A Model of -directed Morphogenesis Part I - Julia Sets

Michael Levin Genetics Dept. Harvard Medical School (617) 432-7758 Inet: [email protected]

Running title: "Julia model of field-driven development" Summary

One paradigm used in understanding the control of morphogenetic events is the concept of positional , where sub-organismic components (such as cells) act in response to positional cues. It is important to determine what kinds of spatio-temporal may be ob- tained by such a method, and what the characteristics of such a morphogenetic process might be. This paper presents a computer model of morphogenesis based on gene activity driven by interpreting a positional information field.

In this model, the interactions of mutually-regulating developmental genes are viewed as a £¡ from ¢¡ to , and are modeled by the algebra. Functions in complex variables are used to simulate genetic interactions resulting in position-dependent differentia- tion. This is shown to be equivalent to computing modified Julia sets, and is seen to be suffi- cient to produce a very rich set of morphologies which are similar in appearance and several important characteristics to those of real organisms. The properties of this model can be used to study the potential role of fields and positional information as guiding factors in morphogene- sis, as the model facilitates the study of static images, -series (movies), and experimental alterations of the developmental process. It is thus shown that gene interactions can be modeled as a multi-dimensional algebra, and that only two interacting genes are sufficient for 1) com- plex formation, 2) chaotic differentiation behavior, and for 3) production of sharp edges from a continuous positional information field. This model is meant to elucidate the properties of the process of positional information- guided bio-morphogenesis, not to serve as a of any particular organism’s develop- ment. Good quantitative data is not currently available on the interplay of gene products in mor- phogenesis. Thus, no attempt is made to link the images produced with actual pictures of any particular real organism. A brief introduction to top-down models and positional information is followed by the formal definition of the model. Then, the implications of the resulting mor- phologies to biological development are discussed, in terms of static , parametrization studies, time series (movies made from individual frames), and behavior of the model in of experimental perturbations. All figures (in grayscale1), formulas, and parameter values needed to re-create the figures and movies are included.

1 Color slides and videotapes are available from the author. Introduction The of Artificial Computer models can serve as biological theories (as in Partridge and Lopez, 1984, Gar- finkel, 1984). The rapidly growing field of uses computer models to emphasize the common processes and characteristics of living things, rather than their physico-chemical implementation. The accent is on the role of bioinformation, cybernetic control processes, com- plexity and theory, rather than the study of specific instances of physicochemical proc- esses (Apter, 1966, Narendra and Thompson, 1986, Langton, 1990, Langton, 1991).

"Computer provide the experimental biologist with a much needed means for doing experiments on theories. We usually think of experiments as something performed on cells, to determine whether the cells’ responses will match predictions made by a theory. However, when dealing with complex phenomena and with correspondingly complex theories, what a given hypothesis predicts can be less than obvious. It can be even more difficult to perceive which components in the hypothesis give rise to a particular prediction, much less to know what alterna- tive classes of hypothesis would have predicted the same results. Lastly, anyone with sufficient insight to predict the consequences of a given theory may need ob- jective methods for proving it to others. Computer simulations can help solve all these problems." (Harris, 1990)

This approach is at the intersection of biology, mathematics, , and computer , and has mainly involved computer models of behavior or evolution. Some attempts to model various developmental phenomena (such as cell proliferation, cleavage, cell migration, specific , etc.) by creating computer simulations of the behavior of the underlying components have been made (Ransom, 1981, and Meinhardt, 1982, are good summaries of such efforts). Interesting special cases are found in Düchting and Vogelsaenger, 1984 and 1985, and Thom, 1983. Good examples of the common "bottom-up" model (where the desired phenome- non emerges from a simulation of empirically-derived behaviors of lower-level components) can be found in Goel and Rogers, 1978, and Rogers and Goel, 1978. In the "top-down" model (sometimes termed "informal", Conrad, 1981), the researcher cre- ates a which behaves like the modeled (in some interesting way), without too much regard for the empirical validity of specific lower-level components; the model’s empirical fitness is tested at a higher level. This approach can be used to study charac- teristics of processes or general schemes. For example, one may create a well-fitting model of some animal’s social behavior based on general principles of cooperation, , and in- formation theory, without any regard as to whether the components of the have any physical counterparts in the animal’s brain (for example, Krebs et al., 1978, Axelrod and Ham- ilton, 1981). These kinds of models usually precede bottom-up models, and are much more common in ethology, evolutionary theory, and the cognitive than in developmental bi- ology. Excellent examples of top-down models are given in Raup and Michelson, 1965, Rozen- berg and Salomaa, 1986, and Slack, 1991. L-systems (Lindenmeyer, 1989, Prusinkiewicz and Lindenmeyer, 1990) are among the best examples of top-down models because they possess the appropriate higher-order behavior even though there is no known physical mechanism in plants

-1- which explicitly implements the symbol manipulation rules used in an L-system. Kauffman’s work (1969, 1974, 1990) with dynamics of abstract logic elements networks is another good ex- ample, since these studies capture higher-order rules governing gene interactions without using characteristics of real genes. Whereas the bottom-up model tries to be as realistic and focused as possible, providing empirically-accurate data of some narrow and specific aspect of development (for example, Ransom et al., 1984, and Meinhardt, 1984), the top-down model attempts to derive the overall pattern of control, information , and higher-order characteristics at the cost of losing some lower-level realism. Bottom-up models are able to provide more concrete information about some narrowly-defined process; the benefit of the top-down model is that it allows insight into the higher-order properties of processes, and can be used when sufficient data and/or theory for a bottom-up model is not available. These models are useful in the study of developmental mor- phogenesis because embryonic development is very likely to involve general mechanisms of in- creasing ; at the same time, much low-level data (such as quantitative mechanisms of gene interactions) is currently unavailable. This paper presents a positional field-driven model of development which is intermediate between the bottom-up and top-down types. Positional information and field concepts in biology The basic problem of pattern formation can be viewed from one (or a combination) of two general perspectives. The developing organism’s resulting morphology can be seen as a product of subunits following a sort of master plan - a blueprint (at some level of detail) of the whole organism. Or, it may be an emergent phenomenon, where the overall pattern emerges as the re- sult of purely local interactions between subunits governed by rules which say nothing about the overall pattern (best exemplified by cellular automata systems, as in Gutowitz, 1991). An intermediate paradigm involves positional information (Wolpert, 1969, 1971, 1989). That is, each unit (usually thought of as a cell, but this could apply equally well to tissues, cell sheets, or sub-cellular components) is able to determine its position within the organism, and act (differentiate, migrate, proliferate, etc.) based upon that positional information. The posi- tional information is usually thought of as coordinates in a field, which then can be a mecha- nism for long-range control and information transfer (see Cooke, 1975 for a review of field theories in biology). The paradigm is thus a combination of the two approaches in that it in- cludes both purely local decision-making (with an emphasis on the distinction between the field itself, and various components’ responses to it), as well as a global entity (the positional infor- mation field itself). In this view, the overall morphology emerges from units following rules which specify actions based on the unit’s position; the process of morphogenesis can be "char- acterized as a series of solutions of the morphogenetic field whose superposition gives way to complex stable patterns." (Goodwin, 1988). The role of positional information in early develop- ment is reviewed in Bard, 1990, and Slack, 1991; there is also evidence that adult animals maintain positional information cues (Harrison, 1918, Raff and Kauffman, 1983). The field is a very attractive theoretical construct for modeling global influences and thus explaining static stability as well as temporal change of a complex morphology, and the ability of numerous processes to combine into a coherent whole and to tend toward that configuration when experimentally perturbed (Goodwin, 1985):

