Binocular Rivalry Stimuli Are Common but Rivalry Is Not

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Binocular Rivalry Stimuli Are Common but Rivalry Is Not GENERAL COMMENTARY published: 24 November 2011 HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00148 Binocular rivalry stimuli are common but rivalry is not Robert Paul O’Shea* Discipline of Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience Research Cluster, School of Health and Human Sciences, Southern Cross University, Coffs Harbour, Australia *Correspondence: [email protected] A commentary on There are two problems for Arnold with These arise for regions off the location in these sorts of examples: space where images would be identical in Why is binocular rivalry uncommon? the two eyes, the horopter (Aguilonius, 1613; Discrepant monocular images in the real 1. Although it is true that one will not Panum, 1858; Ogle, 1953). Binocular rivalry world immediately experience alternations could be common off the horopter – it is Arnold, D. H. (2011). Front. Hum. Neurosci. between the blurry and sharp images, simply not noticed. 5:116. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00116 it is not true that there is no rivalry. In both situations Arnold describes, the Rather, the sharp images’ dominating tendency is for one to look at the nearby Recently Arnold (2011) asked “Why is bin- perception is a form of rivalry, well- object (Mandelbaum, 1960), bringing iden- ocular rivalry uncommon?”. He answered known from laboratory studies as per- tical sharp images onto the foveae. What is in an entertainingly written, provocative manent suppression (Ooi and Loop, needed to resolve the issue of the ecological article, for which I thank and congratulate 1994). optics of binocular vision, rather than exam- him. However, I will argue that Arnold’s 2. If one waits for long enough, the blurry ples, is for them to be quantified by sampling answer falls short in two respects and his image will alternate with the sharp ima- the images the real world presents (cf. Howe assumption that rivalry is uncommon is ges (cf. Blake, 1977). Indeed, George and Purves, 2002; Howe and Purves, 2005). correct for two reasons other than the one (1936) reported that the extremely low- Second, the eyes move about three times he discusses. stimulus-strength image of the back a second (e.g., Otero-Millan et al., 2008). Binocular rivalry is a phenomenon of of one’s closed eyelid will eventually This potentially places fresh images on human visual perception that is easy to dominate the extremely high-stimulus- each retinal region at the same rate. In the demonstrate in the laboratory by using a strength images viewed by the open eye. laboratory, these are likely to be similar to stereoscope to present one image to one eye the previous images (because rival stimuli and a different image to the other: one per- There are at least two reasons other than tend to be two-dimensional stimuli that ceives one image rather than both, and the that Arnold offered for why rivalry is not display rivalry information wherever we image one perceives alternates between the noticed outside the laboratory: look at them). But outside the laboratory, two at random (Wheatstone, 1838). First, although Arnold is correct that these images could be quite different dur- In answering the question, Arnold images of equal stimulus strength rarely fall ing each fixation, and may be identical (2011) identified two situations outside on the foveae of the two eyes, it is common for some corresponding retinal regions. the laboratory in which the view of one that images of equal stimulus strength fall on That is, any pair of corresponding reti- eye differs from that of the other for which corresponding peripheral areas of the reti- nal regions might have rival images for he claimed there is no rivalry. The first is nae. For example, as I type this on my laptop, one fixation and similar, fusible images when an object, such as the trunk of a small images of the rest of the room, several meters for the next. We know from laboratory tree, is near both eyes as we fixate on distant further from my eyes than the laptop screen, research that turning on rival images objects. The trunk projects a blurry, low- fall on the retinae below the fovea. When I briefly and turning them off for longer contrast, low-spatial-frequency (for which attend to these areas, I can see that there are prevents rivalry from occurring by creat- Arnold adopted Levelt’s, 1968, umbrella burry, diplopic images there, for example the ing fusion of the two eyes’ views (Dawson, term “stimulus strength”) image onto the vertical edge of a fireplace appears superim- 1915–1917; Kaufman, 1963; Wolfe, 1983; temporal region of the right-eye retina and posed on the horizontal edges of the bricks O’Shea and Crassini, 1984). Moreover, onto the nasal region of the left-eye retina. of the fireplace. Similarly, when I look up interspersing periods of fusion with peri- In the