Predicting the Difficulty of Multiple Choice Questions in a High-Stakes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Predicting the Difficulty of Multiple Choice Questions in a High-Stakes Predicting the Difficulty of Multiple Choice Questions in a High-stakes Medical Exam Le An Ha1 Victoria Yaneva2 Peter Baldwin2 Janet Mee2 1Research Group in Computational Linguistics, University of Wolverhampton, UK [email protected] 2National Board of Medical Examiners, Philadelphia, USA fvyaneva, pbaldwin, [email protected] Abstract that are too easy or too difficult are less able to distinguish between different levels of examinee Predicting the construct-relevant difficulty of ability (or between examinee ability and a defined Multiple-Choice Questions (MCQs) has the cut-score of some kind – e.g., pass/fail). This is es- potential to reduce cost while maintaining the quality of high-stakes exams. In this paper, pecially important when scores are used to make we propose a method for estimating the dif- consequential decisions such as those for licen- ficulty of MCQs from a high-stakes medical sure, certification, college admission, and other exam, where all questions were deliberately high-stakes applications1. To address these issues, written to a common reading level. To accom- we propose a method for predicting the difficulty plish this, we extract a large number of linguis- of multiple choice questions (MCQs) from a high- tic features and embedding types, as well as stakes medical licensure exam, where questions features quantifying the difficulty of the items of varying difficulty may not necessarily vary in for an automatic question-answering system. The results show that the proposed approach terms of reading levels. outperforms various baselines with a statisti- Owing to the criticality of obtaining difficulty cally significant difference. Best results were estimates for items (exam questions) prior to their achieved when using the full feature set, where use for scoring, current best practices require embeddings had the highest predictive power, newly-developed items to be pretested. Pretest- followed by linguistic features. An ablation ing typically involves administering new items to study of the various types of linguistic features a representative sample of examinees (usually be- suggested that information from all levels of linguistic processing contributes to predicting tween a few hundred and a few thousand), and item difficulty, with features related to seman- then using their responses to estimate various sta- tic ambiguity and the psycholinguistic proper- tistical characteristics. Ideally, pretest data are ties of words having a slightly higher impor- collected by embedding new items within a stan- tance. Owing to its generic nature, the pre- dard live exam, although sometimes special data sented approach has the potential to generalize collection efforts may also be needed. Based on over other exams containing MCQs. the responses, items that are answered correctly by a proportion of examinees below or above cer- 1 Introduction tain thresholds (i.e. items that are too easy or too For many years, approaches from Natural Lan- difficult for almost all examinees) are discarded. guage Processing (NLP) have been applied to esti- While necessary, this procedure has a high finan- mating reading difficulty, but relatively fewer at- cial and administrative cost, in addition to the time tempts have been made to measure conceptual required to obtain the data from a sufficiently large difficulty or question difficulty beyond linguistic sample of examinees. complexity. In addition to expanding the hori- Here, we propose an approach for estimating zons of NLP research, estimating the construct- the difficulty of expert-level MCQs, where the relevant difficulty of test questions has a high prac- 1Examples of well-known high-stakes exams include tical value because ensuring that exam questions the TOEFL (Test of English as a Foreign Language) are appropriately difficult is both one of the most (https://www.ets.org/toefl), the SAT (Scholastic Assessment Test) (https://collegereadiness.collegeboard.org/sat), and the important and one of the most costly tasks within USMLE (United States Medical Licensing Examination) the testing industry. For example, test questions (https://www.usmle.org/). A 55-year-old woman with small cell carcinoma of the lung is admitted to the hospital to undergo chemotherapy. Six days after treatment is started, she develops a temperature of 38C (100.4F). Physical examination shows no other abnormalities. Laboratory studies show a leukocyte count of 100/mm3 (5% segmented neutrophils and 95% lymphocytes). Which of the following is the most appropriate pharmacotherapy to increase this patient’s leukocyte count? (A) Darbepoetin (B) Dexamethasone (C) Filgrastim (D) Interferon alfa (E) Interleukin-2 (IL-2) (F) Leucovorin Table 1: An example of a practice item gold standard of item difficulty is defined through iv) Owing to the generic nature of the features, large-scale pretesting and is based on the re- the presented approach is potentially generaliz- sponses of hundreds of highly-motivated exami- able to other MCQ-based exams. We make our nees. Being able to automatically predict