at improving the conceptualization of the illness and compensated for limited capacities for pathophysiological study. His precocious use of the principles of psychiatric epidemiology were remarkably comparable to modern scientific psychiatry and his classifications served as the foundation for the modern nosological systems in use today (2, 3).

A marked divergence between Kraepelin's work and modern diagnostic systems is the attention to non-clinical factors. Coinciding with the increasing awareness of schizophrenia, the first half of the 20th century experienced a dramatic expansion in

9 psychiatric and medical research on an international level (3). Bourgeoning needs for a unified psychiatric nomenclature led to the development of the first Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual: Mental Disorders (DSM-I) in 1952 which focused on clinical utility rather than the coding of disease which was provided by the International Classification of Disease (1). In 1980, the third version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders was published. The DSM-III was developed to serve as a guideline for establishing universal and reliable psychiatric diagnoses using the manual's explicit diagnostic criteria, a multi-axial system, and descriptive approach (1). The nature of this classification system demanded that a particular psychiatric disorder be delineated by its observable behaviors or symptoms. The goal was to provide a reliable, clinical guideline for the field of psychiatry such that formally trained clinicians could use the manuals as a reference but not a finality when making diagnoses (2). As the versions of the DSM developed so did dependence on the manual for communication, education, and training.

The DSM became a textbook rather than a guideline and the skill of diagnosis was often replaced by a checklist of symptoms (32). This is particularly true with researchers in psychiatry, neuroscience, and epidemiology who applied the diagnostic criteria rigidly, often without formal clinical training. In reference to schizophrenia, conceptual shifts throughout the century refocused attentions toward the symptoms associated with the disorder, particularly positive symptoms, and were reflected in the DSM by concomitant shifts toward positive symptoms in the criteria for diagnosis (33, 34). Despite the fact that both Kraepelin and Bleuler emphasized that the disorder was not defined by clinical symptoms (31), current diagnostic classification systems use symptom constellations to diagnose schizophrenia.

10 Modem diagnostic classification systems may have sacrificed validity for the sake of reliability (35). Attempts over the last 100 years at finding a test, any test, which can give a deterministic diagnosis of SZ have been unsuccessful. It is quite possible that these failures are due, in part, to the invalidity of symptom-based assessments for diagnosis and the failure to incorporate the biological principles of schizophrenia in the study of the disease. The DSM-IV addresses the limitations of its categorical system, stating that "a categorical approach to the classification works best when all members of a diagnostic class are homogeneous, when there are clear boundaries between classes, and when the different classes are mutually exclusive" (I). In the case of SZ, none of these conditions is met. In tune with the modem conceptual shifts that emphasize biology and genetics in the development of psychiatric diseases, diagnostic classification systems that utilize a "definition of the phenotype ... that is heuristic for studying mechanisms of the illness" (31) may greatly improve both the validity and reliability of the diagnoses. The current diagnostic system with its dependence on descriptive psychopathology creates a divide between the goals of clinicians and the goals of researchers. For an effective transition of information between fields, clinicians and researchers need to utilize the same constructs.

Compilation of the original diagnostic manuals occurred prior to the emergence of extensive scientific study in neuroscience, pathophysiology, and genetics. Research over the past two decades in each of these fields has imparted unique contributions to conceptualization of the etiology and presentation of the illness. With advancements in science and technology, it is now possible to test whether schizophrenia, as postulated by Kraepelin, is an expression of altered brain pathology and to reformulate the

11 diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia to encompass the nature of the illness as a

fragmentation of the mind while still lending itself to both clinical application and

scientific study. Psychiatric epidemiology lies at the forefront of this endeavor. Before

an improved nosological system can be created, the foundation for the system must be

established. "The acceptance of a particular diagnostic concept or a classification scheme

is usually based on the interpretation of converging evidence from multiple sources,

including descriptive psychopathology, neuropathology, pathophysiology, genetics, and

epidemiology" (3). The study and treatment of SZ would benefit greatly from an

itillovative epidemiological perspective which seeks to improve upon current diagnostic

practice in each of these fields.

12 CHAPTER 3: Psychiatric Epidemiology ofNeuroanatomy

One of the most defining characteristics of schizophrenia as a disease concept is its complexity, both clinically and pathophysiologically. Interest in the illness has incited extensive academic pursuit evidenced by a overabundance of research reports over the past century. Based on these reports and clinicians' experience, several theories regarding the etiology of schizophrenia have evolved, often in parallel with secular trends

and scientific/technological advancements. The 1990's, referred to as the 'decade of the brain', experienced substantial growth in in-vivo imaging as technological improvements

in computerized axial tomography (CT), positron emission tomography (PET), and

magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) combined with researchers' increased access to

scanners. Psychiatric researchers, previously constrained by the limitations of post­

mortem assessments, now had opportunities to explore both structure and function in the

living brain. Several theories about the etiology of schizophrenia as

neuroanatomical in nature have been supported by these research findings. "Evidences

from these studies have strongly demonstrated that schizophrenia is a chronic

brain disorder, structurally and functionally affecting various cortical and

subcortical regions involved in cognitive, emotional, and motivational aspects of

human behavior" (36). As of yet, however, no single abnormality of brain

structure has been found to be 'pathognomonic' for SZ (37).

Two theories have prominently addressed the role of brain anatomy 111 the

development of the illness. Andreasen's "cognitive dysmetria" theory proposes that the

root of the disorder is a disruption of neuronal connectivity of the prefrontal-thalamic­

cerebellar circuitry which evokes the clinical manifestations (38, 39). Pearlson's

13 "heteromodal association cortex" theory asserts that schizophrenia is caused by aberrant network function due to structural impairments to brain regions that are uniquely

'human'. such as the heteromodal association cortex (HASe) (40). Both theories recognize that schizophrenia is not the result of a single focal abnormality nor does it appear to be the result of pathological neural markers like those detected in Alzheimer's disease (plaques, tangles) (31, 41). The evidence of widespread, distributed anatomical aberrations supports the perspective that SZ results from disruption in neural circuitry, either anatomically or neurochemically (31, 37).

Applying the principles of epidemiology to the study of psychiatric neuroscience creates many opportunities for innovative explorations of brain-behavior phenotypes.

Epidemiology is a propitious next step in schizophrenia research espoused by several researchers in the field. Excerpts from two review articles are presented here:

"In the 20th century the range of methodological and analytical tools available in psychiatric epidemiology was enormously expanded (Jablensky, this volume). Epidemiological research into the frequency, distribution and course of abnormal brain morphology (Jones et al. 1994), already underway, and the dawning epidemiology of functional anomalies and brain processes will be developed further on the neurobiological and psychological level" (42).

liThe relationship between schizophrenia epidemiology and molecular genetics is considered to be one of incremental mutual support; the power of molecular genetic studies depends critically on the ability of epidemiological work to delineate homogeneous subgroups of the disorder in whom the share of genetic risk is assumed to be particularly large" (43).

Specifically, two prime uncertainties can be addressed with the epidemiological study of neuroanatomy. One uncertainty addresses the conceptualization of the illness

14 itself. The heterogeneity in the expression of the illness has fueled long-standing doubts about the diagnosis of SZ representing a single disease entity or a mixture of various disease processes (35, 44, 45). Studying variations in clinical and anatomical phenotypes and their relationships with each other may reveal how aberrations in neuroanatomy manifest clinically. Jointly characterizing multiple phenotypic presentations may uncover the commonalities and differences of various subgroups of patients which provide insight into the validity of current subtypes and the disease process overall.

Identifying both unique and common etiologies of brain structure and function is one promising approach to determining whether schizophrenia is a single illness with a broad spectrum or an amalgamation of several illnesses into one diagnosis. Thus, large scale studies of both clinical expression and pathophysiology using epidemiological principles are vital to the development of a revised taxonomy.

The second uncertainty is whether neuroanatomy can be so well characterized that an individual can be reliably and validly diagnosed with schizophrenia based on neuroanatomical measurement. Variability in affected brain areas as well as variability in the severity of the morphological aberration can obscure attempts to discriminate patients from controls based on brain structure. In addition, aberrations in morphology occur along a continuum that overlaps considerably with normal controls and diminishes the ability to define 'abnormal'. These two inherent qualities of SZ continue to impede efforts to define distinct phenotypes. Scientific methodologies also have hindered efforts to distinguish anatomical phenotypes. Many research reports in psychiatric neuroscience employ small sample sizes (N<40) which undermine the success for detecting meaningful differences. Specifically, smaller sample sizes increase the likelihood for

15 type II errors, especially with the high variability in brain morphology in patients but not control subjects. Despite the fact that neuroanatomy is theorized to play an integral role in the etiology of SZ, few studies have assessed the ability for neuroanatomical measures to predict diagnosis and/or outcomes (28) perhaps for the reasons discussed here. The collection of large scale neuroanatomical studies that embody the principles of epidemiology may achieve new heights in the characterization of neuroanatomy on an individual level and foster interest in its predictive potential.

Several epidemiological principles, in particular, have been lacking in the majority of modem research endeavors. First, as discussed above, small sample sizes inherently weaken the likelihood of detecting differences between patients and controls, limit the potential for detecting more uniform subgroups of patients, and limit the generalizability of the results. Large-scale studies, in the order of 100 or more per diagnostic group, can substantially reduce these limitations (see (46-48) for examples).

Second, neuroimaging studies often require the comparison group to be both physically and mentally 'healthy' in order to reduce confounds and increase the likelihood for detecting differences. A population study of brain anatomy using community controls, not 'healthy' research controls, will provide a normative and generalizable characterization of brain morphology. The inclusion of other mental illnesses in the development of nomlative measures will allow for the emergence of aberrations that are specific to SZ and not to mental illness. Third, associations between patients' presentation and course and the patient's brain morphology are rarely tested. Exceptions to this are the growing number of researchers who attempt to correlate symptomology and/or neurocognitive performances with neuroanatomical measures. Patients' course

16 and outcome are intrinsic qualities of the illness which necessitate increased attention.

The development of hypotheses a priori and the application of more advanced statistical tests (which will also be feasible with larger sample sizes) will help researchers capitalize on these variables. To recapitulate, "closer links between epidemiology and structural neuroimaging ... will enable us to further define structural abnormalities associated with each [psychiatric] disorder and their specificity" (49).

17 CHAPTER 4: Evidence ofStructural Differences and their Clinical Correlates

Identification of structural abnormalities in SZ has been a primary area of interest since the increased accessibility of in vivo assessment techniques. Reports of volumetric differences between patients and controls have been reported for most parts of the brain

although this literature has been pray to inconsistent reports regarding the location and

direction of volumetric differences. Several reviews attempt to summarize the vast

literature on this topic and I refer you to them for a full understanding of the reported

structural aberrations in SZ (see (37, 50) for reviews). In this chapter, I will comment on

the most consistent and relevant findings.

Chronicling the morphometric differences between healthy control subjects and

persons with schizophrenia provides insight into where disruptions in normal functioning

may occur. Associating a brain structure with an observable behavior creates a

conceptual bridge that is important to cross when attempting to determine the role of

structure in clinical expression and in the definition of subtypes. Evaluation of the

existence of morphology-symptom and morphology-cognition associations increases our

understanding of how numerous brain regions may synergize to create the heterogeneous

clinical expression of SZ. "It is unclear if there are subgroups of SZ with distinct clusters

of particular volume abnormalities that involve discrete neuroanatomic circuits" (37).

Several morphometric studies of brain structure have found significant associations

between regional brain volumes and severity of symptoms and cognitive performances.

Relevant studies are included in the discussion of structural differences.

For the purposes of this thesis, I will limit the scope of structural abnormalities to

brain volumes. Structural assessments that involve shape, connectivity (e.g. diffusion

18 tensor imaging), or occur at a cellular level are beyond the purvIew of this thesis.

Volumetric structural measurements will predominately come from MRI assessments.

Differences between volumetric measurement tools, such as region of interest (ROI) vs. voxel-based morphometry (VBM), can introduce variability in findings. I give equal credit to reports using either methodology, however, for direct comparison of our findings to previous work, I will focus on prior VBM studies in the final chapters.

Global Volumes

Global brain volumes can be divided into 4 categories: whole brain, gray matter

(GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Assessments of whole brain size in schizophrenia have not typically found significant size differences between patients and controls (51) but global GM has been found to be reduced in patients in the order of 2-4% (52, 53). Literature reviews that included WM have reported tendencies for both decreases (32) and increases (52). White matter increases may occur in proportion with GM decreases (52) offsetting any whole brain volume loss in patients with GM decreases. The ratio of gray to white matter within the brain has been suggested to be of interest, although no major findings have been repOlied. Pearlson and

Marsh (1999) suggest that global volume reductions such as those seen with GM do not always reach statistical significance despite focal deficits of 10%-15% (37).

Overall, global measures of brain volume have not been a fruitful area of study in schizophrenia research with the exception of cognitive function. Whole brain volume has a non-specific association with cognitive performances in both patients and controls (6,

54-56). For example, Antonova (2005) reported smaller global GM volume correlated with lower premorbid IQ in patients but also in controls. Kareken (1995) reported that

19 deficit but not non-deficit patients had reduced whole brain volumes compared to controls. Since cognitive impairments are more severe in deficit patients, brain volume deficits may be a key element to understanding the clinical manifestations of select subgroups of patients.

Ventricle,')-

Ventricular enlargement is a highly consistent finding in SZ. The ventricles are sub-divided into lateral, third, and fourth. The lateral ventricles are further defined with a frontal hom, body, occipital hom, and temporal hom. Lateral ventricular enlargement, especially in the temporal hom, has been well documented, as well as enlargement of the third ventricle (50) while the fourth ventricle has not shown enlargement in patients.

Pearlson and Marsh (1999) remark that it is difficult to determine if increased ventricular

size is itself an anatomic abnormality or if it is a proxy for changes in neighboring brain

structures (37). Enlargement of the temporal horns may reflect GM reductions in the

temporal gyri (50) and enlargement of the third ventricle may relate to the reported

reductions in thalamic volume (57). It is important to note that despite the consistency of

enlarged ventricles in SZ, ventricular enlargement is commonly found in a variety of

other psychiatric and non-psychiatric conditions (37), thereby minimizing the

contribution of this abnormality to the isolation of schizophrenia's etiology. A recent

study of the heritability of ventricular size suggested that genetics have a stronger

influence over ventricle size than schizophrenia-related changes (58) further supporting

an indirect association between ventricle size and schizophrenia.

