DOCUMENT RESUME

ED 059 625 EM 009 666

AUTHOR Comstock, George A., Ed..; Rubenstein, Eli A., Ed. TITLE Television and Social Behavior; Reports and Papers, Volume III. Television and Adolescent Aggressiveness. INSTITUTION Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, Washington, D.C. PUB DATE Apr 72 NOTE 444p.; See also EM 009 435, EM 009 664, EM 009 665, EM 009 667, EM 009 668 AVAILABLE FROMSuperintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, Dr,C. 20402 (DHEW Pub. No. HSM 72-9058)

EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$16.45 DESCRIPTORS *Adolesctents; *Aggression; Children; Commercial Television; *Correlation; Family Environment; Family Influence; Programing (Broadcast); Social Behavior; Socioeconomic Status; Television Research; *Television Viewing; Viewing Time; *Violence ABSTRACT The question which guided the studies in this third volume of technical reports to the Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior is whether aggressive social behavior by adolescents can be attributed in some degree to violent television programming. After an overview which sets the studies in a comparative context, the studies are reported. They include a followup longitudinal study by Lefkowitz, Eron, Walder, and Huesmann with two comments on cross-lagged correlation. Also included are . study dealing with family influences by Chaffee and McLeod, "Adolescent Television Use in the Family Context"; two studies by McLeod, Atkin, and Chaff ee which are concerned with self-report and other-report measures of television use and aggression; and a study by Dominick and Greenberg, "Attitudes Toward Violence; The Interaction of Television Exposure, Family Attitudes, and Social Class.11Other studies reported include Friedman andJohnson,"Mass Media Use and Aggression; A Pilot Study's; Johnson, Friedman, and Gross, HFour Masculine Styles in Television Programming; A Study of the Viewing Preferences of Adolescent Males"; Robingon and Bachman, "Television Viewing Habits and Aggression"; and McIntyre and Teevan, "Television Violence and Deviant Behavior." (Author/SH) A_ Publications of the Television and Social Behavior Program

Report of the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior Media Content and Control (Reports and Papers, Volume I) Tekvision and Social Learning (Reports and Papers, Volume 2) Television and Adolescent Aggressiveness (Reports and Papers, Vol- ume 3) Television in Day-to-Day Life: Patterns of Use (Reports and Papers, Volume 4) Television's Effects: Further Explorations (Reports and Papers, Vol- ume 5) Television and Social Behavior: An Annotated Bibliography of Re- search Focusing on Television's Impact on Children

For solo by tho Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing 0111co, Washington, D.C. 20402 TELEVISION AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

REPORTS AND PAPERS, VOLUME III: TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

A TECHNICAL REPORT TO THE SURGEON GENERAL'S SCIENTIFIC ADVISORY COMMITTEE ON TELEVISION AND SOCIAL BEHAVIOR

U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION & WELFARE OFFICE OF EDUCATION THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO- DUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIG- INATING IT. POINTS OF VIEW OR OPIN- IONS STATED DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL Or rICE OF EDU- CATION POSITION OR PJ.iCY. Edited By George A. Comstock and Eli A. Rubinstein K'ditorial Coordination:Susan Lloyd.Jones

U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION, AND WELFARE Health Services and Mental Health Administration

National Institute of Mental Health 5600 Fishers Lane Rockville, Maryland Staff Members

Eli A. Rubinstein Vice Chairman. Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee George A. Comstock Senior Research Coordinator John P. Murray Research Coordinator Michael Adler Staff Assistant Eileen Marchak Research Assistant Susan S. Lloyd-Jones Editor Joseph D. Reck ley Administrative Officer Margaret D. Salladay Secretary Laura A. De Lisi Secretary

Former Staff Members

Douglas A. Fuchs Senior Research Coordinator (through 6/70) John P. Robinson Research Coordinator (through 9/70) Harold Leigh Ad m inistrative Officer (through 10/70) Thomas Brubeck Information Officer (through 5/71) Deborah Cutler Research Assistant (through 8/70) Jan W. Lipkin Secretary (through 4/70)

Advisory Com mittee Members

Ira H. Cisin Charles A. Pinderhughes Thomas E. Coffin Ithiel de Sola Pool Irving L. Janis Alberta E. Siegel Joseph T. Klapper Anthony F. C. Wallace Harold M endelsohn Andrew S. Watson Eveline Omwake Gerhart D. Wiebe

4` Preface

This document is one of five volumes of technical reports resulting from a broad scientific inquiry about television and its impact on the viewer. In the spring of 1969. by Congressional request. the DHEW ini- tiated a special program under the general auspices of a Surgeon Gener- al's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior. The major emphasis was to be on an examination of the relationship between televised violence and the attitudes and behavior of children. During the ensuing two years. more than fifty scientists participated di- rectly in this program of research and produced over forty scientific reports. The reports which art. included in these five volumes are the inde- pendent work of the participating researchers. These results have all been made available to the Scientific Advisory Committee as evidence which the Committee could then evaluate and draw its own conclusions in the preparation of its own report. However, this work is of signifi- cance in its own right and is being published independently as source material for other researchers and for such interest as the generpl public niay hi.ve in these technical reports. In any broad scientific undertaking of this nature, where many indi- viduals are involved, a careful balance between collaboration and inde- pendence of responsibility must be established. During the two and half years that this program of research was active, a constant effort was made to protect the scientific independence of the individual investiga- tors and, at the same time: I) to foster both cooperation and exchange among the researchers, 2) to develop as much of a total program struc- ture as possible, and 3) to permit maximum communication and feed- back among the researchers, the full-time staff responsible for planning and implementing the total research program, and the Scientific Adviso- 'ry Committee responsible for the final assessment and evaluation of the research. VA This is not the place to describe in detail how that balance of collabo- ration and independence was established and maintained.I believe, however, that these five volumes of technical reports provide an accur- ate and meaningful indication of our success in achieving the goal. The reports themselves are the products of the respective authors. They have been edited only to insure some comparability of format and to delete any excessive redundancies in review of the literature or intro- ductory material. In some instances, where a report seemed initially too

Ill iv

long the author was requested to reduce the report without deleting any critical material. All editing done by staff was submitted for the author's approval. We believe the result has made each of these five volumes a more readable and integrated totality than would otherwise be expected from a collection of research reports produced under the time con- straints of this program. In each instance, the integration of the five volumes was further estab- lished by the inclusion of an overview paper which attempts to summa- rize and relate the papers in that volume. These overview papers are also the independent work of the respective authors. It would be difficult to convey to the reader the extraordinary efforts required by all participants in this research program to bring the endeav- or to its published conclusion within the time allotted. Despite that time pressure, these volumes demonstrate an unusually high level of both productivity and quality for an area of research which has had more than its share of complexity and controversy. In addition to the work of all persons directly engaged in this program, a very large number of individuals at one time or another provided ad- vice and guidance to the researchers, to the staff, and to the Scientific Advisory Committee. It would be impossible to provide a complete list of these additional consultants. The total count is in the hundreds. While their names are not visible in these products, their counsel was often a very significant factor in the course of an individual piece of research or in a decision on !he direction of the research program. To all those indi- viduals, this program owes a special debt of gratitude for the collective wisdom made available to us. And finally, on behalf both of the members of the Scientific Advisory Committee and of the staff who served the program, I wish especially to express much appreciation to the participating researchers who did the work and wrote the reports that contributed the new knowledge con- tained in these volumes.

Eli A. Rubinstein Vice-Chairman, Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior Contents

Page

Television and Adolescent Aggressiveness (Overview). Steven H. Chaffee

Television Violence and Child Aggression: A Followup Study.

Monroe M. Lefkowitz. Leonard D. Eron.1.copold O. Wald- . and L. Rowell Huesrnann 35

Two Corn ments on Cross-lagged Correlation Threats to thc Internal Validity of Cross-lagged Panel Infer- ence. as Related to "Television Violence and Child Ag- gression: A Followup Study". David A. Kenny 136 Comment on "Television Violence and Child Aggression: A Followup Study". John M. Neale 141

Adolescent Television Use in the Family Context. Steven H. ChaffeeandJackM. McLeod 149

4., Adolescents, Parents, and Television Use: Adolescent Self-Re- port Measures from Maryland and Wisconsin Samples. Jack M. McLeod, Charles K. Atkin, and Steven H. Chaffee 173

Adolescents, Parents, and Television Use: Self-Report and Oth- er-Report Measures from the Wisconsin Sample. Jack M. McLeod, CharlesK. Atkin. and Steven H. Chaffee 239

Attitudes Toward Violence: The Interaction of Television Expo- sure, Family Attitudes, and Social Class. Joseph R. Dominick and Bradley S. Greenberg 314

Mass Media Use and Aggression: A Pilot Study. Herbert L. Friedmhn and Raymond L. Johnson 336

Four Masculine Styles in Television Programming: A Study of the Viewing Preferences of Adolescent Males. Raymond L. Johnson, Herbert L. Friedman, and Herbert S. Gross 361 vi

Television Viewing Habits and Aggression. John P. 'Robinson mut Jerald G. Bachman 372

Television Violence and Deviant Behavior. Jennie J. McIntyre and James J. Teevan. Jr 383 f Lt; Lit Lt. Television and Adolescent Aggressiveness (Overview)

Steven H. Chaffee

This paper is an introduction to reports of eight field studies of adoles- cents, focusing on their exposure to violent television programs and on their aggressive social behaviors and feelings. The concepts, samples, measures, and modes of analysis of these studies differ greatly. The purpose of this paper, then, is to attempt to set the studies in a compara- tive context so they can be interpreted in relation to one another and to other research. This is neither a substitute for, nor a critique of, the studies themselves, and cannot itself be interpreted without direct refer- ence to the original research reports (which can be found elsewhere in this volume).

9 2 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

To avoid pointless redundancy, the eightpresent studies will be con- sistently referred to by number throughout thetext, as follows: ( /) Steven H. Chaffee and Jack M. McLeod. Adolescent television use in the family context. (2) Herbert L. Friedman and Raymond L. Johnson.Mass media use and aggression: a pilot study. (3) Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Leonard D. Eron, Leopold 0.Walder, and L. Rowell Huesmann. Television violence and childaggres- sion: a followup study. (4) Jennie J. McIntyre and James J. Teevan, Jr. Televisionand de- viant behavior. (5) Jack M. McLeod, Charles K. Atkin, and Steven H. Chaffee.Ado- lescents, parents, and television use. This reportwas issued in two parts, which will be referred to where necessaryas follows: (5a) Adolescent self-report measures from Maryland and Wiscon- sin samples. (5b) Self-report and other-report measures from the Wisconsin sample. (6) Joseph R. Dominick and Bradley S. Greenberg. Attitudestoward violence: the interaction of televisionexposure, family attitudes and social class. (7) John P. Robinson and Jerald G. Bachman. Television viewing habits and aggression. (8) Raymond L. Johnson, Herbert L. Friedman, andHerbert S. Gross. Four masculine styles in television programming:a study of the viewing preferences of adolescent males. The central research question guiding all these studies is whetherag- gressive or violent social behavior by adolescentscan be attributed in some degree to violent television programming. This paper will build systematically toward evidence on that hypothesis, accordingto the fol- lowing outline: 1. Samples and settings for the nine studies II. Measures of adolescent television use and their correlates A. Time spent viewing B. Program preferences C. Frequency of viewing D. Perceptions of television 111.Measures of adolescent aggressiveness and their correlates A. Reports by others B.Self-reported aggressive behavior C.Feelings of aggressiveness IV. Correlations between viewirtg and aggressivenessmeasures A. Correlations with violent program preferences B.Correlations with viewing of violentprograms C.Possible causal inferences TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 3

D.Potential approaches to social control V. Summary

SAMPLES AND SETTINGS

The procedures for data-gathering are described indetail in each of the studies. Only a few elementary comparisonswill be made here, to introduce the studies in a com mon context. Two of the studies use small (N=80) samples of boys fromracially mixed junior high schools in Baltimore, Md. (2: 8). Neitherof the sam- ples is random; instead, both are purposefully stratified onthe basis of high or low "aggressiveness" as determined by schoolofficials. Three studies used data from ongoing surveys that hadbegun before initiation of the Television and Social Behavior project. In twoof these. a new wave of data-gathering wasadded to the previous measures (3: 7). Secondary analyses from the third of these (1) provided the"pretest" basis for a new study with a different sample (5). The major longitudinal study (3) was conducted in a rural countyof upstate New York, and spans a ten-year period inthe lives of the panel members. They were originally measured in school in thethird grade, when their modal.age was 8. A five-year followupstuCly was attempted but provided quite incomplete data. ten-year folio wup, called the "thirteenth" grade wave, was completed in 1969 as part ofthe.Televi- sion and Social Behavior project. There are 436 respondents for whom both third- and thirteenth-grade data are available. The other ongoing study is longitudinal to the limited extent that some measures were repeated one year apartin 1969 and 1970 (7). It is an all- male sample', consisting of some 2,200 post-high school students(age 19) "scattered throughout the ." The secondary analysis report is based on data from some 1,300junior and senior high students and their parents, gatheredin five eastern Wis- con cities in 1968I). In the later study by the same research group, two samples, were interviewed (5). The first consists of separate measures from adolescents and their mothers in a south-centralWisconsin suburb. There were two waves, in fall 1969 and fall 1970. The first waveof child measures occurred'at school(siXth and ninth grades), and all other mea- sures took place at home.This sample included 225 mother-child pairs in the first wave; attrition reduced it to 151 in the second wave.The other sample consisted of 473 seventh and tenth graders, whofilled out ques- tionnaries at school in Prince Georges County, Md., in spring 1970. Prince Georges County was also the site of the Most'comprehensive sample among the present studies (4). In this survey2,260 students, evenly distributed across grades 7 through 12,completed questionnaires at school in spring 1970. The sampleincludes large bumbers of both 11 4 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS black and white adolescents, a variable that was also coded in three oth- er studies (2; 7; 8). The remaining study (6) barely reaches the borderline of what is usual- ly considered "adolescence," since the samples consist of fourth, fifth, and sixth graders in six Michigan schools in spring 1970. Data were ana- lyzed separately for the 434 males and the 404 females. The studies are included in this group despite the age discrepancy, primarily because the measures are similar to those in several of the other surveys here.

ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE

These studies use three types of measures of adolescent viewing be- havior: time spent viewing, program preferences, and frequency of viewing recurrent programs. Although these measures obviously 'have something in common, they do not produce consistently similar results. Separate examination of each can tell us something about the uses ado- lescents make of television, and about the specific role of violent pro- gram mihgin these patterns bf use. Time spent viewing

Most of the present studies use variants of the Roper (1969) method of asking the respondent to estimate his viewing on "an average day" (3; 4: 7); others ask about the prior day (Lyle and Hoffman, 1971) or combine the "average day" and "yesterday" items intd a single best-estimate (1; :5). Two studies note that estimates tend to be higher for "average day" than for "yesterday" (2;*LoSciuto, 1971). The wisdom of asdertainitig separate estimates for weekend vs. daily viewing (as in 3) is indicated by the detailed measures gathered by Lyle and Hoffman (1971). They pre- sented local program logs for each day and asked their respondents to check the ones they had watched the previous day. Total viewing time on Sunday was about double the figures for weekdays. Schramm et al. (1961) had found a less dramatic but similar difference ten'jfears earlier. The "average" amount of time spent viewing by the "average" ado- lescent is a decidedly elusive quantity, one that varies greatly according to details of sampling, question phrasing, and the method of averaging. But it appears a reasonable guess ihai on a given day, 'approximately one-half of American adolescents spend some three hours or more with an operating televisionset. A substantial number watch twice that much, and of course a good many do not watch television at all. But overall there is a general impression of "many yoUngsters spending hours in front of a television set" (4). More interesting than the synthetic notion of an "average"are the relationships between viewing time and other variables. Themost con- sistent finding is that viewing .time decreases throughout adolescence. 12 TELEVNION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 5

Schramm et al. (1961) estimated that children's viewing is heaviest at about the sixth grade. which is also roughly the beginning of adoles- cence. Their finding of a steady decline after the sixth gradeis replicat- ed, with rare exceptions, in each of the present studies that make age- group or grade-level comparisons (1: 4: 5a: Lyle andHoffman. 1971). F,urther. a study of junior high boys comments that they "watch a good deal of television." some two to six hours daily. often simply to "kill time" (2); by contrast, a study of boys one year beyond high school remarks on how little they watch television, in comparison with general

population norms (7). , The evidence on sex differences is mixed. Schramm et al. (1961) found no significant male-female difference, nor does LoSciuto (1971). Two of the present studies report that adolescent males view more than femiles ( I: 5a). but two others show slight trends in the opposite direc- tion (4: Lyle and Hoffman, 197 1). These same data indicate a greater adolescent developmental trend away from heavy viewing among girls

in three studies (1: 4; 5) but not in the fourth (Lyle and Hoffman, 971)1, . Mental ability presents a more consistent picture. Almost all the stud- II ies agree that brighter adolescents watch less television. Schramm et al. (1961) found a negative correlation between IQ and viewing time among adolescents, and one of the present studies replicates this (3). Another study shows'a negative correlation with academic performance in school (5a). If we assume that continued education beyond high school indi7 cates higher mental ability, there is pertinent evidence in two samples of post-high-school boys. One finds the expected negative correlation be- tween viewing time and years of education,(3). but the other reports no more than a "trivial difference" between those who arein college and those who are not (7). Viewing time is also negatively related to indices of social skills such as, leadership and achievement (3), popularity (3), and integration with peers (5b). Socioeconomic status tends to be negatively related to adolescent viewing time. That is, those whose parents' occupations and education levels place them in the lower SES strata spend more time with televi- sion (3; 4; 5a). In several studies the authors control SES statistically, to examine residual relationships between viewing time and other variables

, . (1: 4; .; 7). 1

. The possibility,of parental influences on adolescent viewing,has been examined in several.ways. There is a significant correlation between viewing time measures for adolescents and their parents, particularly their mothers (1: 5b). Although Schramm et al. (1961) concluded that "parental example" exercises "a very potent kind of influencel' over adolescent viewing, a review of more extensive data does not support so strong an inference (1; see also Chaffee et al., 197 1). ,It seems that ado- lescents are at least as likely to influence their parents' viewing as vice- [ versa, and a good portion of theparent-adolescent viewing similarity_. 13 6 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

can probably be explained by the fact that two persons living in the same household will tend to be exposed to the same televisionprogram s with- out influencing one another socially. Their mutual opportunity to view the same programs is simply greater, regardless of "example." As will be seen shortly (below), a considerable portion of adolescent viewing time is spent with programs that often depict violence. It is not surprising, then, that part-whole correlations between viewing time and violence-viewing measures are reported in several studies ( I: 3: Sa). There is also evidence that those who watch more television tend slight- ly toward a stronger preference for violent programs (4). Many factors could account for the dwindling of "the TV habit" dur- ing adolescence. The developing youngster increasingly finds competing activities that might occupy his time. The developmental trend away from television is particularly marked among those who are most likely to find other things to do: the brighter and more socially skilled, and those in higher socioeconomic strata. It is against this background of developmental and "life-space" factors that we turn to specific mea- sures of preference for and viewing of violent programs. Program preferences Four studies employ measures of the violent content in theyoung- sters "favorite" programs (2; 3; 4; 7). One_ftf-these (7)uses a list of three or four favo,*tes, and the meanyioience ratingacross this list based on the Greenberg and Gorprr(-I97l) ratings. Another (4)ascer- tains both the single favorite p.rogiam and the four favorites, andapplies the Greenberg-Gordon,tliigs to each; the one-favorite-programmea- sure is used in mova-nalyses. In the longitudinal study, differentmea- sures were ued1n different waves of interviews. At the third-grade stage, th ild's parents were asked what the youngster's three favorite prog,aThs were; in the five- and ten-year followupmeasurements, the m6lescent'sown list of his four favorites was used. The third grade par- ent report is probably more a measure of observed "frequent viewing" by the child than of "program preferences," since the child's viewing behavior is the most likely indicatora parent has of his preferences; therefore this measure is classed with the "frequency of viewing"mea- sures (below). This assumption, that the parent-reports of child's favor- ites differ from the child's self-reports, is supported by the fact thatthey do not correlate significantly over time, for eithersex (3). These differ- ences in the viewing measure seriously affect attempts at longitudinal causal inference, as discussed later here. In bothwaves, staff coders rated the programs, and their ratings (3) correlate highly with those of Greenberg and Gordon. The fourth study (2) used botha checklist and an open-ended "favorite program" question, again applying the Green- berg-Gordon ratings; the two formsof the question'yielded similar re- sults. TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 7 Even more than in the case of viewing time, it is bootless to attempt to ascertain the "average" level of violence in adolescent program prefer- ences. Lyle and Hoffman (1971) constructed a list of the 20 most popular programs, based on the youngsters' four favorite shows. Only twofrom their sixth-grade list, and three from their tenth-grade list, are also among the 20 most-violent shows (Greenberg and Gordon, 1971).How- ever, Lyle and Hoffman elicited a decidedly more violentflavor when they asked which programs the youngsters actually watched (see discus- sion below). Preference for violent programs predominates in the favorite shows of a group of junior high boys that was partly sampled for high aggres- siveness (2). Of their nine most-preferred programs, five are also among the six most-violent according to ratings by critics and by the public (Greenberg and Gordon, 1971). Rather few of these boys complained of too much violence on television (2), whereas this was the second most frequent complaint about television in a national sample of adolescents and adults (LoSciuto, 1971). Lyle and Hoffman (1971) also report that few adolescents complain about television violence, especially at the tenth-grade level. Adolescent boys also have a strong interest in football telecasts, most of which are of the maximally violent professional game (2; 7). Unlike total viewing time, age appears not to be much of a factor in preferences for violent programs. If anything, there is a slightly greater tendency for older adolescents to name a violent program as theirfavor- ite (4; Lyle and Hoffman, 1971). The longitudinal study shows rather strong correlations betw een eighth- and thirteenth-grade preferences for violent programs (3). Nei- ther of these measures is significantly related to the earlier third-grade violence-viewing measure, which could be due to developmental shifts of to the fact that the third-grade measure was derived from parent re- ports (3). At any rate, a youngster's preference for (or against) violent programs appears to be well defined early in adolescence, and topersist despite the general drift away from heavy viewing during that' period in life. Sex and SES are both related to violence-preference. Males and high- SES youngsters are more likely to have no "favorite" program than are girls and low-SES respondents. When a favorite is named, it is again the males, but also the low-SES youngsters, who are most likely to mention a violent one (4). A seemingly relatedfinding is that boys with high mo- bility aspirations are more likely to prefer violent programs than are those whose aspirations are lower,(3). In all, the present studies add rather little to our knowledge of the psychological origins of preferences for television violence, other than the degree to which they can be attributed to the youngster's general .level of aggressiveness. That question is considered in detail later in this paper. 15 8 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Frequency of viewing

The most common method of ascertaining violence viewing inthese studies was to present the respondent witha checklist of recurrent pro- grams, and ask him how often he watched each (5: 6: 8). A more labori- ous but also more precise technique was to show him the previous day's local television log and ask him to check theprograms he had watched; this procedure was repeated daily foran entire school week by Lyle and Hoffman (1971). A decidedly imprecise methodwas to list program cate- gories ("westerns" and "spy and adventure shows"),and ask for an es- timate of the frequency of viewing each (I). These raw frequency measures were converted into violence-viewing indices by a variety of methods. Inone study 28 programs were listed, including the 20 that had been judged "most violent" in the Greenberg and Gordon (1971) study; the number of shows from this 20 that theres- pondent viewed each week was hisor her violence-viewing score (6). A more elaborate procedure was to develop a composite violence rating based on the two Greenberg and Gordon samples plusa Minnesota study (Murray et al., 1970); then to multiply this bya five-level viewing- frequency m Asure for each of 65 programs; and finallyto sum the 65 products of frequency-by-violence measures (.5a). As mentioned above, the third-grademeasure of viewing consisted of asking the parent to list the child's three favoriteprograms; these pro- grams were then coded according to their frequency of violence (3). While it is difficult to determine exactly what behavior of the child'sis being measured by these reports, itseems reasonable to assume that parents would be better able to name programs the child frequently watches than to assess the child's liking ofprograms. The latter would have required information about affectiveresponses to programs that we have no reason to expect parents would possess. Lyle and Hoffman (1971) made no attempt to builda violence-viewing index from their data, but their list of most-viewedprograms indicates a developmental trend toward more violent (and quite probablymore adult) shows. Of the 22 most-viewed prime timeprograms, the number that are also among the 20 most-violent (Greenberg and Gordon,1971) jumps from four at the sixth grade to nine in the tenth-gradenorms (Lyle and Hoffman, 1971). As mentioned earlier, thismeasure of actual view- ing contrasts markedly with program-preferencemeasures in the same study, in which the youngsters listed only twoor three violent shows among their favorites. There seems to be a tendency for adolescentsto watch violent programs more than to admit that they like them. In terms of real viewing, exposure to television violence probably does not increase during adolescence, because total viewing time de- creases (see above). For example, Mod Squad is the second-most- watched program among tenth graders and ranks only fifthon the sixth-