item dif- code available2 at: https://github.com/ ficulty from item text has the potential to save sig- anomymous1/Survival-Prediction. nificant resources by eliminating or reducing the need to pretest the items. These savings are of 2 Related Work even greater importance in the context of some au- The vast majority of previous work on difficulty tomatic item generation strategies, which can pro- prediction has been concerned with estimating duce tens of thousands of items with no feasible readability (Flesch, 1948; Dubay, 2004; Kintsch way to pretest them or identify which items are and Vipond, 2014; Franc¸ois and Miltsakaki, 2012; most likely to succeed. Furthermore, understand- McNamara et al., 2014). Various complexity- ing what makes an item difficult other than manip- related features have been developed in readabil- ulating its reading difficulty has the potential to aid ity research (see Dubay(2004) and Kintsch and the item-writing process and improve the quality Vipond(2014) for a review), starting from ones of the exam. Last but not least, automatic diffi- utilising surface lexical features (e.g. Flesch culty prediction is relevant to automatic item gen- (1948)) to NLP-enhanced models (Franc¸ois and eration as an evaluation measure of the quality of Miltsakaki, 2012) and features aimed at capturing the produced output. cohesion (McNamara et al., 2014). Contributions i) We develop and test the pre- There have also been attempts to estimate the dictive power of a large number of different types difficulty of questions for humans. This has been of features (e.g. embeddings and linguistic fea- mostly done within the realm of language learn- tures), including innovative metrics that measure ing, where the difficulty of reading comprehension the difficulty of MCQs for an automatic question- questions is strongly related to their associated answering system. The latter produced empiri- text passages (Huang et al., 2017; Beinborn et al., cal evidence on whether parallels exist between 2015; Loukina et al., 2016). Another area where question difficulty for humans and machines. ii) question-difficulty prediction is discussed is the The results outperform a number of baselines, area of automatic question generation, as a form showing that the proposed approach measures of evaluation of the output (Alsubait et al., 2013; a notion of difficulty that goes beyond linguis- Ha and Yaneva, 2018). In many cases such evalua- tic complexity. iii) We analyze the most com- tion is conducted through some form of automatic mon errors produced by the models, as well as measure of difficulty (e.g., the semantic similarity the most important features, providing insight between the question and answer options as in (Ha into the effects that various item characteristics 2The questions cannot be made available because of test have on the success of predicting item difficulty. security. and Yaneva, 2018)) rather than through extensive or options, and grammatical cues (e.g., correct an- evaluation with humans. Past research has also fo- swers that are longer, more specific, or more com- cused on estimating the difficulty of open-ended plete than the other options; or the inclusion of the questions in community question-answering plat- same word or phrase in both the stem and the cor- forms (Wang et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2013); how- rect answer). Item writers had to ensure that the ever, these questions were generic in nature and produced items did not have flaws related to vari- did not require expert knowledge. Other studies ous aspects of validity. For example, flaws related use taxonomies representing knowledge dimen- to irrelevant difficulty include: Stems or options sions and cognitive processes involved in the com- are overly long or complicated, Numeric data not pletion of a test task to predict the difficulty of stated consistently and Language or structure of short-answer questions (Pado´, 2017) and identify the options is not homogeneous. Flaws related to skills required to answer school science questions “testwiseness” are: Grammatical cues; The cor- (Nadeem and Ostendorf, 2017). We build upon rect answer is longer, more specific, or more com- previous work by implementing a large number of plete than the other options; and A word or phrase complexity-related features, as well as testing var- is included both in the stem and in the correct an- ious prediction models (Section4). swer. Finally, stylistic rules concerning preferred While relevant in a broad sense, the above usage of terms, formatting, abbreviations, conven- works are not directly comparable to the current tions, drug names, and alphabetization of option task. Unlike community question answering, the sets were also enforced. The goal of standardizing questions used in this study were developed by items in this manner is to produce items that vary experts and require the application of highly spe- in difficulty and discriminating power due only cialized knowledge. Reading exams, where com- to differences in the medical content they assess. prehension difficulty is highly associated with text This practice, while sensible, makes modeling dif- complexity, are also different from our medical ficulty very challenging.
