<<

Memory & Cognition 2007, 35 (4), 610-620

The roles of encoding, retrieval, and in change detection

MELISSA R. BECK AND MATTHEW S. PETERSON George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia

AND

BONNIE L. ANGELONE Rowan University, Glassboro, New Jersey

In the experiment reported here, we examined the processes by which expected (probable) changes are detected more frequently than are unexpected (improbable) changes (the change probability effect; Beck, An- gelone, & Levin, 2004). The change probability effect may be caused by a bias toward probable changes during encoding of the prechange aspect, during retrieval of the prechange aspect, or during activation of an explicit response to the change. Participants performed a change detection task for probable and improbable changes while their eye movements were tracked. Change detection performance was superior for probable changes, but long-term performance was equivalent for both probable and improbable changes. Therefore, although both probable and improbable prechange aspects were encoded, probable prechange aspects were more likely to be retrieved during change detection. Implicit change detection was also greater for probable changes than for improbable changes, suggesting that the change probability effect is the result of a bias during the retrieval and comparison stage of change detection. The stimuli used in the change detection task may be downloaded from www.psychonomic.org/archive.

What is the process by which people detect visual tection). Here, we will focus on three important steps in changes? In the absence of bottom-up information (e.g., change detection; the encoding process, the retrieval and abrupt onsets or motion transients) to direct visual- comparison process, and the explicit response process (see processing resources to the location of a change, changes Simons, 2000, and Simons & Rensink, 2005, for other dis- in the visual environment often go undetected (change cussions of the steps necessary for change detection). In blindness: for reviews, see Rensink, 2002, and Simons, order for a change to be accurately detected, sufficiently 2000). However, visual changes are not always missed, accurate, detailed, and stable prechange representations because knowledge about the visual world can improve must first be encoded in memory. to the pre- change detection performance (Beck, Angelone, & change aspect (i.e., the part of the prechange scene that Levin, 2004; Hollingworth & Henderson, 2000; Rensink, is different form the postchange scene) is necessary for O’Regan, & Clark, 1997; Shinoda, Hayhoe, & Shrivas- these representations to be formed (Levin & Simons, 1997; tava, 2001). Beck et al. found that changes that are likely Rensink et al., 1997; Simons & Levin, 1998). Second, the to occur in the real world (probable changes; e.g., a lamp prechange memory representation must be retrieved for turning from off to on) are detected more frequently than comparison with the postchange aspect. Finally, the com- are changes that are unlikely to occur in the real world parison process must lead to the activation of an explicit (improbable changes; e.g., a blue lamp changing into a response to the change. When change blindness occurs, it green lamp). Therefore, visual information associated suggests that the aspect of the visual world that changed with probable changes is selected for processing over vi- was not sufficiently processed at one or more of these sual information associated with improbable changes. The steps. Consequently, probable changes are detected more goal of the present article is to determine the locus of this frequently than are improbable changes because the infor- selection of probable changes over improbable changes. mation associated with probable changes is more likely to There are several steps of processing necessary for be sufficiently processed during one or more of these steps change detection to occur, during which information rel- in change detection. evant to probable changes could be more likely to be se- lected than information relevant to improbable changes The Encoding Process (see Figure 1; see Simons, 2000, and Simons & Rensink, Successful change detection depends on encoding an 2005, for discussions of the steps necessary for change de- accurate and sufficiently detailed representation of the

M. R. Beck, [email protected]

Copyright 2007 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 610 THE ROLES OF ENCODING, RETRIEVAL, AND AWARENESS 611

1 Maintenance of encoded prechange representation.

Access to prechange representation for 2 retrieval and comparison.