Studies testing the correlations between cognition and ventricular size have failed

to find consistent associations in patients, although studies utilizing ventricular-to-brain

20 volume ratios have been more reliable (6). Enlargement of ventricles may be a non­ specific indicator of decreased brain tissue for which more localized assessments of tissue volume correlate better with cognitive measures.

Temporal lobe

The temporal lobe is comprised of 1) the superior temporal gyrus which includes

the primary (Heschl's gyrus) and secondary auditory cortices and the planum

temporale, 2) the middle and inferior temporal gyri, and 3) the medial temporal

structures of the limbic system. Smaller volume in the superior temporal gyrus (STG)

of the temporal lobe is the most robust finding for a focal region in SZ research (59) and

is principally GM in origin (50). Volume loss in the STG is also highly specific to SZ

unlike the ventricular volumes noted above (37). Reports for volume changes in the

middle or inferior temporal gyri are less consistent (60,61).

The role of the temporal lobe in processing sensory inputs, speech and

language has incited extensive interest in the relationship between temporal

structure and function and the existence of auditory hallucinations, delusions,

and impaired language(61). Numerous studies have studied the associations

between structure and clinical symptoms with promising results. For positive

symptoms, GM volume loss in the STG, in particular, has correlated with

hallucinations (28, 62-64) and formal thought disorder (60, 64-66) but not with

delusions (28). Negative symptoms have also correlated with temporal lobe

volume loss (67, 68). Neuropsychological performance in a variety of cognitive

domains have positively correlated with temporal or STG volume in patients (65,

21 69-72) although some studies have not shown associations (6, 73).

The Planum Temporale (PT) is a discrete region of the posterior STG which is believed to be the "neural substrate of language" (50, 74). The PT displays a discernible left greater than right asymmetry in the normal population that is diminished in patients with schizophrenia (75-80). The disrupted asymmetry is attributed to enlarged right PT volumes and/or left PT reductions (77, 78, 81, 82). Pearlson (1996, 1997) suggests that the PT is a primary ROI in the structural basis of SZ (40, 61) for which anatomical aberrations have yet to be identified in any other psychiatric disorder (40, 50). Although the PT is a portion of the STG which has been heavily correlated with symptomology, the

PT as a discrete sub region of the STG has also correlated with thought disorder (65, 66,

82-84) and higher scores on the suspiciousness/persecution subscale of the Positive and

Negative Symptom Scale (78).

Medial Temporal Lobe

The medial temporal structures are the amygdala, hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus. These structures, also known as the limbic system, mediate emotion and memory and are implicated as aberrant in SZ based on the observation that memory is one of the most marked impairments exhibited in schizophrenia (6).

Early post-mortem studies reported tissue loss in these medial regions (50) followed by MRI studies reporting reductions in the amygdala-hippocampal complex

(83), amygdala alone (53, 69, 85), hippocampus alone (53, 69, 85), and parahippocampal gyrus (53) in SZ both in chronic and first episode patients (36, 59, 68). Volume in limbic structures have been positively correlated with thought disorder (83), symptom severity

(37), and negative symptoms (68). Results of studies relating performance on memory-

22 related tasks to medial temporal lobe volumes in patients have been mixed: several report positive correlations (70, 86-88) while others report no association (86, 87, 89) depending on the structure and the memory task being assessed. No studies that assessed medial temporal volumes and cognition found an association between parahippocampal gyrus volume and cognition in controls suggesting a specificity of the parahippocampal gyrus to cognitive functions in SZ (6).

Frontal lobe

Frontal lobe dysfunction is considered to be an important cause of the specific abnormalities of cognition and behavior that are commonly seen in SZ (6, 38, 40).

Conventionally divided into the superior, middle, inferior, medial, and orbital gyri, the frontal cortex is highly connected with every other cortical structure in the brain (90).

The frontal cortex is also a significant part of neural circuitry and a number of cortical-subcortical networks evidenced by reciprocal connections to all other areas of the cortex, as well as to limbic and basal ganglia structures (91, 92).

While reports using whole lobe volume measurements have been inconsistent, recent parcellation studies of prefrontal subregions have reported specific volume reductions in discrete areas including the inferior, middle, and orbital gyri, and dorsolateral and dorsomedial regions (6, 36, 50, 59). Frontal lobe volumes have correlated with both positive (62) and negative symptoms (68, 93) but not as robustly as the temporal lobe.

On the other hand, impaired cognitive performance and frontal lobe reductions have been well documented. Irrespective of volumetric differences between patients and controls, several studies have reported significant relationships between volumes in the frontal lobe and performance on cognitive measures-particularly on tasks that engage

23 the frontal cortex (executive functioning, working memory, verbal fluency, immediate memory) (6, 7, 3 L 54, 59, 70, 94). Variability in the existence of and associations between cognitive deficits in frontal function and volumetric deficits in the prefrontal cortex indicate that abnormalities in other brain regions may impact frontal functions (6) and that there may be select subgroups of patients who present with disruptions in either neuronal circuitry or anatomy.

Parietal lobe

The parietal lobe is comprised of the post-central gyrus, superior parietal lobule, and inferior parietal lobule. Although the parietal lobe has not been a major ROI in investigating volume abnormalities in SZ, the supramarginal and angular gyri that constitute the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) are critical parts of the heteromodal association cortex (40, 92, 95). Located close to the PT, the IPL is hypothesized to be involved with language development and comprehension (40, 91, 95). The IPL is also implicated in visuospatial processing, visual working memory, and attention (50).

Similar to the PT, the IPL shows a left greater than right asymmetry in normal subjects which is important for normal language development (50, 96). Of the few studies that have assessed subregions of the parietal lobe in schizophrenia, volume or GM density reductions were reported for the supramarginal gyrus (85, 97) and IPL (98, 99). Further, there was reported reversal of the left greater than right asymmetry in the angular gyrus in males and in the supramarginal gyrus (50, 100, 101).

Exploration of the association between symptoms or cognition in SZ and parietal lobe size is minimal. Sullivan (1996) reported no significant differences in parietal or parietal-occipital volumes between male patients and controls nor did they find

24 significant correlations between parietal volumes and cognitive functioning in 4 domains

(executive, verbal, memory, motor). Antonova (2004) suggests that the global

measurement of the parietal region may have negated the detection of associations with

cognitive functions given the differences in functionality for subregions of the parietal

and occipital region. Bilateral parietal lobe volume reductions were found to be

associated with the auditory oddball fMRI task in schizophrenia, using joint independent

component analysis (102).

Cerebellum

The cerebellum has been implicated as a ROI in SZ because of its role in a variety

of motor tasks (neuromotor function is often irregular in SZ), connectivity with cortical

and limbic structures, and role in cognition (30, 38, 50). Decreased cerebellar volumes in

patients compared to controls are typically found (38, 73, 99, 103); increased volumes

(104) but the reports of correlations to symptoms are lacking. Bottmer (2005) found

smaller cerebellar volumes in patients inversely correlated with soft signs but showed no

relation to clinical course (105) while Szeszko (2003) reported a lack of association

between cerebellar volume and overall cognition in patients that was evident in healthy

controls (106). Smaller cerebellar volumes were associated with neuromotor

abnormalities which were in turn (but not directly) associated with poorer cognition and

more severe symptomology (107). Associations of cerebellar volumes with cognitive

functions were found for controls and affected women but not affected men (6). Further

research is needed to clarify the associations of cerebellar structure with clinical

presentations.

Thalamus

25 Known for its role as a major relay station in the brain, the thalamus is responsible for much of the communication between cortical areas by modulating the incoming sensory information and outputting information to other cortical areas. It has been implicated specifically in attention, information processing, and gating (50) and has reciprocal connections with a variety of brain structures as evidenced by the delineation of several cortical circuits that involve the thalamus (108-112). Thalamic explorations

have been thwarted by the difficulties in reliable delineation of the structure using MRI.

While several groups have reported decreased thalamic volumes, others have reported no

differences between diagnostic groups. Even with improvements in imaging technology

and assessment/quantification methods (113), recent studies continued to report mixed

findings regarding volume reductions in patients (73, 99, 114-116) but often report

correlations with other regional brain volumes (57, 113, 117), symptoms (118, 119), and

illness course (120-123).

Basal Ganglia

The Basal Ganglia (BG) is comprised of the caudate, putamen, and globus

pallidus. The caudate and putamen are often referred to as the striatum. Each structure

of the basal ganglia is differentially involved in cognitive, sensory, and motor processing

(124). Volumetric studies of the BG via MRI have typically reported increases (125,

126) although there is growing evidence for the confounding of BO size from atypical

neuroleptics (6, 127, 128). In studies of drug-nai"ve patients, caudate volumes were found

to be no different (128) or decreased (50, 127) compared with controls.

A handful of studies have explored associations between BG volumes and clinical

manifestations. Sigmundsson (2001) measured numerous brain regions in patients with

26 enduring negative symptoms and reported that these patients had increased BG GM volume compared to controls which positively correlated with positive symptoms.

Buchanan (1993) reported a trend for deficit patients to have larger right caudate volumes

(118). Spinks (2005) reported that volume of the extemal segment of the globus pallidus was correlated with severity of global symptoms although there were no volumes differences between patients and controls (129). Two studies reported an inverse relationship between volumes in BG structures and cognitive flexibility/abstraction (123,

130).

It is important to note that there are a number of factors which can confound, or at

least complicate, detection of structural differences in schizophrenia, such as sex, age at

assessment, age at onset, course, medication, treatment regimens, diagnostic criteria, and

comorbidities (36). Although the majority of studies comparing patients to controls

attempt to match for age and sex on a group level, the potential for additional

confounders creates the need for continued study of brain structures, preferably with

larger samples sizes and well characterized patient groups. One of the largest obstacles

in studying correlates of schizophrenia is that the range of a particular construct usually

overlaps considerably with the normal population (37) which prevents the ability to

demarcate a point or line for abnormality. Improved characterization of brain anatomy in

normal population groups can help define size, shape, variability, patterns and

intercorrelations of brain regions.

There are several limitations ofthe studies associating cognition to brain volumes.

One is the variability in measures and interpretation of measures used to assess cognitive

27 domains, as well as the lack of attention to construct validity (6). Another is equating the level of difficulty to that of controls; it can be difficult to determine whether poorer performance is the result of smaller volumes, overall impaired neuronal networks and circuitry, or inequality in cognitive load (6).

28 CHAPTER S: Alternative SUD(J!ping

Past efforts to define subgroups of schizophrenia that do not conform to the criteria set forth in the DSM-IV have been fueled by salient issues. Subgrouping patients based on neuropsychological function (131-133), familial or sporadic (134, 135), course and outcome (136, 137) or symptom presentation (135, 138-141) strive to identify homogeneous subpopulations that may be more conducive to exploration of biological etiologies. Second, these subgroups may address lingering concerns over the validity of the current diagnostic subtypes and SZ as a single unified pathological process. In addition, sub setting patients based on positive responses to certain medications and exploring associated factors may improve treatment regimens (142), essentially using evidence based practices. However, alternate subtyping in SZ is defunct if no additional information can be gleaned from the revised classification. Although there are a number of concerns over the validity of current diagnostic subtypes and there have been a number of attempts to define alternative subtypes, only a few have been examined for biological or phenotypic correlates (34).

Leonhard's classification of SZ subtypes is one alternative that has been studied for a variety of disease correlates. Leonhard subdivided schizophrenia into three subtypes based on different types of symptomology, long-tern1 course, and clinical outcome: systematic schizophrenia, unsystematic schizophrenia, and cycloid psychosis

(36). Several researchers have utilized these subtypes in their studies with promising results. Systematic SZ had significantly more maternal gestational infections during pregnancy and significantly more obstetric complications than unsystematic SZ or controls (143). Differences in P300 topographies and latencies between Leonhard's three

29 subtypes have been reported (36). Genetic studies have reported different genetic backgrounds between the subtypes; for example, systematic catatonia is usually sporadic not familial (144). A recent MRI study that used both DSM-IV and Leonhard's classifications reported significant aberrations in brain morphology for Leonhard's systematic SZ compared with controls but not for non-systematic forms or for any of the

DSM-IV subtypes (145). Thus, alternative subtypes show promise for detecting associations with a variety of physiological correlates that have not been accomplished using DSM-IV subtype classitications.

There is also great potential for biological endeavors to suggest more homogenous phenotypes. Reporting that a substantial subgroup of patients showed ongoing cerebral degeneration, Knoll proposed that a degenerative type of SZ may exist and that this subgroup is similar to the intellectually deteriorated subgroup of patients defined using neuropsychological testing (36). Pearlson and Marsh (1999) also acknowledge that cerebral degeneration may be limited to a subset of patients, notably the Kraepelinian subset (37). Sponheim (2001, 2003) explored whether biological measures known to be deviant in psychosis could discriminate patients from controls

(146, 147). Sponheim et al. measured ocular motor functioning, electroencephalogram frequency characteristics, nail fold plexus visibility, and electrodermal activations in 5 study groups: patients with SZ, 1st degree relatives of patient with SZ, patients with affective disorder, 1st degree relatives of patients with affective disorder, and non­ psychiatric control subjects (146). The authors compared the sensitivity and specificity of each measure in differentiating diagnostic group pairs. Three out of four of the biological measures significantly differentiated SZ patients from non-psychiatric controls. One of

30 these measures also significantly differentiated SZ patients from atlective patients.

Although Sponheim's predictive values werc modest, the potential to tap the predictive ability of biological phenotypes has been considered. Additional statistical methods such as principal components, principal factors and cluster analyses may also advance the delineation of biological subtypes. Future studies should employ more efforts to capitalize on biological indices known to be affected in psychosis.

Beno/its and LimilatiOllS qf Sub(ypillg Structural Dilforences and their Cililical

COITelates

Patterns of brain morphology are biological markers, or endophenotypes, which

are traits that covary with an illness without being causative agents. Brain morphology

represents a halfway point between the genetic/environmental etiologies and the varied

clinical presentations (148). As such, these markers are likely to associate with both

etiology and expression thereby bridging the gap between cause and effect.