Recommended publications
  • Linguistics: Theory and Applications”
    National and Kapodistrian University of Athens Department of English Language and Literature MA Programme “Linguistics: Theory and Applications” L2 C level Writing Proficiency: KPG Integrated Tasks and Language Interaction in Rated Scripts Eleni Pappa 217022 Supervisor: Bessie Mitsikopoulou 2019 Declaration This submission is my own work. Any quotation from, or description of, the work of others is acknowledged herein by reference to the sources, whether published or unpublished. 2 Other supervisors: Elly Yfantidou Anna Xatzidaki 3 Abstract1 Research into L2 writing assessment has largely focused on mapping textual features onto rater-judged candidate performance, exploring issues related to writing quality and rater reliability. However, due to issues such as the ambiguous wording of the marking scale (Lumley, 2002) or the raters’ difficulty to score borderline essays (Gebril & Plakans, 2014), very little has been found with respect to fine distinctions between adjacent levels of language proficiency, C level (C1-C2) in particular. In this line, the current research aimed to investigate KPG C level rater-judged candidate performance in integrated tasks of two types, an intralingual and an interlingual mediation task. Using a sample of 66 rated scripts (33 candidates), three points were addressed: a) the effect of two different types of texts, an expository blog and a narrative encyclopedic entry with an expository task requirement, on language realization, b) interrater variation and c) cohesion and coherence as a potential candidate performance differentiating language criterion within C level. Quantitative analysis results indicate that, first and foremost, Coherence & Cohesion and, second, Vocabulary Range, can allow for distinctions within C level. What is more, their accentuated predictive strength when combined with the Appropriacy criterion can yield a more global (in terms of tasks) account of observed 1 A Greek version of this abstract can be found on the last page of this dissertation.
    [Show full text]
  • The Bilingual Mental Lexicon
    The Bilingual Mental Lexicon The Bilingual Mental Lexicon By Longxing Wei The Bilingual Mental Lexicon By Longxing Wei This book first published 2020 Cambridge Scholars Publishing Lady Stephenson Library, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE6 2PA, UK British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Copyright © 2020 by Longxing Wei All rights for this book reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without the prior permission of the copyright owner. ISBN (10): 1-5275-4093-6 ISBN (13): 978-1-5275-4093-4 Dedicated to my grandsons, Dylan and Gavin CONTENTS Preface ......................................................................................................... x Acknowledgements .................................................................................. xiii Symbols and Abbreviations ...................................................................... xiv 1 ................................................................................................................... 1 Toward a New Approach to Studies of the Bilingual Mental Lexicon 1 Introduction ...................................................................................... 1 2 Code-switching and Issues Involved ................................................ 3 3 Second Language Acquisition and Issues Involved ......................... 7 4 A Preview of the Matrix
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Input Revisited: Nativist Versus Usage-Based Models
    UC Berkeley L2 Journal Title The role of input revisited: Nativist versus usage-based models Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/647983hc Journal L2 Journal, 1(1) Author Zyzik, Eve Publication Date 2009 DOI 10.5070/l2.v1i1.9056 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California L2 Journal, Volume 1 (2009), pp. 42‐61 http://repositories.cdlib.org/uccllt/l2/vol1/iss1/art4 The Role of Input Revisited: Nativist versus Usage-Based Models EVE ZYZIK University of California, Santa Cruz E­mail: [email protected] This article examines the role of input in two contrasting theories of language acquisition: nativist (UG) theory and the usage-based (emergentist) approach. Although extensive treat- ments of input are available for first language acquisition (cf. Gathercole & Hoff, 2007), such research rarely incorporates findings from second language acquisition. Accordingly, this paper examines a range of linguistic phenomena from both first and second language contexts (e.g., yes-no question formation, constraints on want-to contraction) in order to illustrate how each theory might explain their acquisition. The discussion of input presented here addresses various constructs, including the problem of the poverty of the stimulus, the lack of negative evidence, the role of indirect (missing) evidence, recovery from overgener- alization, and frequency effects. The article concludes with a reappraisal of the poverty of the stimulus problem in SLA from a usage-based perspective. _______________ Recent publications in the field of second language acquisition (SLA) show a marked trend towards usage-based approaches to describe the development of linguistic knowl- edge (cf.