-2- "In real organisms, there must at critical or even continuously be an overlay of global instructions that keeps the ramifying network of inductive interactions in the correct relationship by specifying reference to whole- organism properties." (Thomson, 1988)

Fields have recently become truly useful in the study of development through mathematical models (see for example French et al., 1966, Goodwin and Cohen, 1969, Goodwin and Trainor, 1980, Ransom, 1981, Trainor 1982, Goodwin and LaCroix 1984, Goodwin et al., 1987, Nagor- cka, 1989, Brandts and Trainor, 1990a, 1990b, Kondo, 1992) and empirical confirmations of the their existence and crucial role in development (Burr, 1941, Lund, 1947, Cooke, 1972-1973, Jaffe, 1979, Kropf and Caldwell, 1983, Nucitelli, 1986, and Gow, 1987). Field theories are use- ful in explaining the phenomena of cleavage (for example, Goodwin and Trainor, 1980), regen- eration (for example, Tevlin and Trainor, 1985, Mittenthal and Nuelle, 1988), supernumerary limb production (for example, Bryant and Iten, 1976, Mittenthal and Trevarrow, 1984, Gardiner and Bryant, 1989), and cancer and malignancy (Matioli, 1987). Two interesting top-down field-based models are given by Evans (1986), and Thompson (1961). Evans’ model involves generation of morphologies very similar to that of the human fetus, based on electromagnetic field characteristics of the . Thompson (1961, Palmer, 1957) show that it is possible to convert between the morphologies of various existing animals by applying simple mathematical deformations to their 2-dimensional field representa- tions. Field theories must specify several characteristics of the process: the physical nature of the field, what exactly perceives this information, what the time characteristics of field percep- tion are, and how the field is in turn modified by the units which read it. A field can consist of a chemical morphogen gradient or system of gradients (for example, Russel, 1985, French, 1988, Green et al., 1992), an electromagnetic field (patterns of ionic cur- rents and potential differences; for example, Nucitelli, 1984, Nucitelli, 1986), an osmotic field (O’Shea, 1988), a field of differential adhesiveness (haptotactic fields, as in Murray and Oster, 1984, Zackson and Steinberg, 1988, Lord and Sanders, 1992) or a visco-elastic tension field (for example, Lakirev and Belousov, 1986, Hart et al., 1989, Brière and Goodwin, 1990). The salient is that there is some physical factor which forms a field throughout the embryo1, such that units (cells, for example) can interact with it and thus obtain information about their position within the embryo. The model described in this paper is not committed to any specific physical nature of the field; it applies to all Cartesian fields. The basic unit of field-reading need not necessarily be individual cells. It may be a sub- cellular component (for example, Frankel, 1974), or a cell group or sheet (as in Hart and Trai- nor, 1989). The temporal characteristics of field reading have also been studied; for example, Savel’ev (1988) shows that neuroepithelial cells in the amphibian brain read their positional in- formation (in this case, maintained as a mechanical strain field) at discrete times, exhibiting time-limited field events. This model likewise assumes the field interpretation to oc- cur in discrete steps. The dynamics of interactions between sources of positional information fields and the fields themselves are a very important topic. As a simplifying assumption, how- ever, in this paper the focus will be on an existent static field and the interactions of receivers

1Though it is not necessarily the case that every part of the embryo utilizes the information in it.

-3- with the information it carries. Many attempts to model specific developmental processes exist (see Ransom, 1981, and Meinhardt, 1982 for reviews). Some are based on continuous functions and differential equa- tions (such as Yates and Pate, 1989) while others are based on finite-state automata and discrete finite-mathematical methods (for example, Herman and Rozenberg, 1975, and MacDonald, 1983). Although the field concept has been useful in various specific applications, it is an open question as to whether positional information in general can account for the morphological complexity of real organisms. In order to assess the potential role and implications of field theo- ries in development, it is first necessary to show that this type of mechanism is capable of gen- erating the kind of order found in biological morphogenesis. It is also interesting to examine some of the features of the morphogenetic process which is controlled in this way. To these ends, a computer model was developed. This model turned out to be remarkably similar to algo- rithms used to generate images, which have been extensively used to model complete biological shapes (for example, Prusinkiewicz and Lindenmeyer, 1990). The original model described in this paper attempts a wider view of the control processes of development, by modeling cell decisions controlled by field-specified positional information. The model has a strong bottom-up component in that it is based on a process which is known to be crucial in bio-morphogenesis - determination of cell fates via gene interactions. It is top- down in that it is not a simulation of any specific actual organism, but of a type of process - positional information interpretation based on genetic interactions. It is not being claimed that the specific formulas (used below) describe any existent organism; rather, it is a computer im- plementation of a possible morphogenetic control process. Thus, the strength of the model is not based on the fact that the images produced may look like biological forms; it lies in the fact that the model can be used to study the higher-order properties of field-directed gene interac- tions in morphogenesis. It makes several concrete predictions, and explains several key phe- nomena. When quantitative information on morphogenetic gene interactions becomes available, it can be used within the context of the model described herein to make concrete predictions about specific organisms. The model Interpretation Morphogenesis is seen to be a three-stage process: 1) a positional information field is speci- fied (by factors in the external environment, my the mother’s organism, or by components within the embryo itself), 2) cells go through a complex set of genetic interactions and pheno- typic effects, and then achieve some final genetic state based on their positions within this field, and 3) this genetic state determines the behavior of the cell (migration, proliferation, apoptosis, adhesion, etc.). The mechanical interactions of the cells finally determine the overall morphol- ogy of the organism. The current model consists of a computer which simulates cell behavior within a field. Each cell finds itself within some unique position in a two-dimensional field; a mathe- matical (shown to be isomorphic to a modified Julia set, after Pickover, 1990) then de- scribes the behavior of the cell at that position. Of course it is not being suggested that there is a mechanism within the cell that computes this function in the sense of formal computation; rather, the function is descriptive of the cell’s behavior (much as ’s laws describe the

-4- motion of the moon, without any need for the moon to formally compute the functions). This model is similar to Thompson’s (1961) model since applying the deformations to the axes is equivalent to applying a deformation to the coordinates points making up the image. Thus, Thompson’s model also represents a positional-field interpretation model, but this is different from the model presented in this paper because his model applies a deformation to an existing form, while the model described here generates the morphology de novo. Specifically, the simulation works as follows. In achieving some fate, each cell goes through a complex cycle of reactions between various key gene products (similar to that in Kauffman, 1969, 1974, 1990). Below, "gene interactions" refers to regulation of expression of gene prod- ucts by other gene products in a mutually-regulating network (such as the MyoD family, Wein- traub, 1993). Thus a cell at a certain time can be viewed as being controlled by a collection of concentrations of gene products , which interact in time to up- and down-regulate each other’s expression. These interactions can be viewed as the instantiation of a function Φ() which converts a given cell state into a different cell state with passage of time (which, as a simplification, is taken to be discrete). The cycle of interactions between the genes which finally stabilizes with some differentiation path is modeled by apply- ing the function F recurrently to some initial (totipotent) cell state, until a stable point is reached. The stable point can be an instance of one of two possibilities. After N cycles, either some of the gene products have increased beyond a certain threshold and activated a specific program (the cell has differentiated into a specific fate), or, this has not happened, meaning that the cell is like a stem cell - it stays perpetually undifferentiated. In this model, only two gene products are considered, X and Y. Though in reality a larger set of genes is clearly responsible for the morphogenesis of features in organisms, it is one of the important results of this study that two genes are sufficient to produce a very rich set of mor- phologies with properties similar to those of real morphogenetic processes. Each cell is at a cer- tain position x,y within the field (two-dimensional, as a simplification). This position is inter- preted (perhaps by a mechanism similar to that of Hox genes, as in Tabin, 1991) to arrive at a specific initial concentration of 2 gene products, . Then, a function is applied (simulating gene regulation) which alters X and Y in an interdependent manner (for example, it might map Xt to Xt-1-c·Yt-1, modeling a linear inhibition of expression of X by the product of Y). The function is taken to be a map in complex numbers1, so that is represented as X+Yi, and is applied for a number of iterations. At the end, either the magnitude of X or the magnitude of Y has reached a certain threshold value, or neither has. Whether the limit has been reached, and if so, how quickly, determines the "type" of the cell. This algorithm is extremely similar to one used in generating various fractal images, and is isomorphic to Julia set computation. in biology Julia sets are generated by a fractal algorithm, and are closely related to Mandelbrot sets. While the mathematics behind these objects is fairly well researched, their higher-order (mor- phological) characteristics are relatively poorly understood. Some considerations of the mathe- matical properties of complex iterations are found in Beardon 1990, and Beardon 1991. This is a new (the first articles began to appear around 1968) and very active topic in mathematics. Fractals’ main features are widely differing behaviors for close initial points (chaos, due to their