"Th[e] clinically observed presentation of schizophrenia may not in fact represent the true phenotype, since it is comprised of varied symptoms that derive from diverse systems, occurs in non-overlapping patterns, and does not breed true. Instead, the phenotype may perhaps be best defined by a process that lies behind the symptoms, or a metaprocess, sometimes also referred to as an endophenotype, which is a 'final common pathway' that defines the illness." (31)

The use of brain endophenotypes as markers for schizophrenia has a number of

advantages in the search for schizophrenia etiologies. Brain structure is more tangible

and less state dependent than clinical symptomology allowing for bettcr quantification

and classification of affliction. Brain structure is measurable for the entire popUlation,

allowing for mapping of risk across the general population and the potential for

identifying de novo/isolated incidences of schizophrenia (21). Discrete structural

31 aberrations may produce specific clinical manifestations which can lead to pharmacological treatments designed to target the specific abnormalities as well as an improved understanding of the relationship between brain structure and function. There is the potential for data-driven techniques, such as factor or cluster analyses, to provide a starting point for exploring whether brain patterns do exist and how they relate to clinical presentation. Two main limitations common to SZ research still remain. Brain sizes and shapes occur on a continuum with no set point of abnormality. Any demarcation made by researchers would, at this time, be arbitrary. Anatomical structure varies extensively from patient to patient (more so that in the general population) with some patients expressing gross morphological abnormalities. Variability in morphology of numerous structures complicates the discrimination of structures that impact the illness from structures that do not impact the disease process.

Previous Attempts to D¢ne Stmctural Subtypes

Previous attempts to define structural subtypes have been largely restricted to one over arching construct-outcome. Poor outcome has been associated with ventricular enlargement (120,149-151), general GM loss (120,152), and GM volume reductions in the posterior cingulate (151), retrosplenial cortices (151), cerebellum (153), frontal lobe

(120), temporal lobe (28, 152), and thalamus (113). Although many of these studies use a correlational design rather than a comparison of good and poor outcome groups, the association of poor outcome with numerous structures suggests that a dichotomization between outcome groups may successfully associate with brain anatomy.

Deficit symptoms and Kraepelinian subtypes are highly predictive of clinical features of poor outcome (136). It is possible that the "more enduring" nature of deficit

32 symptoms which lead to poorer outcomes increases the likelihood of an association with pathophysiology compared with the more state-dependent positive symptoms (37, 154).

Comparing patients using a Kraepelinian/non-Kraepelinian dichotomy in a series of neuroanatomical reports, Mitelman has detected differences in GM volumes and III patterns of intra-cortical volume correlations in the Kraepelinian subtype compared with the non-Kraepelinian and controls (117, 151, 152, 155, 156) indicating that subtyping by outcome is more discriminatory of structural phenotypes than traditional DSM subtyping

(145).

33 CHAPTER 6: Intercorre/utions between Bruin I/o/limes

Exploration of the role of brain structure and volume in SZ usually "follow a traditional "lesion' model in which changes in one anatomic structure are postulated to lead to change in function, symptoms, or outcome ...." (28). A major limitation of this approach is the failure to incorporate the dynamic relationships between multiple brain regions (28). Theories regarding the etiology of SZ have emphasized the disruption of brain networks (38, 40, 110, 124, 157) and recognize that no one region is responsible for the illness. Considering there are numerous neural and cognitive networks in the human brain, the progression from exploring static groups differences to exploring intercorrelations between brain structures within groups is natural. A handful of studies have focused specifically on detecting patterns of structural variation in SZ with varying success (100, 117, 155, 156, 158-160).

Considering the 'extent and complexity' of the cortical connections between the prefrontal and the temporal lobe structures, Wible (1995) proposed that the variability in significant findings for either region may result from the primary dysfunction of the other region (158). Selecting a small group of patients (N=15) already shown to have volume deficits in left temporal lobe regions, Wible reported no significant differences in prefrontal GM volumes between these patients and matched controls. When entered into a regression analysis, the authors found that of all the temporal lobe structures measured, only the left anterior hippocampal-amygdala complex volume accounted for a significant portion of the variance of the left prefrontal cortex (Rsq = .47) and that this association was not evident in controls. Although the study sample was relatively small, an inter­ correlation in patients but not controls between two brain regions that are of primary

34 interest III SZ suggests that patients may expenence a dynamic disruption III brain morphology that has previously been undetected.

The heteromodal association cortex (HASC) is a network of higher-order neural circuits, which mediate complex cognitive tasks such as working memory, language, and attention (161). Primary HASC regions include the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,

Broca's motor speech region, the STG and PT, and the inferior parietal lobule (40).

Abnormalities in the anatomy of primary HASC regions or disruptions in functional connectivity may underlie the abnormalities of speech, language, and perception, as well as the abnormalities in social and occupational functioning that are cardinal features of schizophrenia (40, 161, 162). To explore the pattern of correlations among heteromodal regions, Buchanan (2004) quantified the GM volumes of the STG, supramarginal and angular gyri, and prefrontal volumes parcellated into four ROIs (100). Using Pearson's partial correlations with each pairwise correlation adjusted for all other volumes considered, the authors reported 7 significant correlations between HASC regions that were similar in patients and controls. In contrast, patients with SZ demonstrated a significant positive association between inferior prefrontal and angular volumes and between supramarginal and angular volumes in the left hemisphere while controls exhibited an inverse association. Although 42 comparisons were made, only 9 intercorrelations were significant in either patients or controls suggesting that volumes of the heteromodal regions may be generally unrelated or that Buchanan's methodology was unable to detect such associations. Buchanan's findings are partially consistent with

Niznikiewicz (2000) finding that volumes of the inferior parietal cortices correlated with several prefrontal regions in patients but not in controls (98).

35 In a series of recent reports, Mitelman (117, 155. 156) investigated the potential associations between 39 regional brain volumes in 106 patients with schizophrenia and

42 normal controls. These studies constitute the largest and most extensive assessments of volumetric intercorrelations to date. Given the exhaustive detail in each of these reports, a summarization of the exact findings would still be lengthy. In general, patients showed an "abundance" of stronger than normal positive correlations among temporal, frontal and occipital regions as well as a weakening of the inverse correlations between the frontal and temporal regions (155). The fact that Mitelman identified numerous correlations between Brodmann's areas (the majority of which were positive) indicates that widespread volumetric change in the same direction usually occurs in both patients and controls. The presence of differences in the pattern of correlations between patients and controls supports the theory of disrupted cerebral networks in SZ. Mitelman also comments that "anatomical connection between two regions is not a sufticient condition for significant inter-correlation of their volumes.... [Instead] the connection must be strong enough or an additional superimposed process, such as consistently coordinated use of the areas, must be at play" (156). Considering that SZ is theorized to result from aberrant connectivity, patterns of regional volumes in patients that deviate from normal controls might implicate alternative 'altered' neural networks that either contribute to the expression of the disease or strive to compensate for disrupted neural structures.

Investigating the patterns of regional variation is a necessary step in understanding the etiology of SZ.

36 CHAPTER 7: AIMSond HYPOTHESES

Independent component analysis (ICA) IS a statistical approach that has the potential to identify brain patterns that may differentiate intrinsic subtypes. ICA was developed to solve problems similar to the "cocktail party" problem (163) in which multiple conversations can obscure attempts to isolate a single conversation. The ICA algorithm, assuming independence in time or space, can separate mixed signals (noise)

into individual sources (voices). Tn reference to structural imaging, ICA allows for the

decomposition of brain areas into those regions that vary together in GM volume from

individual to individual (102, 163-165). Each brain is transformed from a three

dimensional plane into a two dimensional vector and the vectors for each subject are

arrayed into a matrix. An algorithm is then applied to the matrix to detect elements that

vary together across subjects (commonalities between vectors) and across the brain areas

(commonalities between arrays).

Although this statistical approach is similar to well-known factor analytic

approaches (e.g., principle components analysis), a major difference between TCA and

other factor analyses is the statistical theory used to detect patterns. The goal of lCA is to

detect independence (166). Traditional factor analyses are based on an assumption that

the data under study are normally (gaussian) distributed. In gaussian distributions,

uncorrelated components are by definition independent so the factors can be extracted

successfully. When data are not normally distributed (as traditional methods assume), the

assumptions of the model are not met and the results become suspect. ICA detects

independent factors with data that are non-gaussIan usmg alternative mathematical

assumptions. "ICA could be considered as nongaussian factor analysis, since in factor

37 analysis, we are also modeling the data as linear mixtures of some underlying factors"

(166). I refer you to a technical paper by Hyvarinen (166) for details of the estimation process and the algorithm used to decompose the matrix into independent components.

In addition, ICA requires no a priori knowledge of how regions vary between individuals. Thus, using rCA to explore variations in GM segmentation images provides a unique opportunity to identify natural groupings of regions and further the conceptualization of our anatomical network.

The use of alternative diagnostic paradigms is a powerful tool for studying this specific population but, as of yet, no attempts have been made to subtype patients based on brain morphology. Brain anatomy has been explored well enough to conclude that no one aberration lies at the root of the illness (167) and that the brain anatomy in patients is as varied as the manifestations. Considering the hypothesized role of brain anatomy in the etiology of the disease, subtyping patients based on anatomic phenotypes may prove to be more successful for identifying valid subtypes and etiologies. Such a method provides two meaningful benefits to current practice. First, the subtypes are defined by the variation in the population rather than by a somewhat arbitrary clinical definition.

Second, since aberrations in physiology are hypothesized to underlie the expression of symptoms, an association of structural subtypes to clinical features may improve the accuracy and validity of a subtype definition. Improved validity can impact both clinical and research endeavors. Furthermore, mapping of structural subtypes using larger study groups can provide information about each of the goals of psychiatric epidemiology and eventually public health's ultimate goal of prevention.

The HASC hypothesis of schizophrenia postulates that the regions involved in the

38 HASC and their interconnections are disproportionately disrupted (40). This hypothesis has gathered support from both structural (discussed previously) and functional data (40).

Based on previous work by Pearlson and colleagues, I predicted that the regions of greatest distinction between patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls will predominately involve the primary areas of the heteromodal association cortex.

An overarching goal of anatomical research in schizophrenia is to characterize structural variations so brain morphology can be used in tandem with current clinical methods to properly diagnose patients and to reveal underlying pathophysiology. As noted by Milev (2003), "in comparison with the abundance of outcome studies using demographic and psychopathologic variables, the number of studies that address the predictive potential of neuroanatomical measures is [surprisingly] small" (28). Although

ICA summarizes the anatomical relationships in the dataset, the subsequent application of logistic regression to the independent components extends our knowledge further by assessing which components can best differentiate patients with schizophrenia from healthy controls. The combination of these two strategies provides a useful way to increase understanding of how structural changes in anatomical networks may contribute to the disease.

39 CHAPTER B: Doto Anolysis Methods

Sub/eels.'

One hundred and thirty three patients with chronic schizophrenia (53 females, mean age = 41.6, SD = 12.6, range 19-81) and 133 matched healthy control (HC) subjects

(69 females, mean age = 41.8, SD = 16.2, range 19-79) were scanned at Johns Hopkins

University. Patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder were included based on a diagnostic approach that utilized the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III­

RlDSM-IV (SCID) (168, 169), direct assessment, family informants, and past medical records. Clinical measures of onset, course, symptoms, or cognition were not available for the current study. Demographic measures were limited to age and sex of the subject at the time of MRI.

Healthy control subjects were recruited usmg random-digit dialing as part of

Phase 1 of the Johns Hopkins aging brain and cognition study. All HC subjects were evaluated with the same structured interview as the patient group. Subjects were excluded if they had a history of DSM-III-RiDSM-IV Axis lor Axis II disorder based on the SCID. Exclusion criteria for all samples included a history of overt brain disease, mental retardation, head injury with loss of consciousness for greater than 30 minutes, or a diagnosis of substance abuse or dependence within the last 12 months.

MR imagingparamelel:r:

Whole brain MRIs were obtained on a single 1.5T Signa GE scanner (GE Medical

Systems, Milwaukee) in the coronal plane using an SPGR (spoiled gradient recall) 3D imaging sequence (35msec repeat time, 5msec echo time, 45° flip angle, 1 excitation,

1.5mm slice thickness, 24cm field of view, and a matrix size of 256 x 256).

40 Optimized ~)o.:rel-ba.fed mOlphometIY.·

Optimized voxel-based morphometry (VBM) (170) has recently gained favor in the field of neuroimaging as a method for comparing localized volumes of brain tissue.

Unlike ROI studies which traditionally employ manual tracing of regions over a series of brain slices, VBM is an automated technique that segments GM, WM, and cerebrospinal

fluid using structural MRI images for comparison of whole brain or localized measures of

volume. As an automated process, VBM offers many advantages over manual tracing

methods by assessing both whole brain and localized regions in one process, increasing

reliability between studies, and avoiding the time-consuming method of manual tracing

which also allows for larger sample sizes. In contrast, the automated method requires

that all structural images be normalized to the same stereotactic space. As a result, VBM

analyses are less sensitive to shape differences creating a trade-off of validity for the sake

of reliability. This is of particular relevance to schizophrenia given that these patients are

more likely to show gross aberrations in brain structure. Nonetheless, an increasing

number ofVBM studies have confirmed and extended the findings of ROI studies (171).

MR images were visually inspected for orientation and movement artifact before

preprocessing. VBM analyses employed the SPM2 toolbox (statistical parametric

mapping, developed by the Wellcome Institute, London, UK) running in MATLAB (The

MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). All images were preprocessed using the optimized

VBM approach described in detail by Good et al. (47, 170). Particulars relevant to this

study will be briefly described here. A study-specific anatomical template set consisting

of a Tl-weighted image and a priori gray, white and cerebrospinal fluid images was

created from He for the VBM analysis in order to control for intensity differences in MR

41 images due to unique aspects of these data, such as scanner and acquisition parameters.

To construct the templates (47), images were first reoriented and volumes roughly normalized, using a 12-parameter affine model, to the 152 average T 1 MNI (Montreal

Neurological Institute) template provided as part of the SPM2 package. Normalized images were interpolated to voxel dimensions of 1.5mm x I.5mm x 1.5mm and then segmented into GM, WM, and CSF using a modified mixture model cluster analysis technique, with a correction for image intensity nonuniformity (172). Images were then smoothed with an 8mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel and averaged across subjects to create Tl, OM, WM and CSF templates.