corresponding regions of the other at the fireplace, I can see the blurry vertical ods of rivalry tends to prevent rivalry by eye’s retina, the distant objects project dif- edge of my laptop screen crossing the blurry promoting fusion (Julesz and Tyler, 1976; ferent, sharp images. The second is when a horizontal edge of a nearby table. If I pay Buckthought et al., 2008). similar object, the tree trunk again, is closer attention to these diplopic areas, I see slow In conclusion, Arnold (2011) posed to one eye, blocking its view. The trunk pro- alternations of binocular rivalry. Laboratory an interesting question of why we do not jects a blurry image onto the fovea of that studies show that rivalry rate in peripheral notice rivalry when we look at the visual eye, and the distant objects project different, vision is much slower than in central vision world outside the laboratory. In answering sharp images onto the fovea of the other (e.g., Blake et al., 1992). it, he identified two situations in which dif- eye. In both these situations, Arnold said the The ubiquity of diplopic images away ferent strength images are projected into the sharp images dominate perception forever, from fixation was discovered by al-Haytham two eyes, and claimed that rivalry does not preventing rivalry. in the eleventh century (Alhazen, 1989). occur. I have argued, to the contrary, that: Frontiers in Human Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org November 2011 | Volume 5 | Article 148 | 1 O’Shea Why binocular rivalry is uncommon • Perception in the situations Arnold world. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:116. doi: 10.3389/ Ooi, T. L., and Loop, M. S. (1994). Visual suppression and identified is consistent with what we fnhum.2011.00116 its effect upon color and luminance sensitivity. Vision Blake, R. (1977). Threshold conditions for binocular Res. 34, 2997–3003. know about rivalry and offers no chal- rivalry. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 3, O’Shea, R. P., and Crassini, B. (1984). Binocular rivalry lenge to theory; 251–257. occurs without simultaneous presentation of rival • Different images in the two eyes are Blake, R., O’Shea, R. P., and Mueller, T. J. (1992). Spatial stimuli. Percept. Psychophys. 36, 266–276. much more common in peripheral zones of binocular rivalry in central and peripheral Otero-Millan, J., Troncoso, X. G., Macknik, S. L., Serrano- regions of the retinae than in central vision. Vis. Neurosci. 8, 469–478. Pedraza, I., and Martinez-Conde, S. (2008). Saccades Buckthought, A., Kim, J., and Wilson, H. R. (2008). and microsaccades during visual fixation, explora- vision; Hysteresis effects in stereopsis and binocular rivalry. tion, and search: foundations for a common sac- • We do not notice potential rivalry in Vision Res. 48, 819–830. cadic generator. J. Vis. 8, 1–18. http://journalofvision. peripheral vision because it is slower Dawson, S. (1915–1917). The experimental study org/8/14/21/ than in central vision and anyway we of binocular colour mixture. Br. J. Psychol. 8, Panum, P. L. (1858). Über die einheitliche Verschmelzung do not attend to it; and 510–551. verschiedenartiger Netzhauteindrucke beim Sehen mit George, R. W. (1936). The significance of the fluctuation zwei Augen. Kiel. • Movement of the eyes prevents rivalry experienced in observing ambiguous figures and in Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiol- from developing. binocular rivalry. J. Gen. Psychol. 15, 39–61. ogy of vision – Part the First. On some remarkable, Howe, C. Q., and Purves, D. (2002). Range image statistics and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular ACKNOWLEDGMENTS can explain the anomalous perception of length. Proc. vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 128, I am grateful to Urte Roeber for commenting Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 13184–13188. 371–394. Howe, C. Q., and Purves, D. (2005). Natural-scene geom- Wolfe, J. M. (1983). Influence of spatial frequency, lumi- helpfully on earlier version of this paper and etry predicts the perception of angles and line orien- nance and duration on binocular rivalry and abnor- to Derek Arnold for helpful discussion and tation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 1228–1233. mal fusion of briefly presented dichoptic stimuli. for sharing a draft of his reply to this paper. Julesz, B., and Tyler, C. W. (1976). Neurontropy, an Perception 12, 447–456. entropy-like measure of neural correlation, in bin- ocular fusion and rivalry. Biol. Cybern. 23, 25–32. Received: 07 November 2011; accepted: 08 November 2011; REFERENCES Kaufman, L. (1963). On the spread of suppression and published online: 24 November 2011. Aguilonius, F. (1613). Opticorum libri sex. binocular rivalry. Vision Res. 3, 401–415. Citation: O’Shea RP (2011) Binocular rivalry stimuli are Philosophis juxta ac mathematicis utiles. Antwerp: Levelt, W. J. M. (1968). On Binocular Rivalry. The Hague: common but rivalry is not.
