The Role of NKCC2A on the nervous system activity

A Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science

By Annu Kavya Bachelor of Pharmacy, Osmania University, 2013

2015 Wright State University

WRIGHT STATE UNIVERSITY GRADUATE SCHOOL

Date: May 8, 2015

I HEREBY RECOMMEND THAT THE THESIS PREPARED UNDER MY SUPERVISION BY Annu Kavya ENTITLED The Role of NKCC2A on the nervous system activity BE ACCEPTED IN PARTIAL FUFILLMENT OF THE REQUIRMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF Master of Science.

______James B. Lucot, Ph.D. Thesis Director

______

Jeffrey B. Travers, M.D., Ph.D. Chair, Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology Committee on Final Examination

______James B. Lucot, Ph.D.

______Mauricio Di Fulvio, Ph.D.

______Nadja Grobe, Ph.D.

______Robert E. W. Fyffe, Ph.D. Vice President for Research and Dean of the Graduate School

ABSTRACT

Annu Kavya, M.S. Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology, Wright State University, 2015. The Role of NKCC2A co-transporter in the nervous system activity.

The NKCC2A is a splice variant of the Na+K+2Cl- co-transporter 2 (Slc12a1), which is abundantly expressed in macula densa and in the apical membrane of the tubular cells in the kidney. Most of our knowledge regarding NKCC2 function is limited to the kidney, the organ where NKCC2 is abundantly expressed. However, recent studies have demonstrated that NKCC2 is also expressed in extra-renal tissues. This study was designed to determine if NKCC2A, a splice variant of NKCC2 has an impact on behavior and nervous system activity of mice. To these ends, we used mice (wild type, WT), lacking a single or both alleles of NKCC2A (heterozygous, HE and homozygous, HO, respectively) which were subjected to battery of standardized behavioral tests. NKCC2A-HO mice exhibited a significant gating impairment when compared to the WT or HE genotypes. Interestingly, NKCC2A-HO mice also exhibited a significantly lower immobility time compared to the WT and HE mice in the forced swim test (FST), a finding that could not be reproduced in the tail suspension test (TST). Nevertheless, the caudate nucleus of NKCC2A-HO mice exhibited low content of serotonin (5-HT) and dopamine metabolites, which might account for the reduced immobility time assayed by the FST. NKCC2A-HO mice had significantly lower levels of epinephrine (EPI) and norepinephrine (NE) in the adrenals, with the HE mice having intermediate levels suggesting a role of NKCC2A iii

in sympathetic activity, a conclusion suggested by the finding that these mice excreted low urinary EPI and NE. Together, the results of this study indicate that NKCC2A impacts behavior and neurochemistry in specific regions of the brain and the periphery.

iv

Table of Contents

1. Introduction ...... 1

1.1 SLC12A Gene family ...... 1

1.2 NKCC1 ...... 1

1.3 NKCC2 ...... 3

1.4 NKCC2 Isoforms ...... 3

1.5 NKCC2A ...... 4

1.6 Expression and role of NKCC2 in extra-renal tissues ...... 4

1.7 Behavior...... 6

Table 1 - Summary of behavioral tests...... 7

1.8 Neuronal systems ...... 9

1.9 Brain - monoamines...... 15

1.10 Role and Metabolism of DA and 5-HT ...... 15

1.11 Kidney Dopaminergic Systems ...... 16

1.12 NKCC2 and Dopamine in the Kidney ...... 17

2. Development of Hypothesis ...... 18

2.1 Specific Aims ...... 18

3. Experimental Design ...... 20

4. Materials and Methods ...... 21

4.1 Animals ...... 21

4.2 NKCC2A-HO mice generation ...... 21

4.3 NKCC2A-HO, HE and WT genetic analysis ...... 21

4.4 Behavioral Testing ...... 22

4.5 Brain dissection ...... 29

v

4.6 Neurochemical studies ...... 31

4.7 Spot Urine Collection ...... 33

4.8 Urine catecholamine extraction ...... 33

4.9 Determination of catecholamines by High Performance Liquid Chromatography (HPLC) ...... 34

4.10 Adrenal isolation and preparation of the tissue sample ...... 34

5. Data Analysis ...... 35

6. Results ...... 35

6.1 Behavioral tests...... 35

6.2 Neurochemical Analysis ...... 36

6.3 Urine ...... 37

6.4 Adrenals ...... 37

7. Results ...... 38

7.1 Behavior Results ...... 38

7.2 Neurochemistry Results ...... 40

7.3 Spot Urine Results ...... 42

7.4 Adrenal Results...... 43

Table 2 - Summary of Behavioral results...... 45

Table 3 - Summary of Neurochemistry results ...... 46

8. Discussion ...... 47

9. Acknowledgements ...... 53

10. References ...... 54

vi

List of Figures

Figure 1 - Diagrammatic representation of NKCC2 functional role in the kidney ...... 2

Figure 2 - Dopaminergic cell bodies (A8, A9, and A10) projects to the ventral tegmental area (VTA), substantia nigra (SNc) and the retrorubral field (RRF)...... 10

Figure 3 - Dopamine pathways in the brain ...... 13

Figure 4 - 5-HT pathways in the brain ...... 14

Figure 5 - DA metabolism in the dopaminergic nerve terminal and synapse...... 16

Figure 6 - Experimental design ...... 20

Figure 7 - Brain blocker – used to dissect the brain into 1 mm slabs...... 30

Figure 8 - Sagittal section of the mouse brain. The vertical line rostral to the intraural line indicates the slots where razor blades were placed for dissection...... 31

Figure 9 - Functional Observational Battery ...... 38

Figure 10 - Forced swim test ...... 38

Figure 11 - Tail suspension test ...... 39

Figure 12 - Open field test - resting time ...... 39

Figure 13 - Open field test - rearing ...... 40

Figure 14a - 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios in caudate ...... 40

Figure 14b - DOPAC/DA ratios in caudate ...... 41

Figure 14c - DOPAC/DA ratios in septum ...... 41

Figure 15a - Norepinephrine levels in spot urine ...... 42

Figure 15b - Epinephrine levels in spot urine ...... 42

Figure 15c - Dopamine levels in spot urine ...... 43

Figure 16a - Norepinephrine levels in adrenals ...... 43

Figure 16b - Epinephrine levels in adrenals ...... 44

vii

List of Tables

Table 1 - Summary of Behavioral tests ...... 7

Table 2 - Summary of Behavioral results ...... 45

Table 3 - Summary of Neurochemistry results ...... 46

viii

1. Introduction

1.1 SLC12A Gene family

The electroneutral inorganic cation-chloride co-transporters (CCCs) belong to the solute carrier 12 gene family (SLC12) which consist of nine homologous members:

SLC12A1-9. They play several important roles in human physiology, including cell volume regulation, hydro electrolytic balance, fluid secretion, neurotransmission and hormone secretion. This family of genes encode plasma membrane proteins that mediate the movement of inorganic sodium (Na+) and/or (K+) cations coupled to the movement of chloride (Cl-) anions. For instance, the SLC12A1 and

SLC12A2 genes encode Na-dependent K-Cl co-transporters widely known as

NKCCs, whereas SLC12A3 encode several Na-Cl co-transporters. These proteins upload Cl- into the cells by using the thermodynamic energy produced by the inward movement of Na+ (Gagnon and Delpire. 2013). All members of the SLC12A family are structurally closely related but differ in functional properties, transcriptional regulation and post-transcriptional modulation of activity which may contribute to their relative role in different functions (Hubner et al. 2001).

1.2 NKCC1

The SLC12A2 gene encodes the Na+/K+/2Cl- co-transporters-1 (NKCC1) which are ubiquitous and secretory in nature (Gamba et al. 1994). Multiple studies on NKCC1 knock out mice confirmed that it is not essential for the survival. Head bobbing and circling behavior is the most confounding phenotype of NKCC1 knockout mice

(Delpire et al. 1999). They had defects in hearing and balance suggesting both cochlear and vestibular defects (Gagnon and Delpire. 2013; Hubner et al. 2001).

NKCC1 was found to be involved in a variety of adult organ functions including the

1

central nervous system (CNS). Active Cl- accumulation via NKCC1 activated the

GABAA receptor mediated depolarization in immature (Clayton et al. 1998).

As the neurons mature, the intracellular Cl- concentration decreases which result in a shift towards GABAA–mediated inhibition. It was also shown that GABA was excitatory in arginine vasopressin (AVP) neurons which are located in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus that produce and release oxytocin and vasopressin into the blood circulation and so, this effect was increased by chronic hyperosmotic stress of salt loading. In this study, these effects were attributed to

NKCC1. However, NKCC2 may also be important in this regard. Indeed, it has been recently demonstrated that NKCC2 is expressed in AVP neurons and that its expression is increased in response to chronic dehydration (Konopacka et al. 2015).

Figure 1 - Diagrammatic representation of NKCC2 functional role in the kidney

2

1.3 NKCC2

The SLC12A1 gene encodes several Na+/K+/2Cl- co-transporters-2 (NKCC2) which are expressed in the apical membrane of the tubular cells of the thick ascending limb

(TAL) of Henle and in the macula densa (Gamba et al. 1994; Gagnon and Delpire.

2013; Hubner et al. 2001). NKCC2s are considered absorptive isoforms of the

NKCCs. They are responsible for 25-30% of body’s total renal salt retrieval and therefore, they participate in the concentration capacity of the kidney (Castagné et al.

2001). Indeed, approximately 80% of the NaCl uptake across the apical membrane of the TAL cells is mediated by NKCC2 (Castrop and Schnermann. 2008). NKCC2s as well as NKCC1 are sensitive to furosemide and bumetanide the most commonly prescribed diuretics. Inactivating mutations in the SLC12A1 gene cause Bartter’s syndrome type 1, an autosomal recessive salt-wasting disorder characterized by polyuria, renal tubular hypokalemic alkalosis, and hypercalciuria (Simon et al. 1996;

Gagnon and Delpire. 2013).

1.4 NKCC2 Isoforms

The SLC12A2 gene produce at least three variants named NKCC2A, NKCC2B and

NKCC2F. They are the result of alternative selection of three mutually exclusive exons (Gamba et al. 1994; Carota et al., 2010; Payne and Forbush, 1994; Vargas-

Poussou et al., 1998). These isoforms differ in their localization and transport characteristics along the TAL. The renal localization of the NKCC2 isoforms follows a general pattern despite a few interspecies differences. They are found in the apical membrane and in subapical vesicles along the TAL and in the macula densa segment.

NKCC2A is expressed in the outer stripe of the outer medulla and in the cortical portions of the TAL in rodents (Oppermann et al., 2006; Oppermann et al., 2007).

NKCC2A is the dominant isoform in the human cells and is found along the entire

3

kidney. The expression of different isoforms in humans is

NKCC2A>>NKCC2F>NKCC2B whereas in the mice it is

NKCC2F>>NKCC2A>NKCC2B (Castrop and Schnermann. 2008).

1.5 NKCC2A

NKCC2A exhibits a markedly higher Cl⁻ affinity than NKCC2F whereas NKCC2B has highest Cl⁻ affinity but lower transport capacity (Plata, Meade, Vazquez, Hebert, and Gamba, 2002). At low tubular Cl⁻ concentrations, Cl⁻ reabsorption was significantly reduced in NKCC2B-deficient mice when compared to WT mice, whereas no differences were observed for NKCC2A-HO mice (Oppermann et al.,

2007). However, the lack of NKCC2A in mice resulted in reduced Cl⁻ absorption at high Cl⁻ concentrations. Under low Cl⁻ load, TAL reabsorption becomes dependent on the activity of the high- Cl⁻-affinity NKCC2B isoform. Conversely, absence of

NKCC2A function becomes obvious in mice subjected to a high load of NaCl including that high tubular Cl⁻ concentrations modulate the transport activity

NKCC2A. This suggests that NKCC2A transport activity exceeds that of NKCC2B, which might be related to both the higher transport capacity of NKCC2A essentially and its higher expression levels (Castrop and Schnermann, 2008).