For the optimized VBM analysis, all 266 images were segmented in native space into GM, WM and CSF compartments and resulting OM images were normalized to the customized GM template. Normalization parameters were recorded and applied to the raw TI images (170). The resulting images are probabilistic segmentations of GM; the addition of the first segmentation step minimizes the number of non-brain voxels misclassified as GM (47). Gray matter concentration images (OMC) were then smoothed with a 12mm FWHM isotropic Gaussian kernel to compensate for the inexact nature of normalization and to ensure statistical validity under parametric assumptions (173). To calculate differences in GM volume, the voxel values in the segmented images were multiplied by the Jacobian detelminants derived from spatial normalization (47). This modulation step preserves the original volume of the segmented image, which is altered during the transformation into stereotactic space. It is important to note that GM

'volumes' as defined in this paper are relative volumes based on the optimized VBM modulation step and are not the absolute volumes as calculated by ROI analyses.

42 lildependent Component Analvrif:

Images were processed using the GIFT ICA toolbox (http://catb.sourceforge.net).

All GM volume images were entered into the ICA toolbox with the HC images entered first. Each GM image was converted into a one-dimensional vector and each vector was centered to a mean of zero. The 266 vectors were arrayed into a matrix. This subject-by­ gray matter data matrix was decomposed into 30 components with the mixing matrix indicating the amount each subject contributed to each component. The number of components was determined using the order estimation tool in the GIFT toolbox (174).

Output images and mixing matrix columns for each component were scaled to unit standard deviation.

Regrefsion Ana(yses

After estimating components, the mlxmg matrix parameters (i.e., component loading scores) for each subject were extracted to SAS v9.1. Since both diagnostic groups were entered into the same ICA matrix, we first compared the diagnostic groups for significant differences in component loading scores using an independent samples t­ test. This step aimed to reduce the number of components of interest and to limit further analyses to components where the patient and control groups differed in the magnitude to which they loaded on each component. To test the association of each remaining component with the outcome variable, component loadings that were significantly diflerent by diagnostic group at p<0.05 using a t-test were entered into a logistic regression.

Since the outcome variable is dichotomous, binary logistic regression was used

1) to assess which of the independent components significantly predicted diagnostic

43 group, 2) to rank the relative importance of each predictor, and 3) to determine the amount of variance in the actual diagnoses that can be explained by the observed components. Logistic regression uses the maximum likelihood estimation after first transforming the outcome variable into a logit variable (175). Logit is the natural log of the odds of the outcome occurring or not occurring. Discriminant function analyses could also have been used to test the ability of these components to correctly classify subjects' diagnosis. In this instance, logistic regression was preferred because it provides the coefficient for each component which allows for the reporting of odds ratios with confidence intervals for each component's association with the outcome measure (176).

Yisualizatiol1 ..

For visualization of group differences identified from logistic regression analyses, voxels for significant components which contributed to the component at a value of

IZI> 3.1 were superimposed onto SPM2' s spatially normalized template brain.

Coordinates were transformed from the MNI coordinate system to the coordinates of the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (177) using a MATLAB conversion program written by Matthew Brett (MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge,

England). Once converted, Talairach coordinates were entered into the Talairach

Daemon (178) for localization.

44 CHAPTER g: RESULTS

Previous structural MRI studies compare patients and control groups for absolute or relative differences between volumes at a single location in the brain. Although this approach may be useful in the identification of brain regions involved in the expression of the disease for the entire group, it provides minimal information about the natural patterns of brain volumes within groups. ICA decomposes the GM volume variation from the large subject-by-voxel matrix into a smaller subject-by-component matrix. Each component represents a distinct GM volume pattern extracted from the dataset. The component loading score is a quantitative measure of how much each subject correlates with that component/GM volume pattern. Thus, ICA is in a position to detect natural constellations of brain volumes within groups of subjects which may reflect valid neural networks that have been poorly detected using correlational or factor analyses (100, 117,

122, 155, 156, 159).

Thirty components were estimated using ICA. Each component was displayed over a single subject brain image in the GIFT toolbox and visually inspected to identify the major brain regions involved in each component. The primary brain region detected by each component is listed in table 1. Eight components displayed patterns that were suggestive of artifact, such as ringing around the edges of the cerebrum. Three components represented the areas of the ventricles where no GM tissue was expected.

These eleven components may have been detecting artifactual variation in brain volume introduced during the image normalization process. Artifact and reduced variability are both limitations incurred by the normalization process that have been acknowledged and accepted by the neuroimaging community as an acceptable trade-off for increased

45 automation. Since brain size and shape are of particular interest in this study, areas of the brain that are more susceptible to artifactual volume (GM bordering ventricles) are interpreted with caution since patients with SZ are more likely to have gross structural aberrations which require more extensive reshaping during normalization.

T-test

Of the 30 components estimated using lCA, a comparison of the patient group to the control group using an independent samples t-test identified 11 components where subjects' component loading scores were significantly different by diagnostic group.

Only one of the 11 components previously considered to be artifictual was found to be significant. Component 30 was significant at p<0.05 with mean SZ scores greater than

HC scores. Five of the 11 significant components had mean component loadings that were greater in the HC group. The remaining six components had mean component

loadings that were greater in the SZ group.

Regression ana[vres

To estimate the potential for independent components to predict diagnostic group,

a multiple logistic regression was run with the 11 components above as the independent variables and with diagnostic group as the outcome measure. Since this analysis is exploratory rather than hypothesis driven, three entry methods (forward conditional, backward conditional, and stepwise) were used to assess the model. Each method resulted in the same six components signiticant at p

at p

significant (X2 =3.05, df=8, p=O.93) indicating that there is no difference between the

outcomes observed and the outcomes predicted by the six variables.

46 Linear regression uses the R-square statistic to quantify the amount of variance in the outcome variable that is accounted for by the predictor variables. In logistic regression, the variance of a dichotomous outcome measure is dependent on its frequency distribution. Since the distribution can vary between samples, there is no accurate measure of variance for comparison between models. Despite this, several alternatives have been proposed that seek to adjust for the limitations of the logistic model (175). Of these alternatives, the Nagelkerke (1991) measure of variance is the most reported (175).

In this study, the Nagelkerke R-square estimates that approximately 34.82% of the variation in diagnosis is explained by these six components.

Assessing the accuracy of a predictive model such as this one is a critical to evaluating its utility. ROC curves provide a standardized way of quantifying the trade­ off between specificity and sensitivity and provide an overall measure of the ability of these components to predict a diagnosis of SZ. The curves are also visually intuitive for the interpretation of predictive accuracy. The overall accuracy of this model in correctly discriminating diagnostic group was 80.5% (using a predicted probability of 0.5 or greater) and is represented by the area under the curve (AUC) (figure 1). The maximum percentage of individuals who were correctly classified by diagnostic group was 73.3, with a sensitivity of 70.7% and a specificity of 75.9% (table 3). In this sample, the positive predictive value of the 6 components was 74.6% and the negative predictive value was 72.1 % (table 3). However, in this study there are equal numbers of patients and controls. In the general population, prevalence of SZ is less than 1%. In order for the "true" predictive values of these components to be evaluated, the analyses would need to be repeated with a sample that reflects the actual disease prevalence (176) (see table 3,

47 panel B).

Anatom.)/ ifthe independent Components

Five components represented heteromodal association cortex areas and one component represented sub-cortical thalamic areas. Three components represented areas where GM volumes were relatively greater in HC than patients (table 4, figure 2) while the remaining three represented regions where GM was relatively greater in schizophrenia (table 5, figure 3). The six components will subsequently be referred to by the anatomical regions they represent and are listed here in order of their respective contributions to the prediction of diagnostic group.

Component 24: He Parietal

The strongest association with diagnostic group was component 24. This component consisted of bilateral parietal GM volume that was greater in HC than schizophrenia (blue in figure 2). The GM differences were predominately sub-gyral and located medial to the right IPL in the dorsal portion but became bilateral as the GM difference was tracked into the temporo-parietal junction. Although the "HC parietal" component extended into the temporal gyri, its regions were posterior to the STG regions expressed in the "HC temporal" component (discussed next). This component accounted for approximately 11.4% of the variation in diagnostic according to the Nagelkerke R­ square statistic.

Component 13: HC Temporal

The most extensive contiguous regIOn of GM differences between diagnostic groups was found in the region of the STG (red in figure 2) with healthy controls having more GM than patients with schizophrenia. These GM differences were notably

48 constrained to the STG and its medial counterparts, the transverse temporal gyrus and insula, suggesting a clear distinction between these structures (as a single component) and the rest of the temporal lobe. The GM differences were slightly lateralized (R>L) posteriorly in the PT and became increasingly lateralized as the gyrus descended toward the temporal pole. The larger STG and its lateralization are consistent with previous findings. This component accounted for an additional 7% of the variation in diagnostic group.

Component 27: SZ Thalamic/Hippocampal

Component 27 consisted of substantially larger GM in schizophrenia patients in the thalamus, hippocampi and parahippocampal gyri, and medial temporal regions (green

in figure 3). The thalamic increases in schizophrenia were localized to the medial,

anterior, and ventral nuclei and hypothalamus, but did not extend into the lateral thalamic

areas. Both hippocampi were larger in schizophrenia and this effect extended focally along the parahippocampal gyri. The thalamic and hippocampal regions covaried with two bilateral nodes located medial to the temporal lobe (BA 21, 22). This component accounted for an additional 7% of the variation in diagnostic group.

Component 25: HC Frontal/Occipital

This component represented greater GM volumes in HC compared with

schizophrenia and was expressed bilaterally in the middle frontal gyri, extending

medially into sub-gyral areas (green in figure 2). The isolated region along the middle

frontal gyrus covaried with GM volumes in the posterior regions of the STG bilaterally, the middle temporal-middle occipital junction, and the primary visual areas (BA 17, 18).

This component accounted for an additional 2.5% of the variation in diagnosis.

49 Component 2: SZ Parietal

In the parietal lobe, patient with schizophrenia also showed greater volumes bilaterally in portions of the IPL (red in figure 3). The increases in schizophrenia patients were less medial than those in the "HC Parietal" component and did not extend ventrally into the STG or medial temporal areas. The regions comprising this component were more extensive in the right hemisphere. Furthermore, the IPL regions in this component showed a covariance with a posterior section of the medial temporal gyrus bilaterally and the lingual areas (BA 17, 18). This component accounted for an additional 6% of the variation in diagnostic group.

Component 3: SZ Frontal/Temporal

While the majority of components comprised focal GM differences, this component consisted of several regions scattered throughout the brain (blue in figure 3).

Areas in this component included discrete nodes in the superior, middle, and medial frontal gyri (along the GM/WM border), the temporo-parietal junction, and the posterior

STG. The varied location of these small nodes likely contributed to the diminished significance of this component in the regression model. However, the correlated regions all represent areas of the HASC and are suggestive of a GM volume basis to the HASC network. This component accounted for an additional 1.5% of the variation in diagnosis.

50 CHAPTER I(j: IJISCOSSION

Unlike traditional GLM studies which test for a static, absolute difference in

volume at a given voxel between diagnostic groups, ICA explores patterns of variation that occur within and between subjects. Each component extracted using ICA represents

a set of brain regions that covary in a particular way between subjects (as indicated by the

mixing matrix parameters). In this study, patients with schizophrenia and healthy

controls were analyzed as a single dataset to allow the extraction of natural variations in

structure. Significant components do not represent absolute GM volume differences but

represent areas of GM volume where the individuals of one diagnostic group positively

correlate with the given GM volume pattern (on average) while the subjects in the other

diagnostic group negatively correlate with the component.

Regressioll Alla{yscr

By itself, ICA provides a decomposition of data into a smaller set of structural

patterns. In order to identify group differences, significance testing was performed on the

component loading scores. Since both diagnostic groups were included in our model,

testing for group differences using component loadings identified regions of the brain

where one diagnostic group had significantly different GM patterns than the other group.

A binary multiple logistic regression of the component loadings identified six

components that were highly predictive of diagnostic group: overall the model was able

to correctly discriminate diagnostic group 80.5 percent of the time. With further

research, anatomical differences in GM volumes such as those identified using lCA

might be capable of predicting diagnosis on an individual level.

Predictability

51 Although this model appears to be a good predictor of diagnostic group based on the sample at hand, the utility of this model should be interpreted with caution. Briefly discussed in chapter nine, under real world conditions, this model is unlikely to retain its rather high predictive ability. This is because the samples included in the study are not fully generalizable to their respective populations (176).

The measures of predictive value are based on the proportion of subjects who do or do not have the disease. The study prevalence of disease positive subjects substantially influences measures of predictive value. For example, positive predictive value is a ratio of true positives to all positives. In this study, 94 out of 126 subjects were accurately predicted to be positive (74.6%) with a 1: 1 ratio of patients to controls. In a sample where disease-free subjects are much more common, say a ratio of 1:9 for patients to controls, the sensitivity and specificity of the test remain the same but the predictive ability of the test changes (table 3, panel B). When the prevalence of disease individuals is substantially lower than the disease free individuals, the ability to predict a positive status is reduced while predictive ability of negative status is greatly increased.

Prevalence of SZ is estimated to be less than 1% of the popUlation (less than a 1: I 00 ratio of SZ to non-SZ) suggesting the PPV of these 6 components will be remarkably lower while the NPV will be close to 100%. One additional point to consider is the context in which a test such as this one will be applied. Although prevalence is very low for SZ in the general population, the proportion of individuals who present for diagnosis upon whom this test may be used will have a much higher ratio of SZ to non-SZ. Thus, in actual applied settings. the low prevalence of SZ may be less of an issue.

Spectrum refers to the sampling of the population. Although the patients in this

52 study fall along a wide-spectrum of age, sex, and chronicity, the fact that these patients participated in a research study indicates that they are relatively high functioning and able to provide informed consent. In addition, patients with schizoaffective diagnoses were included in the sample. These factors are likely to impact the sensitivity of this model in

SZ only populations. The same effect is seen on specificity when the comparison population does not adequately reflect the real world population. Neuroimaging studies routinely require control subjects to be psychiatrically and physically healthy. Not only does this make the control sample less generalizable to the real world population, but it undermines the nature of specificity. The goal of specificity is to distinguish one type of illness from another. Thus, as an example for this study, a good control group would include patients with non-SZ psychiatric illnesses, relatives of patients with SZ, and persons that demonstrate similar symptoms or cognitive deficits but for reasons other than SZ. Given that the nature of this study was exploratory, wide spectrum control populations may mask the detection of an effect and are perhaps premature in this context. With more refined hypotheses and meaningful applications, the dependence on wide-spectrum study groups reiterates the importance of large-scale epidemiological studies of brain structure, clinical presentation, and genotypes in the search for anatomical predictors of schizophrenia.

