Spatial Cells in the Subiculum; Influence of Boundary Manipulations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Spatial Cells in the Subiculum; Influence of Boundary Manipulations Spatial cells in the Subiculum; Influence of boundary manipulations Sarah Stewart BSc (Hons) MSc Submitted in accordance with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The University of Leeds Institute of Psychological Sciences, Faculty of Medicine and Health March 2013 The candidate confirms that the work submitted is her own and that appropriate credit has been given where reference has been made to the work of others. This copy has been supplied on the understanding that it is copyright material and that no quotation from the thesis may be published without proper acknowledgement. The right of Sarah Stewart to be identified as the Author of this work has been asserted by her in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. © 2012 The University of Leeds and Sarah Stewart i Acknowledgements I would like to dedicate this thesis to my little cousin Beth. Beth was a pragmatic and curious girl who was fascinated by everything. By the age of 10 she had already decided that when she grew up she wanted to be a scientist like me. Last year we lost Beth to Leukaemia. I would therefore like to dedicate this work to Beth, who we all miss and who will always be in my heart. I would like to give thanks to Dr Colin Lever. I could not have asked for a more enthusiastic and inspirational supervisor. I am grateful to Colin for encouraging my interest in science and for taking me on as PhD student. Colin’s maintained enthusiasm meant that after every meeting I was always left excited and with fresh ideas, for that I am indebted. Further I would like to thank Colin’s wife Kate and his children Rose and Moss for putting up with my home visits for our out of hours supervisory meetings. On a light note, I would also like to thank Colin for not laughing in my face when I first told him I thought I had recorded subicular grid cells. I would also like to thank Professor John Rodgers for his unerring support and for his time and assistance over the years. After Colin had moved from Leeds to Durham John became my first supervisor and took over all of Colin’s administrative roles. For that and for keeping me organised I will be eternally grateful. There are also many other people who have helped with various aspects of my PhD. I would like to thank Neil, Lisa, Mark, Mark and Rob for their assistance and help with all lab-related things. A big thank you also goes to Vincent Douchamps for helping me set up my lab and for constant technical support. I would also like to thank my Colleagues at UCL for help with histology and analyses. Ali Jeewajee, Tom Wills and Francesca Cacucci I would like to thank for their help with the grid cell analyses and useful discussion. Particular thanks must be given to Stephen Burton, who not only was of great help with histology, but also entertained and looked after me during all of my trips to UCL. I would like to also give special thanks to Brian and Agnes Wallwork, for firstly hand-making all of my testing environments, and for secondly becoming treasured friends. Brian and Agnes’ friendship and correspondence has continued to make me smile since our very first meeting three years ago. I would like to give thanks to the friends I have made during my time at IPS; Neil Boyle, Ian Flatters, Emily Norris, Rebecca Graber, Nathan Illman, Anna Rossiter and Sarah Smith. It has been a pleasure working with them and I am so thankful for their support and companionship. In particular I would like to thank Fay Twiston-Davies, Suzi Morson and Christine Wells. I give specific thanks to Fay and Christine for being my lab companions. Both of whom kept me smiling during the long hours of testing, and often kept me from going mad during the dark trials. I would like to thank Suzi for her unofficial role as my PA. Truly without her I would have never got anything done. Thank you also to Suzi and Christine for the continuous supply of wine and cheese nights, without you my PhD would have been impossible. It has been an emotional journey the last few years and I am so grateful and thankful that we were together through it. Lastly I would like to thank my family for their encouragement and for taking my mind off work whenever I visited home. Thank you to my Dad and my brother for being there when I needed them. Thank you to my grandparents Betty, Peter and Barclay for our long phone calls which were always able to cheer me. Also I would like to thank my granny Sally who I miss deeply. Most importantly I thank my Mum who is my rock and whose continued love and support humbles me almost every day. Without my families continued and unwavering support I would not have been able to reach this point. ii iii Abstract External space is mapped by a widespread brain circuit located in interconnected sub regions of the hippocampal-parahippocampal cortices. To date there have been 4 functionally specialised cell types identified providing information about location (place cells), heading direction (head direction cells), self-motion (grid cells) and boundaries (boundary vector cells and border cells). In the present thesis I present novel research identifying and characterising spatial cells located in the subiculum, an under explored region within the hippocampal formation. The subiculum is a key output structure from CA1 but also provides strong projections to the entorhinal cortex and pre- and parasubiculum, placing it ideally within the brain circuitry to contribute to the representations of external space. This thesis presents evidence for a variety of functionally and morphologically diverse spatial cells located in the subiculum. Critically