3 Response activation.

Prechange ISI Postchange Was there a YES change? Display Sequence

Figure 1. The necessary steps for successful change detection. (1) The prechange aspects must be encoded in memory until the postchange scene aspect is attended, (2) the prechange representation must then be retrieved for comparison, and then (3) an explicit behavioral response to the change must be activated. prechange aspect. Several studies have arrived at the con- able changes over improbable changes does not occur clusion that change blindness occurs because memory during encoding, memory performance for both types of representations of the visual world are largely gist based prechange aspects should be equivalent. and contain little visual detail (Irwin & Andrews, 1996; O’Regan, 1992; O’Regan & Noë, 2001; O’Regan, Ren- The Retrieval Process sink, & Clark, 1999; Rensink, 2000; Rensink et al., 1997; Although change blindness can be caused by a failure Simons, 1996; Simons & Levin, 1997). Encoding a mem- to encode a sufficiently detailed and stable memory repre- ory representation of the prechange aspect that will sup- sentation of the prechange aspect, encoding failures may port change detection requires attention (Hollingworth & not be the primary cause of change blindness (see Simons Henderson, 2002; Levin & Simons, 1997; Rensink et al., & Rensink, 2005, for a discussion). Several researchers 1997; Simons & Levin, 1998). However, Levin, Simons, have shown that change blindness can occur even when Angelone, and Chabris (2002) found that participants who LTM tests reveal accurate memory for the changing as- missed a change in the identity of a pedestrian they were pects (Angelone, Levin, & Simons, 2003; Hollingworth talking to also performed worse on a memory test for the & Henderson, 2002; Varakin & Levin, 2006), suggest- identity of the pedestrian. That is, even though the partici- ing that the cause of change blindness may be the failure pants were clearly paying attention to the pedestrian they to retrieve the encoded representation. In these studies, were having a conversation with, they did not necessarily the participants performed a change detection task, and encode and maintain an accurate and sufficiently detailed then their memory for the changing features was tested. memory representation of the pedestrian. Therefore, not Often, LTM performance for the changing aspect was everything that is attended is sufficiently encoded into rather high (between 45% and 80%) but change detection memory; rather, encoding may be biased toward some performance was much lower (between 12% and 50%). attended aspects (e.g., the gender of the pedestrian) of Therefore, change blindness could occur even when a rep- the visual world over others (e.g., what the pedestrian is resentation of the prechange aspect has been encoded in wearing). Most important, when an attended aspect is not LTM, suggesting that a failure to retrieve the representa- encoded, change blindness occurs. tion for comparison with the postchange aspect caused the Here, we will examine whether an encoding failure for change blindness. improbable prechange aspects is the reason why probable Research demonstrating that changes can be missed changes are detected more frequently than are improbable even when a prechange representation is encoded in LTM changes. If so, we would expect that on a long-term mem- suggests that change detection tasks do not necessarily ory (LTM) test for probable and improbable prechange provide access to representations encoded in LTM. This aspects, performance should be higher for the probable may occur in the interest of not overloading a limited ca- prechange aspects. Alternatively, if the selection of prob- pacity processing system, such as , which 612 BECK, PETERSON, AND ANGELONE may be involved in the retrieval process. Rather, only a region longer than they look at regions of the scene that select portion of encoded information may be chosen for did not change, we can conclude that they detected the retrieval. For example, when a postchange cue (e.g., a change implicitly. brief mask or arrow at the change location) is provided Implicit and explicit change detection requires encoding that indicates which object may have changed in a dis- and retrieval of a prechange representation for compari- play, change detection performance is greatly improved son with the postchange aspect. If encoding and retrieval (Hollingworth, 2003, 2005). Presumably, providing the occur for a particular change and the change is not de- cue limits retrieval to the prechange representation so that tected explicitly, presumably it has been detected implic- retrieval capacity is no longer overwhelmed. Therefore, itly. Therefore, if both probable and improbable prechange even if detailed and stable prechange representations are representations are retrieved and compared equally but encoded in LTM, retrieval of the correct representations probable changes are more likely to be explicitly reported for comparison may not occur without a cue’s indicating (as was found in Beck et al., 2004), implicit change de- which representation should be retrieved. tection should be greater for improbable changes. Al- In addition to stimulus cues (e.g., arrows and masks at ternatively, if probable changes are more likely to be re- the change location), knowledge may be used to bias the trieved and compared, implicit change detection should retrieval process toward some aspects of the visual world be equivalent for both types of changes (if the degree to over others (knowledge-based retrieval). For example, which probable changes are retrieved and compared over knowledge about the probability of changes occurring in improbable changes is equivalent to the degree to which the real world may be used to direct processing resources explicit change detection for probable changes is greater toward visual information that is likely to change (i.e., than explicit change detection for improbable changes) or probable change information). When a stimulus cue is greater for probable changes. provided in the postchange scene, all cued prechange as- pects should be retrieved equally. However, when stimulus Explicit Response Activation cues are not provided, only those aspects of the scenes If the prechange aspect is encoded and the represen- associated with knowledge-based retrieval cues (e.g., in- tation is retrieved for comparison with the postchange formation associated with probable changes) should be aspect, a signal (the change signal) should be sent to pro- retrieved. Furthermore, stimulus cues shown to improve cesses involved in providing an explicit behavioral re- change detection performance are presented after encod- sponse to the change. Signals that are strong enough to ing but before retrieval (with presentation of the post- pass a threshold for explicit change detection will result in change scene; Hollingworth, 2003, 2005). This means that an explicit behavioral response to the change (e.g., “Yes, the cues must be biasing retrieval, rather than encoding. there was a change”; see Fernandez-Duque & Thornton, Knowledge-based cues may also be used to direct retrieval 2003; Mitroff, Simons, & Franconeri, 2002). According to some aspects of the scene over others. to signal detection theory, observers set a critical thresh- Probable changes may be detected more frequently than old for deciding whether a particular signal is strong improbable changes because representations of probable enough to result in explicit detection (Green & Swets, prechange aspects are more likely to be retrieved and com- 1966). The threshold for explicit detection of change sig- pared with the corresponding postchange aspect. If this is nals that are consistent with expectation (e.g., probable the case, we would expect not only that probable changes changes) may be lower than that for change signals that would be more likely to be detected explicitly (i.e., par- are inconsistent with expectation (improbable changes). ticipants accurately reporting, “Yes, there was a change”), This would result in an increased likelihood that change but also that they should be equally or more likely to signals for probable changes would result in an explicit be detected implicitly. Implicit change detection occurs behavioral response to the change. The possible influence when participants fail to explicitly report detection of the of a response bias (e.g., a lower decision threshold for change (i.e., participants inaccurately saying, “No, there probable changes) in the change probability effect can be was not a change”) but sensitivity to the change is revealed examined in change detection performance by using the through some other measure, such as