Bias, on the other hand, is less of an issue in this study. Bias can falsely inflate sensitivity and specificity when the examiner is aware of the test result during diagnosis or is aware of diagnosis when evaluating a test result. Any subjectivity in this study is limited to diagnosis of the subject prior to imaging. Since a diagnosis is required for inclusion in the study, all diagnoses are unaffected by the imaging results. Alternatively,

53 the brain scans are evaluated by an automated, bias free process during both VBM and lCA. In this study, experimenter bias is completely independent of the predictor variables and is not likely to impact this study's predictive efficacy. The impact of bias will reemerge in these types of studies, however, should brain morphology become part of the diagnostic workup for schizophrenia.

Strue/ura! D(jJerences

The application of ICA to detect structural GM volume differences between healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia successfully identified six components that diflerentiated diagnosis. Five of the six components corresponded to areas of the heteromodal association cortex that have been implicated in the disorder; the remaining component is hypothesized to sub serve the HASC network. Our findings of smaller GM volumes in the patient group in the STG, IPL, and prefrontal regions are consistent with past research (l00).

The ability for TCA to detect discrete regions of variation is particularly valuable.

The "HC Temporal" component indicates that healthy controls consistently possess more

GM volume bilaterally in a large, continuous region of the temporal lobe that included the STG, PT, transverse temporal gyrus, and insula. Restriction of the volume disparities between groups to this temporal region suggests that GM volumes in these areas, but not in the middle or inferior temporal regions, were a discriminating anatomical difference between patients and controls and is consistent with previous reports of selective reductions in the STG (61, 77, 155). A similar effect was seen with the "HC Parietal" component with relative increases in controls bilaterally in the medial regions of the inferior parietal lobule. These two components suggest that STG and lPL structures are

54 each internally consistent m volumetric changes. This uniformity may underlie the power of these two components in this regression model. These two components alone correctly classified 68.8% of the individual diagnoses.

The remaining four components demonstrated several inter-correlations in volume change throughout the brain. Notably, both the "He Frontal/occipital" and "SZ

Frontal/temporal" components revealed several non-prefrontal GM changes that covaried with the prefrontal volumes. This is consistent with Mitelman's (2005) reports of inter­ correlations between prefrontal volumes and temporal and occipital volumes in patterns that ditJered between patients and controls (156). Although it is not possible to demonstrate connectivity between GM regions based on volume correlations, the association in volume change between these regions implies that a search for abnormal connectivity may be fruitful. In addition, the focal GM differences found in these two components may explain the greater success of parcellation studies over whole lobe measurements in detecting anatomical differences. Although past research has mainly detected differences in the inferior and orbital frontal gyri, our findings of GM deficits in schizophrenia in the middle frontal gyrus were consistent with Gur (179) and

Goldstein(97).

Relative mcreases m GM volume in the IPL were evident in both diagnostic groups. In the "He parietal" component, controls had greater IPL volumes in the right superior parietal lobule and angular gyrus and bilaterally in the supramarginal gyrus and posterior portions of the temporal gyri. In the "SZ Parietal" component, patients exhibited greater GM volumes posterior to and more lateral than the regions in the "He temporal" component and did not extend into temporal areas. Although volumetric

55 studies of the IPL in schizophrenia are limited, GM reductions in schizophrenia were consistent with previous reports of volume reductions in the angular and supramarginal gyri in schizophrenia (85, 97, 98, 101). Different but adjacent regions in the parietal cortex that showed consistent volumetric change within subject groups suggests that GM volumes in the parietal lobe may be not be wholly decreased in patients. Instead, patients may display altered morphology that occurs inconsistently between patients. Since ICA is sensitive to variations common among a subset of the population group, it is possible to

extract components representing GM variations that were not detectable using traditional

VBM approaches. Similar IPL volume reductions in patients were found with joint ICA

(iICA), supporting a potential role of the IPL in the expression of the disease (102, 165).

In the jlCA study as in this study, the smaller GM volumes in patients versus controls

were located medially in the GM regions. It is also possible that reductions of GM in

patients in this region represent increases in WM. Future studies should investigate GM­

WM volume interactions using ICA.

The patient group showed the most intense GM increases in the thalamic and

hippocampal regions. MRI studies of thalamic volumes in schizophrenia generally report

reductions in patients( 57, 180) or no significant differences between patients and

controls. In our study, ICA detected increases in thalamic and hypothalamic volumes for

patients that correlated with GM decreases in controls. Since ICA detects variations in

volumes common within and between individual subjects rather than absolute volume

differences between groups, our results may not be fully comparable to those reported

using standard volumetric methods. The "schizophrenia thalamic\parahipp" component

included sizable GM increases in the parahippocampal gyri and the medial portions of the

56 middle and superior temporal gyri (BA 21, 22) as well.

As an automated procedure, VBM may incorrectly segment voxels along the perimeter of the ventricles as OM (47, 170). The location of the thalamic structures at the base of the ventricles may heighten the likelihood that these structures are misclassified and OM volumes are spurious. Although this is possible, the specificity of the misclassification to one diagnostic group is unlikely, suggesting that anatomical differences detected in this region are rcal, albeit misclassified. Non-automated analyses of this region may determine the validity of the VBM segmentation and localization of diagnostic group differences to thalamic structures. Alternatively, ICA may serve as a powerful tool for exposing underlying variations specific to overlapping subgroups of a population that are not detectable using univariate methods. Further research on volumetric variations in the thalamus is recommended.

Conclusions

The use of ICA to explore OM segmentation allowed for the identification of natural groupings of regions that showed disparate volumetric variations between schizophrenia and HC. These findings agree with previous reports on OM differences in schizophrenia. Our study provides support for the HASC theory of schizophrenia-five of the six components that significantly predicted diagnostic groups were composed of regions of the HASC. An estimated 34.8% of the variation in diagnosis was accounted for by OM volumes in these six components, which supports a substantial role for structural abnormalities in the expression of schizophrenia. The separation of these regions into different components suggests that there are complex and regionally specific structural

57 changes occurring in schizophrenia. This may explain variability in previous findings because most methods have simply identified volumetric changes from HC without grouping together regions which exhibit similar changes.

The application of ICA to structural brain images creates new opportunities to assess variations in regional brain morphology. Comparisons of the independent regional variations between diagnostic groups may reveal differences not detectable using traditional univariate methods and may provide new insight into the complex structural changes occurring in mental illness. However, this is only a first step in the overall goal of reevaluating the utility of the current diagnostic systems. Future research investigating the anatomical phenotypes should consider the aims discussed in this thesis in an effort to further our understanding of the etiologies and construct of schizophrenia.

58 Table 1: Each ICA component and the primary region represented by the component. Component loading scores were grouped by diagnosis and descriptive statistics for each group are listed. Results of each independent samples t-test are listed in the final columns. * = p < 0.05, ** =P < 0.01 ; I I DescriPtive Statistics of Each CO!TJlOnent by Diagnostic Group t-test for E wa1itv of Moons Moon SE of :~~:==r· ··.~.=.:·.. ·.r··:·~.;;·.·:.=~ .:: ... : ...... H C Moon SZ ~mun Maxi~ Std. Deviatk>n Std. Error .. sz HC HC siTHC ··HcT sz sz II' Sig. 1 iAnlerior Cingulate 1 0.023 -0.023 -3.170 i -3.290 2.530 2.460 1.041 i 0.961 0.090 0.083 0.372 264 o 710 .•...... j.. 1.053 . 0.893 2··::::: ...... ::ti~~~I~:r:~:a.:~~~I.pr~~I~~ ••••.••••••.•••••••••.••• -0.224; 0.224 -3.120 : -1.640 1.690 2700 0.091 0.077 -3.745: 264 0000 3 ,Bilateral Prefrontal aoo Left Inferior Parietal -0.171 0.172 -2.110:. -2.110 1.720 2.160 0.979 09941 0.085 ~~ 264 0.005 ·.:.:.:::.:.::.::.:::;.:.:.::.:.:.:::.:::.f.:::.:::.:.::·:::::J~~~~.:I:f.n.e~~u;~;;;lln~ :~:L ::.~: ··::~:~·~I;::~: ~~~:L ~::: :: :;:.i·;;. .;.;;.. ~. ::: 6 i iBilateral SupramarQinal Gyrus 0.054 : -0.054 -2.560 i -2.420 2.620 2.280 1.035: 0.966 0.090 0.084 ::::::~·:::::f::::::::::::·.t::·:::::·::::::.t~.i~!~~~E~~~:;;(j8ry\.\;';;8ijoc(jPii';IIObe -0097' 0.098 .~~!~t -2.170 3070, 2870 ···1015; 0.979 ··0088 0.085 I 8 : !Artiflld: LateralVentncles 0.023 -0.023 -2.310; -2.480 2.100 i 2.410 0.964: 1038 0.084 0.090 ·········· .. ·9 .. ··· .... ·.... i...... I·iiiiiiierai·C8udate Nudeus -0001 0.001 ···::;1·6·50+· -2.290 1.870 + 3.120 0.9091·· 1.087 0.079 0.094 iO··;··..IMiiillipDSie;;()~o;:bit;,ir;,r.i8IRir.gir.9 . ·Oii74'~0.073 -2.8801-3.740 2.070 2.160 0.900 i 1089 0.078 0.094 ;;iiddiePrefiOni8Iii~i ...... • 0.910' 1.084 0.079 i 0.094 L. .. ~ .. 12 iRight Lateral Somatosensory 0.988 1.008 0.086 . 0.087 -1.460 0.081 .+ 0.089 ·.. ··· .. ·· .. ·.. l'3 ...... ::·:L·:···· .. :··:·l~i~i~.ra.Oi:~~p.~r.~~l1Ip°ral.Q.~n ...... 0936 ..•.. 1028 3.208 264 14 . iBilateralTemporal-Occipttaljunclion aoo Right -2090 2.110 3990 0.944 . 1.037 0.082 0.090 -2.291 264 iPrecuneus-Angular Gyrus 15 . i/irtiiilli·:··o·i)iSiii Intr .... hempishere -3350 2.100 0.939 + 1.048 0.081 ! 0.091 -1.937 ...... ls··· .. ······i······ .. ·· .. ···tAiiil·ii··j"·iii·ii·iii.. pole Ring in g -3.250 2.930 0.989 1.011 0.086 . 0.088 -0.937 ~ ...... '!:.::....:; .. ·.·.···lii·i~i~r..... i:i~·~~.O';T em po~iii ...... -0.149 0.149 -2.620 ' -1.770 3.430 0.978 , 1.003 0.085 0.087 18 i iBilateralPrinaryVlsual 0.075 -0.076 -2.400 : -3420 0968r ·10281 0.084 0089 ·············'·9 .. ··· .. ···· .. ;··· .. ······· ····;Ail1ilii·i)r·c·j·iliiiiiilte and Insula -0.060' 0.060··:2:50ii' -2.670 1.007 ;··0.993 0.087 : 0.086 ...... 201·!R~·iiistiiiitii·miGI()Il~sp!illi(jus 0.196 -0.196 ·::;146oi 0.965 . 0.999 0.084 . 0.087 21 ·;tMiiildTempomlpoieRingir.iI -0.014 ' 0.014 -21301 1.066 ;.. 0.9321 0.092 0081 ...... 22 ...... ; ············t.iirtiflld: FDUrth Ventricle -0.029 ,. 0.030 -2.280.. 1 0.963 1039 0.083 . 0.090 •• ,ij. •• 23 1.002 1.001 0.087 i 0.087 ...... 0.993 0.921 0.086 . 0.080 ...... ~~ ...... •...... !.~~.~.t~r.~.I.. ~~.~.~.~r.~.~,,~.81 ...... 1... 0.29. 25 i" 1Bilateral Middle Frontal Gyrus aoo P nmary VIsual 1008 0.926 0.087.! 0.080 ······· .. ···zs···········t···············hil·iili;riii·"'·idiiie·F~;,iii8iGyrus··· ...... 1.009 0.988 0.088 : 0.086 ·····-··· .. ··1·· .. ·"···································· •..•.•..•..••..•...... 27 . halamllS aoo J.ledialTemporal Lobe -0.195 0.195 -2.850 ; -2.430 0.899 0.078 : 0.092 0.085 .. ; .. 0.089 I.. ·········· .. ;· .. ·...... 1.. 0.0 31 I' 'I' 1.060 .~~.··:··: ······:··J~i~:~.:·.~:~~ ~i~81.L0t>e ~inQln.Q.. -0.031 29 i iArtilld: Occipital Pole Ringing -0.096 0.096 ·~~~~T~~;~~ ~:~r ~::~ ...... ~.:~l ~.~~~ 0.091 0.082 264 ...... ~ ········t·································· .... · ...... ,...... i VI 30 ,,Artiflld: Posterior Lateral Ventrides -0.147 0146 -2.390+-2450 2.640 · 2.550· ···1.0181. 0.963 0.088 0083 -2.411 264 \0 Table 2: Regression coefficents and signfICance levels in order of effect size_

Component Brain Region B S.E. Wald Sig. EXP{B)

3 SZ fro ntal/tempora I 0_306 0_155 3_918 0_048 1_359

2 SZ parietal 0_543 0_156 12_147 0_000 1_721

25 He frontal/occipital -0.366 0_152 5_819 0_016 0_694

27 SZ thalamidhippocampal 0_591 0_160 13_664 0_000 1.805

13 He temporal -0_612 0_156 15_370 0_000 0_542

24 He parietal -0_723 0_167 18_748 0_000 0_485

Table 3: Observed diagnoses compared to the diagnoses predicted by the regression model. Panel A provides the predictive values for this study sample in which there is a 1:1 ratio of patients to controls_ Green represents the number of subjects correctly classified by the model. Red represents the subjects that were incorrectly classified_ Overall, the regression model in this study accurately classified 195 (73_3%) of the 266 subjects_ Panel B provides the predictive value of this model if the study sample reflected a low prevalence of patients (a ratio of 1 :9), as would be the case when applied as a screening tool. PPV = positive predictive value_ NPV = negative predictive value_

A. Observed SZ He (N=133) (N=133) -g SZ 94 32 PPV=74.6% U (N=126) :g He r------+------i Q: (N=140) 39 101 NPV=72.1% '------'------'

Sensitivity Specificity =70.7% =75.9% B. Observed SZ He (N=26) (N=240) 19 58 PPV=24.7% -g~ (N=77)sz I r----+------1 -g He NPV=96.3% Q: (N=189) '--__7_-'--_ 1_82_-'

Sensitivity Specificity =73.1% =75.8%

60 Table 4: Talairach labels of regions where healthy controls have greater GM volumes than patients. Voxels above the threshold in figure 2 were converted from Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates to Talairach coordinates and entered into a database to provide anatomic and functional labels for the left (L) and right (R) hemispheres. The volume of voxels in each area is provided in cubic centimeters (cc). Within each area, the maximum Z value and its coordinate are provided.