    [Show full text]
  • "Competition Model" In: the Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics
    Competition Model PING LI AND BRIAN MACWHINNEY The competition model (CM) made its debut in Bates and MacWhinney (1982) as a mechanistic explanation of language acquisition. Since its inception, there have been a large number of studies using this framework to account for not only language acquisition but also language comprehension, language production, and impaired language processes. Because the CM distinguishes itself from many other psycholinguistic theories in its emphasis on language variation, the model has been applied to the analyses of many typologically different languages, including Chinese, English, German, Hungarian, Italian, Russian, and Spanish, to name a few. Like other scientifi c hypotheses, the CM has con- tinued to evolve in order to increase its explanatory power (Bates & MacWhinney, 1987, 1989; MacWhinney, in press). Here we fi rst provide an overview of the model and then discuss its application to the study of second language acquisition. Cues and Cue Competition A basic construct of the CM is the cue, an information source that allows the language user to successfully link linguistic form with meaning. Cues vary in their type (morphological, syntactic, prosodic, semantic, and pragmatic), availability (how often they are present), and reliability (how often they lead to the correct interpretation). Each cue is associated with cue validity, the joint product of availability and reliability, and crucially, the same cue may have different validity in different languages. For example, in English, word order (e.g., a noun before a verb) is a strong and reliable cue for identifying agent versus patient roles in an event (high validity), whereas word order is less predictive of who does what to whom in Chinese (low validity).
    [Show full text]
  • 20 Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition: One Person with Two Languages
    488 Vivian Cook 20 Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition: One Person with Two Languages VIVIAN COOK Most linguistics concerns people who know, use, or learn a single language. Yet such monolinguals are probably in a minority in the world as a whole. Many people use several languages in the course of a day, whether in multilingual countries such as Pakistan or the Cameroon or in apparently monolingual countries such as England and Kuwait. This chapter looks at some of the ques- tions raised by the fact that people know more than one language. General popular questions include: is learning a second language (L2) like learning a first (L1)? Are children better than adults at L2 learning? Can you speak a second language without an accent? Linguists are more concerned with ques- tions such as: how does L2 learning relate to Universal Grammar? Does the language input the learner hears make a difference? How does one language affect the other? They are all fundamentally concerned with how one person can have two languages. Any issue in linguistics can potentially be studied in the context of people who know more than one language. This chapter starts with some historical background and then discusses ten interrelated questions that have driven research into SLA (second language acquisition) within the overall context of one person with two or more languages. The aim is to put the reader in touch with some of the issues that have been investigated, touching on areas of linguistics such as phonology and vocabulary as well as syntax. The account represents one person’s route through a large maze, trying not to stray down paths less connected with linguistics.
    [Show full text]
  • The Competition Model
    Looking at Interlanguage Looking at IL Processes Processes The Monitor Model The Competition Model Modes of Knowledge Representation 2 The Competition Model The Competition Model What is your interpretation of these The cows eat the grass. English sentences? What criteria do you use to interpret them? How do you know? 3 4 The Competition Model The Competition Model The grass eat the cows. The grass eats the cows. How do you know? How do you know? 5 6 1 The Competition Model The Competition Model The pencil see the boys. The pencil sees the boys. How do you know? How do you know? 7 8 The Competition Model The Competition Model Syntactic ambiguity is resolved by cue strengths. Consider: The boy asked the girl to go. A asked B to go. Jimmy asked his mother to go play. Jimmy went to play. The doctor asked Jeff to go see a specialist. How do you know? Jeff went to see a specialist. 9 10 The Competition Model The Competition Model The dog asked the girl to go. The girl asked the chair to go. How do you know? How do you know? 11 12 2 The Competition Model The Competition Model In processing meaning in sentences we use The chair asked the dog to go. these cues: Syntax Word order (()SVO) Morphology (Case of nouns, Agreement between subject and verb) Semantics ± Human How do you know? ± Animate 13 14 The Competition Model The Competition Model In some cases the semantic and Different languages assign different syntactic cues converge to give the same weights to syntactic and semantic interpretation of a sentence.