74: !at 16 -- TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 9 grade list (Lyle and Hoffman, 1971), but the difference between grade levels in the percentage who watched it is small. Similarly, Ironside ranks eighteenth at both grade levels but was seen by slightly more sixth graders. Another study shows a decrease in violence viewing from junior to senior high samples that holds for both sexes and for all four family- communication-patterns types (1). This finding was replicated for both sexes in each of two other samples (5a).Consistently higher violence viewing by males than females is found at both grade levels (1; 5a). The brighter youngsters, and those from higher socioeconomic levels, are less likely to watch violent programs.Significant negative correla- tions with SES and measures of IQ or school performance are reported for three samples (5a). A mild negative relationship between violence viewing and SES can also be discerned from the cell Ns in the study of younger children (6). The negative SES relationshipholds for samples of both sexes at two grade levels (5b). It appears, however, that this raw correlation can be better explained by more functional variables than by the demographic construct of SES. When other measures of television use and parent-child relations are controlled, the SES-violence viewing correlation falls to zero ( /). Look- ing at it another way, controls for SES fail to alter appreciably the corre- lations between violence viewing and a number of aggressiveness ind- ices (5a). By contrast, the negative relationship between IQ and violence view- ing survives extensive partialing (I), and school performance proves an important control variable in the aggressiveness-viewing analyses (5a). The tendency for brighter adolescents to watch less television (see above) is echoed by a corresponding tendency to watch less violent pro- grams; this specific pattern holds regardless of theyoungster's total tele- vision use or various parental factors ( /). In one small-sample analysis (5b), it appears that SES is negatively related to violence viewingonly among boys, and to schoolperformance only among girls. Parental behaviors bear some relationships to the adolescent's fre- quency of viewing violent programs. Perhapsthe strongest, at least at the junior high level, is the parent's own level of violence viewing; this correlates significantly with the child's viewing, even when television time is controlled for both parent and child ( /). These parent-child corre- lations hold for specific programs and for all major categories of pro- grams ;O. Regardless of the child's sex,the correlation with the moth- er's viewing is stronger than with the father's (1). This lack of a sex-role link, plus other data, lead the authors to conclude that the correlations are due to reciprocal causation at most and maybe spurious (1). Three other parent-child interaction variables are positively associat- ed with adolescent violence viewing in several samples. Theseinclude restriction as a m ethod of punishing the child (1: 5a: and 5b);socio- 10 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS oriented family communication (1; A); and parental interpretation of television violence (5a and 5b). These correlationsare readily under- stood for two of the three measures. A restricted youngster has unantici- pated "time at home" on his hands. and television isan easily available activity to fill it. Parental interpretation of television violence is obvious- ly more likely to be offered to youngsters who watchmore violent pro- grams. The correlation with family socio-oriented communication sug- gests somewhat more subtle sociopsychological processes at work; it has survived considerable partialing. at least at the junior high level(1;

Perceptions of television

As has already been mentioned, adolescents do not complain often about the level of violence on television. Their most frequent complaints are that there are too many commercials and news programs (2; Lyle and Hoffman, 1971). It is rare to complain about potential material that is missing; instead, objections are raised against what ispresented, which suggests that adolescents havean essentially passive consumer orientation toward television. It is clear from the Lyle and Hoffman (1971) data thattelevision is primarily associated with entertainment and relaxation inthe typical adolescent mind. When a youngster is angry or has had his feelings hurt, he is more likely to want to converse, go off alone, listento music, or engage in sports than to turn on the television set. A sample of adoles- cent boys cited "enjoyment" and "time-killing"as their most usual rea- sons for viewing and said that about three of every tenprograms they watched "just came on" (2). Violent programs are seen as highly realistic by adolescents,even more so than news and documentary programs (4). Involvement with television violence is reportedmore frequently for crime shows than for westerns (51)). Girls report more feelings of involvement with violent programs, but boys identify more with violent characters (Sa). Younger adolescents are more likely to say they have learned aggressivebehav- iors from television (5a). These specific reactionsto television violence are only mildly correlated with the amount of violence viewing; theyare somewhat more strongly correlated with aggressivebehavior, indicating that they may be key intervening variables inany process of learning of social aggression via television (Sa). One study (4) measured the adolescent's perceptionof the role played by aggression in his favorite program; since thesevariables are related only to indices of aggressive behavior, discussionof them will be de- ferred until later in this paper. A provocative attempt to discern the latent dimensionsof role percep- tions in adolescent viewing was made inone study (8). Beginning with a 18 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 1 I four-fold sample (blacks vs. whites, aggressives vs.normals) of 80 eighth-grade boys, the authors focus on those programsthat have a pe- culiar appeal to only one of the four groups.They conclude that the chief characters in these programs can becharacterized by status (low vs. high) and reactions to problemsituations (active vs. passive) that are analogous to those of the boys who watch them.The authors assume that selective television viewing is a form ofinformation seeking, in which "the adolescent is highly motivated tosearch for prototypes of adult masculine behavior" on television,which provides "a readily ac- cessible source of high-definition portraits of themasculine styles com- mon in our mass culture."This implies a more purposeful approach to the television set than other studies would suggest for mostadolescents, who appear to be quite casual in their viewing(see above). But this as- sumption is supported to an extent by the finding thatone-fifth of a gen- eral adolescent sample felt that aggressivebehavior by the main charac- ters in their favorite programs "shows the waypeople ought to act" (4). Also, boys in early adolescence are the mostlikely to say they have learned aggressive modes of behavior fromtelevision (Sa). The hypothe- sis of role-modeling via television need not extend to allkinds of adoles- cents tu he socially significant.

ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS An enormous variety of conceptions of"aggressiveness" are found in the present studies. The measures can beconveniently broken down into four categories: reports of aggression byothers, self-reports of ag- gression and delinquent behavior, self-reports ofaggressive feelings and attitudes, and self-reports of cognitive andeffective reactions to aggres- sion. As with the various indices of television use(above), these differ- ent measures yield somewhatdifferent results. In this section they will be compared with one another, and their correlateswill be reviewed. The following section deals with theirrelationships to television vio- lence. Reports by others Three kinds of "experts" on an adolescents'saggressive social be- havior have been used in the present studies: schoolofficials, peers, and parents. (A fourth potential source,siblings, was not used, probably because data would not have been comparablefrom one youngster to another due to varying ages and numbers ofbrothers and sisters.) Two studies rely on school administrators andcounselors, to pick samples of students with "records" or"histories" of aggressive behav- ior (2; 8). In the first of these there was avalidity check based on self- reports, which showed asubstantially higher incidence of fighting and 12 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS use of weapons by the "aggressives" than by other students (2). The aggressives were also more likely to come from low-SES homes and to get low grades in school. It is difficult to determine whether the school officials were influenced by these factors in their nomination ofaggres- sives; aggressiveness measures based on self-report do not consistently correlate with SES, but tend to be related to poorer school performance (see below). A single-item teacher report was used in one junior high sample, and it did not discriminate very strongly between the sexes (5b). This measure correlated only modestly with peer ratings, and even less with self- and mother-ratings. In this same study, a ten-item peer report of "assault aggression" appears to have been much more satisfactory. It discriminates by sex and age, in that boys and younger adolescents are rated more aggressive by their peers. This was also the only study to use parent reports, a four- item index based on interviews with the mother. Like the teacher report, this measure did not distinguish between sexes or grade levels, and it correlated quite weakly with other measures of aggressiveness. It was, nevertheless, combined with peer and teacher reports into a single "oth- er-report" index of aggressiveness. Although such a multiple-operation measure would seem to offer maximum validity, this index does not cor- relate as strongly with self-reported aggression as does the peer report alone. Peer reports also provide the most important aggressiveness measures in the longitudinal study; they were repeated, with modifications, in the third, eighth, and "thirteenth" grade waves (3). In the third grade,a ten- item "guess who" list of aggressive acts was given to each child; he was instructed to name the other child in the class who was most likely to have committed the act. In the eighth grade, one of the ten items was dropped, and there werP. some changes in wording; more importantly, only a small portion of the original sample could be re-interviewed. So the eighth-grade measure is unsatisfactory for longitudinal analysis, and the authors make little use of it except as a validity check. Respondents in the larger thirteenth-grade wave were given the nine eighth-grade items, all of which had been changed to retrospective measures by con- verting the wording to past tense. For example, "Who startsa fight over nothing?" from the first two waves became "Who starteda fight over nothing?" Instead of nominating a single "most likely" classmate from iheir own third-grade rooms, the respondents were invited to check the names of "all those people who fit that question." For this purpose, each respondent was given a roster of students with whom he had gone to school. Arithmetical corrections were made to neutralize differences in opportunity for nomination. Correlations among these three waves of peer-rated aggression are highest for pairs of measures nearer in time among the boys, but for girls ,20 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 13 the third:eighth grade correlation is quite the weakest. Measuresof "psychopathology" in the thirteenth grade correlate most strongly with thirteenth-grade peer-rated aggression, and successively less strongly with each earlier measure. The thirteenth-grade aggressiveness indices were positively related to social status andnegatively with IQ for both sexes (3). Overall, the peer-based aggressiveness measures appear to bethe most valid of the various other-ratings in the preselitstudies. This should be expected, since they are based on more items and larger pools of "experts" and since peers are more usual victims of adolescent ag- gression, compared with the various types of adult raters. Self-reported aggressive behavior

It could easily be argued that the most competent "expert" on an ado- lescent's aggressive acts would be that person himself. There is, howev- er, a danger that the youngster would be reluctant toadmit many trans- gressions and would distort his answers to achieve a modicum of social approval from the researcher. In most of the present studies, the re- searchers were willing to take that kind of risk, although most of them opted for various forms of multiple operationism in aneffort to "sur- round" the key variable of aggressiveness. In the study where the main measure of aggressiveness was selection by school officials, the nominated "aggressives" more oftenadmitted fighting, hitting a teacher, and using a gun or knife to "get something" from someone(2).Thus self-report provided a validity check on the main measure. Repeated measures one year apart on an eight-item index were gath- ered in the large-sample study of post-high-school boys (7). The most frequent aggressive acts were fighting, participating in a gang fight, hurt- ing someone so that he needed medical attention, and getting something by threatening another person. These same items were used to an index of "aggressive or violent acts" in another study with a more com- prehensive sample(4). Much less frequently reported by the post-high-school males were hit- ting a parent or other adult and using a weapon to get something. The eight-item index, including these and the more frequent delinquent acts, was negatively related to indicators of SES(7). When it was substituted for the aggressiveness reports of school officials (see above), the results of the study were quite different. In addition to the five-item measure of "aggressive or violent acts" (see above), there were four other indices of delinquent behaviorin the Maryland study(4).At least one of them consists of fairly serious be- havior, as indicated by "involvement with legal officials." Inlieu of di- rect behavioral questions that might haveincriminated the respondents, 2,1 14 TELEVISION AND ADOI.ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

they were asked how often they had been stopped by police, taken to the police station. arrested, brought to juvenile court, and spent time in a juvenile facility. The other three indices were "petty delinquent acts" (trespassing, intentionally damaging school property). "defiance of par- ents" (staying out late, running away from home. arguing or fighting with parents. drinking without permission), and "political action" (join- ing a sit-in, asking a school official to change a policy or course). These latter three measures are dropped from portions of the reported analy- ses, and are clearly less "aggressive" in nature. The authors do not report intercorrelations among these five mea- sures, but they do assess each of them in terms of several demographic indicators (4). On both of the measures of serious deviance, boys report a higher incidence of aggressive acts than girls. SES is negatively related to legal involvement, and accounts for racial differences on that mea- sure. On the measure of aggressive acts, SES is also negatively correlat- ed among white respondents, but positively correlated among blacks. Two batteries of self-report items were adapted from previous re- search to supplement teacher-mother-peer measures in one study (5a). One is a six-item measure called "manifest physical aggression," in which four of the items consist of admitting hitting someone. The other two items indicate a readiness to fight when provoked. A response scale based on the degree to which the item is "like me" is used. The second scale, "aggressive behavioral delinquency," consists of three items on the frequency of fighting. These two scales correlated fairly well with one another and with a number of other self-report aggression measures in two samples (5a). Both show the expected boy-girl difference, and clearly decline with age in adolescence. They are unrelated to SES, but in one sample are nega- tively correlated with school performance. The authors combine them with several other measures to construct major indiceslar their later analyses (see below). The final (thirteenth-grade) wave of the longitudinal study (3) included a number of self-reported aggression and delinquency items. These in- cluded two three-item indices of "aggressive habit" and a measure of "antisocial behavior" consisting of 26 delinquent acts. The respondents were also asked how often they had been arrested. These measures all correlated positively with earlier peer-rated aggressiveness indices and with "psychopathology." But the authors prefer the "external" peer- ratings for their main analyses and rely very little on these self-reported acts in drawing their inferences. Feelings of aggressiveness Whereas reports by other persons may be more objective than self- reports where overt behavior is concerned, only the respondent himself 22 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 15 can explain how he "feels"including the extent to which he feels like behaving aggressively, or his internal reactions to aggression. Four sets of authors included such mentalistic measures in their studies. Two studies used the generalized aggressiveness inventory devised by Zaks and Walters (1959). This measure includes several items that ap- pear only obliquely related to aggression on theirface, but its authors report satisfactory validity checks (Walters and Zaks, 1959). Their full 12-item battery was administered in both the third and thirteenth grades in the longitudinal study (3). The other study adapted seven of the 12 items to a modified response scale(5a).The 12-item version correlated posi- tively with "psychopathology" and negatively with "achievement" (3). The seven-item measure showed boys more aggressive than girls in only one of two samples and correlated rather weakly with otheraggressive- ness measures in both samples (5a). It is somewhat negatively correlated with SES and school performance(5a).Data from mothers on this mea- sure show positive correlations with children of both sexes at twograde levels (5b). It is negatively related to the degree of concept-orientation in the family's communication structure(5b).In all, this measure appears to be somewhat different in nature from more direct assessments of ag- gression, and is best employed as a supplementary index of adolescent aggressiveness; in both studies, the authors use it in that fashion. H ypothetical situations were posed as potential settings for aggressive behavior by the respondent in both these studies, and by the authors of another study(3: 5; 6). Inthe longitudinal study, 24 items assessed the degree to which the respondent said he would use punishment if he were the parent of an eight-year-old child (3). Hypothetical aggressive reac- tions to four possible conflict situations involving peers yielded an index that distinguished as well between sexes and grade levels as any direct report of aggressive behavior, and correlated well with the other mea- sures (5a). This measure was independent of SES and displayed incon- sistent patterns of correlation with school performance(5a).It was very slightly related to both dimensions of family communication structure (50. The other study (6) used four open-ended "What would you do if .?" items involving hypothetical conflict situations with peers, and coded the responses on a dichotomous scale. These measures were neg- atively related to SES for boys only, and positively correlated with per- ceived parental approval of aggression. Various items and indices from the Buss and Durkee (1957; Buss, 1961) inventory were employed in several studies. In one, it was con- cluded that only a few of the items discriminated validly between ag- gressive boys and others(2). Manyof the Buss-Durkee items represent a measurement borderline between feelings and actual behavior; they are included in this section because most of the authors did not ask their respondents for specific reports of their past behavior. 16 TE L EV ISM AND ADOI. ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

