Realistic Conflict Theory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Realistic Conflict Theory 11 Applications and Extensions of Realistic Conf ict T eory: Moral Development and Conf ict Prevention Jessica McKenzie and Gabriel Twose T e study of intergroup confl ict is of central concern to social scien- tists. Confl ict is an inevitable result of human interaction, and learning how to reduce or prevent destructive disputes is vitally important. Few theorists have contributed to the fi eld to the extent of Muzafer Sherif, whose realistic confl ict theory states that contradictory goals lead to group confl ict and cooperation-requiring superordinate goals reduce it (Sherif, 1958). Blending psychological and sociological frameworks, his Robbers Cave experiment (which would form the basis of realistic confl ict theory) remains infl uential to this day. Sherif, Harvey, White, Hood, and Sherif (1954/1988) postulated that intergroup confl ict arises as a result of situational variables, personality characteristics, interindividual diff erences, and group psychology. While we are aware that most readers will be familiar with the work, we will begin with a brief overview of Sherif’s seminal Robbers Cave study and the theory that it grew. We will then delineate subsequent research that has built on Sherif’s fi ndings, and fi nally, turn to an ex- tension of his work, incorporating research from multiple disciplines. Our proposed extension involves a moral education that utilizes concepts similar to those suggested by Sherif and by those that have built on his foundations. We argue that such a moral education off ers a promising confl ict resolution strategy by way of curtailing confl ict before its initiation. 307 Norms, Groups, Conf ict, and Social Change Robbers Cave Summary Sherif’s three-stage Robbers Cave experiment took place in 1954 in Southeastern Oklahoma. Stage one of the study involved the experimental formation of two distinct groups. Twenty-two boys ap- proximately 11 years of age were carefully selected so as to preclude diff erences in socioeconomic, religious, or family backgrounds, as well as any psychological abnormality or maladjustment. None of the par- ticipants knew each other prior to the experiment. T e boys were sepa- rated into two groups before ever meeting each other, and bussed to an isolated summer camp. For the fi rst week, the groups were functionally isolated from one another, as the individuals coalesced into two groups with defi ned hierarchies and norms. More specifi cally, members faced a series of problems (such as cooking, improving a swimming hole, and playing sports), which they solved through coordinated action, resulting in the formation of two delineated in-groups. Leaders and followers became apparent within each group, norms such as “toughness” or “being a good sport” formed, and names (Eagles, Rattlers) and places (camping grounds, swimming holes) were claimed. In stage two, the groups were placed in zero-sum competitive situations with one another so that victory for one meant defeat for the other. Competitions included baseball games, treasure hunts, and tugs-of-war. Relations quickly became antagonistic, with negative stereotypes prevalent, and arguing and fi ghting common. Intergroup relations were assessed through participant observation, sociometric indices, and a series of experimental interventions. In the all-important fi nal stage, intergroup hostility was reduced. Sherif et al. (1954/1988) fi rst showed that contact alone (for example, watching a movie, eating a meal) did nothing to decrease the friction between the two groups. A further strategy was necessary in order to eff ectively reduce intergroup hostility. After considering several options, Sherif and colleagues elected to focus on the introduction of superordinate goals, or “goals of high appeal for both groups, which cannot be ignored by the groups in question, but whose attainment is beyond the resources and eff orts of one group alone” (Sherif et al., 1954/1988, p. 204). T e groups were forced to cooperate in order to overcome situations such as a broken truck and a blocked water pipe. Similar intergroup relations measures to those employed after stage 2 showed a reduction of negative stereotypes and an increase in liking between the groups. 308 Applications and Extensions of Realistic Conf ict T eory Realistic Conf ict T eory T e Robbers Cave study forms the basis of Sherif’s realistic confl ict theory, showing that intergroup confl ict is produced by confl icting goals and reduced by superordinate goals achievable only through cooperation. Intergroup relations therefore cannot be reduced to within-group relations or personality characteristics; the wider social and environmental context is crucial in shaping the relationship. When goals become competitive, such that one’s success results in another’s loss, the out-group will become negatively stereotyped and social dis- tance between groups will increase. Real-world factors may include threat to group security (real or imagined), military might, social status, economic considerations, or general competition for scarce resources (Jackson, 1993; Sherif et al., 1988). Sherif also stressed the importance of in-group identifi