I present the first ever report of subicular grid cells. These were recorded alongside boundary-responsive cells and head direction cells. The thesis characterises the basic properties of subicular grid cells, as tested in a variety of environmental manipulations. This thesis explores grid cell relationships to the environment and in particular to boundary manipulations. Among the key results in this thesis is the discovery that grid scale increases along the anterior- posterior axis of the subiculum, similar to MEC grid scale. This thesis also shows that subicular grid cell patterns can be disrupted with environmental manipulation. Wall removal caused grid patterns to shift orientation and increase grid scale. The grid scale expansion was related to novelty, which supports previous findings from MEC grid cells (Barry et al., 2007; 2012). This thesis also shows that grid patterns were disrupted by barrier insertion e.g. causing an inhibition to grid fields. When recorded in total darkness the grid cell patterns remained stable, suggesting that vision is not required for maintenance of the grid pattern structure. Taken together these findings provide evidence that grid cell firing patterns are at least partially determined by environmental boundaries. In addition this thesis extended upon the work of Lever et al., (2009), by presenting a detailed investigation and characterisation of subicular boundary vector cells (BVCs). I developed an empirical classification criterion, and utilised numerous manipulations to address the issue of what a BVC treats as a boundary. I also identified a new type of boundary related cell: the boundary-off cell. These cells have firing patterns which can be considered very similar to the iv inverse of the BVC. The inhibitory response of boundaries on these cells may provide a mechanism to explain the field inhibition seen in grid cells with barrier insertion. The data in this thesis presents novel research identifying and characterising spatial cells located in the subiculum. Of particular importance, is the discovery of grid cells in this structure that are intermingled with HD and boundary-responsive cells. The thesis focuses on characterising and investigating the importance of environment boundaries to subicular cell firing patterns. The results are discussed in relation to what is known about spatial representation in the hippocampal and parahippocampal regions, and the neural circuitry which comprises this representation. v Abbreviations AC Alternating current BVC Boundary vector cell CA Cornu ammonis Cm Centimetre COP Circular open platform DG Dentate Gyrus EC Entorhinal cortex EEG Electroencephalography HD Head direction Hz Hertz i.m. Intramuscular i.p . Intraperitoneal ITI inter trial interval Kg Kilograms LEC Lateral entorhinal cortex LED Light Emitting Diode LH Left hemisphere LWC Large walled circle LWS Large walled square M Metres MEC Medial entorhinal cortex µm Microns µs Microseconds µv Microvolts ml Millilitres ms Milliseconds RH right hemisphere s.c. Subcutaneous SE Standard error SEM Standard error of the mean SWS Small walled square vi Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................ iii Contents ........................................................................................................................................... vi Figure contents ........................................................................................................................... xiii Chapter 1 Introduction part 1: Anatomy of the hippocampal formation ...... 1 1.1 Nomenclature .......................................................................................................................... 1 1.2 Gross morphology ................................................................................................................
Recommended publications
  • Grid Cells Are Ubiquitous in Neural Networks
    Grid Cells Are Ubiquitous in Neural Networks Songlin Li1†, Yangdong Deng2*†, Zhihua Wang1 1 Department of Microelectronics and Nanoelectronics, Tsinghua University 2 School of Software, Tsinghua University * Corresponding author: Email [email protected] † The two authors contributed equally to this work. Abstract: Grid cells are believed to play an important role in both spatial and non-spatial cognition tasks. A recent study observed the emergence of grid cells in an LSTM for path integration. The connection between biological and artificial neural networks underlying the seemingly similarity, as well as the application domain of grid cells in deep neural networks (DNNs), expect further exploration. This work demonstrated that grid cells could be replicated in either pure vision based or vision guided path integration DNNs for navigation under a proper setting of training parameters. We also show that grid-like behaviors arise in feedforward DNNs for non-spatial tasks. Our findings support that the grid coding is an effective representation for both biological and artificial networks. Introduction: It has been long hypothesized that animals use a “cognitive map”1, namely, an internal knowledge representation to support intelligent behaviors. This proposal was later proved by neuroscience experiments on both spatial2,3 and non-spatial4,5 cognition tasks. Grid cells, which exhibit a spatial periodic firing pattern and thus constitute the hexagonal spatial representation as Fyhn et al has observed2, were discovered in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) of rodents2,3. Subsequent studies proposed models to characterize the behavior of grid cells in terms of spike-timing correlations6-8. The discovery of grid cells as well as the fact that different grid cells forms a multi-scale representation inspired the theory of vector-based navigation9.