eye movements signal detection measure AŒ (Grier, 1971). If the change (Hollingworth, Williams, & Henderson, 2001), reaction probability effect is still present when the signal detec- times (Fernandez-Duque & Thornton, 2003; Thornton tion measure is used, we can conclude that the change & Fernandez-Duque, 2000; Williams & Simons, 2000), probability effect is not caused by a response bias toward and event-related potentials (Fernandez-Duque, Grossi, probable changes. Thornton, & Neville, 2003). In the experiment presented In addition to using signal detection theory to examine here, we measured implicit change detection by monitor- the role of a response bias in the change probability ef- ing participants’ eye movements while they completed the fect, we also examined the extent to which the participants change detection task. Previous research has shown that would choose a probable description for trials on which the duration of eye movements during the processing of they falsely reported that a change had occurred. Occa- real-world visual scenes can indicate the level of implicit sionally, participants will provide affirmative explicit be- change detection (Hollingworth et al., 2001). If partici- havioral responses (“Yes, there was a change”) on trials on pants fail to report the change but look at the postchange which there was no change (i.e., a false alarm). If there is a THE ROLES OF ENCODING, RETRIEVAL, AND AWARENESS 613 response bias toward reporting probable changes, partici- Eyelink 2 tracker has 250-Hz temporal resolution and a gaze position pants should describe these false alarm change detections accuracy of less than 0.5˚ and uses an infrared video-based tracking as probable changes more frequently than they describe technology to compute the center and size of the pupils in both eyes. them as improbable changes. An infrared system also tracked head motion. Even though head mo- tion was measured, the head was stabilized by means of a chinrest. The chinrest was located 61 cm from the monitor. The Present Experiment One goal of the present experiment was to replicate the Materials change probability effect, in which probable changes are Thirty pictures of natural scenes from Beck et al. (2004) were used detected more frequently than are improbable changes in this experiment. The pictures were taken with a digital camera (Beck et al., 2004). However, the main objective was to and then modified in Photoshop. Ten of the scenes were prechange pictures. The other 20 were postchange pictures, which were identi- examine whether the change probability effect is driven cal to the prechange pictures except for one object that changed in by a bias toward probable changes during the encoding, some way. For each prechange picture, there was a corresponding retrieval, or explicit response step of change detection. In postchange picture with an improbable change (e.g., the window was the experiment presented here, participants completed a short in the prechange scene, and then it was longer in the postchange change detection task with probable change trials, improb- scene; see Figure 2) and another corresponding postchange picture able change trials, and no-change trials. In order to exam- with a probable change (e.g., the flag was laying flat in the prechange picture, and then it was blowing in the wind in the postchange pic- ine the role of the encoding step in the change probability ture). There were 10 picture sets, each containing a prechange scene, effect, following the change detection task, the participants an improbable postchange scene, and a probable postchange scene. completed an LTM task for the prechange aspects. If an As has been reported in Beck et al. (2004), the 10 sets of pictures encoding bias toward probable changes is the cause of the used in the experiment were chosen from an original set of 17. Four change probability effect, we would expect higher memory raters rated the probability of the probable and improbable changes performance for the probable prechange aspects. The role in the original set of 17. The raters were instructed to rate the likeli- of the retrieval step was examined by tracking the partici- hood of each change occurring from one glance to the next in their everyday visual environment. They were also told to assume the pos- pants’ eye movements during the change detection task as sibility of people manipulating the environment to cause the change. a measure of implicit change detection. If a retrieval bias The ratings were made on a scale from 1 to 7, where 1 indicated a is responsible for the change probability effect, implicit very likely change and 7 indicated a very unlikely change. The 10 change detection for probable changes should be greater picture sets with the highest differences between the improbable than or equal to the level of implicit change detection for change rating and the probable change rating were used in the ex- improbable changes. The role of explicit response activa- periment. The difference scores for the 10 picture sets used in the experiment were all greater than or equal to 3.75, and all of the rat- tion was tested by using the signal detection measure AŒÅ ings were less than 3 for the improbable changes and greater than 5 and by examining the participants’ descriptions of changes for the probable changes (for the specific ratings of each change, on false alarm trials. During the change detection task, see Beck et al., 2004). when the participants reported that they had detected a Beck et al. (2004) carefully controlled for several other aspects that change, they were presented with a four-alternative forced could vary between the scenes, other than the probability of change. choice (4AFC) and were asked to choose which of the four They reported that probable and improbable change scenes did not differ on the centrality of the changing object or the size of the change. alternatives best described the change they saw. On false Furthermore, all the changing objects were rated as highly typical of alarm trials, two of the alternatives described probable the scene, with improbable change objects rated as slightly more typi- changes, and two described improbable changes. If the cal than were probable change objects. Changes in the scenes could change probability effect is caused by a preference toward be object deletions, additions, replacements, state changes (e.g., turn- reporting probable changes, then on false alarm trials, the ing a lamp from on to off), location changes, or color changes (for change should be described as a probable change more more details on the probable and improbable change scenes, see Beck frequently than as an improbable change, and the change et al., 2004; full-color versions of the pictures used in this study can be found at www.psychonomic.org/Archive). probability effect should not be present when the signal In addition to the pre- and postchange scenes, 22 no-change scenes detection measure is used. that were similar to the change scenes were taken with a digital camera. These scenes did not contain changes. All the pictures were METHOD 36.25  28 cm, presented at the center of a 30.5  40.5 cm screen, and subtended approximately 25º of horizontal visual angle. Participants Thirty-one students (27 of them female and 4 male) from George Procedure Mason University participated in this study for course credit. The The participants completed the experiment one at a time, and average age of the participants was 21 years. All the participants had the entire experiment lasted approximately 30 min. The partici- normal or corrected-to-normal vision. pants first completed the change detection task and then completed the incidental LTM task. The participants’ eye movements were Apparatus tracked during both tasks. A Power Macintosh G4 (Dual 1 GHz), equipped with a 20-in. Change detection task. The participants were instructed that (viewable) ViewSonic P225fb capable of running at 120 Hz and run- their task was to decide whether or not a change had occurred in ning custom software was used to present the stimuli, control the each scene. Each participant completed 32 change detection trials. timing of experimental events, and record the participants’ response For 22 of the trials, no-change scenes were presented (no-change times. This computer was networked to a Dell Pentium 4 machine trials), and for 10 of the trials, change scenes were presented (change that collects eye-tracking data in conjunction with an Eyelink 2 sys- trials). The order of presentation of the 10 change trials and the 22 tem (SR Research). Latency between the machines is 10 msec. The no-change trials was randomized for each participant. Across the 10 614 BECK, PETERSON, AND ANGELONE