The study of preadolescents askedfor agree-disagreeresponses on five Buss-Durkee items and combinedthese into an index of "willing- ness to uie violence" (6). As was thecase with the measure based on hypothetical conflict situations (seeabove), aggre ssive responseswere negatively related to SES for boys only,and were associated withparen- tal approval of aggression. Buss-Durkee items were used in anotherstudy (5a) to constructa four-item index of "assault aggression"and a three-item index of "irri- tability." The assault aggressionmeasure, which included three of the six items from the "manifestphysical aggression"measure (see self- reported behavior, above), correlatedwell with all other aggressiveness indices. It also yielded the expecteddistinctions between sexes and grade levels (males andyounger adolescents being more aggressive), whereas irritability correlated weakly withother aggressiveness indices and was somewhat higher for females.Neither measure correlatescon- sistently with SESor school performance (5a). Mother-child correla- tions are mostly positive for assaultaggression, but not for irritability (5b). Generally, then, the irritabilitymeasure seems to tap a dimension that is rather different frommost of the indices under review here. A four-item Buss-Durkee index of"verbal aggression" was employed as a supplementary measure in this study, andas expected it was higher among boys (5b). However, it did not correlatestrongly with measures of aggressive overt behavior,being instead apparentlya manifestation of aggressive inner feelings. The longitudinal study (3) includeda four-item measure called "ag- gressive drive" thatwas mostly verbal in the nature of aggressiveness represented. However, no relationshipsbetween this and other varia- bles are reported, presumably becausethey were not significant. Ingen- eral, verbal expressions of hostilityappear to be orthogonal to physical- ly aggressive behavior, perhapsproviding a functionally equivalent "outlet" for aggressive feelings. Approval of aggressionor violence was measured in variousways by three sets of authors of thepresent studies. The simplestwas the degree of agreement with two items thatsuggested conditions under which "it's all right to hit an enemy" (52).Boys ranked higher than girlson this measure, and it correlated rather well withmeasures of aggressive be- havior and feelings. Itwas negatively correlated with SES and school perform ance in two samples andshowed positive mother-childcorrela- tions (5b). An eight-item approval-of-violence indexwas used in the study of preadolescents (6). The form of themeasure was agreement with state- ments such as, "It's all right ifa man slaps his wife." Another index that is in the general vein of "approval" inthis study is the "perceivedeffec- tiveness of violence," a five-itemmeasure using such statementsas, "A fight is the best way to settlean argument once and for all." Both these 24 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 17 indices correlated negatively with SES for boys only and positively with parental approval of violence for both sexes. The most extensive analysis of approval of violence broke it down into three aggressor roles: adult males, teenage males, and police offi- cers (4). Five possible provoking situations were posed (e.g., a stranger had broken into the man's house, or had knocked him down and was trying to rob him), and the respondent was asked if he approved of an adult male punching or shooting the stranger. The teenage measure was similar, involving punching or knifing another teenage male under var- ious provoking circumstances. The policeman questions asked about striking or shooting an adult male in response to insults or criminal acts. The authors found no consistent relationships between thesemeasures and either sex or race. This study (4) included one additional index that is at least partly based on inner feelings and perceptious (although it has some basis in objective reality as well). This is the respondent's estimate of the fre- quency of various crimes of violence in his neighborhood, in an adjacent metropolitan center, and in "this part of the country." This measure was related to firsthand experience as a crime victim, but only for the local-neighborhood crime level. Interestingly, the authors report that adolescent belief in a high crime rate beyond the local neighborhood is "nearly unanimous." The crime-rate estimates are higher among older adolescents and whites, but are unrelated to SES or sex.

CORRELATIONS BETWEEN VIEWING AND AGGRESSIVENESS The foregoing review of the major variables in the present studies has established a number of similarities between measures of violence view- ing and measures of aggressiveness. There is some evidence that,both these kinds of behaviors are more common early in adolescence, among males, in the lower socioeconomic strata, and among those with lesser intellectual capabilities. But nOne of those relationships is especially strong or consistent across different samples and measures. In this final section of the paper, the focus turns to the direct issue that has been purposely avoided until this point: empirical relationships between vio- lence viewing and aggressiveness. Two opposing (although not mutually incompatible) research hy- potheses bring us to this juncture. Put very simply, it could be hypothe- sized that aggressiveness leads an adolescent to prefer violent programs, or that the experience of seeing violent programs develops aggressive tendencies in the youngster. Schramm et al. (1961) accepted the first hypothesis and rejected the seconds because they could find no differ- ences in aggressiveness bctween youngsters lin a town with television and a town that lacked it. 25 18 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Absence of correlation between the two variables, or a negative corre- lation, would provide evidence against both hypotheses. Positive corre- lations can be interpreted either way, or as evidence consistent with other hypotheses in which some third factor "causes" both violence viewing and aggressiveness. Correlations that remain when other varia- bles have been partialed out can be taken as evidence that those specific variables are not platriible "third factors" for alternative hypotheses of this latter type. To attempt to make some further use of the present findings for causal interpretations, the present review is divided into two sections. The first examines correlations between aggressiveness and preferences for vio- lent programs, on the premise that this is the most appropriate "depend- ent variable" to consider in testing the hypothesis that aggressiveness induces tendencies to watch violent programs. The second section re- views correlations between aggressiveness and actual viewing of violent programs, a measure that is much closer to the kind of "independent variable" that often leads to minor forms of aggression in laboratory experiments. The distinction between these two types of measures is scarcely iron- clad, of course. We should assume that program preferences correlate rather well with actual viewing, although the study showing comparative data demonstrates marked discrepancies between the two measures (Lyle and Hoffman, 1971). And in the only other research that reports indices of both measures, the correlation between them is a modest .25 (Chaffee and McLeod, 1971). But the scientific purposes of this total body of studies will be best served if we can attempt to eliminate one hypothesis at a time. If some rival hypotheses can be eliminated by dis- confirming evidence, the presumptive case for the other hypotheses that "survive" becomes somewhat stronger. At the same time, however, purely correlational evidence cannot "prove" or "confirm" a hypothe- sis in a positive sense. We are necessarily limited to the cumbersome and plodding procedure of attempting tofalsifyeach alternative hypothesis. A final section of the paper will consider possible causal in- ferences more fully. Correlations with violent program preferences Four of the present studies relate a violence index based on the youngster's "favorite" programs to one or .more measures of aggres- siveness (2; 3; 4; 7). Although all the authors infer that there is some kind of overall correlation, they note that their evidence is not particu- larly strong or consistent. In another study,,the authors were so disap- pointed by the lack of correlation between violent program preference and many aggressiveness measures that they omitted the data from their report, and relied instead on measures of actual viewing (.5). Subsequent analyses that include the viewing-preference measures from that study

26 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 19 have since been reported (Chaffee and McLeod, 1971), and areincorpo- rated into this paper. Only one study reports findings for both kinds of viewing measure (2); the results for actual viewing are reviewed in the next section ofthis paper. The preference measure was asingle item asking the youngster to list his four favorite programs. The sample was equally divided between 40 "aggressives" (as nominated by school officials) and 40 other stu- dents, all urban junior high males. The aggressive boys listed an average of 1.8 violent programs, while the corresponding figure for the nonag- gressives was 1.6. Given the small Ns, this is not a particularly impres- sive difference; no significance test is reported. Larger differences were found on some related measures, such as a greater tendency for the nonaggressives to list "family comedy" shows and to complain that tel- evision offers too much violence and too little comedy. A second measure of aggressiveness, based on a battery ofself-report items about aggressive feelings, produced somewhat stronger results (2). When the sample was split on this index, the aggressives were found to have listed an average of 2.2 violent programs, compared to1.5 for the less aggressives. Regardless of the aggressiveness measure used, the aggressives were found to spend more time watching television, to be more selective in their television use, to pay moreattention to newsv- per stories about robberies and civildisordersand to prefer listening to music over watching television. There is a danger of overinterpreting such findings, since the sampleis so small and specialized and was clearly notselected by anything ap- proaching a random process. Moreover, another member of the same research group has suggested that it is not the violent content that spe- cifically accounts for the varying program preferences that are reported (8). Using similar data and a circumplicial analysis, he suggestsinstead that it is the social role of the major male character in theshow that draws a young adolescent boy's attention. Such a youngsteris in need of a "model for manhood" that isappropriate to his ascribed social status and personality, it is argued. Aggressiveness is a manifestationof a gen- erally "active" personality, which in turn is drawn to observe adultmale protagonists who actively deal with the problems that confront them each week; their methods of coping frequently involveviolence, but not necessarily. An analogous line of reasoning, to account for black-white differences in terms of the male lead's social standing in the program ,is also suggested. The study of post-high-school males ( 7) also found a positive correla- tion between an index of violence in the youngster's four favorite pro- grams and an aggressiveness measure. In this case aggression was in- dexed by eight items self-reporting specific delinquent acts. The largest differences (between those with "a great deal" of violence in their fa- vorite programs and those with "almost none") are found on items 20 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS reporting fighting. (The difference was. strangely.strongest among those who spend very little time watching te!evision.) But when the datawere controlled for aggressiveness indicated ina similar measure the previous year, the differences disappeared except among those who had initially been very aggressive. As the authors point out, this does not necessarily render their findings less disturbing socially. If highly aggressiveyoung men find "reinforcement" for aggressive behavior in the television pro- grams they select, there is ample reason for concern about such pro- grams. And as Klapper (1963) has stated, the usual effect of media vio- lence is to reinforce behavioral tendencies regardless of whether they are "socially wholesome or socially unwholesome." The term "rein- force" generally means that "the probability thata response will recur" in a similar situation has been increased (English and English, 1963). The reinforcement of aggressive behavioral tendencies, whatever their ori- gin, may indeed be considered socially unwholesome bymany. The longitudinal study (3) provides two kinds of viewing-aggressive- ness correlations, synchronous and time-lagged. Surprisingly, the latter data are much stronger than the former. (The strongest data, the time- lagged correlations stemming from the third-grade viewingmeasure, will be taken up in the next section. since they are interpreted hereas mea- sures of actual viewing, not of viewing preference.) At both points in adolescence, in fact, the synchronous correlations between aggressiveness and stated preferences for violentprograms are slightly negative. The time-lagged correlation from eighth-grade viewing preference to thirteenth-grade aggressiveness is positive, however. The eighth-grade data, being drawn from an inadequate subsample of the study's total panel, are inconclusive. The more important longitudinal findings involve the third-grade parental report of the child's viewing (below). It should be noted here, however, that there isno correlation between third-grade aggressiveness and thirteenth-grade violencepro- gram preferences, which there presumably should have been if aggres- siveness were a 9'cause" of viewing preference. Rather weak overall relationshipsare shown in the other study using a "favorite program" measure (4). In this large-samplesurvey spanning most of the adolescent years, the incidence of "high deviance"on five different measures increases by only a few percentage points from "low violence, favorite program" to "high violence, favorite program."For the two measures that most clearly involve serious aggressive behavior, the differences are 2.5 percent and 1.1 percent. When themore reliable measure "average violence rating of four favorite shows" is used, how- ever, the differences are much more convincing: 11.1 percent and 10.2 percent, respectively. This justifies the authors' conclusion that the rela- tionship is positive, albeit weak. In their subsequent partialing analyses they show only the less-reliable single-program measure. These partialed 28 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 11 tables fairly consistently demonstrate that the association between pro- gram preference and behavioral deviance is limited to one of their four major subgroups: male whites. The trend is in the opposite direction for female whites and male blacks, and varies between measures for female blacks. Differerwes on the two principal measures of deviant behavior tend to vanish when perceptions of the instrumental role of violence in the favorite program are controlled; only eight of 16 comparisons are in a positive direction, which is the number that would be expected by chance alone. Controls for demographic and aspirational variables do not appreciably alter the authors' conclusion that the preference-devi- ance correlation is a weak one. Feelings about violence, as indexed by approval of adult, teen, and police violence, are all weakly but positively correlated with a prefer- ence for a violent "favorite" program (4). Breaking the sample down by race and gender, however, shows tha., these findings also hold only for white males. There is no relationship between estimated frequency of violent crime and preference for violent programs, a finding that holds up under a variety of controls for other variables. A consistent thread runs through these diverse studies. in that evi- dence of any link between adolescent aggressiveness and preference for television violence is limited to white males. That is certainly the domi- nant conclusion to be reached from the only sample with large numbers of respondents of both sexes and races (4). There are no relationships in the longitudinal study (3), the synchronous correlations being slightly negative for both sexes. In the large-sample national survey of post- high-school boys. the relationship is monotonic (although weak) for whites, but not for blacks (2). And in the small-sample study of equal numbers of black and white males in junior high, the whites listed more violent shows among their favorites than the blacks; this was true of both aggressives and nonaggressives, as determined by two very differ- ent measures (2). One rather simple explanation for this specific pattern has been sug- gested in the present studies. The overwhelming majority of violent acts on evening series television programs are committed by white males. So if program preferences are guided to any extent by the search for adult role models (see 8). it is the aggressive white male adolescent who would be most likely to be peculiarly drawn to shows that frequently depict violence. The general absence of more positive findings for other sectors of the population in these field studies may. then, be partly an artifact of the kinds of program content that are to be found on television. With more detailed research attention to specific sources of attraction to spe- cific programs, including those that are favored by smaller portions of the total audience, it might be possible to establish closer relationships than the present studies suggest. 29 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Correlations with viewing of violent programs