1.6 Expression and role of NKCC2 in extra-renal tissues

NKCC2 has been typically regarded as a kidney-specific transporter. However, recent findings suggest that it is also expressed in the endolymphatic sac (ES) epithelium (Nishimura, Kakigi, Takeda, Takeda, and Doi, 2009). There was also evidence for NKCC2 mRNA transcripts, and protein expression in the rat gastrointestinal tract (Xue et al. 2009). In fact, NKCC2 appears to be widely distributed and expressed in the enteric neurons in the myenteric plexus and it is

4

considered to play a role in the ion transport and neuronal excitability in the gut. Its expression in other nerve tissues has never been reported previously but there is a recent study showing the expression of NKCC2 in the brain hypothalamo- neurohypophyseal system (HNS), in particular the supraoptic and paraventricular nuclei (Konopacka et al. 2015). HNS is a specialized brain region which contain the neurons that produce peptide hormones, arginine vasopressin (AVP) and oxytocin

(OXT). AVP modulates the ability of the kidney to retain water free of solutes whereas OXT has an important role in sexual reproduction, social behavior, lactation or parturition (Konopacka et al. 2015).

The hypothalamus is a fundamental structure involved in the integration of the nervous and endocrine systems, and a key regulator in the whole body homeostasis including glucose homeostasis (Lam et al. 2009). Although the impact of NKCC2 in glucose homeostasis remains undefined, a potential role of NKCC2 in insulin secretion has been suggested. Indeed, NKCC1 and NKCC2 are co-expressed in insulin-secreting beta-cells (Alshahrani and Di Fulvio. 2012a; Alshahrani and Di

Fulvio. 2012b) and elimination of NKCC1 does not preclude secretion of the hormone in response to bumetanide.

If a mutation or deletion of a gene is thought to modify a physiological response or a single function, there could be other unexpected alterations in the whole animal.

However, this reasoning cannot exclude the possibility that the gene of interest may produce its proteins in previously undescribed locations exhibiting different functions.

This may be the case of mice lacking a single variant of NKCC2, where its “kidney- specific” location and very well-known function cannot represent its potential function in extra-renal tissues including brain, where our preliminary data suggested

NKCC2 expression (unpublished data) and a recent report confirmed (Konopacka et

5

al. 2015). The influence of a given gene on a specific behavior can be evaluated by conducting behavioral studies in mice lacking expression of such gene (Komada et al.

2008). NKCC1 knockout mice had behavioral alterations relating this effects to its presence in the central and peripheral neurons. So, a quick way to evaluate neural functions and activity alterations is behavior. Behavioral effects reflect the changes in nerve cell communication and integration as well as morphological alterations (Kulig et al. 1996). Our preliminary studies indicating presence of NKCC2 in some brain areas which lead us to investigate effects of deletion of NKCC2A in the mice.

1.7 Behavior

The work of the present thesis sprouted upon casual noticing of uncommon behavior in mice with functional deficiency in a single or in both alleles of the SLC12A1 gene.

As mentioned, this gene is responsible for one of the three main splice variants of the

Na+/K+/2Cl- co-transporter-2 (NKCC2), named NKCC2A. These observations were originally made in the laboratory of Dr. Mauricio Di Fulvio and included: unusual aggressiveness and defensive postures toward the corner of cages, absence of withdrawal reflexes, head bobbing, -like behavior or abnormal weight gain in some but not all of these mice. Hence, a battery of standardized behavioral tests was requested and thoroughly conducted by Teresa Garrett in the laboratory of Dr. James

Lucot in the Department of Pharmacology and Toxicology at Wright State University,

Boonshoft School of Medicine.

Several tests to analyze behavior in mice have been developed. Some of them are more specific than others and represent the functional interaction of particular regions within the brain. Several of these tests are summarized in Table 1.

6

Table 1 - Summary of Behavioral tests

Behavioral test Implicated brain regions Use

Functional operational CNS To assess and quantify the behavioral and battery (FOB) physiologic state of the mouse

Open field test (OFT) Amygdala To assess locomotor activity and anxiety in

Hippocampus rodents

Elevated plus maze (EPM) Amygdala To assess anxiety in rodents

Right prefrontal cortex

Y maze test Hippocampus To assess exploratory activity

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) Pons Measure of sensory motor gating

Prefrontal cortex

Forebrain

Rotarod Cerebellum To evaluate motor coordination and balance

Forced swim test (FST) Rostral anterior cingulate To evaluate anti-depressant-like effect

cortex (amygdala, nucleus

accumbens, hypothalamus

Tail suspension test (TST) Anterior cingulate cortex To evaluate anti-depressant-like effects

(prefrontal cortex, amygdala,

nucleus accumbens,

hypothalamus)

Visually observed deficits in the gait of NKCC2A-HO mice led us to design

experiments to evaluate the behavioral abnormalities of this animal model according

to the established tests (Table1). Their unusual overall aggressiveness (unpublished

observation) suggests there might be increased sympathetic activity which can be

7

evaluated by testing urinary catecholamines or by analyzing changes in urine catecholamine output rate.

The behavior of mice is usually assessed by subjecting them to a battery of standardized tests. Collectively known as the functional observational battery (FOB), this systematic observational method is applied to comprehensively assess and quantify the behavioral and physiologic state of the mouse. For instance, locomotor activity and thigmotactic (tolerance to less stressful environment) behavior of mice can be assessed by using the Open field test (OFT) (Castagné, Moser, Roux, and

Porsolt, 2001). It can also be used as a surrogate to assess anxiety in rodents. The elevated plus maze (EPM), however, exploits the natural aversion of mice for open and elevated areas, as well as on their normal exploratory behavior towards novel environments (Komada et al. 2008). The EPM is sensitive to anxiolytic effects in neurotoxic lesions of serotonergic neurons and other effects caused by anxiolytic drugs. The prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI) test is an operational measure of sensory gating, usually disrupted in schizophrenia and some other mental disorders and neurodegenerative diseases. The rotarod test is used to evaluate motor coordination and balance. It assesses the animal’s ability to maintain balance and walk on a rotating rod. The forced swim test (FST) is a rodent behavioral test used for evaluation of the antidepressant efficacy of new compounds and experimental manipulations that are aimed at generating or preventing depressive-like states

(Castagné et al. 2001). The tail suspension test (TST) is used to screen potential antidepressant drugs, and to assess other manipulations expected to affect depression- related behaviors (50 Can, Adem 2012).

Forced swimming or exposure to high intensity acoustic stimuli or any other stress activates the hypothalamo-pituitary axis and has an anxiogenic effect on behavior.

8

This activation is dependent on hypothalamic centers including the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei and the release of hormones which regulate the secretion of adrenocorticotropic hormone from the pituitary as well as sympatho-adrenal outflow.

Stressors that disturb physiological homeostasis activates pituitary-adrenal and sympatho-adrenal axis via extended amygdala inputs to the hypothalamus and brain stem, and exert anxiogenic-like behavioral consequences via actions on a variety of mid and forebrain nuclei. Therefore, changes in the mice behavior as analyzed by using FOB may indicate a disturbance in the physiological homeostasis and involves many brain areas such as the amygdala, hypothalamus, brain stem, mid and forebrain.

1.8 Neuronal systems

The neuronal systems involved in the mice behavior can be evaluated by measuring the neurotransmitter concentrations while correlating them to a particular behavior change. As many major theories suggest, neurobehavioral disorders may result from changes in the dopamine (DA) usage in specific brain areas such as frontal cortex, caudate, brain stem, nucleus accumbens, and hypothalamus (Howes and Kapur. 2009;

Mehler-Wex et al. 2006). Therefore these mouse brain areas are usually selected to analyze changes in DA or the turnover of the neurotransmitter serotonin/5- hydroxytryptamine (5-HT).

9

Figure 2 - Dopaminergic cell bodies (A8, A9, and A10) projects to the ventral tegmental area (VTA), substantia nigra (SNc) and the retrorubral field (RRF).

The dopaminergic and noradrenergic cell groups are designated with letter A, where

A8-A15 are dopaminergic groups. The serotonergic cell groups are designated with letter B and adrenergic with letter C. Fig 2 illustrates different dopaminergic cell groups such as olfactory bulb (OB) dentritic periglomerular (A16) neurons, the hypothalamic (Hyp; A12, A14 and A15) cell groups. Among this A12 provides

10

tuberoinfundibular and tuberohypophysial projections involved in the neuroendocrine regulation. A8-A10 are the mesodiencephalic tegmental cell groups comprising SNc

(A9 neurons), the VTA (A10 neurons) and the retrorubral fields (RRF; A8 neurons).

Brain areas receive DA neuronal projections from distinct dopamine pathways which have different physiological and pathophysiological significance e.g. mesolimbic- mesocortical and nigrostriatal projections (Fuxe et al. 1974; Inglis and Moghaddam.

1999; Oades and Halliday. 1987; Feldman. 1997). Figure 2 illustrates different DA bodies as mentioned above.

Ventral tegmental area (VTA; A10 cell groups) is the origin for the dopaminergic cell bodies of mesocortical system and mainly innervated frontal cortex of the brain (Fuxe et al. 1974; Oades and Halliday. 1987; Feldman. 1997). It is mainly involved in cognition and behavior. It contains neurons projecting from prefrontal cortex to the caudate and brain stem (Bjorklund et al. 1975; Bjorklund and Dunnett. 2007).

In the nigrostriatal pathway, as illustrated in figures 2 and 3, dopaminergic pathways run from the zona compacta of the substantia nigra (A9), and nigrostriatal fibers which produce very dense innervation project into the caudate nucleus, putamen and the globus pallidus (Fig 3). It is especially involved in the production of movement which is a part of basal ganglia motor loop. Loss of dopamine neurons in this pathway is implicated in several disorders like Parkinson’s disease, tardiv dyskinesia (Fahn and

Mayeux. 1980; Feldman. 1997).

In the mesolimbic pathway, the dopamine is transmitted from the VTA (A10) to the limbic system along nucleus accumbens. Mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine axons arising from midbrain nuclear groups retrorubral nucleus (A8), substantia nigra

(A9) and VTA (A10) forms a bundle lateral to hypothalamus and projects into

11

amygdala, hippocampus and some brain regions of the medial prefrontal cortex

(Feldman. 1997, Bjorklund and Dunnett. 2007). Different brain regions receiving dopaminergic neurons are involved in different functions.

The amygdala, a component of mesolimbic DA system, participates in conditioned fear (Fadok et al. 2009). Hippocampus is a primary component of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system involved in cognitive functions and dopaminergic projections to the hypothalamus regulates neuroendocrine functions (Bjorklund et al. 1975).