Comp13: Temporal, HC > Brodmann R: Max-Z (x, y, L: Max-Z (x, y, SZ Area R: cc L: cc z) z) 22, 42,41 , 13, Superior Temporal Gyrus 11 .3 5.8 7.6(-50,-6 ,3) 5.2(50,-20,9) 38, 29,21 Precentral Gyrus 6, 13 , 43, 44 1.4 0.3 7.6(-50,-8,6) 4.5(48,-17,12) Transverse Temporal Gyrus 41 , 42 0.9 1.4 6.4(-48,-17,12) 5.2(45,-26,12) Insula 13, 40,29 3.7 2.1 6.3(-45,-14,9) 4.7(42,-28,15) Postcentral Gyrus 40, 43 2.2 0.3 6.0(-56,-23,15) 3.8(56,-25,15) * Optic Tract 0.4 0.1 5.3(-48,0,3) 3.3(0,-17,4) Middle Temporal Gyrus 21,38 0.7 o 4.7(-53,0,-8) n.s. Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 1.6 o 4.6(-48,14,-6) n.s. Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 0.7 0.1 4.4(-59,-28,24) 3.4(53,-37,24) Sub-Gyral 0.4 o 4.2(-48,-11,14) n.s. Brodmann R: Max-Z (x, y, L: Max-Z (x, y, Comp24: Parietal, HC > SZ R: cc L: cc Area z) z) Superior Temporal Gyrus 39,22,13 1.8 0.6 12.4(-39,-51 ,25) 6.1(39,-54 ,19) Supramarginal Gyrus 40 2.1 0.2 12.1 (-39,-51 ,27) 5.9(36,-54,28) Sub-Gyral 7.3 4 11 .7(-36,-51 ,25) 8.2(33,-54,22) Middle Temporal Gyrus 39,22, 19 0.9 0.6 10.8(-36,-54,25) 8.1(36,-54,22) Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 1.4 o 9.9( -42 ,-48,25) 3.3(36,-45,27) Angular Gyrus 0.6 o 9.9(-39,-54,30) n.s. Precuneus 7 0.5 0.2 7.0(-27,-47,44) 4.0(21 ,-48,30) Insula 13 0.2 o 5.9(-39,-43,21) n.s. Superior Parietal Lobule 7 0.1 o 5.8(-27,-50,44) n.s. Extra-Nuclear 0.3 0.3 5.3(-33,-46,19) 5.3(30,-49,19) Postcentral Gyrus 1, 3, 2 o 0.7 n.s. 3.8(45,-26,62) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 0.2 o 3.5(-30,28,32) n.s. Precentral Gyrus 6, 4 o 0.2 n.s. 3.3(42,-11 ,61) Brodmann R: Max-Z (x, y, L: Max-Z (x, y, Comp25: Frontal, HC > SZ R: cc L: cc Area z) z) Sub-Gyral 2.5 3 7.5(-36,24,24) 8.8(36,21 ,21) Middle Frontal Gyrus 9, 46 2.8 0.9 7.4(-39,24,24) 6.5(36,22,27) Lingual Gyrus 18, 17 1.6 1.9 6.9(0,-91 ,-8) 7.3(3,-91 ,-8) * 0. 1 0.5 4.8(-3,-88,-11) 6.9(3,-91 ,-11) Middle Temporal Gyrus 39 0.6 0.2 5.4(-42,-72,12) 4.3(48,-43,8) Middle Occipital Gyrus 19 1.2 1 5.4(-39,-75,9) 4.6(36,-78,12) Superior Temporal Gyrus 39,13,41 0.8 2.2 4.2(-48,-37,13) 5.4(48,-43,10) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 0.1 o 4.3(-33,10,27) n.s. Insula 13 0.2 o 3.9(-48,-40,19) n.s.

61 Table 5: Talairach labels for regions where patients with schizophrenia have greater r GM volumes than healthy controls. Voxels above the threshold in figure 3 were converted from MNI to Talairach coordinates and entered into a database to provide ~ anatomic and functional labels for the left (L) and right (R) hemispheres. The volume of f activated voxels in each area is provided in cubic centimeters (cc). Within each area, the maximum Z value and its coordinate are provided . Brodmann R: Max-Z (x, y, Comp2: Parietal, SZ > HC R: cc L: cc L: Max-Z (x, y, z) Area z) Sub-Gyral 1.6 0.5 12.4(-33,-53,39) 7.3(33,-59,39) Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 , 39, 7 2.9 0.9 11.7(-36,-56,39) 6.8(36,-59 ,39) Angular Gyrus 39 0.6 0.2 10.8(-33,-54,36) 6.3(33,-59,36) Supramarginal Gyrus 0.4 9.3(-33,-51 ,36) n.s. Superior Parietal Lobule 7 0.5 0°.2 7.9(-30,-56,44) 4.8(33,-59,44 ) Precuneus 19, 39, 7 0.5 1 6.6( -36,-62,36) 6.8(33,-62 ,39) Lingual Gyrus 17,18 1.2 0.5 4.9(-3,-90,-3) 4.9(0,-90,-3) Middle Temporal Gyrus 19, 39,22, 21 1.9 0.9 4.7(-45,-63,14) 4.8(42,-66,17) * 0.4 4.3(0,-85,-1) 4.7(0,-93,-3) Cuneus 17 0°.2 0.2 4.3( -3,-93,0) 4.4(0,-93,0) Middle Frontal Gyrus 6 0.3 4.4( -33,5,44) n.s. Middle Occipital Gyrus 0.1° n.s. 4.2(39,-69,17) Fusiform Gyrus 37,20, 36 0.9° 3.5( -42,-50,-15) n.s. Inferior Temporal Gyrus 37 0.1 ° 3.3( -45,-42 ,-18) n.s. Comp3: FrontallTemporal Brodmann ° R: Max-Z (x, y, R: cc L: cc L: Max-Z (x, y, z) SZ>HC Area z) Sub-Gyral 6 2 1.5 5.4(-21 ,-1,47) 6.1(24 ,50,3) Superior Frontal Gyrus 10, 9 1.9 1.2 5.4(-27,52,0) 6.0(27,47,6) Middle Frontal Gyrus 9, 10, 6, 11 1.8 1.4 5.8(-27,33,26) 5.9(27,50 ,3) Middle Occipital Gyrus 19 1 .1 0.1 5.4(-36,-75,1 5) 3.3(33,-78,15) Med ial Frontal Gyrus 10, 6,9 1 0.5 5.3( -24,36 ,26) 4.5(24,44,6) Midd le Temporal Gyrus 39, 19 0.7 0.4 5.3(-36,-75,18) 3.7(48,-55,11 ) Superior Temporal Gyrus 22, 39 1.1 0.4 5.2(-45,-51 ,19) 4.1(45 ,-57,17) Supramarginal Gyrus 0.1 4.8(-45,-51,22) n.s. Inferior Frontal Gyrus 0.2 0°.1 4.2(-39,5,33) 4.4(36,10,27) Cingulate Gyrus 32 0.2 4.3(-18,2,47) n.s. Precentral Gyrus 9 0.2 ° 4.2(-39,5,36) n.s. Inferior Parietal Lobule 0.1 ° 3.5(-42,-48,22) n.s. Comp27: Thalamic/Hippo Brodmann R: Max-Z (x, y, R: cc L: °cc L: Max-Z (x, y, z) SZ>HC Area z) Extra-Nuclear Optic Tract 1.2 6.7(-3,-17,1) 7.5(0 ,-14,3) Thalamus 2.2 1.8 7 .O( -3,-14,3) 6.4(3,-14,3) Sub-Gyral 1.3 4.6(-45,-32,2) 5.8(48,-32,-1 ) Middle Temporal Gyrus 21 , 22 0.4 1.2 4.4( -48,-32,2) 5.7(48,-35,2) Lateral Ventricle 0.3 0.3 5.2( -3 ,-3,3) 4.6(3,-3,3) Superior Temporal Gyrus 22 , 21, 38 0.4 0.9 4.0( -48,-32,4) 5.1(48,-26,-1) Uncus 20,28 0.4 n.s. 4.9(30,-16,-27) 35, 36, 28, ° Parahippocampal Gyrus 2.2 3.2 3.8(-24,-18,-14) 4.4(30,-16,-24 ) 30 , Amygd. Middle Occipital Gyrus 19 0.1 ° 4.1(-30,-78,20) 3.2(27,-81 ,21 ) Anterior Cingulate 25 0.3 ° 4.1(0,2,-10) n.s. Inferior Parietal Lobule 40 0.1 0.1 3.7(-36,-45,38) 3.7(36,-48,38) Inferior Frontal Gyrus 47 ° 0.2 n.s. 3.6(42,14,-11 )

62 Figure 1: Receiver Operator Characteristic (ROC) curve based on the predicted probabilities of the 6 component model. The area under the curve (AUC) indicates that the overall accuracy of the model is 80.5%.

1 .0

0.8

AUC =0.805

0.4 r~

0.2

0 .0 -F----..------,,----..----r------r---' 0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 .0 1 - Specificity

63 Figure 2: Of the six components that significantly predicted diagnosis, the three components where GM volumes were relatively greater in controls are displayed here. Blobs represent voxels above the threshold of Z>13.11 . Red = He temporal component, blue =He parietal component, green =He frontal-occipital component.

64 Figure 3: Of the six components that significantly predicted diagnosis, the three components where GM volumes were relatively greater in patients are displayed here. Blobs represent voxels above the threshold of Z>13 .11. Red = SZ parietal component, blue = SZ frontal-temporal component, green = SZ thalamic component.

65 REFERENCES:

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC; 1994. 2. Andreasen NC. The evolving concept of schizophrenia: fram Kraepelin to the present and future. Schizophren ia Research 1997;28(2-3): 105-9. 3. Jablensky A. The 1OO-year epidemiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 1997;28(2-3): 111-25. 4. Bramet EJ, Fennig S. Epidemiology and natural history of schizophrenia. 1999;46(7): 871-81 . 5. Nixon NL, Doody GA. Official psychiatric morbidity and the incidence of schizophrenia 1881-1994. Psychological Medicine 2005;35(8): 1145-53. 6. Antonova E, Sharma T, Morris R, Kumari V. The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: a selective review. Schizophrenia Research 2004;70(2- 3):117-45. 7. Flashman LA, Green MF. Review of cognition and brain structure in schizophrenia: profiles, longitudinal course, and effects of treatment. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 2004;27( 1): 1-18. 8. Heinrichs RW. The primacy of cognition in schizophrenia. American Psychologist 2005;60(3 ):229-242. 9. Kessler RC, Birnbaum H, Demler 0, Falloon IRH, Gagnon E, Guyer M, et al. The prevalence and correlates of nonaffective psychosis in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R). Biological Psychiatry 2005;58(8):668-76. 10. Kendler KS, Gallagher TJ, Abelson JM, Kessler RC. Lifetime prevalence, demographic risk factors, and diagnostic validity of nonaffective psychosis as assessed in a US community sample. The National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry 1996;53(11): 1022-31. 11. Levav I, Kohn R, Dohrenwend BP, Shrout PE, Skodol AE, Schwartz S, et al. An epidemiological study of mental disorders in a 1 O-year cohort of young adu Its in Israel. Psychological Medicine 1993;23(3):691-707. 12. Jablensky A, Sartorius N, Korten A, Ernberg G, Anker M, Cooper JE, et al. Incidence worldwide of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 1987; 151 :408-9. 13. Bramet EJ, Dew MA, Eaton WW. Epidemiology of psychosis with special reference to schizophrenia. In: Tsuang MT, Tohen M, editors. Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology (2nd ed). New York, NY: Wiley-Liss; 2002. p. 365-387. 14. Hafner H. Onset and early course as determinants of the further course of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Supplementum 2000(407):44-8. 15. Hafner H. Gender differences in schizophrenia. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003;28 Suppl 2: 17-54. 16. Hambrecht M, Maurer K, Hafner H. Evidence for a gender bias in epidemiological studies of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 1993;8(3):223-31. 17. Jablensky A, Sartorius N, Ernberg G, Anker M, Korten A, Cooper JE, et al. Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures. A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychological Medicine - Monograph Supplement 1992;20:1-97. 18. Gottesman, II. Complications to the complex inheritance of schizophrenia. Clinical Genetics 1994;46( 1 Spec No): 116-23. 19. Cardno AG, Gottesman, II. Twin studies of schizophrenia: fram bow-and-arrow concordances to star wars Mx and functional genomics. American Journal of Medical Genetics 2000;97( 1): 12-7. 20. Prescott CA, Gottesman, II. Genetically mediated vulnerability to schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 1993; 16(2):245-67.