Four studies present new data relating various measures of exposure to violent television content to several indicators of aggressiveness (2: 3: 5: 6). In one small-sample study of junior high boys (2) the results are mixed. at. most. Those boys designated as "aggressives" by school offi- cials reported that they spent more time viewing than did the other youngsters. but they also indicated less viewing of individual programs from a list of evening shows. The lists of ten-most-watched shows for these two groups differ in that there is one more violent program on the aggressives' lists, plus considerably less viewing of family comedy shows. However, when a self-report aggressiveness measure was sub- stituted for the school-report. these differences disappeared. In the study of preadolescent boys and girls (6). the violence-viewing measure consists of the number of programs watched each week, from the list of 20-most-violent (Greenberg and Gordon, 1971). Since all the data tables are partialed on SES and parental attitudes toward violence, they provide in effect a total of eight replications of the viewing-aggres.- siveness relationship in different samples of youngsters. On the measure 1 of willingness to use violence, there were significant positive relation- ships with violence viewing for both sexes; the means are in this direc- tion for three of the four subsamples of boys and all four subsamples of girls. On the measure of suggested use of violence in hypothetical con- flict situations, the results are significant for the girls only, and in the positive direction for all four subsamples. For boys the difference is nonsignificant, holds for only two of four subsamples, and is entangled in an uninterpreted three-way interaction with SES and parental approv- al of violence. There are strong main effects of violence viewing on the perceived effectiveness of violence, for both sexes and in seven of eight subsamples; violence viewing does not interact with other variables in either study. The fourth criterion measure, approval of aggression, is not related to violence viewing for either sex. In the study that provides the greatest number of measures of aggres- siveness, and of replications in different subsamples, the correlations with violence viewing are overwhelmingly positive (5a). The correla- tions with an overall index of self-reported aggression are significant in two widely separated communities. For the four subtests that make up this overall index, all eight correlations are positive and seven signifi- cantly so. The partialed data by sex and grade level provide a total of 38 correlation coefficients, of which 35 are positive, 12 significantly; none of the three negative correlations approaches significance. All told, this is a fairly impressive array of evidence, considering the smallness of Ns. These self-report aggressiveness measures are also correlated to an extent with time spent viewing (5a). But when viewing time is con- trolled, the pattern of positive correlation between violence view:ng and 30 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 23 aggressiveness remains in 34 of the 38 subsample entries. The most strongly related aggressiveness index is the one based on hypothetical situations; it is perhaps noteworthy, though, that this does not hold for the younger males, a finding that corresponds to the study of preadoles- cent boys (6). Unlike the findings reviewed earlier on program prefer- ence, there is no appreciable sex difference. There is, however, an inter- action between sex and grade level, in that the overall partial correlation is stronger (in both samples) for girls at the junior high level but for boys in senior high (5a). This interaction, and the overwhelmingly positive trend of the overall data, tend to hold up well when controls for S ES and school performance are instituted. Viewing of any type of program tends to be correlated with the overall self-report aggressiveness measure, which might be expected from the correlation with viewing time (5a). However, when viewing of other types of programs is controlled, only specific exposure to crime-detec- tive, adventure-drama, and Saturday morning programs retains a partial correlation with aggressiveness. There are several variables that, added to violence viewing, increase the likelihood of high self-reported aggres- siveness; these include the perceived learning of aggression from televi- sion and irritability. Controls for these two variables tend to reduce the power of violence viewing to predict aggressiveness, but it remains sig- nificant. Another variable that reduces the correlation is parental em- phasis on nonaggressive behavior; even when this kind of constraint operates on the youngster, however,there remains a significant correla- tion between violence viewing and aggressiveness. These factors should be allocated to different roles in an attempt to explain what accounts for the viewing:aggressiveness correlations. Irri- tability would seem to be a contingent condition, perhaps one that in- creases the likelihood that viewing violence would instigate aggression. Learning of aggressive behavior, by contrast, is part of the hypothetical process by which exposure to violence might be translated into aggres- sion. And finally, parental emphasis on nonaggression is a form of social control that could be instituted to modify any undesirable influences of violence viewing. This last area is taken up more fully later. When the measure of aggressiveness is based on reports by persons other than the youngster himself, the results are less strong, but they are still positive for all measures except mother-report and significantly positive for the overall index constructed from the various reports by others (5b). This low positive correlation with the overall other-report index holds for both sexes and grade levels, which can be thought of as separate replications of the finding. It also holds up when time spent viewing, SES, and school performance are controlled, although the rela- tionship is limited to junior high girls and senior high boys. Only when irritability, perceived learning of aggression from television, and paren- tal affection and punishment are simultaneously controlled does the par- tial correlation of violence viewing and other-report aggressiveness drop 31 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS to zero for the junior high subsample and to a nonsignificant level for the overall sample(5b). Perhaps the best single index of aggressiveness is the gran, combina- tion of self-reports and those by peers, mothers, and teachers. This measure is significantly correlated with overall violence viewing, even when irritability, perceived learning of aggression from television, and parental affection and punishment are simultaneously controlled(5b). Although not based on adolment viewing, the data from the longitu- dinal study provide some ofthemost impressive evidence of a link be- tween violence viewing and aggressiveness. The third-grade parent-re- port viewing measure correlates significantly with third-grade aggres- siveness and evenmore stronglywith thirteenth-grade aggressiveness for boys (3). (In female sample, both relationships are null and even slightly negative.) This time-lagged evidence of time-order is even more impressive in light of the fact that third-grade aggressiveness does not predict thirteenth-grade aggressiveness much better than third-grade violence viewing does. The parent-report of boys' viewing also corre- lates positively with their self-reported antisocial behavior and "psycho- pathology" ten years later. The authors conclude that "exposure to a diet of violent television" as a child is "a probable cause of the expres- sion of aggressive behavior" in late adolescence, for males. Problems of causal inference will be taken up more fuRy later in this paper. There is other evidence that violence viewing at an earlier age is more closely associated with aggressiveness than is the adolescent's present level of violence viewing. Retrospective self-reported viewing corre- lates somewhat more strongly with the self-report aggressiveness mea- sure(5b)and much more strongly with the combined self-other measure in a multiple regression analysis that also involves two other good pre- dictors of aggressiveness. Approval of aggression, a variable that was unrelated to violence viewing in the study of preadolescents (6);is mildly positively related in this study(5b). Itis more strongly correlated with violence viewing at a younger age than at present. To summarize the findings of this key section of the paper, the differ- ent subsamples from the present studies that relate violence viewing to aggressiveness are roughly compared in Table I. Samples of boys have been separated from samples of girls, and each group has been arrayed in order of grade level. For the boys, there are positive relationships in five of six samples, plus the time-lagged positive relationship from the longitudinal study (3). For the girls, out of six samples only the data from the longitudinal study (3) are null. The pattern of stronger correla- tions for the younger girls and the older boys can also be seen in a gener- al fashion in this table. For comparison, Table 2 summarizes the analogous relationships found between aggressiveness and self-reported preferences for vio- ,1111,

TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 25

Table 1: Summary of correlations between violence viewing and aggressiveness

Self-report Other-report Study Locale Grade N aggressive aggressive behavior behavior

Samples of boys

(3) New York 3 211 ++ 161 Michigan 4-56 434 ++ 151 Wisconsin 6-7 38 + + (5) Maryland 7 122 + 0 121 Maryland 8-9 80 0 161 Wisconsin 9-10 43 + ++ 151 Maryland 10 107 ++ ++

Samples of girls

(3) New York 3 216 0 (a) Michigan 4-5-6 404 +++ (5) Wisconsin 6-7 30 ++ ++ (i) Maryland 7 108 ++ ++ (5) Wisconsin 9-10 40 + ++ 151 Maryland 10 136 + +

Note: Cell entries indicate presence of positive (+) or null MI correlation between the amount of violence viewing reported by the adolescent, and an aggressiveness index based on the type of report listed in the column heading. Stronger or more consistent positive relationships are indicated by repeating the sign (+4 ). These are very approximate esti- mates of the strength of the evidence that the correlation is non-zero.

lence viewing. Since rather few analyses had been reported. Table 2 also includes findings from a later report (Chaffee and McLeod, 1971) based on data from one of these studies (5). Among the samples of boys, null relationships are reported ir four of the eight cases; the other four are positive but low (the highest correlation coefficient iS + .13). No positive relationships are reported for any of the five samples of girls. While one hesitates to generalize when most. of the samples are from one state (Maryland), the relationship appears to be peculiar to the younger white males. Overall. Table 2 contrasts markedly with Table 1, where the cor- relations were stronger and not specific to any sex or age group. {,Race was not coded in the samples in Tabie 1.)