cation, ar- guing that an individual’s identity and personal interests are heavily reliant on group membership (a proposition that forms the basis of social identity theory (SIT) [Tajfel & Turner, 1976]). Since the group is important to one’s sense of self, negative normative intergroup atti- tudes become internalized, thus deepening and extending the confl ict. Concurrently, the negative intergroup relations tend to strengthen intragroup solidarity, again boosting the unfavorable attitudes toward the out-group (Sherif, 1958). Of course, there is far more intricate detail to report than space permits, for example, the eff ect of intergroup confl ict on in-group hierarchies and the role of historical group interactions. And certain qualifi cations must be applied, for example, the limitations in ability to attribute causality in a non-laboratory study and the possibly dubious ethics of uninformed participation. In short though, Sherif thoroughly and convincingly showed that: confl ict between groups arises at least in part from competition for limited resources or confl icting goals and confl ict can be reduced by common striving toward a goal that requires cooperation. Realistic confl ict theory has since received a great deal of support (Jackson, 1993). Subsequent Support for Realistic Conf ict T eory Andreeva (1984) replicated the study in the Soviet Union, while Diab (1970) was forced to abandon his attempts in Lebanon due to danger- ously high levels of intergroup hostility. Tyerman and Spencer (1983) recreated the investigation utilizing a Scout Troop who already knew one another, which they argue better refl ects most real-life scenarios. 309 Norms, Groups, Conf ict, and Social Change In this case, competition led to some out-group stereotyping, but did not result in outright hostility. Psychologists have similarly found that competition may lead to hostility in a variety of contexts. For example, Worchel, Axsom, Ferris, Samaha, and Schweitzer (1978) conducted a laboratory-based study in which two groups either competed or acted independently, wearing either similar or diff erent uniforms. Inter- group attraction was lowest for groups that wore distinctive uniforms and competed with one another. Subsequent intergroup cooperation increased intergroup liking, particularly when the groups succeeded in their task, were not distinguished by dress, and had not previously competed. R e a l i s t i c c o n fl ict theory has also been tested in more real world settings. Blake and Mouton (1961) studied win–loss contests in an industrial confl ict between management and a union, concluding that zero-sum competition should be replaced by an orientation stressing mutual problem solving. In an extended application, Bobo (1983) investigated the reasons behind opposition to busing during the civil rights movement. Although some have disagreed with his analysis of the data (see Kinder & Sears, 1981), Bobo extended Sherif’s idea that competitive goals can lead to confl ict, fi nding that perceived threat was by far the strongest predictor of Whites’ opposition to busing. Other factors such as a general dislike for blacks or a strong belief in segrega- tion proved insignifi cant. Simply the perception that Black Americans could be a competitor for scarce resources, or could challenge valued norms, was enough to incite opposition. Studies within other disciplines have also provided support. An- thropologists Divale and Harris (1976) studied a number of smaller societies, fi nding that competition over limited resources, such as land, is a primary driver of intergroup confl ict. Sociologists have stressed the divisive potential of unequal resource distribution, even when such re- sources are abundant (Cummings, 1980). T is array of mixed-methods, interdisciplinary, and international support has convincingly validated the basic tenets of realistic confl ict theory. Many more recent psychological theories also draw from or build upon Sherif’s realistic confl ict theory. Social Identity T eory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) is based on the premise that intergroup confl ict need not arise from interpersonal variables. T is theory shares the basic tenet of Sherif’s theory: that confl ict can arise simply due to the sheer existence of two groups, without the necessity of overt compe- tition. Addressing the amelioration of confl ict, the contact hypothesis 310 Applications and Extensions of Realistic Conf ict T eory (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew, 1971; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006) also aligns with Sherif’s belief that contact alone is not enough to eliminate con- fl ict. T e contact hypothesis argues that in addition to superordinate goals, additional factors such as equal status, egalitarian norms, and cooperative independence are necessary to change cognitive represen- tations of out-groups and to reduce confl ict. T ese positive forms of contact may result in what has become known as a common in-group identity. Common In-Group Identity Model The influential common in-group identity model (Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000) is heavily indebted to realistic confl ict theory, building from and expanding upon Sherif’s ideas.