Recommended publications
  • 427663 1 En Bookbackmatter 215..297
    Conclusion The intention of this book was to test the interdependence between medieval theories of optics and the practice of perspective. More particularly we chose to examine the question of physiological optics, because the proof of a connection between optics and perspective would be less probative if it took geometric optics as its starting point (for example, it is not possible to separate the respective influences of optics and geometry in the use of similar triangles, a method shared by the two sciences). In contrast, the theory of binocular vision is critically dependent on the particular way in which optics was addressed in the Middle Ages, combining as it did geometric and physical optics with anatomy and ocular physiology. It is within this framework that the differing interpretations of “axial perspective” and “two-point perspective” were examined here. As has been seen, these forms of representations do not correspond to any type of parallel projection that places the spectator at infinity, therefore excluding the possibility that they might constitute axonometric (isometric, dimetric, trimetric) or oblique (cavalier and military) pro- jections. One could go even further in the critical analysis of the two-point system of representation: 1. Two-point perspective is not a linear perspective, because the two vanishing points are separated by distances that are many times greater than the maximal metric error. 2. Two-point perspective bears no relationship to bifocal perspective (Parronchi’s hypothesis). 3. Two-point perspective cannot be considered a form of trifocal perspective, whose object verticals are always represented by parallel lines. 4. The two vanishing points are not the result of an absence of parallelism in the side walls nor of an alteration in the parallelism intended to highlight the subject of the scene.
    [Show full text]
  • Brock String and the Horopter: a Perspective
    Brock String and the with neurological disorders such as cerebral 2 3 Horopter: A Perspective palsy and traumatic brain injury. A question that has been posed to us on Kenneth J. Ciuffreda, OD, PhD, more than one occasion, frequently with a FAAO, FCOVD-A, FARVO look ranging from inquisitiveness to distain, Diana P. Ludlam, BS, COVT is, “Why the horopter?” Certainly, this is a fair question. Unfortunately, the horopter seems Barry Tannen, OD, FCOVD to be misunderstood by many, and hence Naveen K. Yadav, BS Optom, the question. In our lectures, we have always MS, PhD, FAAO emphasized the concept of the horopter and its clinical importance, with its clinical analog being the all-important “Brock String.”4 Our Over the past number of years, we immediate reply to this question has been, have been involved in the teaching “Why not, as the concept of the horopter is of both normal and abnormal the underpinning for, and basis of, nearly ALL binocular vision at several optometry aspects of normal and abnormal binocular colleges, national and international vision”. This answer usually results in a pause, vision meetings, and vision research sometimes followed by the request for an institutes and hospitals. This has explanation, which we gladly provide in great involved both the underlying detail with numerous clinical examples. theory and its multitude of clinical The horopter is a wonderful pedagogical implications, including the sensory, and clinical tool, as it blends and integrates PERSPECTIVE motor, and perceptual domains and basic aspects
    [Show full text]
  • Ibn Al-Haytham Sur La Vision Binoculaire: Un Précurseur De L’Optique Physiologique Dominique Raynaud
    Ibn al-Haytham sur la vision binoculaire: un précurseur de l’optique physiologique Dominique Raynaud To cite this version: Dominique Raynaud. Ibn al-Haytham sur la vision binoculaire: un précurseur de l’optique physi- ologique. Arabic Sciences and Philosophy, Cambridge University Press (CUP), 2003, 13, pp.79-99. halshs-00005585 HAL Id: halshs-00005585 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00005585 Submitted on 15 Nov 2005 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Slightly revised for Arabic Science and Philosophy, 2003, 13: 79-99 Ibn al-Haytham sur la vision binoculaire: un précurseur de l'optique physiologique DOMINIQUE RAYNAUD Université Pierre-Mendès-France (Grenoble) Résumé: L'optique physiologique moderne introduit les notions relatives aux conditions de fusion des ima- ges binoculaires par le concept de correspondance, prêté à Christiaan Huygens (1704), et par une expérience at- tribuée à Christoph Scheiner (1619). L'article montre que la conceptualisation de l'expérience remonte en fait à Ptolémée (90-168) et à Ibn al-Haytham (m. ap. 1040), et précise les connaissances que ce dernier avait des méca- nismes de la vision binoculaire. Il est ensuite expliqué pourquoi Ibn al-Haytham, mathématicien mais ici expéri- mentateur, ne donne pas la forme circulaire de l'horoptère théorique, dont la construction revient à Gerhard Vieth (1818) et Johannes Müller (1826).