Obviously Table 1 oversimplifies a great deal of complex data ,:nd obscures a number of nitx distinctions. Assuming that these mattters have been dealt with sufficiently already in this paper, we can at titis stage direct our attention to the central fact of Table 1. There is clearly a preponderance of eviderice in these studies to support the conclusion that adolescent aggressix eness and the viewing of violent television pro- grams are statistically associated. This relationship is considerably more impressive and pervasiVe than the weak and limited correlations be- tween aggressiveness and expressed preference for violent program 3, which are shown in Table 2. Where tested by partialing out additional

33 26 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 2: Summary of correlations between aggressiveness and preference for violent programs

Selfreport Otherreport Locale Study Grade aggressive aggressive (race) behavior behavior

Samples of boys

(5) Maryland 7 122 + (2) Maryland 8-9 80 + o+ (4) Maryland 712 857 0+ (white) (4) Maryland 7-12 125 0 (black) (5) Maryland 10 107 0 (7) Nationwide 12 1351 + (white) (7) Nationwide 12 167 0 (black) (3) New York 13 211

Samples of girls

(5) Maryland 7 108 0 (4) Maryland 7-12 963 0 (white) (4) Maryland 7-12 159 0 (black) (5)* Maryland 10 136 0 () New York 13 216 0

Note: Cell entries indicate positive (+), negative () or null (0) correlation between amount of violence in the child's self-reported "favorite" programs, and an aggressiveness index of the listed type. Asterisk (') indicates later analyses that do not appear in the report here, but are shown in Chaffee and McLeod (1971). variables, the positive relationships in Table 2 tended to vanish, whereas those in Table I did not. In all, then, there seems to be a strong associa- tion between aggressiveness and violence viewing that cannot be ex- plained by an intervening preference f or violent programs. Possible causal inferences Only one of the present studies (3) ventures so far as to state an infer- ence of positive, unidirectional causation, and even that is in terms of violence viewing as a "probable cause" of adolescent aggressiveness among boys only. Accordingly, let us examine the supporting data in that case first. Surprisingly, it is one of the few studies that find no syn- chronous correlations within adolescence, between a viewing measure and an aggressiveness measure. The positive evidence is entirely time- lagged, and there are no instances of an aggressiveness measure predict- 34 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 27 ing a delayed effect on a viewing measure. The positive findings are ei- ther synchronous within childhood or time-lagged from childhood view- ing to adolescent aggressiveness ten years later. It is this last finding, of course, that provides the single basis of the causal claim by the authors. The finding holds only for males. (It is the only sex-specific relationship between two such measures in the present studies.) The authors take a decidedly statistical approach to interpretation, basing their inference on several variants of cross-lagged correlation. The time-lagged correlation of + .31 is the strongest of the four correla- tions relating a viewing preference measure to an aggressionmeasure. As they point out, such data stand up under several statistical analyses. The simple cross-lagged comparison shows + .31 vs. .01. When third- grade aggression levels are controlled, the partial correlation is still + .21. When the authors make reasonable estimates of reliability and appiy the Rozelle and Campbell (1961) baseline, the + .31 figure is substantial- ly above the baseline of + .06; the latter, in turn, is only slightly greater than the reverse-time correlation of .01, which would suggest that it is unlikely that third grade aggressiveness diminishes the youngster's pref- erence for violent television ten years later. Yet another procedure, which the authors did not attempt, would be to apply Bohrnstedt's (1969) formula that combines the cross-lagged and partial tests (see also Chaffee et al., 1970). That approach would yield time-lagged correla- tions of + .35 (p. <.00I) for boys, and .10 (n.s.) for girls. Those fig- ures would seem to provide an even more conclusive case for longitudi- nal causal inference. But models of statistical inference are no more valid than the assump- tions underlying them. And in the case of all the time-lagged models reported in the previous paragraph, the assumptions are quite difficult to meet in one respect: the measures of the "same" variable at different times must be equivalent. On a narrow statistical basis this is dubious in this study. It is not unlikely that the two measures of viewing preference differ from one another in reliability, and perhaps in their approxima- tions to a normal distribution. The third-grade measures are based on "three favorite programs," for instance, whereas "four favorite pro- grams" provide the basis for the thirteenth-grade index; more items generally improve reliability, as was found in a similar situation in an- other study ( 4). For peer-rated aggressiveness, differential reliabilities are even more likely, since many more peers could nominate a given youngster in the thirteenth-grade measure than in the third-grade ver- sion. In the third grade, one's peers were limited to those in his class- room; in the thirteenth-grade measure, all other respondents in the en- tire sample could conceivably nominate a given boy as aggressive. An increase in the number of "expert" judges could increase reliability of the measure appreciably, which would account for its relating more strongly to a third variable (i.e., third-grade viewing) than did the third- grade aggressiveness measure. (Inthe absence of anyreliability 28 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

coefficients, one can only guess at this, of course.) It is also unlikely that the distribution of aggressive behavior at age eight is particularly similar to the distribution for the same boys ten years later. However. Kenny (1971) has concluded, on the basis of an ingenious iest involvg correla- tions with a third variable (IQ). that the third-grade aggressiveness mea- sure is probably not less reliable than the thirteenth-grade measure. Pending more direct evidence, then, the issue appears unresolved. Equivalence of these measures in terms of substantive meaning and validity is an even less tenable assumption, setting aside statistical nice- ties of reliability and distribution. The measures simply are not the same, even on their face. Parent-report program preferences cannot be assumed equivalent to self-reported preferences, and the two measures are uncorrelated longitudinally. Peer nominations of likely social aggres- sion "now" (the third-grade measure) cannot be assumed equivalent to similar statements about some time in the past (the thirteenth-grade measure). Most importantly, perhaps, the social background and con- text of both variables are surely different in the two time periods. To begin with, television program scirca 1960 quite probably contained more violence than ten years later (Clark and Blankenburg, 1971). Eight- year-olds do not watch the same set of programs, nor even the same time slots, as they will in late adolescence. Television is a relatively new experience, of a few years' duration typically, in the life of a third-grad- er; it is a familiar artifact of daily living, and one that he is growing away from, for the usual "thirteenth" grader. The developmental differences regarding television demonstrated by Schramm et al. (1961), and repli- cated and amplified by Lyle and Hoffman (1971), suggest strongly that even seemingly identical questions will not have the same meaning to youngsters at ten-year intervals in their growth. But it ig the aggressiveness measures that are more critical, since the thirteenth-grade viewing preferences can be ignored insome modes of longitudinal analysis. Developmental change in the personal and social meaning of aggressive behavior must be massive, and constitutes the most serious single threat to the assumption of equivalent measurement. This point can perhaps be amply demonstrated by considering the differ- ent meanings, to children and to late adolescents, of some of the items that were used: saying "mean things," making "unfriendly gestures," pushing or shoving students, taking other students' things without ask- ing, "always getting into trouble," starting fights "over nothing." The fact that the social meaning of these items in adolescencemay be quite different from their meaning in childhood does not necessarily render the measures noncomparable. The third-and thirteenth-gradeaggres- siveness indices do, after all, correlate .38, so they surely havesome- thing in com mon. (One cannot say the same for the corresponding view- ing measures.) These developmental and metrical problems can scarcely be blamed on the researchers. Their attempts to gather equivalent measures over 36

-cytoy TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 29 time could not be substantially improved upon. The "fault" instead seems to be inherent in longitudinal research that cuts across radically different phases in child development. At about the time that the third- grade data were being gathered, Schramm et al. (1961: pp.186-88) con- cluded their major volume on television and children with the suggestion that a ten-year longitudinal study much like the present one was "of first importance, if we are to push steadily ahead in understanding the uses children make of television." Now that a skilled research team has per- severed long enough to complete such a study, it can be seen that this research model held out a false promise. The tools for causal inference from nonexperimental data require assumptions that will be met for few, if any, of the variables that interest us in developmental research. (Schramm et al. also suggested that many more variables should be in- cluded in the ten-year study, but that would not have alleviated the basic problem that is faced here.) A longitudinal study extending over two or three years within a homogeneous life-cycle period (either childhood or adolescence, but not from one to the other) would seem highly desira- ble. While not quite as ambitious in its total scope as the present study ( 3), it would provide a somewhat less ambiguous test of the causal hy- pothesis. When all that is said, however, and the formal time-lagged tests are abandoned (which seems appropriate), an undeniable fact remains: one of the strongest correlation coefficients in all of the present studies is the +.31 between this third-grade parent-report viewing preference mea- sure and retrospective peer-rated aggressiveness ten years later. The first index is, as indicated above, at least questionable; the latter is one of the very few aggressiveness measures that fails to correlate with an adolescent viewing measure in the present studies. With such fragile measures and yet such a healthy correlation, one must suspect that "something" has been going on in this panel of young men. Although a simple causal viewing:aggressiveness function cannot reasonably be considered proven, neither can it be easily rejected. Of the various pos- sible explanations for the finding, it has parsimony on its side, and it is probably the most commonly held "folk hypothesis" about television violence. At this stage in research on television violence, the present longitudinal finding (in the context of a wide variety of supportive exper- imental and field studies) appears to stand as the strongest evidence on behalf of the main causal hypothesis. Any challenge to that hypothesis must come from an alternative ex- planation that is at least equally consistent with the data. One possibility would be "simultaneous" causation by a third variable, whose effect on television preferences occurs in childhood but which does not manifest itself in social aggressiveness until well into adolescence. One such vari- able might be a hypothetical personality trait we could call "attraction to

7 30 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS adult forms of aggression." An eight-year-old boy cannot very effective- ly aggress in adult fashion, but if that sort of behavior fascinates him he may well enjoy watching it on television. Some years later, as he ma- tures physically and acquires various social and combative skills, he should find more situations in which to act out his "predisposition" to adult forms of aggression. Meanwhile, television is a progressively less likely locus for the manifestation of this trait; he spends less time watch- ing it,there are fewer violent scenes, and he has become more or less habituated to the stock cliches of television aggression. Hence we could expect a time-lagged correlation but little or no synchronous correlation in adolescence. All of that is wholly speculative, and a competent developmental psy- chologist could surely improve on it as an alternative explanation. It is sketched out briefly here, simply to demonstrate that plausible explana- tions other than the direct causal hypothesis can account for the data that have been reported. Future research could profitably explore this area of the different sociopsychological meanings of both television and aggressive behavior at different stages in childhood and adolescence. To the time-honored call for "more research" it seem s prudent to add the proviso that future studies should be designed to eliminate alterna- tive explanations that challenge the hypothesis that viewing violence induces aggression. The present studies are of some further help in this regard. When two variables are statistically associated, either could be "caus- ing" the other, without reference to third variables. As suggested ear- lier, any influence of aggressiveness as a "cause" of viewing should be indicated most clearly in an adolescent's preferences for violent pro- grams. The evidence from these studies is that any such link is limited to white males, the particular demographic type that is most likely to be seen behaving aggressively on television. When, on the other hand, we examine correlations between aggressiveness and actual viewing of vio- lence, there is no sex difference. The positive findings here are, then, consistent in a rough fashion with those of experimental studies where exposure to media violence has been manipulated. But that inference is based on a "face-validity" distinction between the two media violence measures, preference and exposure. It would be much preferable to examine each of these with the other held constant, before more than tentative conclusions are drawn. If one were limited to a choice between the two possible two-variable causal hypotheses, there is decidedly more support here for the viewing- induces-aggressiveness interpretation than for the ,reverse. The data supporting that conclusion have survived a number of statistical con- trols for likely "third variables," but of course there is a potentially infi- nite list of such additional factors that might account for the correlations that have been found. TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 31 Potential approaches to social control

The authors of these papers have tended to concern themselves pri- marily with detailed presentation of original data, and secondarily with testing the hypothesis that exposure to television violence increases the f likelihood of aggressive social behavior among adolescents. They have generally not addressed the question of what might be done about this hypothetical influence, if indeed it does exist. As indicated in the pre- vious section, we have in these studies considerably stronger evidence for the hypothesis than had been the case before. So without declaring flatly that the hypothesis of aggressive effects of television violence is "true" in a scientific sense, we might well assume it to be true as a tenta- tive proposition and direct some attention to potential forms of social control over this relationship. In one of these studies, families were divided according to the degree to which the parents emphasized nonaggressive behavior by their young- sters. This parental constraint on aggression, without direct reference to television, appears to reduce the viewing-aggressiveness correlations consistently for eight subsamples of varying grades, sexes, and locales (5a). In subsequent analyses reported elsewhere (Chaffee and McLeod, 1971) these authors found more mixed results from parental controls directed at television itself. Limitations by the parent on the programs the child could view also diminish the viewing-aggressiveness correla- tion, but rather few parents appear to control adolescent viewing in this fashion. Parental discussion of television violence, as an attempt to "in- terpret" its meaning in relation to real life, does not appear to affect the adolescent viewing-aggressiveness relationship at all; this latter type of parental behavior is probably rare. It is possible that either control might be effective if attempted more often. There also appears to be some danger that attempts to control adoles- cent aggression by minimizing violence viewing might "." That inference could be drawn from the widely discussed field experi- ment by Feshbach and Singer (1971). They manipulated the viewing of groups of junior high boys into "high violence" and "low violence" tel- evision conditions. Earlier here it was noted that violent programs are among those most-watched by junior high boys, and another study found a positive correlation between aggressiveness (self-reported) and parental control over the youngster's viewing ( 5b ). So we might expect the counternormative "low violence viewing" condition to be associat- ed with more negative affective reactions to the programs and with morc aggression rather than less; one might posit a frustration-aggres- sion hypothesis in making such a prediction. The "low violence" manip- ulation did produce both significantly more disliking of programs viewed (Feshbach and Singer, 1971) and more reported aggressive behavior. 39 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

The authors adopt a "catharsis" explanation, assuming that the "high violence" manipulation provided more covert outlets for aggressive impulses than the "low violence" manipulation. In the absence ofany direct indicators of the hypothetical construct of "catharsis," this inter- pretation remains conceivable but operationally moot. The catharsis hypothesis is ,inconsistent with the great bulk of evidence from thepres- ent studies. which instead suggest that the usual outcome ofexposure to television violence is more aggressive behavior, not less. In all, the question of effective social controls that might modifyso- cially undesirable aggression that could result from television violence remains practically untouched by research. It seems, then.an obvious direction for future study.