Recommended publications
  • Social Psychology of Violence
    Social Psychology of Violence From "Encyclopedia of Violence, Peace and Conflict" Situational, Cognitive, and Systemic Sources of Violence Three Kinds of Violence: Episodic, Structural, and Cultural Analysis of Conflict and Violence at Multiple Levels Social Psychology of Peace-making Social Psychology of Structural and Cultural Violence Peace Psychology: The Pursuit of Peace-building Further Reading Glossary Enemy Images Exaggerated representations of the ‘other’ as thoroughly diabolical, aggressive, and untrustworthy. Dehumanization The mental process of stripping away the human qualities of the other. Fundamental Attribution Error The tendency to perceive the bad behavior of out-group members to bad character and to perceive the bad behavior of in-group members to situational factors. Structural Violence Indirect violence through social injustice, inequity, and failure to meet basic human needs for items such as food and shelter. Superordinate Goals Goals that lie within the interests of each group in a conflict but that can be attained only through intergroup cooperation, which reduces tensions and destructive conflict. Social psychology examines the psychological influence of people on people. Although violence such as the mass killing at Virginia Tech in April 2007 by a lone gunman is often seen as individual aggression, social psychology analyzes such acts through the lens of social and situational influences. Situational, Cognitive, and Systemic Sources of Violence Following World War II, psychological analyses of violence often emphasized personality variables, such as the degree to which people are receptive to prejudice or have a tendency to endorse authoritarian beliefs. This emphasis arose as an effort to understand the rise of fascism in Europe and later, the problem of race relations in the United States.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotypes and Prejudice
    Stereotypes and Prejudice Their Overt and Subtle Influence in the Classroom CONNIE T. WOLFE University of Michigan STEVEN J. SPENCER Hope College The authors examine overt and subtle forms of stereotyping and prejudice. Two theories that explain overt prejudice are reviewed: realistic conflict theory and social identity theory. Although overt prejudice seems to have declined, subtle stereotyping is still pervasive. The authors review one theory, aversive racism theory, that explains this phenomenon. They also discuss two perspectives, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, which provide accounts of the impact of subtle racism. Both overt and subtle prejudice present challenges for the classroom. The authors describe one intervention called the jigsaw classroom that encourages work toward common goals and helps reduce the expression and impact of overt discrimination. A second intervention program, wise schooling, is presented, which aims to reduce the impact of subtle stereotypes by reducing stereotype threat. Why do prejudice and discrimination exist? Has overt racism been replaced by more subtle forms of prejudice? How does stereotyping affect its targets? In this article we describe two theories, realistic conflict theory and social identity theory, which provide an answer to the first question. We address the second question by noting that although overt discrimination has decreased, subtle forms of prejudice are still quite common and we describe one theory, aversive racism, that provides a compelling account of this change in the expression of prejudice. Finally, we answer the third question by describing two phenomena, attributional ambiguity and stereotype threat, that result from the pervasive nature of subtle stereotyping. This article is a selective overview of what social psychology has to say about these crucial issues.