12

Figure 3 – Dopamine pathways in the brain

5-HT is also a monoamine neurotransmitter with an important role in the regulation of sleep, mood, appetite and cognitive functions. 5-HT pathways are named from B1-B9 where B1, B2, and B3 are called the caudal group and as illustrated in figure 3, caudal group gives rise to descending pathways into the dorsal, intermediate and ventral horns of the spinal cord. B4-B9 makes up the ascending 5-HT pathways where B5 13

and B6 pathways innervate the limbic structures, especially from raphe medianus, hypothalamus and the preoptic area. B7, B8 and B9 gives rise to lateral pathway which innervates cingulate cortex. There is another pathway that originates from B7-

B9 and primarily innervates the extrapyramidal motor pathways which arise from the caudate nucleus. The cerebellum is also innervated by 5-HT. Very fine 5-HT neurons were observed in septum, hippocampus and into the neo- and mesocortex (Figure 4:

Feldman. 1997).

Figure 4 - 5-HT pathways in the brain

14

1.9 Brain – monoamines

Behavioral deficits are usually correlated with neurochemical changes in specific regions of the brain. The monoamine neurotransmitters norepinephrine (NE), dopamine (DA), and serotonin (5-HT) are important to normal functioning of the brain and periphery. Current theories of the basis for major neurobehavioral disorders involve abnormalities in the use/metabolism of NE, 5-HT and DA in specific brain regions. To correlate potential changes in these monoamines with observed behavior, the concentration of NE, DA and their metabolites homovanillic acid (HVA) and dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) as well as, 5-HT and its metabolite 5- hydroxyindole acetic acid (5-HIAA) are usually measured in the frontal cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, hypothalamus, brainstem, accumbens, caudate and hypothalamus by using reverse phase high performance liquid chromatography.

1.10 Role and Metabolism of DA and 5-HT

DA is the precursor to norepinephrine (NE) and epinephrine (EPI). Apart from this, it has many other important functions in the brain.

Dopaminergic neurotransmission in different brain regions such as the frontal cortex

(FC), amygdala and the caudate plays an essential role in cognition, attention, motivation and reward, learning, creativity and motor activity. Catechol-O- methyltransferase (COMT) and monoamine oxidase (MAO) metabolizes DA to HVA and DOPAC. These are the most significant DA metabolites. The neurotransmitter rate of usage can be determined from the ratio of the metabolite to the parent compound e.g. HVA/DA or DOPAC/DA. HVA is the extra-neuronal (metabolized outside the neurons) metabolite whereas DOPAC is the intra-neuronal (metabolized in the neurons) metabolite of DA (Figure 5: (Feldman. 1997)).

15

Figure 5 - DA metabolism in the dopaminergic nerve terminal and synapse.

In addition to nervous tissue, adrenal glands produce a NE and E as sympathetic nervous system transmitters. Thus to further extend behavioral studies, it is convenient to determine the levels of these catecholamines in adrenal glands as well as in the kidney, the major organ involved in excretion.

1.11 Kidney Dopaminergic Systems

The kidney’s intrarenal dopaminergic system is distinct from other neural dopaminergic input. Dopamine acts as an intrarenal natriuretic hormone (Jose et al.

1992). Kidney dopamine levels can reach high nano molar concentrations whereas circulating dopamine levels are in the pico molar range (Zeng and Jose, 2011). The dopamine precursor L-DOPA (L-dihydroxyphenylalanine) is taken up from the circulation by the proximal tubule or through filtration via the glomerulus. It is then converted to dopamine by aromatic amino acid decarboxylase. This dopamine produced in the proximal tubule serves as an autocrine/paracrine agent that inhibits 16

the Na+/K+-ATPase activity in the medullary TAL and in the cortical collecting duct mediated by D1 receptors. Renal proximal tubules produce dopamine which serves as an intrarenal natriuretic factor by direct tubular action independent of hemodynamic mechanisms. Approximately 60% of the sodium excretion is mediated by D1 receptors which in turn is mediated by increased renal dopamine production and increased sensitivity of sodium transporters to dopaminergic inhibition.

1.12 NKCC2 and Dopamine in the Kidney

In the kidney, 25-30% of the body’s total renal salt retrieval is through the reabsorption of NaCl in the TAL, a phenomenon mediated by NKCC2. Dietary salt modulates intrarenal dopamine production (Nishimura et al., 2009). In the mammalian kidney, dopamine receptor activation due to increased dopamine release decreases

NaCl and water reabsorption through inhibition of specific tubule transporter activity along the nephron including the Na+/K+-ATPase in the proximal tubule, and NKCC2 in the TAL (Corrigenda for vol. 283, p. F221.2003; Bacic et al., 2005; Grider, 2003).

Therefore, dopamine receptor activation indirectly inhibits Na/K-ATPase and NKCC2 function. This is due to increased renal dopamine release and increased sensitivity of sodium transporters to dopaminergic inhibition. Therefore, absence of NKCC2A in mice might impact dopamine utilization and this can be tested by analyzing dopamine concentrations in the urine samples.

In the TAL, NaCl reabsorption is stimulated by vasopressin, parathyroid, glucagon, epinephrine and norepinephrine. This mechanism is mediated, either directly or indirectly, by altering NKCC2 activity. Sodium transport via NKCC2A or NKCC2B at the macula densa initiates the tuberoglomerular feedback mechanism. Dopamine in the kidney is known to stimulate NKCC2 activity, but controversially, dopamine

17

inhibits the tubuloglomerular pathway. Interestingly NKCC2A-HO mice exhibited decreased maximum tubuloglomerular feedback responses (Oppermann et al. 2007).

Therefore, this study is designed to determine the effect of NKCC2A on the E, NE and DA levels in the urine a reflex of the basal sympathetic activity of the animal.

2. Development of Hypothesis

The hypothesis is that elimination of NKCC2A in vivo alters behavior and in neurochemical usage in the central and peripheral nervous systems.

NKCC2A has different level of expression in WT, HE and HO mice tissues. HE mice have NKCC2A expression intermediate to HO and WT. WT had much higher expression than HE whereas the HO did not have any expression (unpublished data).

Therefore, to find the effect of NKCC2A, we have included HE mice group along with WT and HO in our studies.

2.1 Specific Aims

Aim 1 -

To test the hypothesis that NKCC2A-HO mice have neurobehavioral deficits.

NKCC2A-HO mice were used as a model to evaluate the behavioral phenotypes.

NKCC2A-HO, HE and WT mice were screened for gross functional deficits, locomotion, psychosis, working memory, anxiety, and depression.

Aim 2 -

To test the hypothesis that NKCC2A-HO mice have altered DA, 5-HT metabolism and changes is NE levels in selective brain regions.

18

Brain regions such as frontal cortex, amygdala, hippocampus, brainstem, accumbens, caudate and hypothalamus were evaluated for DA, 5-HT usage. Reverse-phase high performance liquid chromatography (RP-HPLC) was used to measure concentrations of NE, DA and its metabolites DOPAC and HVA, 5-HT and its metabolite 5-HIAA in selected brain regions with DA and 5-HT neuronal projections.

Aim 3 -

To evaluate the impact of NKCC2A-HO on kidney DA levels as well as on basal sympathetic activity by measuring urinary DA, NE and EPI.

Aim 4 -

To test the hypothesis that NKCC2A-HO might show changes in total NE and EPI in the adrenal glands. HPLC was used to measure them in the adrenal glands.

19

3. Experimental Design

Figure 6 - Experimental design

20

4. Materials and Methods

4.1 Animals

Mice subjected to behavioral studies were generated from mice lacking a single functional allele of NKCC2A (heterozygous) kindly provided by Dr. Hayo Castrop

(University of Regensburg) to Dr. Di Fulvio. Mice were used according to approved animal protocols and the material transfer agreement signed by both parties. Mice were used at the age of 8 weeks and both female and male mice were used in all experiments.

4.2 NKCC2A-HO mice generation

NKCC2A-HO mice were generated by Dr. Hayo Castrop at the University of

Regensburg and all the procedures were approved by the Animal Use and Care

Committee of the National Institute of and Digestive and Kidney Diseases and by the Animal Care Committee of the University of Regensburg. NKCC2A-HO mice were generated according to a similar strategy which was used to generate

NKCC2B-HO mice earlier (Oppermann et al. 2006). They were generated by altering the exon 4A of the SLC12A1 gene by the introduction of in-frame stop codons which result in the premature termination of the translation. Homologous arms were generated by long-distance PCR (Roche, Indianapolis, IN). Progeny lacking the neo gene was intercrossed to generate NKCC2A-HO, HE and WT littermates (Oppermann et al. 2007).

4.3 NKCC2A-HO, HE and WT genetic analysis

To identify WT, HE or HO mice, we took advantage of the genetic strategy followed to create these mice to design primer sets for genotyping. We used the polymerase

21

chain reaction (PCR) to determine them. The genotyping primers recognizing flanking intronic sequences 5’ and 3’ exon A were: 5’-TGC AGG AGT GGG TAG

TCC AA-3’ AND 5’-CCG TAG CAT GTG ACG TGG AT-3’. This primer set amplifies a single DNA fragment of 206 bp or 302 bp from genomic DNA obtained from WT or HO mice, respectively, to the two fragments simultaneously in the case of HE mice. The procedure for genotyping mice was developed and set up in our laboratory by Victor Otano-Rivera (Undergraduate STREAMS student) and Amma

Boayke (Undergraduate HONORS student) and was based on the original protocol written by Glenn Travis in Laboratory of Molecular Systematics, Smithsonian

Institution. Washington, DC 20560, and the protocols of Walsh and collaborators and the one described by Morin and Woodruff in Paternity exclusion using multiple hypervariable microsatellite loci amplified from nuclear DNA of hair cells. Pages 63-

81 of (Martin R.D. (Zürich) Dixson A.F. (Franceville) Wickings E.J. (Franceville).

1992). When genotypes were not clearly defined by the PCR, we troubleshooted this by blindly testing the animals and then regenotyping them from the tissues. All animals were fed on standard chow diet and water and were housed under 12h light:

12h darkness cycles.

4.4 Behavioral Testing a. General Handling Procedures:

Animal handling was performed first. The animals were removed from home cages, placed in clean clothes and handled approximately 30seconds each. All animals were handled before individual tests in order to minimize handling-related stress. All animals were acclimatized for at least 30minutes in the testing room before each test.

The behavioral tests were spaced by a week to minimize interference by previous

22

procedures. Tests were administered in ascending order of novelty in order to reduce carry over effects. All observational scoring was checked for inter-rater reliability.

The behavioral studies are described below in the order in which they were conducted. b. Functional Observational Battery:

The functional observational battery is a noninvasive method used to detect gross functional deficits prior to more specific neurobehavioral evaluation. It is used to comprehensively assess and quantify the behavioral and physiological state of the mouse. Due to the visually observed deficits in ambulation, a functional observational battery was developed from a published industry version (Irwin, 1968) for use with this group of animals. The body position, locomotor activity, bizarre behavior, tremors, finger-withdrawal, touch escape, ataxic gait, and hypotonic gait are observed and scored in these tests (Irwin. 1968).