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Eye Movements Are Adapted to the Natural Environment
    The Journal of Neuroscience, April 10, 2019 • 39(15):2877–2888 • 2877 Systems/Circuits Binocular Eye Movements Are Adapted to the Natural Environment X Agostino Gibaldi and Martin S. Banks Vision Science Program, School of Optometry University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720 Humans and many animals make frequent saccades requiring coordinated movements of the eyes. When landing on the new fixation point, the eyes must converge accurately or double images will be perceived. We asked whether the visual system uses statistical regu- larities in the natural environment to aid eye alignment at the end of saccades. We measured the distribution of naturally occurring disparities in different parts of the visual field. The central tendency of the distributions was crossed (nearer than fixation) in the lower field and uncrossed (farther) in the upper field in male and female participants. It was uncrossed in the left and right fields. We also measuredhorizontalvergenceaftercompletionofvertical,horizontal,andobliquesaccades.Whentheeyesfirstlandedneartheeccentric target, vergence was quite consistent with the natural-disparity distribution. For example, when making an upward saccade, the eyes diverged to be aligned with the most probable uncrossed disparity in that part of the visual field. Likewise, when making a downward saccade, the eyes converged to enable alignment with crossed disparity in that part of the field. Our results show that rapid binocular eye movements are adapted to the statistics of the 3D environment, minimizing the need for large corrective vergence movements at the end of saccades. The results are relevant to the debate about whether eye movements are derived from separate saccadic and vergence neural commands that control both eyes or from separate monocular commands that control the eyes independently.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility SIXTH EDITION
    Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility SIXTH EDITION Binocular Vision and Ocular Motility THEORY AND MANAGEMENT OF STRABISMUS Gunter K. von Noorden, MD Emeritus Professor of Ophthalmology Cullen Eye Institute Baylor College of Medicine Houston, Texas Clinical Professor of Ophthalmology University of South Florida College of Medicine Tampa, Florida Emilio C. Campos, MD Professor of Ophthalmology University of Bologna Chief of Ophthalmology S. Orsola-Malpighi Teaching Hospital Bologna, Italy Mosby A Harcourt Health Sciences Company St. Louis London Philadelphia Sydney Toronto Mosby A Harcourt Health Sciences Company Editor-in-Chief: Richard Lampert Acquisitions Editor: Kimberley Cox Developmental Editor: Danielle Burke Project Manager: Agnes Byrne Production Manager: Peter Faber Illustration Specialist: Lisa Lambert Book Designer: Ellen Zanolle Copyright ᭧ 2002, 1996, 1990, 1985, 1980, 1974 by Mosby, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmit- ted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photo- copy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without per- mission in writing from the publisher. NOTICE Ophthalmology is an ever-changing field. Standard safety precautions must be followed, but as new research and clinical experience broaden our knowledge, changes in treatment and drug therapy may become necessary or appropriate. Readers are advised to check the most current product information provided by the manufacturer of each drug to be administered to verify the recommended dose, the method and duration of administration, and contraindications. It is the responsibility of the treating physician, relying on experience and knowledge of the patient, to determine dosages and the best treatment for each individual pa- tient.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision and Space Perception
    CHAPTER 2 Binocular Vision and Space Perception ithout an understanding of the physiology only one visual object is perceived by the ob- Wof binocular vision it becomes difficult, if server. This phenomenon is so natural to us that not impossible, to appreciate its anomalies. The the naive observer is not surprised by it; he is reader is well advised to study this chapter thor- surprised only if he sees double. Yet the oughly since important basic concepts and termi- opposite—single binocular vision from two dis- nology used throughout the remainder of this book tinct retinal images—is the truly remarkable phe- are introduced and defined. It is of historical inter- nomenon that requires an