    [Show full text]
  • PREJUDICE and International Education
    PREJUDICE and International Education Conrad Hughes PREJUDICE and International Education 1. Whatisit? 2. Theories of the way prejudice develops 3. Can it be measured? 4. Schools and prejudice 5. International education and prejudice 6. How can prejudice be reduced? 1. What is it? praejudicium – “precedent” strong generalisation rationalises “beliefs held on irrational grounds [to protect ] a deep-seated system of emotions” (Thouless, 1930, p. 150) “will to misunderstand” (Shi-Xu, 2001, p. 281) Fixed mind-set Tends to be self-gratifying 2. Theories of the way prejudice develops Psychoanalytical Frustration aggression theory Dollard et al. (1939) Hovland & Sears (1940) Emotional maladjustment Adorno et al. (1950) Relative deprivation Runciman (1966) Realistic conflict theory Sherif et al. (1954) Vivian & Brown (1995) 2. Theories of the way prejudice develops Developmental Psychology “the child’s discovery of his homeland and understanding of other countries is a process of transition from egocentricity to reciprocity” Piaget & Weil (1951) Allport’s 3 stage theory of prejudice formation (1954) Pregeneralization (middle childhood) : children are generally aware of differences but do not categorize others or have strong feelings toward other groups. Early puberty : total rejection of outgroups peaks. Frenkel-Brunswick, 1948; Radke & Stage three (late adolescence) : adolescent’s Sutherland, 1949; Remy, Nathan, thinking becomes more differentiated and less Becker & Torney, 1975; Aboud, generalized. 1988; Doyle & Boud,1995; Aboud & Amato, 2001; Cushner, 2008 2. Theories of the way prejudice develops Social psychology/Sociological Belief Congruence Theory Rokeach (1960, 1971) Black Skin White Masks Frantz Fanon (1952) Social Reflection Bandura (1977) Social Identity Theory Tajfel & Turner (1986) 3. Can it be measured? Implicit Association Test Greenwald, Banaji, Nosek(1998) Clark & Clark Doll Task (1954) (Trait Attribution) The Robbers Cave Experiment Sherif et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Online Intergroup Contact
    OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – REVISES-PROOF, CENVEO 09/25/2012 1 Chapter 12 2 Online Intergroup Contact 3 Béatrice S. Hasler & Yair Amichai-Hamburger 4 Introduction 5 In our globalized world we live and work in close contact with people from various backgrounds. 6 However, globalization does not necessarily cause homogenization; quite the opposite in fact: We 7 now have to deal with differences between ethnic, racial, and religious groups in a more direct 8 way (Nolan, 1999 ). It has become crucial to everyday life to accept differences in values, beliefs, 9 and interests between various social groups while building on commonalities. Nonetheless, we are 10 witnessing a growing trend toward segregation and separatism in many places in the world (Hoter 11 et al., 2009 ). Prejudice and discrimination against rival groups often result in conflict, ranging 12 from mild hostility to all-out war, leading to the loss of thousands of lives each year. Especially in 13 areas of intractable conflict, intergroup bias is characterized and reinforced by “antagonistic 14 group histories, exclusionist myths, demonizing propaganda and dehumanizing ideologies” 15 (Ramsbotham et al., 2011 , p. 103). 16 The preamble to the United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization 17 (UNESCO) Constitution states that “since wars begin in the minds of men, it is in the minds of 18 men that the defenses of peace must be constructed” (UNESCO Constitution, 1945 ). The chal- 19 lenge of reducing intergroup bias within the human mind lays a great necessity upon social psy- 20 chological research. Many social psychologists have attempted to understand the complex 21 phenomenon of intergroup conflict, and to provide solutions to end it.