SUMMARY

What, in the light of these new studies,can be said of the scientific standing of the proposition that viewing of violent televisionprograms induces tendencies toward aggressive behavior in adolescents? Insever- al ways, that rather hazy hypothesis has been enhanced, in comparison with competing theories about the relationship between thesevariables. A significant positive correlation has been found muchmore often than not, and there is no negative correlational evidence. That correla- tion stands up consistently in varying samples of differentsexes, age levels, and locales, and with a variety of measures of aggressiveness. It persists in the face of attempts to partial outmany other variables that might have explained it away. And its'most obvious theoretical rival,the reverse causal hypothesis, has not fared nearly so well here. Measures of viewing preference, as opposed to actual viewing, relateto aggres- siveness only for white males, and generally quite weakly. Longitudinal data on this point are null, whereas longitudinal dataon the viewing-in- duces-aggressiveness hypothesis are aboutas strong as any of the syn- chronous correlational findings in any of these studies. All of this is unsurprising, perhaps, sinceone can hardly expect that media experiences would have absolutely no influenceon the social behavior of any developing child. A minor effecton rather few young- sters can produce positive correlations, and none of the present studies suggests that viewing television violence could account for,more than about ten percent of the total variance in themeasures of adolescent aggressiveness. Whether more precise and reliable measurementwould increase that figure appreciably remainsa question for future research. Meanwhile, the present studies have also demonstrated thatadoles- cent aggressiveness is associated with a number of other factors that have nothing to do with television. Their "effects" tend to remain when violence viewing is controlled statistically, and several of themare more strongly correlated with aggressiveness. These studies rather conclu- 40 l'....V.P...... '...... 0...... r....ell TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 33 sively eliminate the hypothesis that television violence is the sole, or principal, cause of aggressive behavior by adolescents. In all, it appears to make a relatively minor contribution. And the findings here cannot conclusively eliminate the possibility that this apparent contribution is an artifact of other causal processes that have yet to be discovered.

REFERENCES

Bohrnstedt, G.W. Observations on the measurement of change. In Bor- gatta, E.F. (Ed.)Sociological methodology 1969.San Francis- co: Jossey-Bass, 1969. Buss, A.H. Aggression and hostility inventories. InThe psychology of aggression.NewYork: John Wiley, 1961. Buss, A.H., and Durkee, A. An inventory for assessing different kinds ofhostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology,1957, 21, 343-49. Chaffee, S.H., and McLeod, J.M. Adolescent television use in the fami- ly context. InTelevision arid social behavior,Vol. 3 (tIiis volume). Referred to in this paper as (1). Chaffee, S.H., and McLeod, J.M. Adolescents, parents, and television violence. Paper presented at annual' meeting of the American Psy- chological Association, Washington, D.C., September 1971. Chaffee, S.H., McLeod, J.M.., add Atkin, C.K., Parental influences on adolescent media u se.American Behavioral Scientist,1971,14, 323- 40. Chaffee, S.H., Ward, L.S., and Tipton, L.P. Mass com munication and political sOcialization.Journalism Quarterly,1970, 47, 647-59. Clark, D.G., and Blankenburg,W.B. Trends in violent content in selected mass meclia. InTelevision and social behavior,Vol. 1 (this series). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Dominick, J.R., and Greenberg, B.S. Attitudes ,toward violence; the interaction of television exposure, family attitudes, and social class. InTelevision and social behavior,Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as ( 6.). English, H.B., and English, A.C.A comprehensive dictionary of psy- chological and psychoanalystical terms.New York: David McKay, 1958. , Fesh'ba.ch, S., and Singer, R.D.Television and aggression.San Francis- , co: Jossey-Bass, 1971. Friedman, H.L., and Johnson., R.L. Mass media use and aggression: a pilot study. InTelevision and social behayior,ygl. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as (2). Greenb.erg, B.S., and Gordon, T.F. Perceptions of violence in television programs: critics and the public. InTelevision and social behavior, Vol. 1(this series). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Governme,nt Printing Office, 1971. 34 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Johnson, R.L., Friedman, H.L., and Gross, H.S. Four masculine styles in television programming. In Television and social behavior. Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as ( 8). Klapper. J.T. The social effect of mass communication. In Schramm. W. (Ed.) The science of human communication. New York: Basic Books, 1963. Lefkowitz, Eron, L.D.. Walder, L.C., and Huesmann, L.R. Tel- evision violence and child aggression: a followup study. In Televi- sion and social behavior. Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as ( 3 ). LoSciuto, L.A. A national inventory of television viewing behavior. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 4 (this series). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Lyle, J., and Hoffman, H.R. Children's use of television and other me- dia. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 4 (this series). Washing- ton, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. McIntyre, J.J., and Teevan, J.J. Television and deviant behavior. In Television and social behavior. Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as ( 4), McLeod. J.M., Atkin, C.K.. and Chaffee, S.H. Adolescents, parents, and television use: adolescent self-report measures from Maryland and Wisconsin samples. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as (5a). McLeod. J.M.. Atkin. C.K. and Chaffee. S.H. Adolescents, parents. and television use: self-report and other-report measures from the Wisconsin sample. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as (5b). Murray, R.L., Cole, R.R.. and Fedler, F. Teenagers and TV vio- lence: how they rate and view it. Journalism Quarterly, 1970, 47, 247-55. Robinson, J.P., and Bachman, J.G. Television viewing habits and ag- gression. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 3 (this volume). Referred to in this paper as ( 7). Rozelle, R.M., and Campbell, D.T. More plausible hypotheses in the cross-lagged panel correlation technique. Psychological Bulletin. 1969, 71,74-80. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B. Television in the lives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961. Walters, R., and Zaks, M. Validation studies on an aggression scale. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 209-18. Zaks, M., and Walters, R. First steps in the construction of a scale for the measurement of aggression. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 199-208.

42 Television Violence and Child Aggression: A Followup Study

Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Leonard D. Eron, Leopold 0. Walder, and L. Rowell Huesmann

New York State Department of Mental Hygiene Albany, New York

The current projecti is a continuation of a project begun in 1955, enti- tled Psychosocial Development of Aggressive Behavior. The 1955 proj- ect was initiated by the research unit of the Rip Van Winkle Foundation in Columbia County, New York, and supported in part by USPHS Grant No. M1726. Because of the public health orientation of the host institu- tion, the research had originally been conceived as epidemiological and Preventive in nature. The charge to the research unit at that time was to study the prevalance of mental illness in a rural area, Columbia County in New York 'state. Applying epidemiological techniques to the a:.;a of mental health presented two related problems. One problem was arriv- ing at an adequate or appropriate definition of mental health; the other 35

43 36 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS was obtaining a representative sample of the population in which to measure this condition. A first step in resolving the definition problem was selecting for study one behavior which most investigators would accept as an aspect of mental health. This behavior had to be amenable to reliable observation and objective measurement. Aggression was considered to be such a variable. (In the current study the idea that ag-

gression is a facet of psychopathology is also, examined.) . The original definition of aggression was that developed by Dollard, Doob, Miller, Mowrer, and Sears (1939): "an act whose goalresponse is injury to another object." As the research progressed, however,a more limited definition developed: "an act which injures or irritates another person." This definition is concerned specifically with extiapunitive aggression directed toward another person (Walder, Abelson, Eron, Banta, and Laulicht, 1961). The sampling problem was resolved by the decision to studya 100 percent sample of a specific population. All the children in the third grade in Columbia County were selected for the study. The modal age of these children was eight years. It was felt that thiswas the earliest age at which children would cooperate with the group paper-and-pencilproce- dures used in a large-scale survey study. Moreover, stable patterns of behavior amenable to reliable observation have probably been estab- lished by this age (Goodenough and Tyler, 1959). The basic research plan was to obtain data about each child from four independent sources: his classmates, his mother, his father, and him- self. One putpose of studying this population was to determine theex- tent of aggressive behavior at school and at home and 'attempt toac- count for the variation in aggression by familial, social, geographic, ecOnomic, and cultural factors. A second goalwas to gain an under- standing of the learning conditions for aggression. The experimenters tried to relate the ways in which children act out their aggressionto the kinds of training in its expression and control they receive from such socializing agents as parents and peers. A third aimwas to study the consistency of aggressive behavior across timeas a function .of new learning' situations by studying thesathe subjects in the eighth and twelfth grades. This aim is related toa body of theory which holds that behavior is characterized by stability (Goodenough anq Tyler,1959; Cattell, 1965), and that from such stable behavior the construct ofper- sonality is deduced. The major dependent variable, then, was aggression as observed in the school situation; the major class of independent variables was de- fined by parents' socialization practices. These variables includedcon- tingent responses to the expression of aggressive behavior, instigation of aggression, the child's identification with his parents, and sociocultur- al factors (the family's socioeconomic status,. their educational and oc- cupational aspiration for the child, and,the child's IQ). 44 011.-

TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 37 The measure of aggression in the classroom was obtained by a peer rating instrument in which each child could be nominated by every other child in his class for ten "guess who" itclns describing aggressive be- havior. These items were interspersed among a series of other peer nom- ination questions. Since these items with only slight modification consti- tute the major dependent variable in the five- and ten-yearfollowup studies,2 the three versions are presented in Table 1.

Table 1: Comparison of peer rating items for three time periods

5-year follow up 10-year followup 3rd grade 8th grade 13th grade

1. Who does not obey the Who does not obey the Who did not listen to the teacher? teacher? teacher? 2. Who often says, "Give OMITTED OMITTED me thatl"? 3. Who gives dirty looks or Who gives dirty looks or Who gave dirty looks or sticks out their tongue unfriendly gestures to made unfriendly gestures at other children? other students? to other students? 4. Who makes up stories Who makes up stories and Who made up stories and and lies to get other lies to get othei students lies to get other students children into trouble? into trouble? into trouble? 5. Who does things that Who does things that Who did things that bother others? bother others? bothered others? 6. Who starts a fight Who starts a fight Who started fights over nothing? over nothing? over nothing? 7. Who pushes or shoves Who pushes or.shoves Who pushed or shoved children? students? students? 8. Who is alwaYs getting Who is always getting Who was always getting into trouble? into trouble? into trouble? 9. Who says mean things? Who says mean things? Who used to say mean things? 10. Who takes other chil- Who takes other students' Who took other students' dren's things without things without asking? things without asking? asking?.

The development of this peer rating technique, pilot studies concern- ing it, and data pertaining to reliability and validity are presented in Walder et al. (1961) and Eron, Walder, and Lefkowitz (1971). Each child's aggression score was based on the number of judges choosing him as fitting a parLicular behavioral description. Thus, if ten of the 27 members of a class crossed out Johnny Jones's name as some- one who said mean things, Johnny's raw score was ten for that item. If he was selected a total of 45 times for the nine other items, his total raw score on the complete set of aggression items would be 55; his mean raw score on the ten aggression items would be 5.5. These mean raw scores were converted into percentageS (of the total number who were present 45

- I111 38 TELEVIS1')N ANI) ADOI.EgCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