    [Show full text]
  • Activity Strength Within Optic Flow-Sensitive Cortical Regions Is Associated with Visual Path Integration Accuracy in Aged Adults
    brain sciences Article Activity Strength within Optic Flow-Sensitive Cortical Regions Is Associated with Visual Path Integration Accuracy in Aged Adults Lauren Zajac 1,2,* and Ronald Killiany 1,2 1 Department of Anatomy & Neurobiology, Boston University School of Medicine, 72 East Concord Street (L 1004), Boston, MA 02118, USA; [email protected] 2 Center for Biomedical Imaging, Boston University School of Medicine, 650 Albany Street, Boston, MA 02118, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: Spatial navigation is a cognitive skill fundamental to successful interaction with our envi- ronment, and aging is associated with weaknesses in this skill. Identifying mechanisms underlying individual differences in navigation ability in aged adults is important to understanding these age- related weaknesses. One understudied factor involved in spatial navigation is self-motion perception. Important to self-motion perception is optic flow–the global pattern of visual motion experienced while moving through our environment. A set of optic flow-sensitive (OF-sensitive) cortical regions was defined in a group of young (n = 29) and aged (n = 22) adults. Brain activity was measured in this set of OF-sensitive regions and control regions using functional magnetic resonance imaging while participants performed visual path integration (VPI) and turn counting (TC) tasks. Aged adults had stronger activity in RMT+ during both tasks compared to young adults. Stronger activity in the OF-sensitive regions LMT+ and RpVIP during VPI, not TC, was associated with greater VPI Citation: Zajac, L.; Killiany, R. accuracy in aged adults. The activity strength in these two OF-sensitive regions measured during Activity Strength within Optic VPI explained 42% of the variance in VPI task performance in aged adults.
    [Show full text]
  • Marine Vehicles Localization Using Grid Cells for Path Integration
    Marine Vehicles Localization Using Grid Cells for Path Integration Ignacio Carluchoa, Manuel F. Baileya, Mariano De Paulab, Corina Barbalataa [email protected], [email protected], mariano.depaula@fio.unicen.edu.ar, [email protected] a Department of Mechanical Engineering, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, USA b INTELYMEC Group, Centro de Investigaciones en F´ısica e Ingenier´ıa del Centro CIFICEN – UNICEN – CICpBA – CONICET, 7400 Olavarr´ıa, Argentina Abstract—Autonomous Underwater Vehicles (AUVs) are plat- forms used for research and exploration of marine environments. However, these types of vehicles face many challenges that hinder their widespread use in the industry. One of the main limitations is obtaining accurate position estimation, due to the lack of GPS signal underwater. This estimation is usually done with Kalman filters. However, new developments in the neuroscience field have shed light on the mechanisms by which mammals are able to obtain a reliable estimation of their current position based on external and internal motion cues. A new type of neuron, called Grid cells, has been shown to be part of path integration system in the brain. In this article, we show how grid cells can be used for obtaining a position estimation of underwater vehicles. The model of grid cells used requires only the linear velocities together with heading orientation and provides a reliable estimation of the vehicle’s position. We provide simulation results for an AUV which show the feasibility of our proposed methodology. Index Terms—Grid cells, Navigation, Neuro inspired agents, Autonomous underwater vehicles Fig. 1. A modified BlueROV2 used for experiments I. INTRODUCTION In recent years, the interest in exploring our oceans has that strongly correlates with the animal position in space [5].