First the animals were subjected to the least provoking stimuli which involved an initial phase of undisturbed observation and a later manipulative phase. The assessment in this phase and the undisturbed behavior i.e., body position, locomotor activity, bizarre behavior, exophthalmos, respiration, tremors, twitches and convulsions were assessed. The animals were then transferred and were briskly dropped onto the floor of the viewing arena for testing the transfer arousal and spatial locomotion. Observations were then made if tremors were present. Observations for gait and limb rotation were also made. The categories measured included general arousal, gait, reflexes, and motor behavior. Scoring of 0 to 8 range was used throughout FOB, with values 0, 2, 4, 6, and 8 which represents none, slight, moderate,

23

marked and extreme magnitudes of behavior. The scores were summed for each category and then for over all categories (Irwin, 1968). c. Open field test (OFT):

The locomotor activity and thigmotactic behavior can be evaluated using an open field chamber in which infrared photo-beams (Hamilton Kinder, Motor Monitor

Version 3.11; Poway, CA, USA) are employed to measure locomotor activity in real time. The open field arena has dimensions of 40 X 40 cm and is divided into central

(20X20 cm) and peripheral (10 cm wide) zones. The IR beams are interrupted by movements and sorted into different measures based on algorithms in the Motor

Monitor Software (Lad et al. 2010; Tatem et al. 2014). Each mouse was placed in the center of the open field arena under the red light and allowed to explore it for 10 minutes. Measurements such as horizontal beam interruptions, vertical beam interruptions (rearing), distance traveled, time ambulating, spatial distribution of behaviors, and fine movements (repeated activation of one horizontal beam only) were measured. If mice spend more time in the area close to the wall then it is an indicator of increased stress level, whereas if they explore the center of the arena then it indicates that they can tolerate a mildly stressful environment. The open field arena was cleaned with 70% ethanol solution and let dry after testing each mouse. d. Elevated Plus Maze (EPM):

This is a behavioral paradigm that takes advantage of the conflict behavior of rodents between exploration of a novel area and aversion to open and elevated spaces (Hata et al. 2001). The expression of scores as percentage of time and distance in the open arms allows for the correction of overall changes due to exploration of the maze, reducing the activity-induced artifacts (Hogg. 1996).

24

The device has the shape of a cross with each arm being of 5 X 35 cm. Two arms are open and two of them have walls of 15 cm high, with the arms connected by a 5X5 cm central square. The arms are 76 cm above the floor.

Mice were tested on the plus maze in a room with low, indirect incandescent red lighting and very low noise levels. The mouse was placed at the center of the maze with head facing the open arm and allowed to explore for 5 minutes. The number of entries into open and closed arms, as well as the time spent and distance travelled in open and closed arms was recorded with the help of an automated EPM (Hamilton

Kinder, Version 3.11; Poway, CA). Animals sensitive to stressful environments avoid the open arms of the maze. The mazes are cleaned with 70% ethanol and permitted to dry between sessions. Automated software (Kinder Scientific Motor Monitor

Software Package, Built 08356-14) kept a record of the number of entries, distance travelled (inches), and time spent (seconds) in the closed and open arms. Entry was determined when all the four paws of the mouse were on the arm. e. Y maze activity:

The Y-maze measures a mouse’s working memory status and exploits the innate tendency to explore novel areas. The apparatus used for this test is an acrylic maze with 3 arms at 120 degrees to each other, each arm 3.5 cm wide and 20 cm long. The acclimatization period to the room was 1 hour. Then the animal is placed in the center of the apparatus. The sequence of arm entries (entry defined as all four paws within the arm), alternation sequence (entering three different arms in succession e.g. ABC or BCA) are video recorded for 8 minutes for each sequence. The percentage of alternation was determined by dividing the total number of alterations by the total number of choices minus 2, multiplied by 100.

25

f. Pre-pulse Inhibition (PPI):

This is a test to evaluate psychosis-like behavior. Mice were tested in automated startle chambers (SM100 Startle Monitor System Version 6.12; Hamilton Kinder,

Poway, CA, USA) for PPI. They were calibrated prior to the day of testing. Mice were placed in the startle chamber and allowed to acclimate for 5 min. A background noise of 60 dB was on in between trials. The animals were then presented with 30 trials randomly mixed of startle stimuli (loud white noise) and pre-pulse stimuli

(lower dB white noise preceded by loud). The startle stimuli consist of a 20 ms white noise stimulus presented at 2 different intensities (85 or 100 dB). The pre pulse stimulus consists of 20ms, 70 dB white noise pulse, presented 100ms prior to the startle (85 or 100 dB) stimulus (70dB + 100 ms + 85 dB and 70 dB + 100 dB). The inter trial intervals range from 9-16 s. The startle response was recorded on a pressure plate (Sharma, Elased, and Lucot, 2012). g. Rotarod test:

The rotarod test is used to assess motor coordination by measuring the latency to fall off the rotating drum. The rod is 3cm in diameter and designed so that the mouse only drops 25cm onto a plate that stops a timer. The animals were moved to the experiment room from their home cages 30 minutes prior to testing. There were two protocols followed, one that used a constant speed (25rpm) for 2min and a second that used accelerating speeds for 5min (up to 30rpm). Both tests were conducted with all subjects. Learning sessions were conducted for 3 days in which the animals were placed on a rotarod (24rpm) for a maximum of 60s and the latency to fall off was recorded. The animals were then returned to their home cage for 5-10 minutes and then the session was repeated for 3 times each day of training. For the learning sessions (3 consecutive days): Each mouse was placed on a rotating rod (24 rpm) for a

26

maximum of 60s and the latency to fall off was recorded. In test session, two trails were conducted at a fixed speed with a maximum time of 60s, and an hour later the animal was tested on an accelerating rod. The animal was placed on a rotating rod and then the speed was increased every minute up to 44rpm for a maximum of 5 minutes.

The latency was recorded when the animal falls. The same test was performed again

10 minutes later. This procedure was adapted from Current Protocols in

Neuroscience, 2001 and Pallier, Drew, and Morton, 2008. h. Forced Swim Test:

The Forced Swim Test (FST) is used to evaluate depression-like behavior. In this test, mice were placed in a transparent glass cylinder (diameter 10 cm, height 35 cm) which was filled up to 30 cm with water (23-25°C). The mice were tested separately in different cylinders with an opaque white plexi glass separating them so that the mice cannot see each other during the test. This testing is video recorded and scored for durations of immobility. After the test the mice were dried with a towel and returned to their home cage and placed on a thermal blanket heated to 37C. A mouse was considered as immobile when floating motionless or making only those movements necessary to keep its head above the water (Castagné et al., 2001; Sharma et al., 2010).

Scoring

A new time-scoring technique was used to score several behaviors during a single viewing session which has been shown to be both reliable and valid for detecting antidepressant effects of both desipramine and fluoxetine in the FST (Detke et al.,

1995). The scoring had three different categories scored based on definitions,

“floating in the water without struggling and making only those movements necessary

27

to keep the head above water” which is considered immobility; “making active swimming motions, more than necessary to merely maintain the head above water”

(i.e., moving around in the cylinder) called swimming; “making active movements with forepaws in and out of the water, usually directed against the walls” which is climbing; and “having entire body submerged” called diving. In these tests, diving occurred and was not reliably altered by the compounds tested, data for diving are not reported. All of the behavior scoring was done by a single rater, who was unaware of treatment condition. This new procedure to score many active behaviors in the FST was used to re-evaluate the effects of different types of anti-depressant drugs (Detke,

Rickels, and Lucki, 1995). Selective norepinephrine (NE) uptake inhibitors, such as desipramine and maprotiline, reduced immobility and selectively increased climbing without affecting swimming. Previously effects of serotonin (5 hydroxytryptamine; 5-

HT) uptake inhibitors in the FST were considered as false negatives in this test, but this scoring system revealed the effects of serotonin (5 hydroxytryptamine; 5-HT) uptake inhibitors in the FST, such as fluoxetine, sertraline, and paroxetine also reduced immobility but increased swimming instead of climbing. Thus, an analysis of behavioral patterns in the FST distinguished drugs that have a common therapeutic outcome and those that act on distinct neurotransmitter systems. To avoid these false- positives, tail suspension test (TST) can be used to test the depressive-like behavior which has different false positives. A literature search revealed monoaminergics (e.g., amphetamine, paroxetime)-induced decreases in immobility time which might be false positives in the forced swim test (Zhao et al. 2008). i. Tail Suspension Test:

Mice were suspended by their tails for 6 minutes (50 Can, Adem 2012). The subject’s behavior was recorded with a video camera and scored afterwards. A suspension bar

28

(1 cm. height, I cm. width, 60 cm. length) and tape (Time Med Labelling Systems,

Inc., Burr Ridge IL, 1.9 cm width were used. The mice were suspended by their tails by attaching them to the suspension bar with tape in a way that they cannot escape or hold on to nearby surfaces. The distance between mouse nose and floor was approximately 20-25 cm. The mice were separated from each other using opaque plastic to prevent interaction or observance between each other. A polycarbonate tray was placed at the bottom of each mouse suspension to collect feces and urine.

Some strains of mice (mainly C57s) have a behavior called tail climbing and can thus hold on to their tails during the test and negate it. We suspect that this strain of mouse will have that response because their background strain is C57Bl/6. In order to prevent this from happening a clear hollow cylinder approximately 4 cm long, 1.6 cm outside diameter, 1.3 cm inside diameter, and weighing 1.5 grams made from polycarbonate tubing was placed around the tails of the mice to prevent tail climbing behavior of the mice.

The animals were brought to the testing room an hour before testing. The climb stoppers were placed around the tail of the mouse and the tape was applied at the very end of the tail leaving 2-3 mm outside the tape. The mouse was suspended by placing the free end of the tape on the suspension bar. At the end of 6 minutes the animals were returned to their cages and the tape was removed by gently pulling it off.

4.5 Brain dissection

Animals were sacrificed by decapitation. The brains were removed and the frontal cortex was hand dissected. The remainder of the brain was placed in a brain matrix

(Ted Pella, Inc) and sliced into 1mm or 2 mm slabs rostral to the intraneural line,

(Caudate : 5-4, Amygdala/Hypothalamus: 3-2, Hippocampus: 2-1, Brain stem1: 1-0,

29

Brain stem2: 0- -1) which were then stored at -80C on plastic squares until further dissection. Amygdala and the caudate nucleus were dissected using landmarks in The

Mouse Brain Stereotaxic Coordinates (Paxinos and Franklin, 2008) as a guide.

Accumbens was taken using a tissue punch (-20̊ C ice blocks) of 1mm thickness.

Other sections were dissected using landmarks in The Mouse Brain Stereotaxic

Coordinates (2008) as a guide.

Figure 7 - Brain blocker – used to dissect the brain into 1 mm slabs.

30

Figure 8 - Sagittal section of the mouse brain. The vertical line rostral to the intraural line indicates the slots where razor blades were placed for dissection.

4.6 Neurochemical studies a. Preparation of tissue samples

All the brain tissues were homogenized in 0.2N HCl using a homogenizer followed by centrifugation. The supernatants were collected into 2 aliquots, which were stored at -

80ºC until they were analyzed by HPLC.

31

b. Determination of catecholamines by High Performance Liquid

Chromatography (HPLC)

HPLC is high performance liquid chromatography which is used to separate, identify and quantify the components in a mixture. Its function relies on a high-pressure pump which passes a pressurized liquid solvent containing the sample through a short, narrow column which is filled with a solid adsorbent material. Each component of the sample interacts differently with the adsorbent material causing different retention times of each component which helps in separation as they flow through the column.

The electrochemical detector attached to the machine generates a signal proportional to the amount of the sample component eluting from the column. Here we used BAS

LC-4B and BAS LC-4C detector with a potential of 0.75V and maintained across a glossy carbon working electrode with a positive radial flow. This electrochemical

(amperometric) detector is attached to the system to identify the separated components of the sample as they get eluted from the column. In this amperometric cell the column effluent passes electrodes to which a voltage is applied. As the molecules in the column effluent pass the electrodes they become oxidized when the potential is sufficiently large. The amount of current drawn to drive the electrochemical reaction is quantified and concentration determined by reference to an external standard.