    [Show full text]
  • Kootstravanhell&Dijkstra (2009)
    Alignment in code-switching 1 Running head: ALIGNMENT IN CODE-SWITCHING Two speakers, one dialogue An interactive alignment perspective on code-switching in bilingual speakers Gerrit J. Kootstra1, Janet G. van Hell1,2, and Ton Dijkstra1 1Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands 2The Pennsylvania State University, USA Book chapter in: L. Isurin, D. Winford, & K. de Bot (2009), Multidisciplinary approaches to code switching (pp. 129-159). Amsterdam / Philadelphia: John Benjamins. AUTHOR NOTE: Gerrit J. Kootstra, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Janet G. van Hell, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands, and Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, USA; Ton Dijkstra, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour: Centre for Cognition, Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands. The research reported in this paper was supported by a grant from the Behavioural Science Institute, awarded to Janet G. van Hell. Alignment in code-switching 2 Correspondence concerning this chapter should be addressed to Gerrit J. Kootstra, Behavioural Science Institute, Faculty of Social Sciences, Radboud University Nijmegen, P.O. Box 9104, 6500 HE, Nijmegen, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]. Alignment in code-switching 3 Abstract In code-switching research, a distinction can be made between approaches that focus on linguistic and cognitive variables within single individuals and approaches that emphasize processes between individuals and the social and interactive context. These approaches differ in terms of both theory and methodology, and are difficult to integrate. In this chapter, we build on recent theoretical developments in psycholinguistics and propose a model of interactive alignment in code-switching.
    [Show full text]
  • "Multicompetence" In: the Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics
    Multicompetence VIVIAN COOK The term “multicompetence” was originally defi ned as “the compound state of a mind with two grammars” (Cook, 1991); in the context of that paper, “grammar” was used in the Chomskyan sense of the total knowledge of language in the mind (the I-language) leading some people to infer wrongly that multicompetence was restricted to syntax. The current working defi nition is “the knowledge of more than one language in the same mind or the same community”: everything a single person or a single community knows about all the languages they use. Multicompetence thus presents a view of second language acquisition (SLA) based on the second language (L2) user as a whole person rather than on the monolingual native speaker. It is neither particularly a psychological concept, as some have claimed (Kelly Hall, Cheng, & Carlson, 2006), nor particularly sociological. It is a perspective for viewing both broad approaches to SLA and thus applies both to the individual and to the community. It is perhaps closest to approaches that treat language as a continuously changing system (De Bot, Lowie, & Verspoor, 2005). Multicompetence therefore involves the whole mind of the speaker, not simply their fi rst language (L1) or their second. It assumes that someone who knows two or more languages is a different person from a monolingual and so needs to be looked at in their own right rather than as a defi cient monolingual. Multicompetence is thus neither a model nor a theory so much as an overall perspective or framework: it changes the angle from which second language acquisition is viewed.
    [Show full text]
  • Competition Model and Contrastive Lexical Competition Meisam Ziafar1, Ehsan Namaziandost2
    Journal of Humanities and Education Development (JHED) ISSN: 2581-8651 Vol-1, Issue-6, Nov – Dec 2019 https://dx.doi.org/10.22161/jhed.1.6.1 Competition Model and Contrastive Lexical Competition Meisam Ziafar1, Ehsan Namaziandost2 1Assistant Professor, Department of English Language Teaching, Ahvaz Branch, Islamic Azad University, Ahvaz, Iran [email protected] 2Ph.D. Candidate in TEFL, Department of English, Faculty of Humanities, Shahrekord Branch, Islamic AzadUniversity, Shahrekord, Iran [email protected] Abstract— The Competition Model (CM) embraces lexicalist and functionalist approach to language structure and function. What is highly emphasized in this model is a lexicalist functionalism through which syntactic patterns are directed and controlled by lexical items. CM tenets resemble to that of Haliday’s systemic-Functional linguistics in that it only deals with form-meaning relations within a text and not in the real world. A new Competition Model needs to be introduced which is more pragmatic-oriented through taking formulaic sequences as forms to be mapped onto real world pragmatic functions. CM must free itself from the mere focus on sentence processing studies and involve itself with more pragmatic manifestations of form-function relations. It is claimed that within the models in which there is an architecture that utilizes lexical categories to build “valence bridges”, L1-L2 translation equivalents facilitate crossing valence bridges which helps in discovering forthcoming elements and filling syntactic slots.