1Thus an N-dimensional algebra can be defined for modeling the interactions of N genes.

-5- recursiveness which magnifies small differences), their production of images which are self- similar in regions, and the incredible richness of which is produced by very short for- mulas (Peitgen and Saupe, 1988). Note that fractals’ chaotic behavior provides a possible an- swer to the question (Lewis et al., 1977) of how a smooth (continuous) field can give rise to discrete cell states (the problem of sharp thresholds): the potential for two very close complex field values to have widely different fates under iterative transformations is a hallmark of fractal (chaotic) . Thus, after a time of genetic interactions, two close cell states and can diverge to become widely differing cell types, providing sharp differentiation boundaries within a continuous field. Fractal algorithms are highly "epigenetic", and the generativeness is uni-directional. That is, given a fractal formula it is very easy to produce the complex morphology it codes for (as a complex biological organism is formed as a result of chemical reactions driven by the informa- tion content of DNA); however, given a complete morphology, it is exceedingly difficult to find a short formula that codes for it (as would be very difficult, given the morphology of an organ- ism, to determine the content of the DNA which would produce that organism). The same sort of reason that may explain why there is no inheritance of acquired characteristics (that the epi- genetic character of development makes it nearly impossible to directly translate some charac- teristic of an organism back into its DNA) makes it very difficult to find a fractal formula cod- ing for some given morphology. The fact that Julia sets map positions in a plane to discrete scalar values by iterating interde- pendent changes of several independent values makes them a natural candidate for a positional information model of pattern formation due to differential gene activation. Pietronero and To- satti, 1986, and Feder and Aharony, 1990 present overviews of the importance of fractals in the study of physical processes. Fractal and chaotic algorithms form the basis for many physical processes important in development (such as diffusion-limited aggregation in Witten and Sander, 1981 and Witten and Cates, 1986, viscous fingering in Sander, 1987, and , in Oxaal et al., 1987). A great sourcebook for this information is the classic Mandelbrot, 1982. These algorithms have been used in modeling and analyzing biophysical systems (for example, ion channel activity in Liebovitch and Töth, 1990, neural activity in Ishibashi et al., 1988, and dielectric response in biological tissues in Dissado, 1990), various biological phenomena (for example, periodontal disease in Landini, 1991, metabolism in Sernetz et al., 1985, and for many more examples, West, 1991), evolution (Green, 1991) and the development of different compo- nents of embryos, such as nerves, blood vessels, and mosaic organs (see for example Meakin, 1986, Tsonis and Tsonis, 1987, Tsonis, 1988, Tsonis and Tsonis, 1989, Tsonis et al., 1989, Mainster, 1990, Iannaccone, 1990, Smith et al., 1991, Vaidya et al., 1991, Kiani and Hudetz, 1991, Smith et al., 1991, Bailly et al., 1991, and Burlando et al., 1991). Complex systems (of which biological organisms are the quintessential example) have sev- eral characteristics: "they have many attributes with complex interdependencies and rules of in- teractions which are non-obvious, they are usually non-linear, and their dynamics appear sto- chastic, random, or fuzzy" (Goel and Thompson, 1988). These aspects are admirably captured by Julia set behavior. The hypothesis that the differentiation pathway for a cell is determined by iterative chaotic processes has an interesting consequence, mirroring a similar result in the theory of dynamical systems. While for any given cell the final outcome (i.e., which differentiation fate is taken) is

-6- physically pre-determined, in certain (though not all) cases it may be impossible to know what that outcome will be, even with good data about the initial gene product concentrations, since the gene interactions are iterative and thus magnify minute differences in initial conditions. Two widely divergent differentiation pathways may thus result from very close (and perhaps indistinguishable within some technique’s precision) gene activity levels. The formal definition of the Julia set model algorithm The basic algorithm is as follows: for each position in the field (it is assumed that the field overlays a rectangular patch of cells, with every position occupied), the formula is applied, and the resulting cell behavior (for example, a differentiation) is plotted at that position as a given color. The mapping of cell "types" to colors is arbitrary. In pseudocode:

for each X from Xmin to Xmax for each Y from Ymin to Ymax plot a point at (X,Y) in a color T depending on Φ(X,Y,υ);

Thus, the function Φ() maps each position (X,Y) and a constant υ to a type T. X and Y are varied within some range of values, and there is a finite and discrete number of types T. This corresponds to the biological case of a continuous population of primitive (undifferentiated), non-motile cells, within a 2-dimensional positional information field, which differentiate as a function of their position in the field. Thus, formally a given morphology is a quintuple M = <λ, Φ, υ, R, T > where: • λ is the field limitation - the domain of field values seen by the subunits (upper and lower bounds on X and Y values for the points examined). In terms of biological development, this expresses the boundaries on the field values that cells experience. • Φ is the function - a mathematical function describing the input-output relation between the cell’s position and its resulting type (describing the cell’s mechanism for interaction with the field - some type of receptor, or membrane mechanism, which is determined by the cell’s DNA). This function performs the iteration of the initial X,Y values through some complex formula. • υ, the constant used in the function, represents some existing identical information all of the cells have when they begin morphogenesis. • R is the resolution, which determines how many points in the interval from Xmin to Xmax and Ymin to Ymax are sampled. In terms of biological systems, this parameters specifies how much of the morphology is a result of field-cell interactions, and how much is to be provided by purely local interactions which fill in the spaces between the field-specified points. • T is the set of possible cell behaviors (differentiation pathways). Computer implementation In a computer program, the field is implemented as the R2 - a plane where each point corresponds to a complex number a+bi. Thus, for each point there is an X and Y value (corre- sponding to the concentrations of two different and orthogonally diffusing chemicals, in a mor-