The supernatant (sample) in the aliquot is injected directly into the chromatographic system. Chromatographic separation was conducted using a BAS 100 X 3 mm C18 column (BAS model MF-8954), 80 X 4.6mm C18 column (ESA model 68-0100) optimized to 0.75V. The mobile phase consisted of soap (1-octanesulfonic acid), acetonitrile and dimethyl acetamide. The flow rate was kept at 0.3ml/min.

32

c. Brain monoamine analysis

To identify monoamines, a series of standard solutions containing known amounts of monoamines includingnorepinephrine (NE), 3,4 - dihydroxyphenylacetic acid

(DOPAC), 5-Hydroxy Indole acetic acid (5-HIAA), Homovanillic acid (HVA),

Dopamine (DA) and serotonin (5-HT) were injected into the chromatography system before and after each sample analysis. The concentrations of mono amines in the samples were determined by comparing the peak heights from the samples relative to that of the standards. The concentrations of NE, DA, HVA, DOPAC, 5-HIAA and 5-

HT were expressed as ng/mg of wet tissue. The ratio between the metabolite and the parent compounds (e.g. HVA/DA, DOPAC/DA, 5-HIAA/5-HT) was used as a surrogate index of neurotransmitter usage.

4.7 Spot Urine Collection

Urine was collected from each animal by gently scruffing the animal and placed it on clean surface until it urinated. The urine was collected using a pipette and placed in a micro tube which was placed in ice until storage at -80 C until analysis. Subjects that did not urinate after 30s were placed in a clean empty cage until they did.

4.8 Urine catecholamine extraction

Catecholamines in spotted urine were extracted over neutral alumina, activity grade 1, from Sigma Chemical Co. product A-9003 supplemented with an internal standard consisting of 2,3-dihydroxybenzoic acid (DHBA).This preliminary extraction over alumina is necessary for down-stream electrochemical detection of urine catecholamines. This procedure removes electroactive species in the urine that cannot be separated by HPLC (Smith et al. 2013). Extraction was done as follows: alumina was placed in 1.5mL tubes and supplemented with HCl containing sodium

33

metabisulfite. To assay samples spot urine, DHBA internal standard and tris buffer were added. The pH was then adjusted to 8.5±0.05 by adding Tris buffer. Then the tube was vortexed, centrifuged and then the supernatants were removed and diluted with deionized water. This was done for 2 times. Then the supernatants were removed and further diluted with HClO4 containing sodium metabisulfite. The tube was then vortexed; centrifuged and the supernatants were aliquoted into different 0.5 mL tubes and stored at -80C̊ until analysis (Smith et al. 2013).

4.9 Determination of catecholamines by High Performance Liquid

Chromatography (HPLC)

To determine catecholamines in these processed samples, 20µL of them were directly injected into the chromatographic system. Chromatographic separation was conducted using 80 X 4.6mm C18 column (ESA model 68-0100) optimized to 0.55V. The mobile phase consisted of soap (1-octanesulfonic acid), methanol and acetonitrile.

The flow rate was kept at 0.3ml/min.

4.10 Adrenal isolation and preparation of the tissue sample

Animals were sacrificed by decapitation and then the adrenal glands were dissected, weighed onto empty 1.5ml tubes and labelled with the animal identification number.

A single weighed adrenal gland was submerged in PBS buffer and DHBA stock (as an internal standard) and homogenized in the buffer using a homogenizer keeping the samples on ice in between homogenizing cycles. The homogenates were added to a tube containing HCLO4 to be filtered and centrifuged at. Supernatants were then collected into 3 aliquots and stored at -80C until HPLC analysis.

34

5. Data Analysis

A DataQ device attached to the detector records the waveforms and using Windaq software.

The data were analyzed by using STATISTICA data analysis software (Stat soft,

Tulsa, OK). The results are presented as group means±S.E.M. and analyzed using

STATISTICA data analysis software. Data were analyzed by 1-way ANOVA test. It was used to evaluate the overall significance among groups. Tukey-HSD post hoc test was used to identify individual group differences. We also used Kruskal Wallis one way analysis of variance in the FOB and rotarod test.

6. Results

6.1 Behavioral tests a. Functional Observational Battery

The NKCC2A-HO had significant differences in its walking behavior which is gait.

The HO mice group had higher scores compared to the HE and WT mice. As shown in Fig.9, there were significant differences in the gait category (Fig 9) with NKCC2A-

HO having higher gait score compared to NKCC2A-HE and WT [factor ‘genotype’ F

(2, 28) = 3.524, p < 0.05 vs WT and HE]. b. Forced swim test

As shown in Fig.10, NKCC2A-HO mice were immobile for significantly less time compared to WT and HE mice during the 6 minute forced swim test [factor

‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 31.044, p < 0.001 vs WT and HE].

35

c. Tail suspension test

As shown in Fig.11 there were no significant differences between mice of the three genotypes during the 6 minute lasting tail suspension test [factor ‘genotype’, F (2, 22)

= 0.59, p >0.05 vs WT and HE]. d. Peripheral Rest time

As shown in Fig.12, during the 10 min of the test session in the open field NKCC2A-

HEmice spent significantly more time in the periphery than in the central zone [factor

‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 5.7466, p < 0.05 vs WT and HO]. e. Rearing

Fig.13 shows a significant differences between NKCC2A-WT, NKCC2A-HE and

NKCC2A-HO mice in the rearing phenomena in the open field test [factor ‘genotype’

F (2, 28) = 4.03, p < 0.05].

6.2 Neurochemical Analysis a. The caudate of NKCC-HO mice exhibit low 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios

The 5-HIAA/5-HT ratio in the caudate of NKCC2A-HO was significantly low (Fig.

14a, p<0.05) when compared to NKCC2A WT (Fig. 14a) [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28)

= 4.514, p < 0.05 vs WT]. b. The caudate of NKCC2A-HO mice exhibit low DOPAC/DA ratios

The DOPAC/DA ratio in the caudate of NKCC2A-HO was significantly low (Fig.

14b, p<0.05) when compared to NKCC2A-HE and WT (Fig. 14b) [factor ‘genotype’

F (2, 28) = 5.04, p < 0.05 vs WT and HE].

36

c. The septum of NKCC2A-HE mice exhibit high DOPAC/DA ratios

The DOPAC/DA ratio in the septum of NKCC2A-HE mice was significantly higher than that of NKCC2A-HO and WT mice (Fig. 14c, p<0.05). However, there were no significant differences in individual neurotransmitter levels [factor ‘genotype’ F (2,

28) = 4.07, p < 0.05 vs WT and HO].

6.3 Urine

As shown in Fig 15a, 15b, 15c, there were no significant differences in urine NE, EPI and DA levels between the mice of three genotypes.

6.4 Adrenals

There was a significant decrease in the NE (Fig 16a) levels in NKCC2A-HO mice when compared to NKCC2A-HE and WT [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 23) = 4.12, p <

0.05 vs WT].

There was a significant decrease in the EPI (Fig. 16b) levels in NKCC2A-HO mice when compared to NKCC2A-HE and WT [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 23) = 4.744, p <

0.05 vs WT].

37

7. Results

7.1 Behavior Results

Figure 9 - Functional Observational Battery

Sum of the arousal category and the gait categories in WT, HE and HO. The HO group had significantly higher gait scores [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 3.524, p < 0.05 vs WT and HE].

Figure 10 - Forced swim test

Immobility time. Immobility of the HO group was lower compared to the WT and HE [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 31.044, p < 0.001 vs WT and HE].

38

Figure 11 - Tail suspension test

Immobility time. No significant changes in the Immobility of the HO group compared to the WT and HE [factor ‘genotype’, F (2, 22) = 0.59, p >0.05 vs WT and HE].

Figure 12 - Open field test – Peripheral resting time

Time (sec) spent resting in the periphery during a 10 min test. HE mice were different from WT and HO mice. No other differences were significant [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 5.7466, p < 0.05 vs WT and HO].

39

Figure 13 - Open field test - Rearing

Total number of rearing events in the ten min test. The WT were lower than either HE or HO [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 4.03, p < 0.05].

7.2 Neurochemistry Results

Figure 14a - 5-HIAA/5-HT ratios in caudate

5-HIAA/5-HT ratios observed in the caudate area of WT, HE and HO mice [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 4.514, p < 0.05 – significant difference from WT].

40

Figure 14b - DOPAC/DA ratios in caudate

DOPAC/DA ratios in caudate area of WT, HE and HO mice [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 5.04, p < 0.05 – significant difference from WT and HE].

Figure 14c - DOPAC/DA ratios in septum

DA usage levels in Septum area of WT, HE and HO mice [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 28) = 4.07, p < 0.05 – significant difference from WT and HO].

41

7.3 Spot Urine Results

Figure 15a - Norepinephrine levels in spot urine

Figure 15b - Epinephrine levels in spot urine

42

Figure 15c - Dopamine levels in spot urine

7.4 Adrenal Results

Figure 16a - Norepinephrine levels in adrenals

Norepinephrine levels observed in adrenals of WT, HE and HO mice [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 23) = 4.12, p < 0.05 – significant difference from WT].

43

Figure 16b - Epinephrine levels in adrenals

Epinephrine levels observed in adrenals of WT, HE and HO mice [factor ‘genotype’ F (2, 23) = 4.744, p < 0.05 – significant difference from WT].

44

Behavioral test Result

Functional operational battery (FOB) Increased gait in NKCC2-HO compared to the WT and HE

Open field test (OFT) Higher rearing in NKCC2A-HO mice compared to the WT and

HE

Elevated plus maze (EPM) No differences in anxiety across the three groups

Y maze test No significant spontaneous alterations across the 3 groups

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) No significant disruption in PPI

Rotarod No differences in motor coordination and balance among the 3

groups

Forced swim test (FST) Increased immobility time of the NKCC2A-HO mice compared to

the WT and HE

Tail suspension test (TST) No significant change in immobility time of the NKCC2A-HO

mice compared to the WT and HE

Table 2 - Summary of Behavioral results

Accumb Amygdala Brain stem Caudate Hypothalam Septum

ens us

NE Higher in indeterminate Higher in HE Higher in HE HE DOPAC Lower Lower in Lower in Higher in HE Higher in HE in HO HO HO Lower in 5-HIAA HO Higher in HE Higher in HE HVA Lower Higher in HVA Higher in

in HO and HO HE DA indeterm

inate 5-HT Higher in Lower in HO Higher in HE HE

45

DOPAC/DA Lower in Higher in

HO HE HVA/DA

5-HIAA/5-HT Lower in

HO

Table 3 - Summary of Neurochemistry results

46

8. Discussion

We performed a preliminary characterization of the behavioral and neurochemical phenotypes of mice lacking a single (HE) or both (HO) alleles of NKCC2A and compared the results against WT. The HE tissues exhibits intermediate level of

NKCC2A expression, whereas HO does not show any expression with high expression levels in the WT indicating all the three groups have different levels of

NKCC2A expression and its effect might be different across the groups. Based on anecdotal behavior observations noted in NKCC2A-HO mice, we evaluated them by using a functional observational battery followed by specific behavioral tests including open field test, y maze activity, elevated plus maze, pre-pulse inhibition, rotarod test, forced swim test, and tail suspension test.

There were few visually observed deficits in ambulation of the NKCC2A- HO mice.