    [Show full text]
  • TARGET ARTICLE Diversity Science
    Psychological Inquiry, 21: 77–99, 2010 Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 1047-840X print / 1532-7965 online DOI: 10.1080/10478401003676501 TARGET ARTICLE Diversity Science: Why and How Difference Makes a Difference Victoria C. Plaut Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia Jurisprudence and Social Policy Program, University of California Berkeley School of Law, Berkeley, California This article proposes that addressing the complex ethnic and racial issues of the 21st century will require a diversity science. A diversity science will consider how people create, interpret, and maintain group differences among individuals, as well as the psychological and societal consequences of these distinctions. A diversity science will recognize that these significant social distinctions (in the case of this article, race and ethnicity) are not simply natural, neutral, or abstract. Instead they are created and re- created in the process of everyday social interactions that are grounded in historically derived ideas and beliefs about difference and in a set of practices and institutions that reflect these ideas and beliefs and that therefore shape psychological experience and behavior.According to this “sociocultural” framework, psychological experience and behavior, in turn, reinforce particular cultural and structural realities. As an initial step toward a diversity science, this article reviews the roots and consequences of two examples of how to think about difference, color blindness and multiculturalism. Through this sociocultural lens, intergroup behaviors can be understood as more than just products of individual prejudice. This article also proposes that a comprehensive diversity science requires a critical examination of majority group perspectives, minority group perspectives, and their dynamic interaction beyond the typical Black– White binary.
    [Show full text]
  • Principles for Intergroup Projects: a First Look
    PRINCIPLES FOR INTERGROUP PROJECTS: A FIRST LOOK June 1999 ASSOCIATION FOR THE STUDY AND DEVELOPMENT OF COMMUNITY PREFACE This report was produced by the Association for the Study and Development of Community (ASDC) for the Community Foundations/Intergroup Relations Program funded by the Ford and C.S. Mott Foundations. Taryn Higashi, Program Officer for the Ford Foundation and Lori Villarosa, Program Officer for the C.S. Mott Foundation provide guidance and leadership for the project. David Chavis of ASDC serves as the project director. Kien Lee coordinates the project. Kien Lee and Rebecca Buchanan assisted in writing the report. Principles of Intergroup Projects: A First Look page I CONTENTS Preface I 1 Introduction 1 2 Principles 2 3 Applying the Principles 8 4 References 10 Principles of Intergroup Projects: A First Look page II 1 INTRODUCTION This document describes ten principles for effective efforts to strengthen intergroup relations. Supporting evidence for the principles was derived from a review of the social science research literature on intergroup relations and management research on diversity training, review of reports by organizations and foundations supporting intergroup initiatives, and from the evaluation of the Initiative to Strengthen Neighborhood Intergroup Assets (sponsored by Eugene and Agnes E. Meyer Foundation and other foundations in the Washington, DC region). The literature reviewed was selected through electronic searches of databases such as PsychInfo. Representatives of foundations, other non-profit organizations, government agencies, and social science leaders were contacted to recommend any additional research on successful efforts. Recommendations were also sought from participants in relevant listserves such as the Society for Community Research and Action.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Categorization and Social Identification: Making Sense of Us and Them1 Nils Karl Reimer, Katharina Schmid, Miles Hewstone, & Ananthi Al Ramiah
    Self-Categorization and Social Identification: Making Sense of Us and Them1 Nils Karl Reimer, Katharina Schmid, Miles Hewstone, & Ananthi Al Ramiah Social identity processes are central to social psychological theory and research. Social identity plays an important role in many individual and social phenomena, including health and well-being (e.g., C. Haslam, Jetten, Cruwys, Dingle & Haslam, 2018), educational achievement (e.g., Steele, Spencer & Aronson, 2002), and collect- ive action and social change (e.g., van Zomeren, Postmes & Spears, 2008). In this chapter, we provide an overview of social psychological theories on social identity, including social identity theory (Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1979) and self-categorization theory (Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher & Wetherell, 1987). Prior to Tajfel’s and Turner’s seminal writings, two types of theory dominated ex- planations of intergroup relations and intergroup conflict. First, so-called ‘prejudice’ accounts proposed that intergroup conflict results from individual-level variation in a prejudiced personality (Son Hing & Zanna, 2010). Second, realistic group conflict theory (Campbell, 1965; Sherif, 1966) contended that negative intergroup relations stem from conflicting group goals and competition over resources or power (see Jackson, 1993, for a review). Compared to these explanations, social identity theory and self-categorization theory offered a unique and refined account of intergroup relations that remains important to this day. This chapter is dedicated to these interrelated approaches for understanding intergroup relations. Rather than present these theories chronologically—or focus on differences and similarities across the two theories—we take a more encompassing approach to what is often called the ‘social identity perspective’ (Abrams & Hogg, 2010).