-7- phogen field, for example, or the adhesiveness of two separate surfaces, in a tactile field). The function that is applied to the locations in the field is, in this model, a modified Julia set calcula- tion (which differ from true Julia sets in that a point is plotted if and only if |real(z)| < limit or |imaginary(z)| < limit). That, is, for each given point (complex number) Z, its value is recur- sively iterated through a formula such as z = z²+υ, and the original point Z is plotted in a color which is related to how many iterations occurred before |z| exceeded some limit value. Some formal theory regarding these functions can be found in Beardon, 1991. Note that the images thus produced are those of pre-pattern - they are analogous to an in situ mRNA hybridization because they are showing final genetic states of cells, not what the organ- ism actually looks like. The real morphology of an organism is derived from this pre-pattern by cell movements, mechanical deformations, apoptosis, etc., which are governed by the genetic states of the cells. This limit value is a constraint on which points appear in the final image, and its biological interpretation is thus as a of the importance of the positional information. A high limit value would allow relatively many points to be placed, indicating that cells’ fates are heavily influenced by the positional information. A low limit value would allow relatively few points to be placed, indicating that only certain key cells get their fate specification from the positional field, the rest being determined by local near-neighbor interactions. Note that the floating-point routines used in any computer’s math library have a limit on their accuracy. This provides a natural limit to the differences in positional information that the cells will be able to detect, and corresponds to real cells’ limits in sensitivity to field values which differ by very small values. Its important to realize that the formula in z is not identical with the function Φ() mentioned above. The function Φ() includes the formula, iteration, and cutoff decision, and is used only once for each point; it maps a given (x,y) pair to a given type (color). The definition of Φ() subsumes the iteration of the complex function, and the intermediate values of the real and imaginary components of z represent the concentrations of various gene products, as explained above. The actual formulas used to produce the figures are shown in Table 1. Note that on different certain parts of the pictures may look somewhat different than they do in the illustra- tions below. This is due to the accumulation of differences in ’ floating-point calcula- tion errors (which are magnified due to the iterations of the formulas). Biologically-relevant properties of morphologies produced by this model The figures show the of gene expression patterns that can be produced by only two gene products and a smooth linear Cartesian coordinate field. It is clear that this is sufficient to produce extremely rich patterns, which can be even further elaborated by cell movements and rearrangements to produce an embryo’s final morphology. Of course it is not being suggested that all of the features observed in the figures are actually used in organisms’ morphologies; rather these results show that the kinds of patterns found in biological systems can be plausibly produced by such a model. 1. General notes on static shapes and how they resemble real organisms’ morphologies Several general traits of these figures (see for example Figures 3-18) are similar to those of

-8- real organisms. Note how colors never merge - borders are well-defined; differentiation pro- ceeds in layers, with distinct boundaries. There are clear borders between separate structures; there are many instances of objects possessing a definite inside and outside. There are almost no straight lines; curves predominate. As in real biological organisms, the morphology is an epige- netic phenomenon of the formula expressing the genetic interactions - it is very difficult to pre- dict the large-scale morphological properties of the organism from the formula (genome). Repeated magnification reveals ever more intricate detail, but every function described in this paper possesses a top-most view - a scale beyond which nothing interesting happens. This indicates that for every organism, there are limits on which field values are valid (as perceived by the cells); the positional information field has definite edges. As in real organisms, there are recurrent spatial motifs - most of the images build on the same types of morphologies over and over again. Repeated segments are used in many images (see for example Figures 3, 24, and 57). Most figures show clear examples of structural (a of structural hierarchies in biological systems appears in Totafurno et al., 1980). The properties of these functions are also reminiscent of life. Many images are truly symmetrical along one axis (as in Figures 5, 7, and 8); some are radially symmetrical (such as Figure 20). Some are regular, but not symmetrical along any axis (such as Figures 15 and 23). And others, such as Figure 27, seem to be bilaterally symmetrical, but close inspection reveals differences which break the symmetry (look closely at the distribution of the solid black sectors in Figure 27, and at the outgrowth to the right of Figure 59, reminiscent of the kinds of asymmetries found in crabs and lobsters); this resembles the morphology of many organisms which are bilaterally symmetrical, except for various features (such as the heart and liver in the human). Many parts of the images are self-similar (see Figures 4 and 20), though are not exactly repeated units. There is usually some top-level , within which there are self-similar struc- tures. Biological forms display many examples of self-similarity (for examples see West and Goldberger, 1987, Mandelbrot, 1982, Nelson et al., 1990, West, 1991, Guyon and Stanley, 1991). This self-similarity implies that the algorithm itself, with a given formula, dictates a spe- cific shape, and this shape is basically the same in many regions of the field, at different resolu- tions. This means that 1) field values are relative, not absolute, and experiments with field- directed developmental systems can be done to test this, and that 2) parts of the field contain information for the whole organism. This "holographic" property of the model is interesting in light of the ability of many kinds of embryos (at certain stages) to regenerate from pieces, and recover the whole organism when pieces are removed. As with real embryos, not every region contains the crucial information, and only some regions can be "magnified" into the proper morphology. Real embryos also pos- sess this "relativity" of positional information (for example, in certain cases in early Drosophila axis formation, it’s the relative concentrations of two morphogens which matter, not their abso- lute concentrations). During morphogenesis there is a "matching of proportionate spatial struc- tures to the size of the field in which the pattern is formed. It appears as if a size-sensing mechanism exists which controls and adapts the detailed structures in accordance with the vari- able field size, so that their relative extent remains " (Papageorgiou and Venieratos, 1983). This is seen in the experiments on Xenopus (Cooke, 1973c, Cooke, 1979, Cooke, 1981), hydra (Bode and Bode, 1980), and a unicellular ciliate (Bakowska and Jerka-Dziadosz, 1980).

-9- How might this property be supported by a morphogenetic field? In general, patterns generated by diffusion reactions are very sensitive to changes of scale. However, a variation on Turing’s early model (Othmer and Pate, 1980) of spatial formation based on morphogen reaction- diffusion systems also exhibits scale-invariance. A theoretical basis for morphogen gradient fields which exhibit scale-invariance is given in Papageorgiou and Venieratos, 1983. In general, these images show that only two interacting genes are sufficient to produce rich morphologies from a smooth and continuous positional information field. This is an important result because it provides a convenient and biochemically plausible mechanism for how organismic complexity arises from two simple orthogonal vectors. Figure 34 shows a low- morphology. Note the simple circular layers of aggregated (mixed-color) tissue, and the simplicity of the formula by which it is produced. Note also that the image is radially symmetrical, except for a small horizontal on the right. Figure 35 shows the same formula as Figure 34, with an additional component. Notice that this is a some- what more complicated morphology, with modifications to the simple circular membrane of Figure 34, but still much simpler than any of the other figures. Figures 36 and 37 show two morphologies which correspond to gene interactions which fail to produce coherent morphologies. Note the failure of layers to join in many places, and the confused disordered arrangement of internal detail (especially apparent in Figure 37). Another interesting example is seen in Figure 41 (this is just a magnification of the overlaid by the dashed rectangle in Figure 1): note the complex behavior that occurs where two separate struc- tures overlap. This should be studied further in light of the competing field influence found in Xenopus (Cooke 1972a-c, 1973a-b). 2. Parametrization: how forms change in response to parameter changes An important set of questions can be answered by this model through parametrization stud- ies. The role of a given component (gene interaction rule) in producing the morphology can be studied by tracking the changes in the image as its value is slowly changed. Several things can be studied in this way: constants in the formula itself, the limit used in the cutoff point, the in- tercellular constant (υ), etc. The parametrization studies described below show that smoothly changing gene co-regulation functions from R² to R² are sufficient to produce the kinds of spatio-temporal processes that are observed in biological embryogenesis. It is seen that for functions of the form z = zn where n is small (between 1.0 and 4.0), chang- ing υ from 0+0i serves to produce a very rich morphology from an otherwise monotonous one. This suggests that for these kinds of morphologies, field-independent information is an impor- tant determinant of form. However, for higher values of n, making υ different from 0+0i serves to severely disrupt a morphology (causing skews in angles, etc.) which is complex by default. In formula 33 (one example of which is shown in Figure 28), changing the limit variable controls the degree of waviness of the outer layer. Changing the limit variable in Figure 1 changes the shape of one end and of the design in the circular center of the image. Figures 38 and 39 show an interesting phenomenon associated with changing the intercellular constant (υ). The formulas for both of these figures are the same, but the real parts of υ are slightly different. Observe that this change in υ can account for the appearance (or lack of it) of one single struc- ture, leaving the rest of the image unaffected. It is also interesting to note that amidst all the striking complexity of most figures there are which are structurally very simple.