    [Show full text]
  • Emergence of Grid-Like Representations by Training
    Published as a conference paper at ICLR 2018 EMERGENCE OF GRID-LIKE REPRESENTATIONS BY TRAINING RECURRENT NEURAL NETWORKS TO PERFORM SPATIAL LOCALIZATION Christopher J. Cueva,∗ Xue-Xin Wei∗ Columbia University New York, NY 10027, USA fccueva,[email protected] ABSTRACT Decades of research on the neural code underlying spatial navigation have re- vealed a diverse set of neural response properties. The Entorhinal Cortex (EC) of the mammalian brain contains a rich set of spatial correlates, including grid cells which encode space using tessellating patterns. However, the mechanisms and functional significance of these spatial representations remain largely mysterious. As a new way to understand these neural representations, we trained recurrent neural networks (RNNs) to perform navigation tasks in 2D arenas based on veloc- ity inputs. Surprisingly, we find that grid-like spatial response patterns emerge in trained networks, along with units that exhibit other spatial correlates, including border cells and band-like cells. All these different functional types of neurons have been observed experimentally. The order of the emergence of grid-like and border cells is also consistent with observations from developmental studies. To- gether, our results suggest that grid cells, border cells and others as observed in EC may be a natural solution for representing space efficiently given the predominant recurrent connections in the neural circuits. 1 INTRODUCTION Understanding the neural code in the brain has long been driven by studying feed-forward
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Grid Cells and Theta Oscillations
    BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS ARISING Models of grid cells and theta oscillations ARISING FROM M. M.Yartsev, M. P. Witter & N. Ulanovsky Nature 479, 103–107 (2011) Grid cells recorded in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) of freely and in putative velocity-controlled oscillatory inputs identified as inter- moving rodents show a markedly regular spatial firing pattern whose neurons within the septohippocampal circuit7. underlying mechanism has been the subject of intense interest. Yartsev et al.1 recorded the firing of grid cells from bats trained to Yartsev et al.1 report that the firing of grid cells in crawling bats does crawl within the recording environment, a behaviour that they per- not show theta rhythmicity ‘‘causally disproving a major class of form very slowly (a mean speed of 3.7 cm s21 versus 17.6 cm s21 in computational models’’ of grid cell firing that rely on oscillatory our rat data), often stopping entirely (supplementary figure 11 in interference2–7. However, their data may be consistent with these ref. 1). The authors found grid cells with very low firing rates (a mean models, with the apparent lack of theta rhythmicity reflecting slow peak rate of 0.56 Hz versus 5.14 Hz in our data) and little significant movement speeds and low firing rates. Thus, the conclusion of theta modulation. However, matching movement speed is important Yartsev et al. is not supported by their data. for comparisons involving theta. At low speeds movement-related In oscillatory interference models, path integration is performed by theta rhythmicity is strongly attenuated12 and the need for path velocity-dependent variation in the frequencies of theta-band oscilla- integration is reduced.
    [Show full text]
  • Resonating Neurons Stabilize Heterogeneous Grid-Cell Networks
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.10.419200; this version posted December 11, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 Resonating neurons stabilize heterogeneous grid-cell networks 2 3 Divyansh Mittal and Rishikesh Narayanan* 4 5 Cellular Neurophysiology Laboratory, Molecular Biophysics Unit, Indian Institute of 6 Science, Bangalore, India. 7 8 *Corresponding Author 9 10 Rishikesh Narayanan, Ph.D. 11 Molecular Biophysics Unit 12 Indian Institute of Science 13 Bangalore 560 012, India. 14 15 e-mail: [email protected] 16 Phone: +91-80-22933372 17 Fax: +91-80-23600535 18 Number of words: 250 (abstract), 120 (significance statement) 19 20 Abbreviated title: Resonating neurons stabilize heterogeneous networks 21 22 Keywords: grid cells, entorhinal cortex, continuous attractor network, resonance, 23 heterogeneities 24 25 Author contributions 26 D. M. and R. N. designed experiments; D. M. performed experiments and carried out data 27 analysis; D. M. and R. N. co-wrote the paper. 28 29 Competing financial interests 30 The authors declare no conflict of interest. 31 32 Acknowledgments 33 This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust-DBT India Alliance (Senior fellowship to 34 RN; IA/S/16/2/502727), Human Frontier Science Program (HFSP) Organization (RN), the 35 Department of Biotechnology through the DBT-IISc partnership program (RN), the Revati 36 and Satya Nadham Atluri Chair Professorship (RN), and the Ministry of Human Resource 37 Development (RN & DM). The authors thank Dr. Poonam Mishra, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioral Strategies, Sensory Cues, and Brain Mechanisms