A functional observational battery developed from a published industry version

(Irwin, 1968) was used with this group of animals which measured the categories of general arousal, gait, reflexes, and motor behavior. The pattern of movement of the limbs during locomotion is gait and abnormal gait is observed in several neurological and musculoskeletal disorders. There were significant differences in the gait category with the NKCC2A-HO group having higher impairment but no significant differences were observed in rotarod test which suggest that NKCC2A might be involved in areas affecting gait rather than balance. Abnormality in gait generally indicate some gross functional deficits in the CNS which were later assessed using specific behavioral tests. Therefore, this suggests that the NKCC2 might affect the CNS in areas involved in locomotor activity like walking.

47

In FST, which is used to detect the anti-depressant like effect, the NKCC2A-HO mice displayed significantly lower immobility time compared to the HE and the WT mice

(Fig 2) which was not reproduced by TST. Motor stimulants (Betin et al. 1982, De

Pablo et al. 1989) generally produce false positives in the FST (Panconi et al. 1993).

Nicotine (1mg/kg) has a weak but significant anti-depressant like effect consistent with the excitatory substances which constitute false positives for the FST (Porsolt et al. 1978) (Panconi et al. 1993). However, in this study, there were no significant differences in the motor activity (basic movement and fine movement) of NKCC2A-

HO mice. Thus, the decrease in immobility time in FST in NKCC2A-HO was independent of changes in locomotor activity suggesting that NKCC2A might have an effect related to the swimming challenge in the FST rather than behavioral despair challenge in the TST.

There might be the effect of thermogenesis in the FST that is producing decreased immobility in NKCC2A-HO mice. Sympathetic nervous system and thyroid hormone play a major role in generation and regulation of heat (Silva. 2006). Interestingly, preliminary results from our laboratory indicate that NKCC2 mRNAs are expressed in the thyroid at very low levels. Therefore, NKCC2A-HO mice might experience hypothermia in FST condition leading to decreased immobile behavior which cannot be observed in temperature-insensitive TST.

There were lower 5-HT and intraneural DA usage in the caudate nucleus which is mainly associated with motor activity, suggesting its role in the FST effect. Caudate nucleus receives projections from the A9 group of dopaminergic cell bodies which are located in the substantia nigra (Feldman. 1997). The significant neurochemical change in the caudate dopaminergic system was not supported by the results of the locomotor activity in the open field test indicating that NKCC2A is not effecting the overall

48

motor activity. But, there is a study showing significant elevation in prodynorphin

(pDyn) mRNA levels following FST in the caudate putamen but not in the nucleus accumbens, hypothalamus, amygdala, frontal cortex or hippocampus indicating that caudate might be solely involved in this process (Reed et al. 2012). No changes in the

DA and 5-HT usage in frontal cortex, amygdala, nucleus accumbens and hypothalamus which are common areas involved in both FST and TST explain that these areas might not be responsible for the immobile behavior in the tests. We suggest that NKCC2A might have an effect on DA system in the caudate nucleus but not in the other areas involved in motor activity.

In the open field test, there were no significant differences in locomotor activity which measures the fine movements (usually grooming), distance traveled in total or in zones or total time spent in either zone. The NKCC2A-HO exhibited a higher level of rearing compared to the WT mice in the open field test which may relate to more exploratory behavior in these mice. There was also a difference in the time spent resting in the periphery exhibiting thigmotactic behavior. However, this has uncertain significance in the absence of any difference in total time in the periphery, distance traveled in the periphery or any measurement in the central zone.

The elevated plus maze revealed no significant differences among groups in the time spent either in the open arm or in the closed arm. This is usually interpreted as a lack of difference in their ability to tolerate a stressful environment. This test is strongly responsive to benzodiazepine type anti-anxiety treatments and not to other classes of anxiolytics. No significant differences among the groups suggest that the GABA and benzodiazepine receptors are functionally normal which generally effect anxiety and performance in the elevated plus maze.

49

Y maze testing revealed no differences among mice of the three genotypes. Frontal cortex is the anatomical site for working memory and dopamine neurotransmission in the frontal cortex plays a major role in y-maze testing. The working memory was not different across the three groups. This observation is consistent with absence of significant changes in the DA usage in frontal cortex.

We used prepulse inhibition (PPI) of startle to evaluate genotype dependent effect on psychosis-like symptoms in NKCC2A-HO mice. There were no significant differences among groups in PPI of startle which measures the inhibitory response to a loud white noise stimulus (85 or 100 dB) when it is immediately preceded by a softer (70 dB) white noise. This is a basic circuit present in all normal species but deficient in schizophrenics. It is used as a screen for antipsychotic drugs by measuring their ability to reverse impairment of the PPI produced by either a dopamine antagonist or dopaminergic neurotransmission in frontal cortex (Zavitsanou et al.

1999) or phencyclidine (Yee et al. 2004; Bakshi and Geyer. 1997). There was no disruption in the PPI behavior which indicates absence of psychosis-like symptoms in the NKCC2A-HO mice and also indicating that NKCC2A-HO did not affect the hearing or sound perception. Interestingly, NKCC2 is expressed in the endolymphatic sac (Nishimura et al. 2009). Together, these results suggest the possibility that either

NKCC2B or NKCC2F are expressed in ES or that NKCC2A has a minimal if any role in audition. Dopaminergic neurotransmission in the frontal cortex plays a major role in the modulation of startle reflex measured by PPI (Zavitsanou et al. 1999; Yee et al.

2004; Bakshi and Geyer. 1997; Mehler-Wex et al. 2006). Absence of changes in PPI correlates with the insignificant changes in the frontal cortex and amygdala dopamine usage suggesting that NKCC2A is not affecting the dopamine projections innervating these areas.

50

There was a significant decrease in the adrenal NE and EPI levels in the NKCC2A-

HO mice compared to the wild type and heterozygous mice (Fig 16A, 16B).

SLC12A1 gene was found to be expressed in supraoptic and paraventricular neurons of the hypothalamo-neurohypophyseal system during salt load condition. The interactions among the hypothalamus, pituitary and adrenal gland forms the major part of the neuroendocrine system that controls stress related reactions. Expression of

NKCC2 in the hypothalamo-neurohypopheseal system, a specialized brain region involved in the elaboration of peptide hormones that directly control the kidney function explains both the central and peripheral role of NKCC2 in the crucial mechanisms to regulate salt and water balance. In the kidney NKCC2 is regulated by arginine vasopressin (AVP). AVP released from an endocrine source or paracrine/autocrine source might provoke NKCC2 synthesis and activity (Konopacka et al. 2015). Thus we speculate that the NKCC2A-HO mice which had low NE and

EPI adrenal levels but not significantly lower urinary catecholamine levels, compromise their ability to respond to the sympathetic stimulation.

Further studies

There is preliminary evidence of the presence of NKCC2A in some sections of the brain such as the frontal cortex, somatosensory cortex and in the cerebral cortex. Its expression was much higher in the WT mice compared to the HE mice and with no expression in the HO mice (unpublished observation). We did not perform tests to evaluate learning and cognitive behavior, there can be further investigations focused on the attention and cognitive abnormalities of the NKCC2A-HO mice as well as studies related to these brain areas showing NKCC2A expression, to determine the effect of its presence in these areas and to pursue the FST and TST discrepancy which is an interesting finding. Because of greater variability in urinary catecholamine

51

levels, we could not conclude there was a significant effect of NKCC2A on sympathetic activity. High salt-loaded rats were observed to have elevated NKCC2 expression (Haque et al. 2011), therefore there is a need to look at the urinary catecholamine levels in stress (salt-load) induced mice. Further studies can be focused on stress induced urinary catecholamine levels to see the effect of NKCC2A and also with a greater ‘N’ to increase the statistical significance of the study.

52

9. Acknowledgements

I am earnestly thankful to my advisor Dr James Lucot and also Dr Mauricio Di

Fulvio and Dr Nadja Grobe who advised and supported throughout my thesis with their comprehensive knowledge. I am very grateful to Teresa Garrett for her valuable support, guidance, time and help throughout my thesis and teach me working on HPLC. I would specially like to thank Marie Heis and Katherine for their constant support and help. I am heartily thankful to Dr. Mauricio for his valuable suggestions and also Shams Kuran to teach me working on PCR. Thanks to all graduate, undergraduate students and lab-staff for their assistance to conduct experiments.

We are grateful to the Research Incentive Funds awarded to Dr. Lucot by Wright

State University, American Diabetes Association, and the Boonshoft School of

Medicine (MDiF) which partially supported the costs of animals and behavioral studies, the outstanding resources of the Departments of Pharmacology and

Toxicology and Neuroscience, Cell Biology and Physiology and the Animal Facility at Wright State University, Boonshoft School of Medicine.

Note: The results of this Master’s Thesis and their interpretations have been presented in part or in whole in Scientific Meetings or have been submitted for publication. The data presented are the result of a collaborative arrangement between the laboratories of Dr Lucot and Dr Di Fulvio, which were responsible for the generation of data, analysis and interpretation, and the maintenance, genotyping and characterization of the NKCC2A colony of mice, respectively.

Finally I would like to thank my parents, my siblings, and my friends for being with me and for their moral support.

53

10. References

Corrigenda for vol. 283, p. F221 (2003). American Journal of Physiology - Renal

Physiology 284:Fa1-. - 1009.

Abbas, G., Naqvi, S., Mehmood, S., Kabir, N. and Dar, A. (2011) Forced swimming stress does not affect monoamine levels and in rats. Neuroscience

Bulletin 27:319-24.

Alshahrani, S. and Di Fulvio, M. (2012) Expression of the Slc12a1 Gene in Pancreatic beta-cells: Molecular Characterization and in silico Analysis. Cellular Physiology and

Biochemistry 30:95-112.

Alshahrani, S. and Di Fulvio, M. (2012) Enhanced insulin secretion and improved glucose tolerance in mice with homozygous inactivation of the Na+K+2Cl- co- transporter 1. Journal of Endocrinology 215:59-70.

Andersen, S. L. and Teicher, M. H. (1999) Serotonin laterality in amygdala predicts performance in the elevated plus maze in rats. Neuroreport. 1999 Nov

26;10(17):3497-500.

Ares, G. R., Caceres, P. S. and Ortiz, P. A. (2011) Molecular regulation of NKCC2 in the thick ascending limb. American Journal of Physiology - Renal Physiology

301:F1143-59.

Armando, I., Konkalmatt, P., Felder, R. A. and Jose, P. A. The renal dopaminergic system: novel diagnostic and therapeutic approaches in hypertension and kidney disease. 165(4):505-11

Bacic, D., Capuano, P., Baum, M., Zhang, J., Stange, G., Biber, J., Kaissling, B.,

Moe, O. W., Wagner, C. A. and Murer, H. (2005) Activation of dopamine D1-like

54

receptors induces acute internalization of the renal Na+/phosphate cotransporter NaPi-

IIa in mouse kidney and OK cells. American Journal of Physiology - Renal

Physiology 288:F740-7.

Bakshi, V. P. and Geyer, M. A. (1997) Phencyclidine-Induced Deficits in Prepulse

Inhibition of Startle are blocked by Prazosin, an Alpha-1 Noradrenergic Antagonist.

Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 283:666-74.

Betin, C., DeFeudis, F. V., Blavet, N. and Clostre, F. (1982) Further characterization of the behavioral despair test in mice: Positive effects of convulsants. Physiology and

Behavior 28:307-11.

Bissiere, S., McAllister, K. H., Olpe, H. and Cryan, J. F. (2006) The rostral anterior cingulate cortex modulates depression but not anxiety-related behavior in the rat.

Behavioral brain research 175:195-9.