-10- 3. Time series: development through function changes To simulate the time course of development, where the co-regulation of gene products changes with time, the basic algorithm was iterated, resulting in a "movie", where each frame was created using a slightly modified function ∆Φ() (since in this model the DNA of the organ- ism is represented by the function). This provides a continuous image of morphogenesis through time. The function can be changed continuously, or in discrete jumps, along one or more parameters. A custom program was written in C, on a Sun-4, to generate frames using the same Julia set algorithm, and display them rapidly using the Khoros1 package. The results cannot adequately be represented in the static medium of paper (videotapes are available from the author), but de- scriptions of the time series (called "movies" below) that were made can be instructive. The movies were made with the parameters shown in Table 2. Each movie consists of itera- tions of some formula, where all but one parameter is held constant. The remaining parameter is varied in small steps, and for each value of that parameter, a complete morphology is generated. These frames are then played back in order. In each movie, the choice of step size of the vary- ing parameter is arbitrary. Smaller step sizes result in movies with better grain (smoother transi- tions), but requiring correspondingly longer generation times. In general, most movies consist of periods of slow, smooth, continuous change, interspersed with periods of very rapid major changes. These rapid changes generally occur at the beginning of the movie, although some movies exist where there is a rapid period of change in the middle (corresponding to the drastic morphological alterations of metamorphosis, in biological forms). Throughout, it is seen that certain structures (usually, the outermost membrane) stay relatively constant in shape. Most movies show a shape that is generally static in size, while some have periods of growth. Some movies eventually settle down to a state where they change very little, corresponding to a completed morphology - the end of development. Note though that even in this state, very minor modifications can be seen from time to time (biological maintenance). All these characteristics are very similar to the properties of real biological development. Through a large series of experiments, it was determined that the formula is the most interesting parameter to vary. For example, movie 1 shows some interesting morphogenetic changes going on within the shape (reminiscent of the rearrangements accompanying gastrulation); another stunning feature appears about 75% of the way through the movie: a small part of the interior (on both sides of the axis of symmetry) acquires its own independent membrane, moves outward, breaks through the outer membrane of the main figure (which then slowly rejoins to reform perfectly), moves away, and disintegrates. Movie 2 shows a close-up of a membrane disjoinment - one can ob- serve the kinetics of the outer and inner layers as they slowly split and spread apart from each other. Movie 3 shows several interesting membrane properties (below, "membrane" denotes tissue layers which separate macroscopic compartments, not cell membranes). The outermost mem- brane is seen to actually consist of five separate layers, which shrink inwards one at a time; the result of these membrane movements is a circular sack with four separate but identical regions,

1Khoros is produced by the University of New Mexico.

-11- each possessing its own set of membranes. Throughout the movie, a region at the center of the embryo is seen to be in intimate contact with, and to change synchronously with, the deforming membranes. Movie 4 shows several interesting things. Very little change is seen until frame 65, at which point some drastic changes occur (reflecting the kinds of processes which can lead to metamorphosis). Two, and then four detachments (as in movie 1) occur. At the same time, the outer layer expands greatly in size. Movie 5 shows some continuous tissue growth, without the significant morphological re-arrangements seen in movies 1 and 4. Movie 6 shows some inter- esting stalk separations (a single stalk splitting into two shoots). Movie 7 shows a circular outer layer studded with spikes. As the time progresses, new spikes are continually added. This always happens in the same spot, with the other spikes spreading out radially to make room for it. Movie 8 shows a mostly-filled solid structure meta- morphose into an intricately shaped sparse one. Movie 9 shows a worm-like shape with cyclic changes in the the morphology of the outer layer. Movies 10 and 11 show different modes of ternary "mitosis" - a division of a roughly circular structure into three daughter structures, with partitioning of the internal components. The fact that it is possible to make these movies is interesting since the functions themselves are highly non-linear (i.e., they are chaotic: |f(x) - f(x+∆x)| is large even for small ∆x). Thus it was a surprise to the author to discover that a relatively large step size is enough to produce a smooth transition between frames, resulting in a continuous movie. It could have been the case that given the non-linear nature of these functions, small changes in the function produced im- ages that were very different, thus making it impossible to produce a smoothly changing movie. There are two basic ways to produce a movie. One is for each frame to represent a small change in some one parameter in function f(). The other is to produce a transition from one form to another. Suppose that one form is produced by function f(), and the other is produced by a function g(). One can then produce a smooth transition from form f() into form g() by in- troducing two real constants a and b, where in each frame a goes from 0.0 to 1.0, while simulta- neously b goes from 1.0 to 0.0. Thus, in the beginning f() dominates, and at the end of the movie, g() dominates. The magnitude of the step size in changing a and b (the grain) determines the number of frames, and thus, for how continuous the transformation from one shape to the

other is. For each frame, the function iterated becomes:

¤ ¤ ¤

¦ § ¨ = ¥ · ( ) + · ( )

It is interesting to see that different step sizes are needed to produce continuous movies be- tween different shapes. That is, there are shapes which easily evolve one into the other (for ex- ample, figures 11 and 12 are easily interconvertible), and there are shapes which do not (for example, the shape produced by z=sin(z) is very difficult to convert into z=z9.5). Thus, these morphologies form "natural kinds" (much as real organisms do) some of which are more closely related than others. This is all a higher-order property of the morphology, and cannot be determined directly by inspection from the formula itself. Thus, formulas expressing genetic in- teractions form obvious groups, on the basis of the morphologies they give rise to. The grain size necessary to produce a continuous movie is thus a measure of the closeness between two morphologies, and is analogous to evolutionary distance in real organisms. z=z9.5 and z=sin(z) are an example of two far-removed morphologies; z = z6+zz and z = z8.5 are an

-12- example of two closely-related ones. Interestingly, movie 6 (which is a conversion between Figures 11 and 12) shows that most of the change occurs very late in the sequence; in this scheme, it is analogous to an evolutionary change in gene interactions the effects of which are relatively minor and consists of a change late in development. There may also be regions (for example, some of the early regions in movie #1) that are discontinuous, and stay discontinuous no matter how fine a grain is used. This would represent forms which cannot arise from each other as a result of evolutionary processes. 4. : dealing with perturbations Errors in positional information in field theories have been studied (Lewis et al., 1977). It is also possible to use this model to explore the implications of field-directed morphogenesis for the amazing stability and resistance to perturbations that real embryos exhibit. So far, the model has assumed that field values (positions) can be read exactly. This condition will be referred to as M0. Some interesting results can be obtained when this is altered, in one of two ways.