    Intro to Neuroscience: Behavioral Neuroscience Animal Navigation: Behavioral strategies, sensory cues, and brain mechanisms Nachum Ulanovsky Department of Neurobiology, Weizmann Institute of Science Outline of today’s lecture • Introduction: Feats of animal navigation • Navigational strategies: • Beaconing • Route following • Path integration • Map and Compass / Cognitive Map • Sensory cues for navigation: • Compass mechanisms • Map mechanisms • Brain mechanisms of Navigation (brief introduction) • Summary Outline of today’s lecture • Introduction: Feats of animal navigation • Navigational strategies: • Beaconing • Route following • Path integration • Map and Compass / Cognitive Map • Sensory cues for navigation: • Compass mechanisms • Map mechanisms • Brain mechanisms of Navigation (brief introduction) • Summary Shearwater migration across the pacific יסעור Population data from 19 birds 3 pairs of birds Recaptured at their breeding Shaffer et al. PNAS 103:12799-12802 (2006) grounds in New Zealand Some other famous examples • Wandering Albatross: finding a tiny island in the vast ocean • Salmon: returning to the river of birth after years in the ocean • Sea Turtles • Monarch Butterflies • Spiny Lobsters • … And many other examples (some of them we will see later) Mammals can also do it… Medium-scale navigation: Egyptian fruit bats navigating to an individual tree Tsoar, Nathan, Bartan, Vyssotski, Dell’Omo & Ulanovsky (PNAS, 2011) GPS movie: Bat 079 A typical example of a full night flight of an individual bat released @ cave Bat roost Foraging tree 5 Km Characteristics of the bats’ commuting flights: • Long-distance flights (often > 15 km one-way) • Very straight flights (straightness index > 0.9 for almost all bats) • Very fast (typically 30–40 km/hr, and up to 63 km/hr) • Very high (typically 100–200 meters, and up to 643 m) • Bats returned to the same individual tree night after night, for many nights tree cave Tsoar, Nathan, Bartan, With Vyssotski, Dell’Omo & Y.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Linear Navigation: Is Vision Necessary? Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I
    Spatial linear navigation: Is vision necessary? Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I. Giannopulu To cite this version: Isabelle Israël, Aurore Capelli, Anne-Emmanuelle Priot, I. Giannopulu. Spatial linear navigation: Is vision necessary?. Neuroscience Letters, Elsevier, 2013, 554, pp.34-38. 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.08.060. hal-02395669 HAL Id: hal-02395669 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02395669 Submitted on 5 Dec 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Elsevier Editorial System(tm) for Neuroscience Letters Manuscript Draft Manuscript Number: Title: Spatial linear navigation : is vision necessary ? Article Type: Research Paper Keywords: Path integration; self-motion perception; multisensory integration Corresponding Author: Dr. Isabelle Israel, PhD Corresponding Author's Institution: CNRS First Author: Isabelle ISRAEL, PhD Order of Authors: Isabelle ISRAEL, PhD; Aurore CAPELLI, PhD; Anne-Emmanuelle PRIOT, MD, PhD; Irini GIANNOPULU, PhD, D.Sc. Abstract: In order to analyze spatial linear navigation through a task of self-controlled reproduction, healthy participants were passively transported on a mobile robot at constant velocity, and then had to reproduce the imposed distance of 2 to 8 m in two conditions: "with vision" and "without vision".
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Grid Cells and Theta Oscillations
    Publisher: NPG; Journal: Nature: Nature; Article Type: BCA DOI: 10.1038/nature11276 Models of grid cells and theta oscillations ARISING FROM M. M.Yartsev, M. P. Witter & N. Ulanovsky Nature 479, 103–107 (2011) Grid cells recorded in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) of freely moving rodents show a strikingly regular spatial firing pattern whose underlying mechanism has been the subject of intense interest. Yartsev et al.1 report that the firing of grid cells in crawling bats does not show theta rhythmicity “causally disproving a major class of computational models” of grid cell firing that rely on oscillatory interference2–7. However, their data may be consistent with these models, with the apparent lack of theta rhythmicity reflecting slow movement speeds and low firing rates. Thus, Yartsev et al.’s conclusion is not supported by their data. In oscillatory interference models, path integration is performed by velocity- dependent variation in the frequencies of theta-band oscillations, which combine to generate the grid-cell pattern2–4,6,7. In addition, learned associations to environmental sensory inputs (possibly mediated by place cells) ensure that grids are spatially stable over time and are sufficient to maintain firing in familiar environments2,3,8. In rats, the majority of grid cells show theta-modulated firing9,10, and the model predicts specific relationships between modulation frequency, running velocity and grid scale4, which have been verified in grid cells11 and in putative velocity-controlled oscillatory inputs identified as interneurons within the septohippocampal circuit7. Yartsev et al.1 recorded the firing of grid cells from bats trained to crawl within the recording environment, a behaviour that they perform very slowly (a mean speed of 3.7 cm s−1 versus 17.6 cm s−1 in our rat data), often stopping entirely (supplementary figure 11 in ref.