explanation. est that most of these concepts and terms have only been with us since the nineteenth century when they were introduced by three men who may Relative Subjective Visual be considered among the fathers of modern visual Directions physiology: Johannes Mu¨ller, Hermann von Helm- Whenever a retinal area is stimulated by light holtz, and Ewald Hering. The basic laws of binoc- entering the eye, the stimulus is perceived not ular vision and spatial localization that were laid only as being of a certain brightness and color down by these giants of the past form the very and of a certain form but also as always being foundation on which our current understanding of localized in a certain direction in visual space. strabismus and its symptoms and sensory conse- One cannot have a visual impression without quences is based. seeing it somewhere. If the stimulated retinal area is located to the left of the fovea, it is seen in the right half of the field; if it is located to the right Fusion, Diplopia, and the Law of the fovea, it is seen in the left half of the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Ibn Al-Haytham, the Arab Who Brought Greek Optics Into Focus for Latin Europe
    Advances in Ophthalmology & Visual System Literature Review Open Access Ibn al-Haytham, the Arab who brought Greek optics into focus for Latin Europe Abstract Volume 9 Issue 2 - 2019 Ibn al-Haytham remains an unknown figure in the development of optics from the Robert McQuaid OD ancient Greeks to Latin Europe. He put to rest the idea of visual rays being emitted from the eye, demonstrating vision occurs by intromission of light rays to the eye. Through Faculty of Optometry, Ramkhamhaeng University, Thailand observational experimentation he characterized the behavior of light and described many visual phenomena that are now considered visual perception. Ibn al-Haytham’s Correspondence: Robert McQuaid, O.D, Front Range Family most significant work, Kitab al-Manazir was written in the eleventh century and Eye Care, Greenwood Village, CO, USA, Faculty of Optometry, was translated to Latin, as De aspectibus during the middle of the thirteenth century. Ramkhamhaeng University, Bangkok, Thailand, 4920 S Yosemite, Its introduction to Latin Europe stimulated the study of optics within the nascent Ste C5, Greenwood Village, CO 80111 USA, Tel +1 303-810- universities of Europe and provided a model to conduct experiments and influenced 6178, Fax+1 303-220-5938, Email natural philosophers for centuries. Received: March 18, 2019 | Published: April 12, 2019 Keywords: perspectivist optics, intromission, extramission, eidola, arabic philosophers, witelo, pecham, kepler Introduction resources he realized his plan wasn’t feasible, and allegedly feigned madness to avoid the wrath of the labile al-Hakim. He remained under In antiquity there was no differentiation between the eye, light and house arrest for ten years until the Caliph’s death.
    [Show full text]
  • Studies of Human Depth Perception in Real and Pictured Environments By
    Studies of human depth perception in real and pictured environments by Emily Averill Cooper A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Neuroscience in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Professor Clifton M. Schor Professor Dennis M. Levi Professor David Whitney Fall 2012 Abstract Studies of human depth perception in real and pictured environments by Emily Averill Cooper Doctor of Philosophy in Neuroscience University of California, Berkeley Professor Martin S. Banks, Chair Determining the three-dimensional (3D) layout of the natural environment is an important but difficult visual function. It is difficult because the 3D layout of a scene and the shapes of 3D objects are not explicit in the 2D retinal images. The visual system uses both binocular (two eyes) and monocular (one eye) cues to determine 3D layout from these images. The same cues that are used to determine the layout of real scenes are also present in photographs and realistic pictures of 3D scenes. The work described in this dissertation examines how people perceive 3D layout in the natural environment and in pictures. Chapter 1 provides an introduction to the specific 3D cues that will be studied. Chapter 2 describes a series of experiments examining how the visual system’s use of binocular cues might be adaptive to improve depth perception in the natural environment. Chapter 3 describes a series of experiments examining how the visual system interprets monocular linear perspective cues, and how this can lead to perceptual distortions in perceived 3D from pictures.