The first method, M1, corresponds to an inherently imprecise (fuzzy) positional information. This would correspond, for example, to a morphogen gradient that tends to spread out and be- come less sharp with time, or a cell’s field-reading mechanism that operates with some intrinsic error. In the implementation, this is done by using { type = φ(z+∆z) } instead of { type = φ(z) } : for each position, the function is applied not to the position itself, but to the position plus some small offset. If the offset is random, this produces a randomly-distributed fuzziness in position (an inability of the cell to determine precisely where it is). If the offset is not random, it can be described by a function which corresponds to a positional information mutant (whose cells make a consistent error in reading positional field values).

The second method, M2, is to model an inherently precise field, which is read by cells some component of which distorts the positional information, and other components of which do not. The implementation then looks like: { type = φ(z,z+∆z) } : the type is a function of the real field value, but also of a slightly distorted field value. The distortion can be interpreted as described in the previous paragraph. Several of these type of experiments were done. Consider for example the form produced by 3.5 z = z (Figure 44 shows the morphology as computed from M0). A plausible initial hypothesis as to what would happen if one added a small random offset to each point value is that the over- all shape would be the same, but would be more fuzzy and diffuse, with less precise detail. In- terestingly, this is not what happens. Figure 45 shows the figure as computed from M1 (with each ∆z = z ± a random number of range log(z)). Note that part of the image is completely un- touched, while another part is duplicated, and spatially shifted. One of the copies is more dif- fuse than the other, suggesting a primary and supernumerary versions of the structure. Figure 46 shows the morphology produced by formula #7 (at double scale) as computed from M0. Note the sharp and well-defined edges, but lack of horizontal or vertical straight lines. ∆ Figure 47 shows the same thing computed from M2 (with each z = a random number between ±z0.01). Note the fact that the interior detail has been obliterated by a clear rectangle; note also the "picture-frame" rectangle of fuzzy pattern around it, and the perfect (unchanged) detail out- side that. It is interesting that adding a random component to these functions is able to produce a straight edge, where there was none before.

-13- The implication of this is that errors in reading the positional information field produce much more complicated effects than just fuzziness in the resulting image. They can result in multiple copies of certain parts of the morphology, as well as complicated spatial patterns of regions of imprecise and precise structures. Some regions of the field are very resistant to per- turbations; some are not. In all cases there is a tolerance to such influences, since the results remain the same over a wide range of magnitudes for the influences. This is consistent with the considerable resistance of embryos to outside perturbations, and especially with the fact that er- rors in reading the positional information field have been considered as an explanation for the production of supernumerary limbs at regeneration sites (Trainor, 1982, Sessions et al., 1989). Thus, the duplication of certain (but not all) structures as a result of small differences in posi- tional information seen in Figure 45 is consistent with the results of Sessions and Ruth (1990) who showed that implanting small beads into developing amphibian embryos (thus altering the position of various cells relative to a positional information field) can induce supernumerary limbs. These types of experiments suggest many empirical tests for the model. Future directions Work is in progress to extend this model to the 3-D (R3) space. This would require suitable definitions of the operations (as, for example, there is no unique definition of multiplication for R3). Another interesting biological feature to study is the organizer: it is possible to make areas of the field special in some way (for example, by making the errors in field position inversely proportional to distance from the organizer). This model can be directly applied to real organisms by defining operations to correspond to a system in which the interactions between morphogenetically-important genes have been worked out. This awaits definitive quantitative studies on morphological gene interactions. Part II of this paper will discuss another model (which utilizes Iterated Dynamical Systems to model cell migration without differentiation), as well as the characteristics of a much- expanded model of field-directed morphogenesis. Much empirically-testable information may be derived from this model if it were possible to obtain formulas coding for a particular real biological morphology. This is known as the syntactic inference problem. Part III will present work currently in progress, which seeks to develop a method for obtaining the formula for an arbitrary biological image; this method utilizes biologically-inspired algorithms such as neural networks and genetic algorithms. Part IV will present an vector force field approach to morpho- genesis.

Acknowledgments -- Some of the software (for the Sun platform) used in this work was written by Alva Couch. I would also like to thank Alva Couch, Susan G. Ernst, Ben Levin, Jorge Gonzalez, and Dmitry Grenader for helpful discussions.

-14- References Apter, M. J., and Development, Pergamon Press, NY: 1966 Axelrod, Robert, and William D. Hamilton, (1981), The evolution of cooperation, Science, 1390-1396 Bailly, F., F. Gaill, and R. Mosseri, (1991), A for biological development, Acta Biotheoretica, 39: 167-184 Bakowska, J., and M. Jerka-Dziadosz, (1980), Ultrastructural aspect of size dependent regulation of surface pattern of complex ciliary in a protozoan ciliate, Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 59: 355-375 Bard, Jonathan, Morphogenesis: the Cellular and Molecular Processes of Developmental Anatomy, Cambridge University Press, NY: 1990 Beardon, Alan F., (1990), of Julia sets, Bulletin of the London Mathematical Society, 22: 576-582 Beardon, Alan F., Iteration of Rational Functions, Springer-Verlag, NY: 1991 Bode, M. P., and H. R. Bode, (1980), Formation of pattern in regenerating tissue pieces of hydra attenuata: I. Head-body proportion regulation, Developmental Biology, 78: 484-496 Brandts, W. A., and L. E. H. Trainor, (1990a), A non-linear field model of pattern formation: intercalation in morphalactic regulation, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 146: 37-56 Brandts, W. A., and L. E. H. Trainor, (1990b), A non-linear field model of pattern formation: application to intracellular pattern reversal, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 146: 57-86 Brière C., and B. C. Goodwin, (1990), Effects of calcium input/output on the stability of a system for calcium-regulated viscoelastic strain fields, Journal of Mathematical Biology, 28(5):585-93 Bryant, Susan V., and Laurie E. Iten, (1976), Supernumerary limbs in amphibians, Developmental Biology, 50: 212-234 Burlando, Bruno, R. C. Vietti, R. Parodi, and M. Scardi, (1991), Emerging fractal properties in Gorgonian growth forms, Growth, Development, and Aging, 55: 161-168 Burr, Harold Saxton, (1941), Field properties of the developing frog eggs, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 27: 276-281 Chandebois, R., (1977), Cell and the problem of position effect: pattern formation, origin, and role of gradients, Acta Biotheoretica, 26(4): 203-238 Conrad, Michael, (1981), Algorithmic specification as a technique for computing with informal biological models, BioSystems, 13: 303-320 Cooke, J., (1972a), Properties of the primary organization field of Xenopus laevis part I, Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 28(1): 13-26 Cooke, J., (1972b), Properties of the primary organization field of Xenopus laevis part II,