    [Show full text]
  • Second Language Acquisition: an Introductory Course, Third Edition
    SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Praise for the Third Edition: “A tour de force. The authors have taken the best text available for an introduc- tory course in second language acquisition (SLA) and made it even stronger.... The improvements in the third edition are palpable from the very beginning of the text.... The third edition of Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course should form the basis for any introduction to SLA, at either the graduate or undergraduate level.” —Fred Eckman, Center for the Advanced Study of Language, University of Maryland “This text presents a comprehensive overview of SLA in an accessible, highly readable manner appropriate for readers new to this discipline.... The new edition includes even more data samples than the previous edition, both integrated into the main text and in the discussion questions at the end of each chapter.” —Deborah Pilcher, Gallaudet University “Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course presents the most balanced, grounded, and accessible introduction to a broad field. The field has grown rapidly since publication of the second edition in 2001, and a new, updated, and expanded edition is most welcome. [T]he third edition . make[s] this breadth of know- ledge accessible to students with little background in the feeder disciplines ... Second Language Acquisition has now become a living standard in our field.” —Richard Young, Professor of English Linguistics, University of Wisconsin-Madison About the Authors: Susan M. Gass is University Distinguished Professor in the Department of Linguistics and Germanic, Slavic, Asian and African Languages at Michigan State University. She has conducted research in a wide variety of sub-areas of second language acquisition, including language transfer, language universals, second language research methods, and input and interaction.
    [Show full text]
  • Second Language Acquisition and the Competition Model
    CHAPTER FOUR Second Language Acquisition and the Competition Model Brian MacWhinney Carnegie Mellon University Current approaches to second language acquisition (SLA) can be divided broadly into two groups: nativist models and empiricist models. Nativist models attribute language development to the operation of a universal, genetically controlled, language instinct. For researchers in the nativist tra- dition, the learning of the core features of a second language involves little more than the setting of a few switches for the parameters of Universal Grammar (Du Plessis, Solin, Travis, & White, 1987; Gregg, 1989; Liceras, 1989; Mazurkewich & White, 1984; Thomas, 1991; White, 1983, 1985, 1990, 1991). Many nativists view second language acquisition as recapitulating the course of first language acquisition (Bickerton, 1984; Krashen, 1982) because a strong version of the nativist position holds that both first and second language learning are determined by the underlying principles of Universal Grammar. Empiricist approaches to second language acquisition tend to emphasize the extent to which the second language must be actually learned. Some second language researchers who are willing to grant that first language acquisition is strongly influenced by Universal Grammar are not willing to view second language acquisition in the same light (Bley-Vroman, Felix, & loup, 1988; Clahsen & Muysken, 1986; Schachter, 1989). Researchers who accept nativist approaches to first language acquisition and empiricist ap- proaches to second language acquisition
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Salience in Second Language Acquisition Ian South Comeaux Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2015 The Role of Salience in Second Language Acquisition Ian South Comeaux Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_theses Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Comeaux, Ian South, "The Role of Salience in Second Language Acquisition" (2015). LSU Master's Theses. 2155. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_theses/2155 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Master's Theses by an authorized graduate school editor of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ROLE OF SALIENCE IN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in The Department of Psychology by Ian South Comeaux B.S., Louisiana State University, 2012 May 2016 TABLE OF CONTENTS ABSTRACT……………………………………………………………………………………………………..... iii CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………………………………..1 The Competition Model………………………………………………………………………………………. 1 Cue Conflict Resolution………………………………………………………………………………………. 5 The Shallow Structure Hypothesis (SSH)……………………………………………………………………. 6 Summary of Models and Implications for Language Learning………………………………………………. 7 Implicit Learning
    [Show full text]