    [Show full text]
  • Path Integration Deficits During Linear Locomotion After Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy
    Path Integration Deficits during Linear Locomotion after Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy John W.Philbeck 1,Marlene Behrmann 2,Lucien Levy 3, Samuel J.Potolicchio 3,andAnthony J.Caputy 3 Abstract & Animalnavigation studies have implicated structures inand both adecrease inthe consistency ofpath integration and a around the hippocampal formation as crucialin performing systematic underregistration oflinear displacement (and/or path integration (amethod ofdetermining one’s position by velocity) during walking.Moreover, the deficits were observable monitoring internally generated self-motion signals). Less is even when there were virtually no angular acceleration known about the role ofthese structures forhuman path vestibular signals. Theresults suggest that structures inthe integration. We tested path integration inpatients whohad medial temporal lobe participate inhuman path integration undergone left orright medial temporal lobectomy as therapy when individuals walkalong linearpaths and that thisis so to forepilepsy. Thisprocedure removed approximately 50% ofthe agreater extent inright hemisphere structures than left. anterior portion ofthe hippocampus, as wellas the amygdala Thisinformation is relevant forfuture research investigating and lateral temporal lobe. Participants attempted to walk the neural substrates ofnavigation, not only inhumans without vision to apreviously viewed target 2–6 mdistant. (e.g.,functional neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies), Patients withright, but not left,hemisphere lesions exhibited but also
    [Show full text]
  • Path Integration in Place Cells of Developing Rats PNAS PLUS
    Path integration in place cells of developing rats PNAS PLUS Tale L. Bjerknesa,b, Nenitha C. Dagslotta,b, Edvard I. Mosera,b, and May-Britt Mosera,b,1 aKavli Institute for Systems Neuroscience, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, NO-7489 Trondheim, Norway; and bCentre for Neural Computation, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, NO-7489 Trondheim, Norway Contributed by May-Britt Moser, January 2, 2018 (sent for review November 1, 2017; reviewed by John L. Kubie and Mayank R. Mehta) Place cells in the hippocampus and grid cells in the medial entorhinal Firing locations of grid cells and place cells are not determined cortex rely on self-motion information and path integration for exclusively by path integration, however. Position information may spatially confined firing. Place cells can be observed in young rats be obtained also from distal landmarks, as suggested by the fact that as soon as they leave their nest at around 2.5 wk of postnatal life. In place fields (17) as well as grid fields (18, 19) follow the location of contrast, the regularly spaced firing of grid cells develops only after the walls of the recording environment when the environment is weaning, during the fourth week. In the present study, we sought to stretched or compressed. This observation points to local bound- determine whether place cells areabletointegrateself-motion aries as a strong determinant of firing location. On the other hand, information before maturation of the grid-cell system. Place cells other work has demonstrated that place cells fire in a predictable were recorded on a 200-cm linear track while preweaning, post- relationship to the animal’s start location on a linear track even weaning, and adult rats ran on successive trials from a start wall to a when the position of the starting point is shifted (20, 21).
    [Show full text]
  • May-Britt Moser Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway
    Grid Cells, Place Cells and Memory Nobel Lecture, 7 December 2014 by May-Britt Moser Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Trondheim, Norway. n 7 December 2014 I gave the most prestigious lecture I have given in O my life—the Nobel Prize Lecture in Medicine or Physiology. Afer lectures by my former mentor John O’Keefe and my close colleague of more than 30 years, Edvard Moser, the audience was still completely engaged, wonderful and responsive. I was so excited to walk out on the stage, and proud to present new and exciting data from our lab. Te title of my talk was: “Grid cells, place cells and memory.” Te long-term vision of my lab is to understand how higher cognitive func- tions are generated by neural activity. At frst glance, this seems like an over- ambitious goal. President Barack Obama expressed our current lack of knowl- edge about the workings of the brain when he announced the Brain Initiative last year. He said: “As humans, we can identify galaxies light years away; we can study particles smaller than an atom. But we still haven’t unlocked the mystery of the three pounds of matter that sits between our ears.” Will these mysteries remain secrets forever, or can we unlock them? What did Obama say when he was elected President? “Yes, we can!” To illustrate that the impossible is possible, I started my lecture by showing a movie with a cute mouse that struggled to bring a biscuit over an edge and home to its nest. Te biscuit was almost bigger than the mouse itself.
    [Show full text]