    [Show full text]
  • Perspective—A New Look at Conflict Management in Work Groups
    This article was downloaded by: [128.91.90.188] On: 10 May 2017, At: 08:28 Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the Management Sciences (INFORMS) INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA Organization Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://pubsonline.informs.org Perspective—A New Look at Conflict Management in Work Groups Andrew M. Carton, Basima A. Tewfik To cite this article: Andrew M. Carton, Basima A. Tewfik (2016) Perspective—A New Look at Conflict Management in Work Groups. Organization Science 27(5):1125-1141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2016.1085 Full terms and conditions of use: http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used only for the purposes of research, teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher approval, unless otherwise noted. For more information, contact [email protected]. The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of, or references to, products or publications, or inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or support of claims made of that product, publication, or service. Copyright © 2016, INFORMS Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages INFORMS is the largest professional society in the world for professionals in the fields of operations research, management science, and analytics. For more information on INFORMS, its publications, membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org Organization Science Vol.
    [Show full text]
  • Prejudice and Intergroup Relations 14 Thomas Kessler and Amélie Mummendey
    9781405124003_4_014.qxd 10/31/07 3:14 PM Page 290 Prejudice and Intergroup Relations 14 Thomas Kessler and Amélie Mummendey KEY CONCEPTS authoritarian personality category salience common ingroup identity contact hypothesis decategorization ethnocentrism ingroup favouritism intergroup anxiety intergroup behaviour minimal group paradigm mutual distinctiveness positive and negative interdependence positive distinctiveness prejudice reverse discrimination social categorization social discrimination social dominance orientation superordinate goals tokenism 9781405124003_4_014.qxd 10/31/07 3:14 PM Page 291 CHAPTER OUTLINE This chapter introduces prejudice and social discrimination together with explanations of their causes. After a brief discussion of prejudice at the level of the individual, the chapter focuses mainly on prejudice and social discrimination as intergroup phenomena. We present classic and con- temporary intergroup explanations of intergroup conflict such as realistic conflict theory and social identity theory. Furthermore, we introduce basic psychological processes such as social categorization, identification with a social group, group evaluation and group-based emotions, and consider the intergroup motivations regulating intergroup behaviour. The remainder of the chapter presents various approaches explaining when and how prejudice and intergroup conflict can be reduced. Introduction As in most parts of the modern world, social and economic change in Europe is currently charac- terized by an internationalization of all domains of life. Consequently, countries are forced to deal with a permanent and increasing flow of migration of people with different cultural, religious or ethnic backgrounds. We find ourselves confronted with people who appear to be different from us. Obviously, societal changes such as migration are not the only way in which different others are introduced into our social contexts.
    [Show full text]
  • PSYCHOLOGICA 2010, 52 – Vol
    PSYCHOLOGICA 2010, 52 – Vol. II, 585-598 585 Sherif’s theoretical concepts and intergroup relations studies: notes for a positive interdependence1 Joaquim Pires Valentim2 Sherif’s theoretical concepts on intergroup relations are not merely a historical landmark for surpassing individualistic perspectives on that domain. They could also be, nowadays, useful theoretical tools for those who are interested in studying PSYCHOLOGICA, 2010, 52 – Vol. II societal functioning. The main point of this article is to argue that social psychology can benefit from the discussion and use of Sherif’s concepts, in order to provide a more comprehensive analysis for the study of intergroup relations than those deriving from a strict use of the categorization model. This is illustrated here with two examples of supra-national identities (European identity and the lusophone community) studies which could benefit from the use of Sherif’s ideas, especially the notion of superordinate goals. However, despite the pertinence and utility of Sherif’s concepts, several weaknesses of realistic conflict theory of intergroup behaviour should also be noted, namely, the tendency to assume that conflict is always negative. This undermining idea is discussed here based not only on the perspective of disadvantaged groups but also on positive evidence from the benefits of socio-cognitive conflict provided by two fields of European social psychological studies: social developmental psychology and research on minority influence. KEY-WORDS: realistic conflict theory; intergroup relations; social identity; national identity Before M. Sherif Several theoretical concepts started developing in social psychology a few years before the 2nd. World War, aiming to understand complex social phenomena related to intergroup relations, stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination.