-15- Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 28(1): 27-46 Cooke, J., (1972c), Properties of the primary organization field of Xenopus laevis part III, Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 28(1): 47-56 Cooke, J., (1973a), Properties of the primary organization field of Xenopus laevis part IV, Journal of Embryology and Experimental morphology, 30(1): 49-62 Cooke, J., (1973b), Properties of the primary organization field of Xenopus laevis part V, Journal of Embryology and Experimental morphology, 30(2): 283-300 Cooke, J., (1973c), Morphogenesis and regulation in spite of continued mitotic inhibition in Xenopus embryos, Nature, 242: 55-57 Cooke, J., (1975), The and regulation of spatial organization in early animal development, Annual Review of Biophysics & Bioengineering, 4(00): 185-217 Cooke, J., (1979), Cell number in relation to primary pattern formation in the embryo of Xenopus laevis, Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 51: 165-182 Cooke, J., (1981), Scale of body pattern adjusts to available cell number in amphibian embryos, Nature, 290: 775-778 Dissado, L. A., (1990), A fractal interpretation of the dielectric response of animal tissues, Physics In & Biology, 35(11): 1487-1503 Düchting, W., and T. Vogelsaenger, (1984), Analysis, forecasting, and control of three-dimensional tumor growth and treatment, Journal of Medical Systems, 8(5):461-475 Düchting, W., and T. Vogelsaenger, (1985), Recent progress in modeling and simulation of three-dimensional tumor growth and treatment, Biosystems, 18(1): 79-91 Evans, John, Mind, Body, and Electromagnetism, Element Books, London: 1986 Feder, J., and A. Aharony, eds., Fractals in Physics, Media Magic, CA: 1990 Frankel, Joseph, (1974), Positional information in unicellular organisms, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 47: 439-481 French, Vernon, Peter J. Bryant, and Susan V. Bryant, (1966), Pattern regulation in epimorphic fields, Science, 193: 969-981 French, V., (1988), Gradients and insect segmentation, Development, 103 supplement: 3-16 Gardiner, D. M., and S. V. Bryant, (1989), Organization of positional information in the axolotl limb, Journal of Experimental Zoology, 251(1): 47-55 Garfinkel, David, (1984), Modeling of inherently complex biological systems, Mathematical Biosciences, 72: 131-139 Goel, Narendra S., and Gary Rogers, (1978), Computer simulation of engulfment and other movements of embryonic tissue, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 71: 103-140 Goel, Narendra S., and Richard L. Thompson, Computer Simulations of Self-organization in

-16- Biological Systems, Macmillan Publishing Co., NY: 1988 Goodwin, B. C., and Morel H. Cohen, (1969), A phase-shift model for the spatial and temporal organization of developing systems, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 25: 49-107 Goodwin, B. C., and L. Trainor, (1980), A field description of the cleavage process in morphogenesis, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 85: 757-770 Goodwin, B. C., and N. LaCroix, (1984), A further study of the holoblastic cleavage field, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 109: 41-58 Goodwin, B. C., (1985), Developing Organisms as Self-organizing Fields in Antonelli, Peter L., Mathematical Essays on Growth and the Emergence of Form, University of Alberta Press, 1985 Goodwin, B. C., C. Biere, and P. S. O’Shea, (1987), Mechanisms underlying the formation of spatial structures in cells, in Poole, P. K., and A. P. Trinci, Spatial Organization in Eukaryotic Microbes, Society for General Microbiology, Washington DC: 1987 Goodwin, B. C., (1988), Problems and prospects in morphogenesis, Experientia, 44:633-637 Gow, Neil A. R., (1987), Polarity and branching in fungi induced by electrical fields, in Poole, P. K., and A. P. Trinci, Spatial Organization in Eukaryotic Microbes, Society for General Microbiology, Washington DC: 1987 Green, David M., (1991), Chaos, fractals, and nonlinear dynamics in evolution and phylogeny, Trends in and Evolution. 6(10): 333-337 Green, Jeremy B. A., Helen V. New, and J. C. Smith, (1992), Responses of embryonic Xenopus cells to activin and FGF are separated by multiple dose thresholds and correspond to distinct axes of the mesoderm, Cell, 71: 731-739 Gutowitz, Howard, Cellular Automata, Media Magic, CA: 1991 Guyon, E., and H. Eugene Stanley, Fractal Forms, Media Magic, CA: 1991 Harris, Albert K., (1990), Testing cleavage mechanisms by comparing computer simulations to actual experimental results, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 582: 60-77 Harrison, R. G., (1918), Experiments on the development of the forelimb of Ambystoma, a self-differentiating, equi-potential system, Journal of Experimental Zoology, 25: 413-462 Hart, Trevor N., L. E. H. Trainor, and B. C. Goowdin, (1989), Diffusion effects in calcium-regulated strain fields, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 136: 327-336 Hart, Trevor N., and L. E. H. Trainor, (1989), Geometrical aspects of surface morphogenesis, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 138: 271-296 Herman, Gabor T., and Grzegorz Rozenberg, Developmental Systems and Languages, North-Holland Publishing Co., Amsterdam: 1975 Iannaccone, P. M., (1990), Fractal geometry in mosaic organs: a new interpretation of mosaic pattern, FASEB Journal, 4: 1508-1512

-17- Ishibashi A., K. Aihara, and M. Kotani, (1988), Chaos in brain and and an analysis on the fractal , Japanese Journal of Medical Electronics & Biological , 26(1): 57-61 Jaffe, Lionel, F., (1979), Control of development by ionic currents, in Membrane Transduction Mechanisms, edited by Cone, Richard A., and John E. Dowling, Raven Press, NY: 1979 Kauffman, S. A., (1969), Metabolic stability and epigenesis in randomly constructed genetic nets, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 22: 437-467 Kauffman, S. A., (1974), The large scale structure and dynamics of gene control circuits, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 44: 167-190 Kauffman, S. A., (1990), Requirements for evolvability in complex systems, p. 151-, in Complexity, , and the Physics of Information, ed. W. H. Zurek, Addison Wesley, MA: 1990 Kiani, M. F., and A. G. Hudetz, (1991), Computer simulation of growth of anastomosing microvascular networks, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 150(4): 547-560 Kondo, Shigeru, (1992), A mechanistic model for morphogenesis regeneration of limbs and imaginal discs, Mechanisms of Development, 39: 161-170 Krebs, John R., Alejandro Kacelnik, and Peter Taylor, (1978), Test of optimal sampling by foraging great tits, Nature, 275: 27-31 Kropf, D. L., and Lupa Caldwell, (1983), Cell polarity: endogenous currents precede and predict branching in the water mold Achyla, Science, 220: 1385-1387 Lakirev, A. V., and L. V. Belousov, (1986), Computer modeling of gastrulation and neurulation in amphibian embryos based on mechanical tension fields, Ontogenez, 17(6): 636-647 Landini G., (1991), A fractal model for periodontal breakdown in periodontal disease, Journal of Periodontal Research, 26(3 Pt 1): 176-179 Langton, Christopher, Artficial Life I, Proceedings of the 1st Artificial Life Conference, Santa Fe Complexity Sciences series, NM: 1990 Langton, Christopher, Artficial Life II, Proceedings of the 2nd Artificial Life Conference, Santa Fe Complexity Sciences series, NM: 1991 Lewis, J., J. M. W. Slack, and L. Wolpert, (1977), Thresholds in Development, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 65: 579-590 Liebovitch, L. S., and T. I. Töth, (1990), Using fractals to understand the opening and closing of ion channels, Annals of Biomedical Engineering, 18(2): 177-194 Lindenmeyer, Arstid, and Przemyslaw Prusinkiewicz, (1989), Developmental models of multicellular organisms, in Langton, Artificial Life I, pp. 221-247 Lord, E. M., and L. C. Sanders, (1992), Roles for the extracellular matrix in plant development and pollination, Developmental Biology, 153: 16-28