Prechange

Probable Postchange Improbable Postchange

Figure 2. Example of a prechange scene and its corresponding probable and improbable postchange scenes. In the probable postchange scene, the flag is blowing in the wind, and in the improbable postchange scene, the bottom right window is longer. change trials, each prechange scene was presented once; for half of responding probable or the improbable postchange scene. A green the trials, the probable postchange scene was presented, and for the arrow pointed to the part of the scene that was different between other half, the improbable postchange scene was presented. Which 5 the two scenes, so the participants would know on which part of the change trials would contain probable postchange scenes was ran- scene to focus their attention for the memory test. The participants’ domly determined for each participant. task was to decide which of the two scenes they had seen during the For the change trials, the prechange scene was presented for 6 sec, change detection task. The postchange scene presented during the followed by a 100-msec white screen interstimulus interval (ISI). memory task was always the postchange scene not shown during After the ISI, the postchange scene appeared for 6 sec. For the no- the change detection task. For example, if a participant had seen change trials, the no-change scene was presented for 6 sec, and then, the probable postchange scene during the change detection task after the ISI, the same no-change scene was presented for another (e.g., the picture with the flag blowing out and the short window 6 sec. For all the trials, a question appeared on the screen asking in Figure 2), they were presented with the improbable postchange the participants to press one key if they saw a change in the scene scene (e.g., the picture with the flag laying flat and a long window and another key if they did not see a change in the scene. If the par- in Figure 2) and the prechange scene (e.g., the picture with the flag ticipants pressed the key indicating that they saw a change, a 4AFC laying flat and a short window in Figure 2) during the LTM test. In appeared, and the participants chose which of the four alternatives this example, the participants had not yet seen the improbable post- best described the change they saw (see Figure 3). change scene and should report that they had seen the prechange The 4AFC for the change trials was the same for both the probable scene, not the improbable postchange scene, during the change de- and the improbable changes for each scene. One of the alternatives tection task. was the probable change, one was the improbable change, and the As instructions for the LTM task, the participants were told that final two were changes that never occurred. The two options that they would see pairs of scenes and that one of the scenes in each were changes that never occurred were the most common written de- pair would be a scene that they had seen during the change detection scriptions of changes that the participants in Beck et al. (2004) had task and the other would be a scene that they had not seen before. given when they incorrectly detected the change. For the no-change They were then instructed that their task was to choose which of scenes, two of the alternatives were possible probable changes, and each pair was the scene that they had seen during the change detec- two of the alternatives were possible improbable changes. tion task. The scenes were presented one at a time for 6 sec each, Incidental long-term memory task. Following completion of and the order (prechange scene presented first or postchange scene the change detection task, the participants were asked to complete presented first) was randomly determined, so that on half of the tri- the incidental LTM memory test. For each of 10 trials, the partici- als, the prechange scene was presented first and on half of the trials, pants were presented with the prechange scene and either the cor- the postchange scene was presented first. Following presentation of THE ROLES OF ENCODING, RETRIEVAL, AND AWARENESS 615

Press the appropriate key (1–4) indicating which option best describes the change you saw.

1. The flag was laying flat then blowing in the wind. 2. The window on right side of house was short then longer. 3. The fence was whole, then parts of fence were lying against the fence. 4. More snow appeared.

Did a change occur in the scene you 4AFC: presented if “z” was just saw? pressed on previous screen.

Press the “z” key if a change occurred and the “?” key if a change did not occur. Change response scene: response terminated.

Postchange scene: 6,000 msec. ISI: 100 msec Prechange scene: 6,000 msec.

Figure 3. Sequence of events in the change detection task. the second scene, a question appeared on the screen asking the par- ability effect found by Beck et al. (2004) was replicated, ticipants to press one key if they had seen the first scene presented with higher change detection performance for probable in the first part of the experiment (during the change detection task) changes than for improbable changes. and to press another key if they had seen the second scene during the first part of the experiment. The correct answer was always the prechange scene. Encoding Bias: LTM Performance on the LTM test was examined to test RESULTS whether or not the change probability effect found in the change detection task was caused by a bias toward encod- Change Probability Effect Overall, the participants accurately responded yes, there was a change to the change trials and no, there was 100 not a change to the no-change trials on 70% of the tri- Probable als. The participants accurately responded that a change 90 Improbable had occurred on 60% of the 10 change trials, and they 80 inaccurately responded that a change had occurred (false 70 alarms) on 25% of the 22 no-change trials. The percent-