Bjorklund, A., Lindvall, O. and Nobin, A. (1975) Evidence of an incerto- hypothalamic dopamine neuron system in the rat. Brain research 89:29-42.

Bjorklund, A. and Dunnett, S. B. (2007) Dopamine neuron systems in the brain: an update. Trends in neurosciences 30:194-202.

Borsini, F., Evangelista, S. and Meli, A. (1986) Effect of GABAergic drugs in the behavioral 'despair' test in rats. European journal of pharmacology 121:265-8.

Borsini, F. and Meli, A. (1988) Is the forced swimming test a suitable model for revealing antidepressant activity? Psychopharmacology 94:147-60.

Can, A., Dao, D. T., Arad, M., Terrillion, C. E., Piantadosi, S. C. and Gould, T. D.

(2012) The Mouse Forced Swim Test: e3638. Journal of Visualized Experiments. J

Vis Exp. 2012 Jan 29;(59):e3638

55

Can, A., Dao, D. T., Terrillion, C. E., Piantadosi, S. C., Bhat, S. and Gould, T. D.

(2012) The Tail Suspension Test: e3769. Journal of Visualized Experiments. (JoVE),

J Vis Exp. 2012 Jan 28 ;( 59):e3769

Carota, I., Theilig, F., Oppermann, M., Kongsuphol, P., Rosenauer, A., Schreiber, R.,

Jensen, B. L., Walter, S., Kunzelmann, K. and Castrop, H. (2010) Localization and functional characterization of the human NKCC2 isoforms. Acta Physiologica

(Oxford, England) 199:327-38.

Castagné, V., Moser, P., Roux, S. and Porsolt, R. D. (2001) Rodent Models of

Depression: Forced Swim and Tail Suspension Behavioral Despair Tests in Rats and

Mice. In: Current Protocols in Neuroscience. John Wiley and Sons, Inc.

55:8.10A:8.10A.1–8.10A.14.

Castrop, H. and Schnermann, J. (2008) Isoforms of renal Na-K-2Cl cotransporter

NKCC2: expression and functional significance. American Journal of Physiology -

Renal Physiology 295:F859-66.

Chaouloff, F. (2000) Serotonin, stress and corticoids. Journal of Psychopharmacology

14:139-51.

Chatterjee, M., Jaiswal, M. and Palit, G. (2012) Comparative Evaluation of Forced

Swim Test and Tail Suspension Test as Models of Negative Symptom of

Schizophrenia in Rodents. ISRN Psychiatry Volume 2012 (2012), Article ID 595141, doi:10.5402/2012/595141

Chen, M., Gao, J., He, X. and Chen, Q. (2012) Involvement of the Cerebral

Monoamine Neurotransmitters System in Antidepressant-Like Effects of a Chinese

Herbal Decoction, Baihe Dihuang Tang, in Mice Model. Evid Based Complement

Alternat Med. 2012;2012:419257 56

Chodavarapu, H., Grobe, N., Somineni, H. K., Salem, E. S. B., Madhu, M. and

Elased, K. M. (2013) Rosiglitazone Treatment of Type 2 Diabetic db/db Mice

Attenuates Urinary Albumin and Angiotensin Converting Enzyme 2 Excretion. PLoS

ONE 8:e62833.

Chorpita, B. F. and Barlow, D. H. (1998) The development of anxiety: The role of control in the early environment. Psychological bulletin 124:3-21.

Clayton, G. H., Owens, G. C., Wolff, J. S. and Smith, R. L. (1998) Ontogeny of cation

Cl- cotransporter expression in rat neocortex1. Developmental Brain Research

109:281-92.

Costa, A. P. R., Vieira, C., Bohner, L. O. L., Silva, C. F., Santos, E. C. d. S., De Lima,

T.,Christina Monteiro and Lino-de-Oliveira, C. (2013) A proposal for refining the forced swim test in Swiss mice. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and

Biological Psychiatry 45:150-5.

Crowley, J., Blendy, J. and Lucki, I. (2005) Strain-dependent antidepressant-like effects of citalopram in the mouse tail suspension test. Psychopharmacology 183:257-

64.

Cryan, J. F. and Lucki, I. (2000) Antidepressant-Like Behavioral Effects Mediated by

5-Hydroxytryptamine2C Receptors. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental

Therapeutics 295:1120-6.

Cryan, J. F., Mombereau, C. and Vassout, A. (2005) The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: Review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews; Animal Models of

Depression and Antidepressant Activity 29:571-625.

57

De Pablo, J. M., Parra, A., Segovia, S. and Guillamón, A. (1989) Learned immobility explains the behavior of rats in the forced swimming test. Physiology and

Behavior 46:229-37.

Delpire, E., Lu, J., England, R., Dull, C. and Thorne, T. (1999) Deafness and imbalance associated with inactivation of the secretory Na-K-2Cl co-transporter.

Nature genetics 22:192-5.

Detke, M. J., Johnson, J. and Lucki, I. (1997) Acute and chronic antidepressant drug treatment in the rat forced swimming test model of depression. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology 5:107-12.

Dunlop BW, N. C. (2007) THe role of dopamine in the pathophysiology of depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 64:327-37.

Fadok, J. P., Dickerson, T. M. K. and Palmiter, R. D. (2009) Dopamine is Necessary for Cue-Dependent Fear Conditioning. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29:11089-97.

Fahn, S. and Mayeux, R. (1980) Unilateral Parkinsons Disease and Contralateral

Tardive Dyskinesia: A Unique Case with Successful Therapy That May Explain the

Pathophysiology of These Two Disorders, eds. A. Carlsson, K. Jellinger and P.

Riederer, [179-185 pages]. Springer Vienna. J Neural Transm Suppl. 1980;(16):179-

85

Felten, A., Montag, C., Markett, S., Walter, N. T. and Reuter, M. (2011) Genetically determined dopamine availability predicts disposition for depression. Brain and

Behavior 1:109-18.

58

Flatman, P. W. (2002) Regulation of Na-K-2Cl cotransport by phosphorylation and proteinase“protein interactions. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA) -

Biomembranes; Epithelial Ion Transport-A Tribute to Hans H.Ussing 1566:140-51.

Fuxe, K., Hokfelt, T., Johansson, O., Jonsson, G., Lidbrink, P. and Ljundahl, Ã.

(1974) The origin of the dopamine nerve terminals in limbic and frontal cortex.

Evidence for meso-cortico dopamine neurons. Brain research 82:349-55.

Fuxe, K., Hokfelt, T., Johansson, O., Jonsson, G., Lidbrink, P. and Ljundahl, Å.

(1974) The origin of the dopamine nerve terminals in limbic and frontal cortex.

Evidence for meso-cortico dopamine neurons. Brain research 82:349-55.

Gagnon, K. B. and Delpire, E. (2013) Physiology of SLC12 transporters: lessons from inherited human genetic mutations and genetically engineered mouse knockouts.

American Journal of Physiology - Cell Physiology 304:C693-714.

Gamba, G., Miyanoshita, A., Lombardi, M., Lytton, J., Lee, W. S., Hediger, M. A. and Hebert, S. C. (1994) Molecular cloning, primary structure, and characterization of two members of the mammalian electroneutral sodium-(potassium)-chloride cotransporter family expressed in kidney. Journal of Biological Chemistry 269:17713-

22.

Geng, Y., Byun, N. and Delpire, E. (2009) Behavioral Analysis of Ste20 Kinase

SPAK Knockout Mice. Behavioural brain research 208:377-82.

Greger, R. (1985) Ion transport mechanisms in thick ascending limb of Henle's loop of mammalian nephron. Physiological Reviews 65:760-97.

59

Grider, J. S., Ott, C. E. and Jackson, B. A. (2003) Dopamine D1 receptor-dependent inhibition of NaCl transport in the rat thick ascending limb: mechanism of action.

European journal of pharmacology 473:185-90.

Holzel, B.,K., Carmody, J., Vangel, M., Congleton, C., Yerramsetti, S. M., Gard, T. and Lazar, S. W. (2010) Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density. Psychiatry research 191:36-43.

Hagenbuch, N., Feldon, J. and Yee, B. K. (2006) Use of the elevated plus-maze test with opaque or transparent walls in the detection of mouse strain differences and the anxiolytic effects of diazepam. Behav Pharmacol. 2006 Feb;17(1):31-41

Haque, M. Z., Ares, G. R., Caceres, P. S. and Ortiz, P. A. (2011) High salt differentially regulates surface NKCC2 expression in thick ascending limbs of Dahl salt-sensitive and salt-resistant rats. American Journal of Physiology - Renal

Physiology 300:F1096-104.

Harris, R. and Zhang, M. (2012) Dopamine, the Kidney, and Hypertension. Current hypertension reports 14:138-43.

Hata, T., Nishikawa, H., Itoh, E. and Funakami, Y. (2001) Anxiety-Like Behavior in

Elevated Plus-Maze Tests in Repeatedly Cold-Stressed Mice. The Japanese Journal of

Pharmacology 85:189-96.

Hebert, S. C., Reeves, W. B., Molony, D. A. and Andreoli, T. E. (1987) The medullary thick limb: function and modulation of the single-effect multiplier. Kidney international 31:580-9.

60

Hogg, S. (1996) A review of the validity and variability of the Elevated Plus-Maze as an animal model of anxiety. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior; Anxiety,

Stress and Depression 54:21-30.

Howes, O. D. and Kapur, S. (2009) The Dopamine Hypothesis of Schizophrenia:

Version III—The Final Common Pathway. Schizophrenia bulletin 35:549-62.

Hubner, C. A., Lorke, D. E. and Hermans-Borgmeyer, I. (2001) Expression of the Na-

K-2Cl-cotransporter NKCC1 during mouse development. Mechanisms of development 102:267-9.

Inglis, F. M. and Moghaddam, B. (1999) Dopaminergic Innervation of the Amygdala

Is Highly Responsive to Stress. Journal of neurochemistry 72:1088-94. Irwin, S.

(1968) Comprehensive observational assessment: Ia. A systematic, quantitative procedure for assessing the behavioral and physiologic state of the mouse.

Psychopharmacologia 13:222-57.

Isenring, P., Jacoby, S. C., Payne, J. A. and Forbush, B. (1998) Comparison of Na-K-

Cl Cotransporters: NKCC1, NKCC2, AND THE HEK CELL Na-K-Cl

COTRANSPORTER. Journal of Biological Chemistry 273:11295-301.

Jose, P. A., Raymond, J. R., Bates, M. D., Aperia, A., Felder, R. A. and Carey, R. M.

(1992) The renal dopamine receptors. Journal of the American Society of Nephrology

2:1265-78.

Karolewicz, B. and Paul, I. A. (2001) Group housing of mice increases immobility and antidepressant sensitivity in the forced swim and tail suspension tests. European journal of pharmacology 415:197-201.

61

Knable, M. B. and Weinberger, D. R. (1997) Dopamine, the prefrontal cortex and schizophrenia. Journal of Psychopharmacology 11:123-31.

Komada, M., Takao, K. and Miyakawa, T. (2008) Elevated Plus Maze for

Mice:e1088. Journal of Visualized Experiments (JoVE) Vis Exp. 2008; (22): 1088.

Konopacka, A., Qiu, J., Yao, S. T., Greenwood, M. P., Greenwood, M., Lancaster, T.,

Inoue, W., de Souza Mecawi, A., Vechiato, F. M. V., de Lima, J. B. M., Coletti, R.,

Hoe, S. Z., Martin, A., Lee, J., Joseph, M., Hindmarch, C., Paton, J., Antunes-

Rodrigues, J., Bains, J. and Murphy, D. (2015) Osmoregulation Requires Brain

Expression of the Renal Na-K-2Cl Cotransporter NKCC2. The Journal of

Neuroscience 35:5144-55.