-18- Lund, E. J., Bioelectric Fields and Growth, University of Austin Press, TX: 1947 MacDonald, N., Trees and Networks in Biological Models, John Wiley & Sons, NY: 1983 Mainster, Martin A., (1990), The fractal properties of retinal vessels, Eye, 4: 235-241 Mandelbrot, Benoit, The Fractal Geometry of Nature, Media Magic, NY: 1982 Matioli, G. T., (1987), Cancer clonality and field theory, Haematology and Blood Transfusion, 31: 286-289 Meakin, Paul, (1986), A new model for biological pattern formation, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 118: 101-113 Meinhardt, Hans, Models of Biological Pattern Formation, Academic Press, NY: 1982 Meinhardt, Hans, (1984), Models for positional signaling: the threefold subdivision of segments and the pigmentation pattern of molluscs, Journal of Embryology and Experimental Morphology, 83: 289-311 Mittenthal, J. E., and W. W. Trevarrow, (1984), Intercalary regeneration in legs of crayfish: proximal segments, Developmental Biology, 101(1): 40-50 Mittenthal, J. E., and J. R. Nuelle, (1988), Discontinuities of pattern and rules for regeneration in limbs of crayfish, Developmental Biology, 126: 315-326 Murray, J. D., and G. F. Oster, (1984), Cell traction models for generating pattern and form in morphogenesis, Journal of Mathematical Biology, 19(3): 265-279 Nagorcka, B. N., (1989), Wavelike isomorphic prepatterns in development, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 137: 127-162 Nelson, T. R., B. J. West, and A. L. Goldberger, (1990), The fractal lung: universal and species-related patterns, Experientia, 46(3):251-254 Nucitelli, Richard, (1984), The involvement of transcellular ion currents and electric fields in pattern formation, in Malacinski, G. M., and Bryant, S. V., Pattern Formation, MacMillan Publishing Co., NY: 1984 Nucitelli, Richard ed., Ionic Currents in Development, Alan R. Liss, NY: 1986 O’Shea, P. S., (1988), Physical fields and cellular organization: field-dependent mechanisms of morphogenesis, Experientia, 44: 684-694 Othmer, H. G., and E. Pate, (1980), Scale-invariance in reaction-diffusion models of spatial pattern formation, Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 77(7): 4180-4184 Oxaal, U., M. Murat, F. Boger, A. Aharony, J. Feder, and T. Jossang, (1987), Viscous fingering on percolation clusters, Nature, 329: 32-37 Palmer, Alison R., (1957), Ontogenetic development of two olenellid trilobites, Journal of Paleontology, 31(1): 105-128 Partridge, Derek, and P. D. Lopez, (1984), Computer programs as theories in biology, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 108: 539-564

-19- Peitgen, Heinz-Otto, and D. Saupe, eds., The Science of Fractal Images, Springer Verlag, NY: 1988 Pickover, Clifford, Computers, Pattern, Chaos, and Beauty, St. Martin’s Press, NY: 1990 Pietronero, L., and E. Tosatti, eds., Fractals in Physics, Amsterdam, North-Holland: 1986 Prusinkiewicz, Przemyslaw, and Aristid Lindenmeyer, The Algorithmic Beauty of Plants, Springer Verlag, NY: 1990 Raff, Rudolf A., and Thomas C. Kaufman, Embryos, Genes, and Evolution, Macmillan Publishing Co., NY: 1983 Ransom, Robert, Computers and Embryos, John Wiley & Sons, NY: 1981 Ransom, Robert, (1984), Computer modeling of during development using a topological approach, Journal of embryology and experimental morphology, 83: 233-259 Raup, D. M., and A. Michelson, (1965), Theoretical morphology of the coiled shell, Science, 147: 1294-1295 Rogers, Gary, and N. S. Goel, (1978), Computer simulation of cellular movements: cell sorting, cell migration through a mass of cells, and contact inhibition, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 71: 141-166 Rozenberg, G., and A. Salomaa, The Book of L, Springer-Verlag, NY: 1986 Russel, M. A., (1985), Positional information in imaginal discs: a Cartesian coordinate model, in Antonelli, Peter L., Mathematical Essays on Growth and the Emergence of Form, University of Alberta Press, 1985 Sander, Leonard M., (1987), Fractal growth, Scientific American, 256: 94-100 Savel’ev S. V., (1988), The mechanism of encoding positional information by neuroepithelial cells from the brain of amphibian embryos, Doklady Akademii Nauk Sssr, 301(6): 1479-83 Sernetz, M., B. Gelléri, and J. Hofmann, (1985), The organism as bioreactor, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 117: 209-230 Sessions, S. K., D. M. Gardiner, and S. V. Bryant, (1989), Compatible limb patterning mechanisms in urodeles and anurans, Developmental Biology, 131(2): 294-301 Sessions, S. K., and S. B. Ruth, (1990), Explanation for naturally occurring supernumerary limbs in amphibians, Journal of Experimental Zoology, 254(1): 38-47 Slack, J. M. W., From Egg to Embryo, Cambridge University Press, NY: 1991 Smith T. G. Jr., T. N. Behar, G. D. Lange, W. B. Marks, and W. H. Sheriff Jr., (1991), A of cultured rat optic nerve glial growth and differentiation, , 41(1):159-66 Tabin, Clifford J., (1991), Retinoids, homeoboxes, and growth factors: toward molecular models for limb development, Cell, 66: 199-217

-20- Tevlin, Patrick, and L. E. H. Trainor, (1985), A two- model of limb regeneration and transplant phenomena, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 115: 495-513 Thom, René, Mathematical Models of Morphogenesis, John Wiley, NY: 1983 Thompson, D. W., (1961), , Cambridge University Press, NY: 1961 Thomson, K. S., Morphogenesis and Evolution, Oxford University Press, NY: 1988 Totafurno, John, C. J. Lumsden, and L. E. H. Trainor, (1980), Structure and function in biological hierarchies, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 85: 171-198 Trainor, L. E., (1982), A field approach to pattern formation in , Physics in Canada, 38: 117-120 Tsonis, A. A., and P. A. Tsonis, (1987), Fractals: a new look at biological shape and patterning, Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 30(3): 355-361 Tsonis, P. A., (1988), Pattern formation: from theoretical models to molecular biology, In Vivo, 2: 201-208 Tsonis, A. A., J. B. Eslner, and P. A. Tsonis, (1989), On the dynamics of a forced reaction-diffusion model for biological pattern formation, Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA, 86: 4938-4942 Tsonis, P. A., and A. A. Tsonis, (1989), Chaos: principles and implications in biology, Computer Applications in the Biosciences, 5(1): 27-32 Vaidya, P. G., V. G. Vaidya, and D. G. Martin, (1991), An application of the non-linear to tumor growth modeling, International Journal of Bio-Medical Computing, 27(1): 27-46 Weintraub, Harold, (1993), The MyoD family and myogenesis: redundancy, networks, and thresholds, Cell, 75: 1241-1244 West, Bruce, and Goldberger, (1987), in Fractal Dimensions, American Scientist, 75: 354- West, Bruce, Fractal Physiology and Chaos in Medicine, Media Magic, CA: 1991 Witten, T. A. Jr., and L. M. Sander, (1981), Diffusion limited aggregation, a kinetic critical phenomenon, Physics Review Letters, 47: 1400-1403 Witten, T. A. Jr., and M. E. Cates, (1986), Tenuous structures from disorderly growth processes, Science, 232: 1607-1612 Wolpert, Lewis, (1969), Positional information and the spatial pattern of cellular differentiation, Journal of Theoretical Biology, 25: 1-47 Wolpert, Lewis, (1971), Positional information and pattern formation, Current Topics in Developmental Biology, 6: 183-224 Wolpert, Lewis, (1989), Positional information revisited, Development (1989 supplement), p. 3-12 Yates, Kemble, and Edward Pate, (1989), A cascading development model for amphibian

-21- embryos, Bulletin on Mathematical Biology, 51(5): 549-578 Zackson, Saul L., and Malcolm S. Steinberg, (1988), A molecular marker for cell guidance information in the axolotl embryo, Developmental Biology, 127: 435-442

-22-