y 60

age of yes responses on probable change trials (M  65%) c a was higher than that on improbable change trials (M  r

u 50

2 c 54%) [F(1,30)  4.38, MSe  0.209, p  .05, partial h  c 0.127]. Unless otherwise noted, in the remaining analyses A 40 accuracy on the change trials was coded as an accurate 30 change detection only if the participants indicated that a 20 change had occurred and then chose the correct change from the 4AFC. Accuracy on the change trials was 45%, 10 and overall accuracy (change trials and no-change trials) 0 was 65%. Figure 4 displays the mean percentage correct Change Detection Memory for the probable and improbable change detection trials. The percentage of accurate change detections for probable Figure 4. Accuracy (in percentages) for the change detection changes (M  52%) was higher than that for improbable and memory tasks. Accuracy in the change detection task for the probable and improbable change trials was coded by saying that changes (M  38%) [F(1,30)  6.23, MSe  0.372, p  there was a change and accurately choosing the correct answer  .02, partial h2 0.172; see below for analysis of change to the four-alternative forced choice. Bars represent the standard probability effect using AŒ]. Therefore, the change prob- errors. 616 BECK, PETERSON, AND ANGELONE ing probable prechange representations over improbable 450 prechange representations. The percentage correct for On change probable prechange scene (M  79%) was not signifi- 400 Off change cantly different from the percentage correct for improbable 350 prechange scene (M  75%) [F(1,30)  1.0, MSe  0.032, p  .33, partial h2  0.0321; see Figure 4]. Therefore, the 300 participants were not more likely to have encoded a repre- 250 sentation of the prechange aspect of the probable scenes.

ixation Duration 200

Retrieval Bias: Eye Movements 150 A change region was defined for each probable and im- erage F

v 100 probable change. The change regions were defined as the A outline of the portion of the scenes that differed between 50 pre- and postchange scenes, and then this portion of the scene was extended in all directions by 2˚ of visual angle. 0 Each fixation during the change trials was coded as being Probable Improbable either on the change region or off the change region. If the change probability effect were the result of bi- Figure 5. The average durations (in milliseconds) for fixations in the postchange scene as a function of fixation location (on or ased retrieval for probable prechange representations, off the postchange region) for trials on which the change was not we would expect higher or equivalent levels of implicit accurately detected. Bars represent the standard errors. change detection for probable changes. In order to exam- ine the prevalence of implicit change detection for proba- ble and improbable changes, we examined the duration of bias toward retrieval and comparison for the probable fixations on the postchange region when the participants prechange representations over the improbable prechange failed to report the change. If these fixation durations representations. were longer than those on nonchange regions, we would It is important for the interpretation of the implicit have evidence suggesting that the changes were detected change detection data to demonstrate that when changes implicitly. The average durations of the fixations for tri- were detected, both probable and improbable postchange als on which the change was not accurately detected were regions were looked at longer than no-change regions. entered into an ANOVA with fixation location (on change The average durations of fixations for trials on which or off change) and change type (probable or improbable) the change was accurately detected were entered into an as within-subjects factors.2 The main effect for fixation ANOVA with fixation location (on change or off change) location was significant; the average fixation duration on and change type (probable or improbable) as within- the postchange regions (M  320 msec) was longer than subjects factors. The main effect for fixation location the average duration for fixations off the postchange re- was significant; the average fixation duration on the post- gions (M  283 msec) [F(1,24)  4.907, MSe  33,874, change region (M  379 msec) was longer than the aver- p  .036]. The main effect for change type was also sig- age duration for fixations off the postchange region (M  nificant. The participants’ fixation durations when they 303 msec) [F(1,20)  8, MSe  121,529, p  .01]. How- were looking at the probable postchange scenes (M  ever, the main effect for change type was not significant. 318 msec) were longer than those when they were look- The participants’ fixation durations when they were look- ing at the improbable postchange scenes (M 285 msec) ing at the probable postchange scenes (M  307 msec) [F(1,24)  4.89, MSe 25,730, p  .037]. Furthermore, were not different from those when they were fixat- the significant interaction between fixation location and ing the improbable postchange scenes (M 264 msec) change type [F(1, 24)  4.47, MSe  23,575, p  .045] [F(1,20)  2.07, MSe 12,100, p  .17]. The interaction indicated that the main effects were driven by a significant between fixation location and change type was not sig- difference for the probable change trials between fixation nificant [F(1,20)  0.62, MSe  6,725, p  .44; see Fig- duration on (M  351 msec) and off (M  284 msec) the ure 6]. When a change was accurately detected, fixation change [t(25)  2.7, p  .01] but not for the improbable durations on the change regions were longer than fixation change trials (on, M  289 msec; off, M  282 msec) durations off the change regions, presumably reflecting [t(25)  0.33, p  .75; see Figure 5]. Therefore, when the the added time necessary for retrieval and comparison of change was not explicitly reported, the participants looked the prechange representation. at the probable postchange region longer than they looked To further examine whether the longer durations of the at the no-change regions. Furthermore, fixation durations fixations on the probable postchange regions on inaccu- on the probable postchange regions (M  351 msec) rate change trials (when the change was not accurately de- were longer than fixation durations on the improbable tected) is evidence of implicit change detection, we must postchange regions (M  289 msec) [t(25)  12.9, p rule out the possibility that fixations on probable change .01]. Therefore, implicit change detection was found for regions are longer even when a change has not occurred. probable changes, but not for improbable changes, sug- Therefore, we examined the average durations of the fixa- gesting that the change probability effect is driven by a tions on the prechange regions. For the prechange scenes, THE ROLES OF ENCODING, RETRIEVAL, AND AWARENESS 617