    [Show full text]
  • From Swarms to Summer Camps : a Theoretical Deconstruction of Cohesion Among Groups of Latency Aged Boys
    Smith ScholarWorks Theses, Dissertations, and Projects 2009 From swarms to summer camps : a theoretical deconstruction of cohesion among groups of latency aged boys Prairie Youn-Yuen Chiu Smith College Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Chiu, Prairie Youn-Yuen, "From swarms to summer camps : a theoretical deconstruction of cohesion among groups of latency aged boys" (2009). Masters Thesis, Smith College, Northampton, MA. https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses/1183 This Masters Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations, and Projects by an authorized administrator of Smith ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Prairie Chiu From Swarms to Summer Camps: A Theoretical Deconstruction of Cohesion Among Groups of Latency Aged Boys ABSTRACT This thesis is an interdisciplinary theoretical deconstruction of cohesion among groups of latency aged boys. Process research on the development of cohesion among informally organized groups of latency aged boys is lacking. Also, much of cohesion research is adult-oriented, rather than child-oriented. This project seeks to elucidate some of the hows of cohesion among this population by applying a biological theory examining cohesion among aggregates of animals to a social psychological theory considering cohesion among latency aged boys. It is the author’s hope that such an approach will yield valuable insights into the mechanisms of cohesion and its facilitation, while increasing validity for both theories through a complimentary dialogue. Realistic conflict theory, developed by Muzafer Sherif on the basis of a 1961 large-scale, ambitious experiment involving two groups of latency aged boys, hypothesizes about intergroup relations.
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of the Relationship Between Groups - Stephen Worchel
    PSYCHOLOGY-Vol III-The Psychology of The Relationship Between Groups - Stephen Worchel THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN GROUPS Stephen Worchel University of Hawai’i at Hilo, USA Keywords: Intergroup contact, common enemy, superordinate goal, stereotype, prejudice, social identity, minimal group paradigm, self-categorization, intergroup perceptions Contents 1. Introduction 2. Expanding the Field: Types of Groups and Events Defining Relationships between Groups 2.1. The Social Identity Perspective 2.2. Self-Categorization 2.3. The Domains of Intergroup Relations 3. Grapes of Wrath: Behaviors That Comprise the Relation between Groups 3.1. Attraction and Discrimination 3.2. Proximity 3.3. Similarity 3.4. Ambivalence 4. In the Eye of the Beholder: Intergroup Relations and Intergroup Perceptions 4.1. They Are All the Same 4.2. Stereotypes: Cognitive Pictures of Groups 5. Improving the Relationship between Groups 6. Conclusion Acknowledgements Glossary Bibliography Biographical Sketch Summary The relationshipUNESCO between groups is often – characterized EOLSS by conflict and hostility. The basis for this conflict may be competition for limited resources and/or the desire for individuals to enhance the status of groups to which they belong. Social identity theory argues that partSAMPLE of each individual’s identity CHAPTERS is formed by the groups to which they belong. In order to have a positive social identity, individuals favor their own group and discriminate against out-groups (groups that they do not belong to). Individuals are typically more attracted to their own group than to out-groups. Similarity and proximity generally leads individuals to be more attracted to in-group members. However, similarity and proximity enhance conflict and discrimination against out-groups.
    [Show full text]