    [Show full text]
  • This Dissertation Has Been 64-9559 Microfilmed Exactly As Received INVESTIGATION of the HOROPTER and the Ohio State University
    This dissertation has been 64-9559 microfilmed exactly as received ESKRIDGE, Jess Boyd, 1 9 2 8 - INVESTIGATION OF THE HOROPTER AND THE APPARENT FRONTAL PLANE. The Ohio State University, Ph.D., 1964 Physiology University Microfilms, Inc., Ann Arbor, Michigan INVESTIGATION OP THE HOROPTER AND THE APPARENT FRONTAL PLANE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By JESS BOYD ESKRIDGE, B.Se., M. Opt., M.Sc. ****** The Ohio State University 1964 Approved by: ISLXJKAAA/\ v/\ • rv Ad vise i* School of Optometry ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A list of those individuals who have aided me in the attainment of this goal would indeed be long, and the acknowledgment of each would be impractical. I would be amiss, however, if I did not give special recognition and express my sincere appreciation to these few. I am grateful to Dr. Fred Hebbard for his interest and for the incentive that he gave me to begin graduate study, to the members of the American Optometric Foundation for the Research Fellowship awarded me made it possible for me to pursue this program, and to Dickson Call and the staff at the Ohio State University Numerical Computation Laboratory for their assistance in programming and operating the 1620 IBM computer. Only those who have "trod this path" can fully appre­ ciate the encouragement, guidance, and help given by my adviser, Dr. Glenn A. Fry. His devotion and the unselfish giving of his time, effort, and knowledge have been and will always be a stimulating example to those that know him.
    [Show full text]
  • The Experimental Study of Binocular Vision by Ibn Al-Haytham and Its Legacy in the West*
    127 Hist Ophthal Intern 2018, Vol.2: 127-151 The Experimental Study of Binocular Vision by Ibn al-Haytham and Its Legacy in the West* Dominique Raynaud** Abstract Early modern physiological optics introduced the concept of correspondence through the study of the con- ditions for the fusion of binocular images. The formulation of this concept has traditionally been ascribed to Christiaan Huygens (1667) and to an experiment often attributed to Christoph Scheiner (1619). How- ever, Scheiner’s experiment had already been conceptualized, first by Ptolemy (90–168 AD), then by Ibn al-Haytham (d. after 1040). The extent of the latter’s knowledge of the mechanisms of binocular vision is analyzed. It is explained why Ibn al-Haytham, who addressed this problem as an experimentalist, suc- ceeded in discovering the theoretical horopter (the locus of points in space that yields single vision) and yet failed to recognize that the horizontal line of the horopter could be described as a circular plane around the viewer’s head, credit for which goes to Vieth (1818) and Müller (1826). Through his experiments, Ibn al-Haytham established the notion of corresponding points, explored the cases of homonymous (direct) and heteronymous (crossed) diplopias, and prepared the ground for the discovery of Panum’s fusional area. Finally, the influence of al-Haytham’s pioneering treatise Kitāb al-manāẓir (Book of Optics) on West- ern science is examined, by studying the translations and commentaries that were available in the Latin world. Introduction Galen attempted to rectify this lacuna by A key chapter in the physiology of optics con- deriving a definition of the disparity between siders the conditions for the fusion of the the quasi-images produced by the two eyes quasi- or displaced images generated by the from one of Euclid’s propositions (Optica, two eyes.
    [Show full text]
  • Are Corresponding Points Fixed?
    Vision Research 41 (2001) 2457–2473 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Are corresponding points fixed? James M. Hillis a, Martin S. Banks a,b a Vision Science Program, Uni6ersity of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-2020, USA b Department of Psychology, Uni6ersity of California, Berkeley, CA 94720-2020, USA Received 16 October 2000; received in revised form 30 January 2001 Abstract Several investigators have claimed that the retinal coordinates of corresponding points shift with vergence eye movements. Two kinds of shifts have been reported. First, global shifts that increase with retinal eccentricity; such shifts would cause a flattening of the horopter at all viewing distances and would facilitate fusion of flat surfaces. Second, local shifts that are centered on the fovea; such shifts would cause a dimple in the horopter near fixation and would facilitate fusion of points fixated at extreme viewing distances. Nearly all of the empirical evidence supporting shifts of corresponding points comes from horopter measurements and from comparisons of subjective and objective fixation disparity. In both cases, the experimenter must infer the retinal coordinates of corresponding points from external measurements. We describe four factors that could affect this inference: (1) changes in the projection from object to image points that accompany eye rotation and accommodation, (2) fixation errors during the experimental measurements, (3) non-uniform retinal stretching, and (4) changes in the perceived direction of a monocular point when presented adjacent to a binocular point. We conducted two experiments that eliminated or compensated for these potential errors. In the first experiment, observers aligned dichoptic test lines using an apparatus and procedure that eliminated all but the third error.
    [Show full text]