450 als (M  .12) [F(1,22)  0.147, MSe  0.0004, p  .71]. On change The interaction between accuracy and change type was 400 Off change not significant [F(1,22)  0.202, MSe  0.001, p  .66]. 350 Therefore, more frequent fixations on the prechange re- gion improved change detection performance in general, 300 but probable prechange regions were not fixated more 250 frequently than were improbable prechange regions. This further supports the idea that attention is not biased toward

ixation Duration 200 probable change regions prior to the change. 150 Explicit Response Bias: AŒ and False Alarm erage F v 100 Change Descriptions A To examine the role of a response bias in the change 50 probability effect, we revisited change detection perfor- 0 mance, using the signal detection measure AŒ. The AŒ value Probable Improbable for performance on probable change trials (AŒ  .60) was significantly higher than that for performance on the im- Figure 6. The average durations (in milliseconds) for fixations probable change trials (AŒ  .52) [t(31)  4.43, p .01].3 in the postchange scene as a function of fixation location (on or off the postchange region) for trials on which the change was ac- Therefore, the change probability effect demonstrated by curately detected. Bars represent the standard errors. higher change detection performance for probable changes than for improbable changes was still present even when the data were examined in such a way that response biases the duration of the fixations on both the probable and the were ruled out. improbable change regions was calculated for all of the The participants false alarmed to 25% of the no-change change trials (e.g., for a probable change trial, we exam- trials. Data from 2 participants were not included in the ined the duration of the fixations on both the probable analysis because they did not false alarm to any of the tri- prechange region and the improbable prechange region). als. The response chosen for the 4AFC on the false alarm The average duration of the fixations on the probable pre- trials was a description of a probable change 53% of the change regions (M  302 msec) was not different from time, which is not different from chance (50%) [t(28)  the average duration on the improbable change regions 0.474, p  .64]. Therefore, there was no top-down bias (M 288 msec) [F(1,30)  1.2, MSe  3,017, p  .29; toward choosing probable change descriptions, suggest- see Figure 7]. Therefore, participants do not look at prob- ing that the bias toward probable changes does not occur able change regions longer than they look at improbable during explicit response activation. change regions prior to a change’s occurring, providing further support that the longer durations on the prob- able postchange regions on the inaccurate change trials is evidence for implicit change detection for the probable 400 changes. Implicit change detection for probable changes, but not for improbable changes, suggests that the change 350 probability effect is the result of a retrieval bias toward probable prechange representations. It is also important to note that the participants fixated 300 the change region more frequently when they accurately detected a change but that they did not fixate the prob- 250 able prechange regions more frequently than they did the improbable prechange regions. The proportion of all fixa- 200 tions on the prechange region for each type of trial was ixation Duration entered into a two-factor ANOVA with change detection 150 accuracy (accurate or inaccurate) and change trial (prob- erage F v able or improbable) as within-subjects factors. There was A 100 a main effect for accuracy, with a greater proportion of fixations on the prechange region when the change was accurately detected (M  .14) than when the change was 50 not detected (M  .11) [F(1,22)  6.78, MSe  0.023, p  .016; see Figure 8]. However, there was no main ef- 0 fect for change trial. The proportion of fixations on the Probable Improbable probable prechange region for probable change trials Figure 7. The average fixation durations (in milliseconds) for (M  .12) was not different from the proportion on the fixations on the probable and improbable prechange regions. improbable prechange region for improbable change tri- Bars represent the standard errors. 618 BECK, PETERSON, AND ANGELONE

.40 changes and no implicit change detection for improbable Correct changes. Implicit change detection requires that the pre- .35 Incorrect change memory representation be encoded, retrieved and compared with the postchange aspect but that the result of .30 this comparison not be available for explicit response acti- vation (Ryan & Cohen, 2004). If the bias toward probable

xations .25 i

F changes occurred after encoding and retrieval (i.e., dur- .20 ing explicit response activation), implicit change detection would be greater for improbable changes than for probable tion of .15 changes. This would be the case because the same number

opor of probable and improbable changes would be encoded r P .10 and then retrieved and compared but more of these changes .05 would be explicitly reported for the probable changes (as demonstrated by the change probability effect), leaving 0 more of the improbable changes to be detected implicitly. Probable Improbable However, we found implicit change detection for prob- able changes, but not for improbable changes. This dem- Figure 8. The proportions of all the fixations on the prechange onstrates that even before the stage of explicit response scene that were on the probable and improbable prechange re- gions as a function of accuracy in the change detection task. Bars activation, a bias toward probable changes has occurred. represent the standard errors. This, together with the evidence from the LTM test show- ing that the bias toward probable changes does not occur during encoding, supports the conclusion that the bias to- DISCUSSION ward probable changes occurs during retrieval.