66 21. Bray NJ, Owen MJ. Searching for schizophrenia genes. Trends in Molecular Medicine 2001 ;7(4): 169-74. 22. Jones P, Cannon M. The new epidemiology of schizophrenia. Psychiatric Clinics of North America 1998;21(1):1-25. 23. Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, et al. Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley twin psychosis series. Archives of General Psychiatry 1999;56(2): 162-8. 24. Brown S, Birtwistle J. People with schizophrenia and their families. Fifteen year outcome. British Journal of Psychiatry 1998; 173: 139-144. 25. Murray CJL, Lopez AD, editors. The global burden of disease and injury series, volume 1: a comprehensive assessment of mortality and disability from diseases, injuries, and risk factors in 1990 and projected to 2020. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1996. 26. Commander M, Sashidharan S, Rana T, Ratnayake T. North Birmingham assertive outreach evaluation. Patient characteristics and clinical outcomes. & Psychiatric Epidemiology 2005;40( 12):988-993. 27. Davidson L, McGlashan TH. The varied outcomes of schizophrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 1997;42(1):34-43. 28. Milev P, Ho B-C, Arndt S, Nopoulos P, Andreasen NC. Initial magnetic resonance imaging volumetric brain measurements and outcome in schizophrenia: a prospective longitudinal study with 5-year follow-up. Biological Psychiatry 2003;54(6):608-15. 29. Rice DP. The economic impact of schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 1999;60(Suppl I ):4-6; discussion 28-30. 30. Andreasen NC. Understanding the causes of schizophrenia. New England Journal of Medicine 1999;340(8):645-7. 31. Andreasen NC. Schizophrenia: the fundamental questions. Brain Research Reviews 2000;31 (2-3): 106-112. 32. Schultz SK, Andreasen NC. Schizophrenia. Lancet 1999;353(9162): 1425-30. 33. McGlashan TH, Fenton WS. Classical subtypes for schizophrenia: literature review for DSM-IV. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1991;17(4):609-32. 34. an der Heiden W, Hafner H. The epidemiology of onset and course of schizophrenia. European Archives of Psychiatry & 2000;250(6):292-303. 35. Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Schultz S, Duzyurek S, Miller D. Diagnosis, methodology and subtypes of schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 1997;35(2):61-3. 36. Kasai K, Iwanami A, Yamasue H, Kuroki N, Nakagome K, Fukuda M. Neuroanatomy and neurophysiology in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Research 2002;43(2):93-110. 37. Pearlson GO, Marsh L. Structural brain imaging in schizophrenia: a selective review. Biological Psychiatry 1999;46(5):627-49. 38. Andreasen NC, Paradiso S, O'Leary OS. "Cognitive dysmetria" as an integrative theory of schizophrenia: a dysfunction in cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry? Schizophrenia Bulletin 1998;24(2):203-18. 39. Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, O'Leary OS, Miller DO, Wassink T, Flaum M. Defining the phenotype of schizophrenia: cognitive dysmetria and its neural mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry 1999;46(7):908-20. 40. Pearlson GO, Petty RG, Ross CA, Tien A Y. Schizophrenia: a disease ofheteromodal association cortex? Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 14( 1): 1-17. 41. Pearlson GO. Neurobiology of schizophrenia. Annals of Neurology 2000;48(4):556-66. 42. Hafner H. Epidemiology of schizophrenia. A thriving discipline at the turn of the century. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience 2000;250(6):271-3. 43. Lichtermann 0, Karbe E, Maier W. The genetic epidemiology of schizophrenia and of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience 2000;250(6):304-10.

67 44. Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR. A case against subtyping in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 1995; I 7(2): 147 -52. 45. Stompe T, Ortwein-Swoboda G, Ritter K, Marquart B, Schanda H. The impact of diagnostic criteria on the prevalence of schizophrenic subtypes. Compr Psychiatry 2005;46(6):433-9. 46. Giuliani NR, Calhoun VD, Pulver A, Schretlen OJ, Cascella N, Keshavan M, et al. A Large Scale, Multi-Site Investigation of Gray Matter Differences in Schizophrenia Using Optimized Voxel-based Morphometry. American Journal of Psychiatry 2006;Submitted. 47. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel­ based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. NeuroImage 2001;14(1 Pt 1):21-36. 48. Wen W, Sachdev PS, Chen X, Anstey K. Gray matter reduction is correlated with white matter hyperintensity volume: a voxel-based morphometric study in a large epidemiological sample. Neuroimage 2006;29(4): 1031-9. 49. Jones PB, Tarrant CJ. Developmental precursors and biological markers for schizophrenia and affective disorders: specificity and public health implications. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience 2000;250(6):286-91. 50. Shenton ME, Dickey CC, Frumin M, McCarley RW. A review ofMRl findings in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2001 ;49(1-2): 1-52. 51. Ward KE, Friedman L, Wise A, Schulz Sc. Meta-analysis of brain and cranial size in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 1996;22(3): 197-213. 52. Lawrie SM, Abukmeil SS. Brain abnormality in schizophrenia. A systematic and quantitative review of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging studies. British Journal of Psychiatry 1998; 172: 11 0-20. 53. Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, WoodruffPW, David AS, Murray RM, Bullmore ET. Meta­ analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2000; 157(1): 16-25. 54. Antonova E, Kumari V, Morris R, Halari R, Anilkumar A, Mehrotra R, et al. The relationship of structural alterations to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometry study. Biological Psychiatry 2005;58(6):457-67. 55. Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Swayze V, 2nd, O'Leary OS, Alliger R, Cohen G, et al. Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals. American Journal of Psychiatry 1993; 150( 1): 130-4. 56. Haier RJ, Jung RE, Yeo RA, Head K, Alkire MT. Structural brain variation and general intelligence. Neuroimage 2004;23(1 ):425-33. 57. Gaser C, Nenadic I, Buchsbaum BR, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS. Ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia related to volume reduction of the thalamus, striatum, and superior temporal cortex. American Journal of Psychiatry 2004; 161 (1): 154-6. 58. Styner M, Lieberman JA, McClure RK, Weinberger DR, Jones DW, Gerig G. Morphometric analysis of lateral ventricles in schizophrenia and healthy controls regarding genetic and disease-specific factors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 2005; 102(13):4872-7. 59. Honea R, Crow TJ, Passingham 0, Mackay CEo Regional deficits in brain volume in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis ofvoxel-based morphometry studies. American Journal of Psychiatry 2005; 162( 12):2233-45. 60. Onitsuka T, Shenton ME, Salisbury OF, Dickey CC, Kasai K, Toner SK, et al. Middle and inferior temporal gyrus gray matter volume abnornlalities in chronic schizophrenia: an MRI study. American Journal of Psychiatry 2004; 161 (9): 1603-11. 61. Pearlson GO. Superior temporal gyrus and planum temporale in schizophrenia: a selective review. Progress in Neuro- & Biological Psychiatry 1997;21 (8): 1203- 29.

68 62. Gaser C, Nenadic I, Volz HP, Buchel C, Sauer H. Neuroanatomy of "hearing voices": a frontotemporal brain structural abnormality associated with auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex 2004; 14( 1):91-6. 63. Flaum M, O'Leary OS, Swayze VW, 2nd, Miller DO, Arndt S, Andreasen NC. Symptom dimensions and brain morphology in schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research 1995;29(4):261-76. 64. Barta PE, Pearlson GO, Powers RE, Richards SS, Tune LE. Auditory hallucinations and smaller superior temporal gyral volume in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1990;147(11):1457-62. 65. Vita A, Dieci M, Giobbio GM, Caputo A, Ghiringhelli L, Comazzi M, et al. Language and thought disorder in schizophrenia: brain morphological correlates. Schizophrenia Research 1995; 15(3):243-51. 66. Menon RR, Barta PE, Aylward EH, Richards SS, Vaughn DO, Tien AY, et al. Posterior superior temporal gyrus in schizophrenia: grey matter changes and clinical correlates. Schizophrenia Research 1995; 16(2): 127-35. 67. Turetsky B, Cowell PE, Gur RC, Grossman Rl, Shtasel DL, Gur RE. Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia. Relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype. Archives of General Psychiatry 1995;52(12):1061-70. 68. Sigmundsson T, Suckling J, Maier M, Williams S, Bullmore E, Greenwood K, et al. Structural abnormalities in frontal, temporal, and limbic regions and interconnecting white matter tracts in schizophrenic patients with prominent negative symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry 2001; 158(2):234-43. 69. Gur RE, Turetsky 81, Cowell PE, Finkelman C, Maany V, Grossman Rl, et al. Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 2000;57(8):769-75. 70. Sanfilipo M, Lafargue T, Rusinek H, Arena L, Loneragan C, Lautin A, et al. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia: relationship to regional brain volumes and psychiatric symptoms. Psychiatry Research 2002;116(1-2):1-23. 71. Hoff AL, Riordan H, O'Donnell 0, Stritzke P, Neale C, Boccio A, et al. Anomalous lateral sulcus asymmetry and cognitive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1992; 18(2):257-72. 72. Nestor PG, Shenton ME, McCarley RW, Haimson J, Smith RS, O'Donnell B, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of MRI temporal lobe abnormalities in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1993; 150( 12): 1849-55. 73. DeLisi LE, Sakuma M, Tew W, Kushner M, Hoff AL, Grimson R. Schizophrenia as a chronic active brain process: a study of progressive brain structural change subsequent to the onset of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 1997;74(3):129-40. 74. Geschwind N, Levitsky W. Human brain: left-right asymmetries in temporal speech region. Science 1968; 161 (83 7): 186-7. 75. Shapleske J, Rossell SL, Woodruff PW, David AS. The planum temporale: a systematic, quantitative review of its structural, functional and clinical significance. Brain Research Brain Research Reviews 1999;29(1):26-49. 76. Barta PE, Pearlson GO, Brill LB, 2nd, Royall R, McGilchrist IK, Pulver AE, et al. Planum temporale asymmetry reversal in schizophrenia: replication and relationship to gray matter abnormalities. American Journal of Psychiatry 1997; 154(5):661-7. 77. Hirayasu Y, McCarley RW, Salisbury OF, Tanaka S, Kwon JS, Frumin M, et al. Planum temporale and Heschl gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study of first-episode patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 2000;57(7):692-9. 78. Kwon JS, McCarley RW, Hirayasu Y, Anderson JE, Fischer lA, Kikinis R, et al. Left planum temporale volume reduction in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 1999;56(2): 142-8.

69 79. Petty RG, Barta PE, Pearlson GO, McGilchrist IK, Lewis RW, Tien AY, et al. Reversal of asymmetry of the planum temporale in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1995; 152(5):715-21. 80. Rossi A, Stratta P, Mattei P, Cupillari M, Bozzao A, Gallucci M, et al. Planum temporale in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance study. Schizophrenia Research 1992;7( 1): 19-22. 81. Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury OF, Hirayasu Y, Onitsuka T, Spencer MH, et al. Progressive decrease of left Heschl gyrus and planum temporale gray matter volume in first-episode schizophrenia: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry 2003;60(8):766-75. 82. Barta PE, Powers RE, Aylward EH, Chase GA, Harris GJ, Rabins PV, et al. Quantitative MRI volume changes in late onset schizophrenia and Alzheimer's disease compared to normal controls. Psychiatry Research 1997;68(2-3):65-75. 83. Shenton ME, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Pollak SO, LeMay M, Wible CG, et al. Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. New England Journal of Medicine 1992;327(9):604-12. 84. Marsh L, Harris 0, Lim KO, Beal M, Hoff AL, Minn K, et al. Structural magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities in men with severe chronic schizophrenia and an early age at clinical onset. Archives of General Psychiatry 1997;54(12): 1104-12. 85. Hulshoff Pol HE, Schnack HG, Mandl RC, van Haren NE, Koning H, Collins DL, et al. Focal gray matter density changes in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 2001;58(12):1118-25. 86. DeLisi LE, Hoff AL, Schwartz JE, Shields GW, Halthore SN, Gupta SM, et al. Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry 1991 ;29(2): 159-75. 87. Gur RC, Alsop 0, Glahn 0, Petty R, Swanson CL, Maldjian JA, et al. An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain and Language 2000;74(2): 157-70. 88. Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Greenberg R, Griffin S, Schulz SC, Pickar 0, et al. Contrasts between patients with affective disorders and patients with schizophrenia on a neuropsychological test battery. American Journal of Psychiatry 1993; 150(9): 1355-62. 89. Torres 11, Flashman LA, O'Leary OS, Swayze V, 2nd, Andreasen NC. Lack of an association between delayed memory and hippocampal and temporal lobe size in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Biological Psychiatry 1997;42( 12): 1087-96. 90. Goldman-Rakic PS, Selemon LD. Functional and anatomical aspects of prefrontal pathology in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1997;23(3):437-58. 91. Mesulam MM. Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Annals of Neurology 1990;28(5):597-613. 92. Mesulam M. Neurocognitive networks and selectively distributed processing. Revue Neurologique 1994; 150(8-9):564-9. 93. Brazo P, Delamillieure P, Morello R, Halbecq I, Marie RM, Dollfus S. Impairments of executive/attentional functions in schizophrenia with primary and secondary negative symptoms. Psychiatry Research 2005; 133(1 ):45-55. 94. Suzuki M, Nohara S, Hagino H, Takahashi T, Kawasaki Y, Yamashita I, et al. Prefrontal abnormalities in patients with simple schizophrenia: structural and functional brain-imaging studies in five cases. Psychiatry Research 2005; 140(2): 157-71. 95. Pearlson GO, Barta PE, Powers RE, Menon RR, Richards SS, Aylward EH, et al. Ziskind­ Somerfeld Research Award 1996. Medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry 1997;41(1): 1- 14. 96. Eidelberg 0, Galaburda AM. Inferior parietal lobule. Divergent architectonic asymmetries in the human brain. Archives of Neurology 1984;41 (8):843-52.