Kulig, B., Alleva, E., Bignami, G., Cohn, J., Cory-Slechta, D., Landa, V.,

O'Donoghue, J. and Peakall, D. (1996) Animal behavioral methods in neurotoxicity assessment: SGOMSEC joint report. Environmental health perspectives 104:193-204.

Lad, H. V., Liu, L., Paya-Cano, J., Parsons, M. J., Kember, R., Fernandes, C. and

Schalkwyk, L. C. (2010) Behavioural battery testing: Evaluation and behavioural outcomes in 8 inbred mouse strains. Physiology and Behavior 99:301-16.

Lam, C. K. L., Chari, M. and Lam, T. K. T. (2009) CNS Regulation of Glucose

Homeostasis. Physiology 24:159-70.

Lazar, N. L., Rajakumar, N. and Cain, D. P. (2007) Injections of NGF Into Neonatal

Frontal Cortex Decrease Social Interaction as Adults: A Rat Model of Schizophrenia.

Schizophrenia bulletin 34:127-36.

Maria Schießl, I., Rosenauer, A., Kattler, V., Minuth, W. W., Oppermann, M. and

Castrop, H. (2013) Dietary salt intake modulates differential splicing of the Na-K-2Cl

62

cotransporter NKCC2. American Journal of Physiology: Renal Physiology

305:F1139-48.

Martin R.D. (Zürich) Dixson A.F. (Franceville) Wickings E.J. (Franceville). (1992)

Paternity in primates: genetic tests and theories. Pages 63-81 (eds).

Mechiel Korte, S. and De Boer, S. F. (2003) A robust animal model of state anxiety: fear-potentiated behaviour in the elevated plus-maze. European journal of pharmacology 463:163-75.

Mehler-Wex, C., Riederer, P. and Gerlach, M. (2006) Dopaminergic dysbalance in distinct basal ganglia neurocircuits: Implications for the pathophysiology of parkinson’s disease, schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

Neurotoxicity Research 10:167-79.

Mineur, Y. S., Obayemi, A., Wigestrand, M. B., Fote, G. M., Calarco, C. A., Li, A.

M. and Picciotto, M. R. (2013) Cholinergic signaling in the hippocampus regulates social stress resilience and anxiety- and depression-like behavior. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences 110:3573-8.

Moser, V. C. (2011) Functional Assays for Neurotoxicity Testing. Toxicologic pathology 39:36-45.

Nakasato, A., Nakatani, Y., Seki, Y., Tsujino, N., Umino, M. and Arita, H. (2008)

Swim stress exaggerates the hyperactive mesocortical dopamine system in a rodent model of autism. Brain research 1193:128-35.

Nishimura, M., Kakigi, A., Takeda, T., Takeda, S. and Doi, K. (2009) Expression of aquaporins, vasopressin type 2 receptor, and Na+-K+-Cl- cotransporters in the rat endolymphatic sac. Acta Otolaryngol 129:812-8.

63

Oades, R. D. and Halliday, G. M. (1987) Ventral tegmental (A10) system: neurobiology. 1. Anatomy and connectivity. Brain Research Reviews 12:117-65.

Oppermann, M., Mizel, D., Huang, G., Li, C., Deng, C., Theilig, F., Bachmann, S.,

Briggs, J., Schnermann, J. and Castrop, H. (2006) Macula Densa Control of Renin

Secretion and Preglomerular Resistance in Mice with Selective Deletion of the B

Isoform of the Na,K,2Cl Co-Transporter. Journal of the American Society of

Nephrology 17:2143-52.

Oppermann, M., Mizel, D., Kim, S. M., Chen, L., Faulhaber-Walter, R., Huang, Y.,

Li, C., Deng, C., Briggs, J., Schnermann, J. and Castrop, H. (2007) Renal Function in

Mice with Targeted Disruption of the A Isoform of the Na-K-2Cl Co-Transporter.

Journal of the American Society of Nephrology 18:440-8.

Panconi, E., Roux, J., Altenbaumer, M., Hampe, S. and Porsolt, R. D. (1993) MK-801 and enantiomers: Potential antidepressants or false positives in classical screening models?. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 46:15-20.

Payne, J. A. and Forbush, B. (1994) Alternatively spliced isoforms of the putative renal Na-K-Cl cotransporter are differentially distributed within the rabbit kidney.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

91:4544-8.

Pechnick, R. N., Chesnokova, V. M., Kariagina, A., Price, S., Bresee, C. J. and

Poland, R. E. (2004) Reduced Immobility in the Forced Swim Test in Mice with a

Targeted Deletion of the Leukemia Inhibitory Factor (LIF) Gene.

Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of

Neuropsychopharmacology 29:770-6.

64

Pietropaolo, S. (2010) Mood and Anxiety-related Phenotypes in Mice:

Characterization Using Behavioral Tests - Edited by T. D. Gould. Genes, Brain and

Behavior Volume 9, Issue 5, page 544.

Plata, C., Meade, P., Vazquez, N., Hebert, S. C. and Gamba, G. (2002) Functional

Properties of the Apical Na+-K+-2Cl- Cotransporter Isoforms. Journal of Biological

Chemistry 277:11004-12.

Porsolt, R. D., Bertin, A. and Jalfre, M. (1978) Behavioural despair in rats and mice:

Strain differences and the effects of imipramine. European journal of pharmacology

51:291-4.

Reed, B., Fang, N., Mayer-Blackwell, B., Chen, S., Yuferov, V., Zhou, Y. and Kreek,

M. J. (2012) Chromatin alterations in response to forced swimming underlie increased prodynorphin transcription. Neuroscience 220:109-18.

Robert S. Feldman, ed. Jerrold S. Meyer, Linda F. Quenzer (1997). Principles of

Neuropsychopharmacology. Sunderland MA, Sinauer Associates Inc.; 277-344.

Sahyoun, C., Floyer-Lea, A., Johansen-Berg, H. and Matthews, P. M. (2004) Towards an understanding of gait control: brain activation during the anticipation, preparation and execution of foot movements. NeuroImage 21:568-75.

Sakas, D. E. and Panourias, I. G. (2006) Rostral cingulate gyrus: A putative target for deep brain stimulation in treatment-refractory depression. Medical hypotheses

66:491-4.

Sango, K., Yamanaka, S., Hoffmann, A., Okuda, Y., Grinberg, A., Westphal, H.,

McDonald, M. P., Crawley, J. N., Sandhoff, K., Suzuki, K. and Proia, R. L. (1995)

65

Mouse models of Tay-Sachs and Sandhoff diseases differ in neurologic phenotype and ganglioside metabolism. Nature genetics 11:170-6.

Sharma, A. N., Elased, K. M., Garrett, T. L. and Lucot, J. B. (2010) Neurobehavioral deficits in db/db diabetic mice. Physiology and Behavior 101:381-8.

Sharma, A. N., Elased, K. M. and Lucot, J. B. (2012) Rosiglitazone treatment reversed depression- but not psychosis-like behavior of db/db diabetic mice. Journal of Psychopharmacology 26:724-32.

Silva, J. E. (2006) Thermogenic Mechanisms and Their Hormonal Regulation.

Physiological Reviews 86:435-64.

Simon, D. B., Karet, F. E., Hamdan, J. M., Pietro, A. D., Sanjad, S. A. and Lifton, R.

P. (1996) Bartter's syndrome, hypokalaemic alkalosis with hypercalciuria, is caused by mutations in the Na-K-2CI cotransporter NKCC2. Nature genetics 13:183-8.

Smith, E. A., Schwartz, A. L. and Lucot, J. B. (2013) Measurement of urinary catecholamines in small samples for mice. Journal of pharmacological and toxicological methods 67:45-9.

Starremans, P. G. J. F., Kersten, F. F. J., Knoers, N. V. A. M., van den Heuvel, L. P.

W. J. and Bindels, R. J. M. (2003) Mutations in the Human Na-K-2Cl Cotransporter

(NKCC2) Identified in Bartter Syndrome Type I Consistently Result in Nonfunctional

Transporters. Journal of the American Society of Nephrology 14:1419-26.

Stevenson, C. W. and Gratton, A. (2004) Role of basolateral amygdala dopamine in modulating prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition in the rat. Psychopharmacology

176:139-45.

66

Tatem, K. S., Quinn, J. L., Phadke, A., Yu, Q., Gordish-Dressman, H. and Nagaraju,

K. (2014) Behavioral and Locomotor Measurements Using an Open Field Activity

Monitoring System for Skeletal Muscle Diseases:e51785. J Vis Exp. 2014 Sep

29;(91):51785

Thierry, B., Stéru, L., Simon, P. and Porsolt, R. D. (1986) The tail suspension test:

Ethical considerations. Psychopharmacology 90:284-5.

Tibaldi, J. M. (2014) Evolution of Insulin: From Human to Analog. The American

Journal of Medicine 127:S25-38.

Trullas, R., Folio, T., Young, A., Miller, R., Boje, K. and Skolnick, P. (1991) 1- aminocyclopropanecarboxylates exhibit antidepressant and anxiolytic actions in animal models. European journal of pharmacology 203:379-85.

Vargas-Poussou, R., Feldmann, D., Vollmer, M., Konrad, M., Kelly, L., van den

Heuvel, ,L.P., Tebourbi, L., Brandis, M., Karolyi, L., Hebert, S. C., Lemmink, H. H.,

Deschênes, G., Hildebrandt, F., Seyberth, H. W., Guay-Woodford, L., Knoers, N. V. and Antignac, C. (1998) Novel molecular variants of the Na-K-2Cl cotransporter gene are responsible for antenatal Bartter syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics

62:1332-40.

Xue, H., Liu, S., Ji, T., Ren, W., Zhang, X. H., Zheng, L. F., Wood, J. D. and Zhu, J.

X. (2009) Expression of NKCC2 in the rat gastrointestinal tract.

Neurogastroenterology and Motility 21:1068-e89.

Yee, B. K., Chang, D. T. and Feldon, J. (2004) The Effects of Dizocilpine and

Phencyclidine on Prepulse Inhibition of the Acoustic Startle Reflex and on Prepulse-

Elicited Reactivity in C57BL6 Mice. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 29:1865-77. 67

Zangen, A., Overstreet, D. H. and Yadid, G. (1999) Increased catecholamine levels in specific brain regions of a rat model of depression: normalization by chronic antidepressant treatment. Brain research 824:243-50.

Zavitsanou, K., Cranney, J. and Richardson, R. (1999) Dopamine Antagonists in the

Orbital Prefrontal Cortex Reduce Prepulse Inhibition of the Acoustic Startle Reflex in the Rat. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 63:55-61.

Zeng, C. and Jose, P. A. (2011) Dopamine Receptors: Important Antihypertensive

Counterbalance Against Hypertensive Factors. Hypertension 57:11-7.

Zhao, Z., Zhang, H., Bootzin, E., Millan, M. J. and Donnell, J.,M. (2008) Association of Changes in Norepinephrine and Expression with the Long-

Term Behavioral Effects of Antidepressant Drugs. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1467-

81.

Zhong, P., Liu, W., Gu, Z. and Yan, Z. (2008) Serotonin facilitates long-term depression induction in prefrontal cortex via p38 MAPK/Rab5-mediated enhancement of AMPA receptor internalization. The Journal of physiology 586:4465-79.

68