The change probability effect (Beck et al., 2004) was Causes of Change Blindness replicated here, with the participants detecting more prob- The results presented here also provide additional able changes than improbable changes. This demonstrates evidence not only for the cause of the change probabil- the importance of knowledge in directing processing re- ity effect, but also for the causes of change detection in sources toward some changes over others. The research general. Specifically, accurate change detection was as- presented here suggests that a bias toward probable sociated with higher fixation rates on the pre- and post- changes during the retrieval process is the cause of the change regions (for similar findings, see Henderson & change probability effect. First, equivalent performance on Hollingworth, 2003; Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002). the LTM test for probable and improbable changes ruled Therefore, an attentional bias, during encoding, toward out the possibility that encoding is biased toward probable some aspects of the visual world over others leads to better prechange aspects over improbable prechange aspects. change detection performance for these aspects. Second, implicit change detection was found for prob- Although an attentional bias during encoding may be able changes, but not for improbable changes. Therefore, the cause of change blindness in some instances, it does not only are probable changes detected more explicitly, not appear to be the cause of the change probability effect. but also they are detected more implicitly. This suggests Both probable and improbable prechange aspects were that the retrieval and comparison process is biased toward looked at with equivalent frequency and duration. This probable changes over improbable changes. Finally, two demonstrates not only that probable prechange regions fail additional sources of evidence were found against the pos- to attract attention preferentially, but also that, when atten- sibility that a response bias toward probable changes is the tion arrives, it does not dwell for longer periods of time cause of the change probability effect. The first source than it dwells on improbable prechange regions. There- of evidence was that change detection performance for fore, the amount of time spent encoding the prechange re- probable changes was still higher than change detection gion appears to be related to change detection in general, performance for improbable changes when a signal de- but it is not related to the change probability effect. tection measure was used. The second source of evidence Both the change probability effect and change detection was that the participants were no more likely to choose a in general appear to be related to a failure to retrieve en- description of a probable change to describe a false alarm coded prechange representations. Memory performance than they were to choose a description of an improbable was between 75% and 79%, suggesting that the majority change. Taken as a whole, these results indicate that the of the prechange aspects of the scenes were encoded, but change probability effect is caused by a limited capacity change detection performance was only between 37% and retrieval process that is biased toward probable changes 52%. Therefore, in general, more information is encoded over improbable changes. than is retrieved during change detection. This finding is The hypothesis that the change probability effect is consistent with those of a growing number of reports sup- driven by a bias toward probable changes during retrieval porting the once controversial idea that encoding failures is supported by implicit change detection for probable account for only a small proportion of change detection THE ROLES OF ENCODING, RETRIEVAL, AND AWARENESS 619 failures (Angelone et al., 2003; Hollingworth & Hender- Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: son, 2002; Varakin & Levin, 2006). If encoding failures Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin, 75, 424-429. were the main cause of change blindness, the high per- Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (2003). Eye movements and visual memory: Detecting changes to targets in scenes. Per- formance levels on the memory task would predict better ception & Psychophysics, 65, 58-71. performance on the change detection task than was found Hollingworth, A. (2003). Failures of retrieval and comparison con- here. Similarly, if encoding failures were the cause of the strain change detection in natural scenes. Journal of Experimental change probability effect, we would expect that memory Psychology: Human & Performance, 29, 388-403. Hollingworth, A. (2004). Constructing visual representations of natu- performance for probable changes would be better than ral scenes: The roles of short- and long-term visual memory. Journal memory performance for improbable change. However, of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 30, the data presented here suggest that encoding failures are 519-537. not the sole cause of change detection in general and that Hollingworth, A. (2005). The relationship between online representa- they are not the cause of the change probability effect. tions of a scene and long-term scene memory. Journal of Experimen- tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 396-411. Hollingworth, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2000). Semantic informa- Conclusion tiveness mediates the detection of changes in natural scenes. Visual Research demonstrating participants’ inability to de- Cognition. 7, 213-235. tect visual changes has been used to argue for the absence Hollingworth, A., & Henderson, J. M. (2002). Accurate visual memory for previously attended objects in natural scenes. Journal or sparseness of memory representations of the external of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 28, visual world (O’Regan, 1992; Rensink, 2000; Rensink 113-136. et al., 1997; Simons & Levin, 1997). This argument has Hollingworth, A., Williams, C. C., & Henderson, J. M. (2001). To recently been called into question by data showing pre- see and remember: Visually specific information is retained in mem- served memory representations in the face of undetected ory from previously attended objects in natural scenes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 761-768. visual changes (Angelone et al., 2003; Hollingworth & Irwin, D. E., & Andrews, R. (1996). Integration and accumulation of Henderson, 2002; Hollingworth et al., 2001; Simons, information across saccadic eye movements. In T. Inui & J. L. McClel- Chabris, Schnur, & Levin, 2002; for a review, see Simons land (Eds.), Attention and performance XVI: Information integration & Rensink, 2005). This suggests that memory representa- in perception and communication (pp. 125-155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. tions of the visual world are frequently encoded but that Levin, D. T., & Simons, D. J. (1997). Failure to detect changes to at- change detection tasks do not always serve to success- tended objects in motion pictures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, fully retrieve these . The results presented here 4, 501-506. speak to the importance of using change detection tasks to Levin, D. T., Simons, D. J., Angelone, B. L., & Chabris, C. F. (2002). discover when LTMs are retrieved and used to aid visual Memory for centrally attended changing objects in an incidental real- world change detection paradigm. British Journal of Psychology, 93, processing. Performance on the LTM test suggests that a 289-302. fair amount of information in the visual world is encoded Mitroff, S. R., Simons, D. J., & Franconeri, S. L. (2002). The siren into LTM. However, the encoding of visual information song of implicit change detection. Journal of Experimental Psychol- may be functionally irrelevant if the memory is not re- ogy: Human Perception & Performance, 28, 798-815. O’Regan, J. K. (1992), Solving the “real” mysteries of visual percep- trieved at the critical moment (e.g., when a change must tion: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychol- be detected). ogy, 46, 461-488. O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). Acting out our sensory experience. AUTHOR NOTE Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 955-975. O’Regan, J. K, Rensink, R. A., & Clark, J. J. (1999). Change-blindness This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant R01 as a result of “mudsplashes.” Nature, 398, 34. MH64505 to M.S.P. Address correspondence to M. R. Beck, Depart- Rensink, R. A. (2000). Seeing, sensing, and scrutinizing. Vision Re- ment of Psychology, 236 Audubon Hall, Louisiana State University, search, 40, 1469-1487. Baton Rouge, LA 70803 (email: [email protected]). Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245-277. REFERENCES Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychologi- Angelone, B. L., Levin, D. T., & Simons, D. J. (2003). The relation- cal Science, 8, 368-373. ship between change detection and recognition of centrally attended Ryan, J. D., & Cohen, N. J. (2004). The nature of change detection and objects in motion pictures. Perception, 32, 947-962. online representations of scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychol- Beck, M. B., Angelone, B. L., & Levin, D. T. (2004). Knowledge ogy: Human Perception & Performance, 30, 988-1015. about the probability of change affects change detection performance. Shinoda, H., Hayhoe, M. M., & Shrivastava, A. (2001). What controls Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Perfor- attention in natural environments? Vision Research, 41, 3535-3545. mance, 30, 778-791. Simons, D. J. (1996). In sight, out of mind: When object representations Fernandez-Duque, D., Grossi, G., Thornton, I. M., & Neville, H. J. fail. Psychological Science, 7, 301-305. (2003). Representation of change: Separate electrophysiological Simons, D. J. (2000). Current approaches to change blindness. Visual markers of attention, awareness, and implicit processing. Journal of Cognition, 7, 1-15. , 15, 491-507. Simons, D. J., Chabris, C. F., Schnur, T., & Levin, D. T. (2002). Evi- Fernandez-Duque, D., & Thornton, I. M. (2003). Explicit mecha- dence for preserved representations in change blindness. Conscious- nisms do not account for implicit localization and identification of ness & Cognition, 11, 78-97. change: An empirical reply to Mitroff et al. (2002). Journal of Experi- Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cog- mental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 846-858. nitive Sciences, 1, 261-267. Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to peo- psychophysics. New York: Wiley. (Reprinted 1988, Los Altos, CA: ple during real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Peninsula) 5, 644-649. 620 BECK, PETERSON, AND ANGELONE

Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change Blindness: Past, pres- changes, we would need to run 124 subjects. In order for this effect ent, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 16-20. size to have a power of the ideal level (.8), we would need to run 245 Thornton, I. M., & Fernandez-Duque, D. (2000). An implicit mea- subjects. Therefore, although there may be a slight advantage of memory sure of undetected change. Spatial Vision, 14, 21-44. for probable over improbable changes, this advantage is so small that its Varakin, D. A., & Levin, D. T. (2006). Change blindness and visual contribution to the change probability effect, which has an effect size of memory: Visual representations get rich and act poor. British Journal .172 and a power of .676, is at best minimal. of Psychology, 97, 51-77. 2. Six participants were excluded from the analysis because they had Williams, P., & Simons, D. J. (2000). Detecting changes in novel, com- missing data in one of the cells; 2 did not have any incorrect probable plex three-dimensional objects. Visual Cognition, 7, 297-322. trials, 1 did not have any incorrect improbable trials, 2 never looked at the probable postchange region, and 1 never looked at the improbable ARCHIVED MATERIALS postchange region. For the remaining analyses, partici- pants were similarly excluded if they had missing data in any of the cells The following materials and links may be accessed through the necessary for the analysis. Psychonomic Society’s Norms, Stimuli, and Data Archive, www.psycho- 3. AŒ is a signal detection measure with a range between . 5 (chance) nomic.org/Archive. and 1.0 (perfect sensitivity). In other reports of change detection tasks To access these files or links, search the archive for this article, using with a yes/no response, AŒ has been used to examine change detection the journal (Memory & Cognition), the first author’s name (Beck), and performance (e.g., Hollingworth, 2004, 2005). The formula used to cal- the publication year (2007). culate AŒ was taken from Grier (1971): FILE: Beck-Memory and Cognition-2007.zip DESCRIPTION: The compressed archive file contains one file: h f 1 h f A`  1 + , File1.ext, containing the 10 picture sets (prechange scene with cor- 2 4h 1 f responding improbable and probable postchange scenes) used in the  present experiment. where h is the percentage correct for either probable or improbable AUTHOR’S E-MAIL ADDRESS: [email protected]. changes calculated on the basis of the yes/no response to the change, ignoring the accuracy on the 4AFC task, and f is the percentage of yes NOTES responses on the no-change trials (false alarms). 1. The effect size of change probability on memory performance is .032 as measured by partial eta-squared in a repeated measures ANOVA. In order for an effect size this small to result in a significant difference (Manuscript received September 8, 2005; ((  .05) in memory performance between probable and improbable revision accepted for publication February 5, 2006.)