70 97. Goldstein JM, Goodman JM, Seidman LJ, Kennedy ON, Makris N, Lee H, et al. Cortical abnormalities in schizophrenia identified by structural magnetic resonance imaging. Archives of General Psychiatry 1999;56(6):537-47. 98. Niznikiewicz M, Donnino R, McCarley RW, Nestor PG, Iosifescu OV, O'Donnell B, et al. Abnormal angular gyrus asymmetry in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2000: 157(3 ):428-3 7. 99. Jayakumar PN, Venkatasubramanian G, Gangadhar BN, Janakiramaiah N, Keshavan MS. Optimized voxel-based morphometry of gray matter volume in first-episode, antipsychotic­ naive schizophrenia. Progress in Neuro Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 2005;29( 4 ):587-91. 100. Buchanan RW, Francis A, Arango C, Miller K, Lefkowitz OM, McMahon RP, et al. Morphometric assessment of the heteromodal association cortex in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2004; 161 (2):322-31. 101. Frederikse M, Lu A, Aylward E, Barta P, Sharma T, Pear1son G. Sex differences in inferior parietal lobule volume in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2000; 157(3):422- 7. 102. Calhoun VO, Adali T, Stevens MC, Kiehl KA, Pekar JJ. Semi-blind ICA offMRI: a method for utilizing hypothesis-derived time courses in a spatial lCA analysis. Neurolmage 2005;ln press. 103. Andreasen NC, Flashman L, Flaum M, Arndt S, Swayze V, 2nd, O'Leary OS, et a l. Regional brain abnormalities in schizophrenia measured with magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA 1994;272(22): 1763-9. 104. Suzuki M, Nohara S, Hagino 1-1, Kurokawa K, Yotsutsuji T, Kawasaki Y, et al. Regional changes in brain gray and white matter in patients with schizophrenia demonstrated with voxel-based analysis ofMRI. Schizophr. Res. 2002;55( 1-2):41-54. 105. Bottmer C, Bachmann S, Pantel .I, Essig M, Amann M, Schad LR, et al. Reduced cerebellar volume and neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 2005; 140(3):239-50. 106. Szeszko PR, Gunning-Dixon F, Goldman RS, Bates J, Ashtari M, Snyder P J, et al. Lack of normal association between cerebellar volume and neuropsychological functions in f1rst­ episode schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003; 160( 10): 1884-7. 107. Ho B-C, Mola C, Andreasen NC, Cerebellar dysfunction in neuroleptic naive schizophrenia patients: clinical, cognitive, and neuroanatomic correlates of cerebellar neurologic signs. Biological Psychiatry 2004;55( 12): 1146-53. 108. Danos P. Pathology of the thalamus and schizophrenia--an overview. Fortschritte der Neurologie-Psychiatrie 2004;72( 11 ):621-34. 109. Lewis OA. GABAergic local circuit neurons and prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews 2000;31(2-3):270-276. 110. Bunney WE, Bunney BG. Evidence for a compromised dorsolateral prefrontal cortical parallel circuit in schizophrenia. Brain Research - Brain Research Reviews 2000;31(2- 3):138-46. 111. Andreasen NC, O'Leary OS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Ponto LL, et al. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positron-emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal­ thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 1996;93( 18):9985-90. 112. Andreasen NC. The role of the thalamus in schizophrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 1997;42(1 ):27-33. 113. Brickman AM, Buchsbaum MS, Shihabuddin L, Byne W, Newmark RE, Brand J, et al. Thalamus size and outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2004;71(2-3):473- 84.

71 114. Arciniegas D, Rojas DC, Teale P, Sheeder J, Sandberg E, Reite M. The thalamus and the schizophrenia phenotype: failure to replicate reduced volume. Biological Psychiatry 1999;45( 10): 1329-35. 115. Volz H, Gaser C, Sauer H. Supporting evidence for the model of cognitive dysmetria in schizophrenia--a structural magnetic resonance imaging study using deformation-based morphometry. Schizophrenia Research 2000;46( 1):45-56. 116. Ananth H, Popescu I, Critchley HD, Good CD, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ. Cortical and subcortical gray matter abnormalities in schizophrenia determined through structural magnetic resonance imaging with optimized volumetric voxel-based morphometry. Am. J. Psychiat.2002;159(9):1497-505. 117. Mitelman SA, Brickman AM, Shihabuddin L, Newmark R, Chu KW, Buchsbaum MS. Correlations between MRI-assessed volumes of the thalamus and cortical Brodmann's areas in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2005;75(2-3):265-81. 118. Buchanan RW, Breier A, Kirkpatrick B, Elkashef A, Munson RC, Gellad F, et al. Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1993; I 50( 1):59-65. 119. Dazzan P, Morgan KD, Orr KG, Hutchinson G, Chitnis X, Suckling J, et al. The structural brain correlates of neurological soft signs in AESOP first-episode psychoses study. Brain 2004;127(Pt 1):143-53. 120. Staal WG, HulshoffPol HE, Schnack HG, van Haren NE, Seifert N, Kahn RS. Structural brain abnormalities in chronic schizophrenia at the extremes of the outcome spectrum. American Journal of Psychiatry 200 I; 158(7): 1140-2. 121. Preuss UW, Zetzsche T, Jager M, Groll C, Frodl T, Bottlender R, et al. Thalamic volume in first-episode and chronic schizophrenic subjects: a volumetric MRI study. Schizophrenia Research 2005;73(1 ):91-1 0 1. 122. Mitelman SA, Byne W, Kemether EM, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS. Correlations between volumes ofthe pulvinar, centromedian, and mediodorsal nuclei and cortical Brodmann's areas in schizophrenia. Neuroscience Letters 2006;392( 1-2): 16-21. 123. Jeste DV, McAdams LA, Palmer BW, Braff 0, Jernigan TL, Paulsen JS, et al. Relationship of neuropsychological and MRI measures to age of onset of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 1998;98(2): 156-64. 124. Middleton FA, Strick PL. Basal ganglia and cerebellar loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Research Reviews 2000;31(2-3):236-250. 125. Hokama H, Shenton ME, Nestor PG, Kikinis R, Levitt J1. Metcalf 0, et al. Caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus volume in schizophrenia: a quantitative MRI study. Psychiatry Research 1995;61(4):209-29. 126. Kawasaki Y, Suzuki M, Nohara S, Hagino H, Takahashi T, Matsui M, et al. Structural brain differences in patients with schizophrenia and schizotypal disorder demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience 2004;254(6):406-14. 127. Gur RE, Maany V, Mozley PD, Swanson C, Bilker W, Gur RC, Subcortical MRI volumes in neuroleptic-naive and treated patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1998;155(12):1711-7. 128. Taylor S, Christensen JD, Holcomb JM, Garver DL. Volume increases in striatum associated with positive symptom reduction in schizophrenia: a preliminary observation. Psychiatry Research 2005; 140(1 ):85-9. 129. Spinks R, Nopoulos P, Ward J, Fuller R, Magnotta VA, Andreasen NC. Globus pallidus volume is related to symptom severity in neuroleptic naive patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2005;73(2-3):229-33.

72 130. Stratta P, Mancini F, Mattei P, Daneluzzo E, Casacchia M, Rossi A. Association between striatal reduction and poor Wisconsin card sorting test performance in patients with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 1997;42(9):816-20. 131. Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan ML Weinberger DR. Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Archives of General Psychiatry 2000:57(9):907-13. 132. Hill SK, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE. Neuropsychological profiles delineate distinct profiles of schizophrenia, an interaction between memory and executive function, and uneven distribution of clinical subtypes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24:765-80. 133. Turetsky BI, Moberg PJ, Mozley LH, Moelter ST, Agrin RN, Gur RC, et al. Memory­ delineated subtypes of schizophren ia: relationsh ip to clinical, neuroanatomical, and neurophysiological measures. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:481-90. 134. Malaspina 0, Friedman JH, Kaufmann C, Bruder G, Amador X, Strauss 0, et al. Psychobiological heterogeneity offamilial and sporadic schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 1998;43(7):489-96. 135. Van Os J, Marcelis M, Sham P, Jones P, Gilvarry K, Murray R. Psychopathological syndromes and familial morbid risk of psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry 1997; 170:241-6. 136. Roy MA, Merette C, Maziade M. Subtyping schizophrenia according to outcome or severity: a search for homogeneous subgroups. Schizophrenia Bulletin 2001;27(1):115-38. 137. Marengo J. Classifying the courses of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1994;20(3):519-36. 138. Andreasen NC, Olsen S. Negative v positive schizophrenia. Definition and validation. Arch ives of General Psychiatry 1982;39(7): 789-94. 139. Kendler KS, McGuire M, Gruenberg AM, Walsh D. Examining the validity of DSM-III-R schizoaffective disorder and its putative subtypes in the Roscommon Family Study. American Journal of Psychiatry 1995; 152(5):755-64. 140. Grube BS, Bilder RM, Goldman RS. Meta-analysis of symptom factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 1998;31(2-3):113-20. 141. Zakzanis KK. Neuropsychological correlates of positive vs. negative schizophrenic symptomatology. Schizophrenia Research 1998;29(3):227-33. 142. Davis JM, Chen N. The effects of olanzapine on the 5 dimensions of schizophrenia derived by factor analysis: combined results of the North American and international trials. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 200 1;62(10):757-71. 143. Stober G, Franzek E, Beckmann H. The role of maternal infectious disease during pregnancy in the etiology of schizophrenia in the offspring. Eur Psychiatry 1992;7: 147-152. 144. Beckmann H. Franzek E, Stober G. Genetic heterogeneity in catatonic schizophrenia: a family study. American Journal of Medical Genetics 1996;67(3):289-300. 145. Sallet Pc' Elkis H, Alves TM, Oliveira JR. Sassi E, de Castro CC, et al. Rightward cerebral asymmetry in subtypes of schizophrenia according to Leonhard's classification and to DSM-IV: a structural MRI study. Psychiatry Research 2003; 123( 1):65-79. 146. Sponheim SR. Iacono WG, TllLlras PO, Nugent SM, Beiser M. Sensitivity and specificity of select biological indices in characterizing psychotic patients and their relatives. Schizophrenia Research 2003;63(1-2):27-38. 147. Sponheim SR, Iacono WG, Thuras PO, Beiser M. Using biological indices to classify schizophrenia and other psychotic patients. Schizophrenia Research 2001 ;50(3): 139-50. 148. Tsuang MT, Faraone SV. The frustrating search for schizophrenia genes. American Journal of Medical Genetics 2000;97(1): 1-3. 149. Rossi A, Bustini M, Prosperini P, Marinangeli MG, Splendiani A, Daneluzzo E, et al. Neuromorphological abnormalities in schizophrenic patients with good and poor outcome. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 2000; 101 (2): 161-6.

73 150. Ho BC, Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, Arndt S, Magnotta V, Flaum M. Progressive structural brain abnormalities and their relationship to clinical outcome: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study early in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 2003 ;60( 6):585-94. 151. Mitelman SA, Shihabuddin L, Brickman AM, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS. Volume ofthe cingulate and outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2005;72(2-3):91-108. 152. Mitelman SA, Shihabuddin L, Brickman AM, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS. MRI assessment of gray and white matter distribution in Brodmann's areas of the cortex in patients with schizophrenia with good and poor outcomes. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003; 160(12):2154-68. 153. Wassink TH, Andreasen NC, Nopoulos P, Flaum M. Cerebellar morphology as a predictor of symptom and psychosocial outcome in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 1999;45( 1):41-8. 154. Deister A, Marneros A. Long-term stability of subtypes in schizophrenic disorders: a comparison of four diagnostic systems. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience 1993;242(4): 184-90. 155. Mitelman SA, Shihabuddin L, Brickman AM, Buchsbaum MS. Cortical intercorrelations of temporal area volumes in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2005;76(2-3):207-29. 156. Mitelman SA, Buchsbaum MS, Brickman AM, Shihabuddin L. Cortical intercorrelations of frontal area volumes in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 2005;27(4):753-70. 157. Benes FM. Emerging principles of altered neural circuitry in schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews 2000;31 (2-3):251-269. 158. Wible CG, Shenton ME, Hokama H, Kikinis R, Jolesz FA, Metcalf 0, et al. Prefrontal cortex and schizophrenia. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry 1995;52(4):279-88. 159. Tien A Y, Eaton WW, Schlaepfer TE, McGilchrist IK, Menon R, Powers R, et al. Exploratory factor analysis of MRI brain structure measures in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1996; 19(2-3):93-1 0 1. 160. Wright IC, Sharma T, Ellison ZR, McGuire PK, Friston KJ, Brammer MJ, et al. Supra­ regional brain systems and the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex 1999;9(4):366-78. 161. Mesulam MM. Schizophrenia and the brain. New England Journal of Medicine 1990;322(12). 162. Schlaepfer TE, Harris GJ, Tien A Y, Peng LW, Lee S, Federman EB, et al. Decreased regional cortical gray matter volume in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 1994; 151 (6):842-8. 163. Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Computation. 1995;7(6): 1129-59. 164. Nakai T, Muraki S, Bagarinao E, Miki Y, Takehara Y, Matsuo K, et al. Application of independent component analysis to magnetic resonance imaging for enhancing the contrast of gray and white matter. Neuroimage 2004;21(1):251-60. 165. Calhoun VD, Adali T, Giuliani NR, Pekar JJ, Kiehl KA, Pearlson GO. Method for muItimodal analysis of independent source differences in schizophrenia: combining gray matter structural and auditory oddball functional data. Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27(1):47-62. 166. Hyvarian A, Oja E. Independent component analysis: algorithms and applications. Neural Networks 2000; 12(4-5):411-30. 167. Pearlson GO. New insights on the neuroanatomy of schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reports 1999; 1(1 ):41-5. 168. Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM­ III-R-Patient Version (SCID-P, 511/89). New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1989.

74 169. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams J13W. Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis Disorders (SCID-IV). New York, NY: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1997. 170. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Voxel-based morphometry--the methods. Neuroimage 2000; 11 (6 Pt 1):805-21. 171. Giuliani NR, Calhoun VD, Pearlson GD, Francis A, Buchanan RW. Voxel-based morphometry versus region of interest: a comparison of two methods for analyzing gray matter differences in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 2005;74(2-3): 135-47. 172. Ashburner J. Friston K. Multimodal image coregistration and partitioning--a unified framework. NeuroImage 1997;6(3):209-17. 173. Jones OK, Symms MR, Cercignani M, Howard RJ. The effect of filter size on VBM analyses of DT-MRI data. Neuroimage 200526(2):546-54. 174. Li Y, Adali, T., Calhoun, V.D. Sample Dependence Correction For Order Selection In FMRI Analysis. Proc. ISBI 2006. 175. Hosmer OW, Lemeshow S. Applied logistic regression. 2nd ed. New York: Wiley­ Interscience; 2000. 176. Kramer MS. Clinical epidemiology and biostatistics: a primer for clinical investigators and decision-makers. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1988. 177. Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotactic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system - an approach to cerebral imaging. New York, NY: Thieme Medical Publishers; 1988. 178. Lancaster JL, WoldorffMG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, et al. Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2000; I 0(3): 120-31. 179. Gur RE, Cowell PE, Latshaw A, Turetsky BI, Grossman RI, Arnold SE. et al. Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 2000;57(8):761-8. 180. Csernansky JG, Schindler MK, Splinter NR, Wang L, Gado M, Selemon LD, et al. Abnormalities of thalamic volume and shape in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161 (5):896-902.

75