and made nominations) in order to make scores of subjects who were in different sized classrooms more comparable. In addition to the aggres- sion measure, other peer ratings were obtained from items designed to measure aggression anxiety. success in aggression. popularity, and ac- tivity level. Such measures as IQ. masculine-feminine identification. occupational aspirations. human figure drawings. and self-description of expressive behavior were also obtained from the children. These mea- sures are described in greater detail in Eron. Walder. and Lefkowitz (1971). Measures of the independent variables were primarily obtained from an objective. precoded child rearing interview administered face-to-face with mothers and fathers independently. The interview contained 286 items comprising 41 variables, which were categorized largely into four types: reinforcers of aggression. instigators to aggression. identification of child with family. and sociocultural variables. A copy of the interview and a history of its development and psychometric properties are pre- sented in Eron et al.. 1971. A number of individual questions were included in the interview be- cause of popular notions about the antecedents of aggressive behavior. These questions concerned frequency of television watching in the home, types of programs watched, the child's reading of comic books. parents' PTA membership, and parents' familiarity with Dr. Spock and other child care publications. Because of their content, these questions 2iso tended to serve as buffer items: the insouciance with which they were treated or viewed by the investigators at that time is shown by their grouping under the initials LHJ (for Ladies Home Journal). In 1959-60 all the children in 38 third-grade classrooms in all public and parochial schools in Columbia County were tested in their classrooms. This population was 900 children, of whom 875 were third graders. Dur- ing this period 713 of their mothers and 570 of their fathers were inter- viewed separately; data on 557 mother-father pairs were thereby ob- tained.3 Because of the increasing interest in the effect of television viewing on children'sbehavior,theinvestigatorsanalyzed the re- lation betweentheviolenceratingsof thechild'sfavorite television programs (as reported by parents) and the child's peer ratings of aggression in the classroom (Eton. 1963). Ratings of the violencecon- tained in these programs were made independently by two raters, and each child's putative television diet was assigned a violence rating. This rating. as well as the number of hours the child was said to watch televi- sion, was compared in an analysis of variance design with his peer rating aggression score. A significant positive relationship was found among boys between television violence and peer ratings of aggression. In addi- tion, but again only for boys, a significant inverse relationship was found 46 TV VIOLENCE ANDCHILD AGGRESSION 39 between the number of hours subjects watched tei4vision and peer rat- ings of aggression. Funding for the project was terminated in 1962 and the research team was dispersed. However, an attempt wasmade by the present research- ers to carry out the second step inthe longitudinal design. Five years later, in 1964-65, when the modal age of the subjects was 13 years and their modal grade was eight, the schools in Columbia County werehp- proached and asked for their continuing cooperation. Becauseof ad- verse newspaper publicity andopposition to the program by some dissi- dent groups during the third-grade study (Eron and Walder. 1961). sever- al of the schools did not, wish to participate. However,382 children were tested in eighth-grade classrooms. Of this group. 252 were ir. theoriginal third-grade population. Slight modifications of the peer rating items were necessary to insure face validity for 13-year-old subjects (see Table 1). The nominating pro- cedure was altered slightly, and the peer rating measure andother tests were administered in the classroomby teachers. school psychologists, and other school personnel. The subjects themselves, ratherthan their parents, were asked to report their favoritetelevision programs and the amount of time they watched television. The measuresused in Columbia County at this stage of the study had previously beenmodified and test- ed in pilot studies of eighth graders in the Duke Street School inAlexan- dria, Virginia. The attenuation of the number of subjects tested in the eighthgrade in Columbia County reflected the limited effort the research team wasable to muster rather than attrition from the third tothe eighth grades. Una- ble to obtain support for this phase of the study. the teamconfined itself to those schools most readily accessible and to acircumscribed number of subjects. Lists of television programs were assembled and rated for violenceby three Columbia County school personnel. Each rated thetelevision pro- grams independently, and each received$10 for this service. A violence score could range from a low of 0 to ahigh of 5. Each child's television violence score was the average rating of theshows he named. For example, if the child named three programswith violence scores of 2, 3. and 3 respectively,his total violence score would be 2.67. If he named only two programs with ratings of 2 and 4,his average score would be 3. Separate ratings of these same programs weremade by two NBC television censors.4 The relation between television violence and aggressivebehavior among the eighth grade subjects inAlexandria, Virginia, was also ana- lyzed. These subjects differed from the Columbia Countypopulation in that they lived in an urban area. Again, the ratings oftelevision violence and aggressive behavior remainedmethodologically independent. A 47 40 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCE1T AGGRESSIVENESS

statistically significant relation in the same direction (r= .31. p <.01) as that found for the third graders in Columbia Countywasfound for the 73 eighth-grade male and female subjects in Alexandria. Virginia: the great- er the amount of television violence viewed, the higher the peer ratings of aggression. The peer rating procedure was also administered to large groups of third graders in Cedar Rapids. Iowa. andscores were meaningfully relat- ed to available school data (Semler and Eron. 1967: Semler. Eron. Mey- erson. and Williams. 1967) and to overt behavior in a controlld labora- tory situation (Williams. Meyerson. Eron, and Semler, 1967). In 1968 the third-grade study was replicatedon a smaller scalein Am- sterdam under the direction of Dr. Eron. Ihe peer rating procedue and the parent interview were translated into Dutch. The classroompoce- dures were administered in six classrooms of eight-year-old children located in divergent socioeconomic areas of Amsterdam.An attempt was made to interview all the mothers and fathers of these children. bu the effort met with considerably less success than it had in Columbia County. However. scorable interviews were obtained from 72 mothers: these data were analyzed in relation to the peer ratingscores and other classroom measures of their children. The results were surprisingly sim- ilar to the Columbia County findings (Stroo, 1970). These cross-national data are presented in Eron et al. (1971). Because of limited access to tel- evision and rigorous control of programming by the Dutch government, data on television behavior were not collected in the Amsterdam study. The present research is directly related to the foregoing third-grade and eighth-grade studies and to the cross-national study. The current project has three specific aims: (1) to complete (within the financial limi- tations of NIMH support) the longitudinal study of the psychosocial development of aggressive behavior for which data were collected in Columbia County in 1960 and 1965: (2) to investigate the longitudinal relationships between violence content in television and aggressive be- havior on the part of viewers: and (3) to examine theconsequences of childhood aggression during late adolescence andyoung adulthood. A relationship between violent content and viewer aggression could be determined if the present subjects selected thesame level of violence in their television preferences that they had selected in the eighth grade and if these television preferences were still related to aggression. The third aim deals with the ramifications of aggressiveness (as assessed in childhood) on critical spheres of functioning during late adolescence. Specific questions about educational and occupational achievement and aspiration, psychopathology, prosocial and antisocial behavior in the community, and military status ate scrutinized. A corollary of this objective was determining the relation between variables obtained in the third gtade and aggressiveness as measured in the ten-year followup. Moreover, the relation between the early measures of aggression and 4t3 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 41 later measures of psychopathology is important in determining if aggres- sion was indeed well chosen as a component of mental health. Hypotheses Three hypotheses were derived from the foregoing rationale: 1. When examined longitudinally, ,positive relationships obtain be- tween violent television preferences and aggressive behavior. 2. Early aggressive behavior is positively related to later aggressive behavior. 3. Aggressive behavior is positively related to psychopathology. To test these hypotheses, the investigators adduced data concerning the manner and extent to which viewing of violent television programs is related to aggressive behavior. The relation between violent television preferences and a host of other variables is also open to inspection through the test of the foregoing hypotheses.

METHOD The present research employed survey techniques. The overall goal was to obtain data from as many as possiblebut atleast halfof the original 875 third-grade subjects. Each subject was to give two hours of his time, the first hour in an individual face-to-face interview and the second taking written psychological tests. Two incentives were offered to encourage the subjects to participate. The importance of theresearch was explained to the subjects in a letter, andthey were offered $20 for their time. Subjects In order to estimate the number of subjects available for the ten-year followup study, a preliminary survey of the Columbia County high school graduates of June 1969 was undertaken. Information was ob- tained from New York State Department of Education records and from various newspaper files in Columbia County. It was determined that approximately 52 percent of the original subject pool was still in Colum- bia County as of June 1969. The investigators assumed that this was a conservative estimate of the number of subjects potentially available, since this tally did not account for those subjects who dropped out of school or who had not yet been graduated. Letters were written to the seven district superintendents of the county's public and parochial schools (see Appendix A). They were reminded of the investigators' past efforts and of their own cooperation with those efforts. The superintendents were asked to supply addresses for these former third grade children and other information from their school records. Most of these officials were very cooperative and asked 49 42 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

their principals and guidance counselorsto furnish the requested infor- mation. Perhaps because of problems encounteredwith certain parents and community groups during the third gradesurvey in the Chatham and Hudson areas (Eron and Walder, 1961) and because ofadverse publicity there in 1965, the district superintendents in thesetwo localities thought it necessary to present our requestto their school boards. In both cases, the school boards were concerned about the possiblecontroversy which might again arise from the study and refusedto cooperate in providing address lists or information from school records. Despitethese refusals, more than 400 addresses for third-grade subjects were furnished by offi- cials of the other school districts. A majoreffort was exerted to locate as many of the remaining subjects as possibk, especially those in thenon- cooperating school districts. It was reasoned that sampling bias would beminimized by inviting for interview as many of the original subjectsas could be traced. An inten- sive search was made for those subjects forwhom the schools refused to give current addresses and for those subjects for whomcooperating schools could provide no addresses. We examinedhigh school year- books, old and current telephone directories,voter lists, tax lists, and a county directory. In addition, each of the interviewees who didappear was questioned about the whereabouts of any of the missingsubjects. This effort, in conjunction with theaddresses furnished by the schools, resulted in letters sent to 735 of the875 subjects, or 84 percent of the original sample. Ifa subject did not respond within three to four weeks and the letter was not returned by thePost Office for insufficient address, a followup letter was sent. (Copies ofthese letters are present- ed in Appendix A.) Consequently, 236 secondletters were sent, totaling 971 first and second letters. Of the 735 subjectsto whom letters were sent (see Appendix A), 460 indicated willingnessto be interviewed; the remaining 275 subjects were categorizedas shown in Table 2.

Table 2: Classification of 735 subjects to wnom letterswere sent

96

Acceptances 460 63 Post office returns 45 6 Definite refusals 81 11 In military service 38 5 Deceased 4 .5 In prison 2 .2 No replies 105 14

TOTAL 735 99.7

Of the 460 subjects who responded positivelyto the interview re- quest. 436 were interviewed. (The remaining 24 subjects either livedat too great a distance, did not appear after several appointments,or re- quested an appointment after the field operationwas terminated.) Of the 50 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 43 42,6 subjects interviewed, 427 contributed data which could becompared across the ten-year span. Thesample was composed of 211 boys and 216 girls. The modal age of this group was 19 years. The mean numberof years of school completed was 12.57 +.82. Based on 103 cases for whom curl znt test scores were available, the mean IQ was109.12 + 11.57. Using father's occupation as reported by subject todetermine socioeconomic status (Warner, Meeker and Eells, 1960), thesample may be described as predominantlymiddle class. Sampling bias was evinced in the ten-year followup when an analysis of the number of subjects in the upper and lower quartiles ofaggression in the third grade was performed. Of the 130 boys in the lowerquartile of aggression at age eight 74 (or 57 percent) consented to beinterviewed in the ten year followup at age 19. However, of the 125 boys in the upper quartile of aggression at age eight. only 34 (or 27 percent) consented tobe interviewed at age 19. Girls responded similarly: 63 percent of the low aggressive group consented to be interviewed in the ten-year followup. while only 33 percent of those in the upper quartile of aggression con- sented to be interviewed at age 19. That approximatelytwice as many high as compared to low aggressive subjectsof both sexeswere una- vailable for interview stands as a datum by itself and is relevant to sur- vey research. In part the effectof this sampling bias was controlled sta- tistically where the data were analyzed by analysis of variance. In this technique, the independent variable was partitioned in a manner so that the upper and lower 10 percent and the middle 80 percententered into the analysis. Thus both extremes were equally represented. A field office was established in Hudson, N. Y., seatof Columbia County. from June 1 through September 30. 1970. The resultsof the pre- liminary survey had indicated that this time of year would be the most propitious for maximizing the number of subjects sampled. Inaddition to the authors of this report. additional temporarystaff was employed.5 Shortly after the field office was established, staff trainingsessions were held. The confidentialityof the data being gathered and the neces- sity of maintaining locked files were stressed. The ethics ofconducting research with human subjects was discussed. Thetrainees first familiar- ized themselves with the interview schedule, beingthemselves inter- viewed and interviewing one another. Finally they observed asenior staff member, who had participated in the third grade study,in a live in- terview session. The trainees were observed in a liveinterview session. Throughout, they received feedback on their performances from the staff members and from an interviewer manual which hadbeen devel- oped early in the data collection phase. Measures: peer ratings The measure of aggressive behavior forthese 19-year-old subjects included essentially the same items used inthe eighth-grade study, TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

which in turn had been slightly modifiedfrom the third-grade items (see Table l). Tested ina brief pilot study in the Washington. D.C.,area for wording, the measurewas made final at the Hudson field officeon the first few Columbia County subjects. In addition to the nine aggressionitems, the questionnaire contained six items which yieldedscores on aggression anxiety, popularity, activi- ty level, and leadership. The fifteen itemswere administered to each subject individually with relevantinstructions (see Appendix B). "Ten years ago, when you were in the third grade, you answereda series of questions about yourselfand about your classmates. I would like your cooperation in answeringa similar series of questions. "I have lists of people who mighthave gone to school withyou. Put a check in the box next to thenames of the people you know well enough to answer some questions about. Generally,these would be people who had been in class withyou. You might remember the way they acted in school.- (At this point the subjectwas given appropriate class lists to check [see below].) After thesubject checked thenames of those he knew he was given the followinginstructions: "I shall ask you questions,one at a time. For each question, tellme the identification numbers of all thosepeople who fit that question. Do not name yourself for any of these questions.You may name any num- ber of people for each question. Noticethat to these questions thereare no answers that are right for everybody. Baseyour answers on what you last knew of each person from personalobservation and contact. "You may give any number ofanswers. You may check more names if you think of themas I ask the questions." Since the subjects wereno longer grouped together in classroomsas they had been in the third and eighthgrades. the problem aroseas to how nominations would be made andwho was to make them. This prob- lem was resolved by asking each subjectfor the schools he attended af- ter the third grade. The subjectwas then presented with rosters of all the original third-grade subjects who mighthave attended the last Columbia County school the subject himself saidhe attended in addition to his own third-grade class. For example, if the subjectsaid the last school he attended was Hudson High School, hewas presented with rosters of the twelve grade school classes in Hudson andits environs which were the feeder elementary schools for HudsonHigh School. On these rosters were only those subjects who were in the originalthird-grade study. Thus. subjects in the larger schools might reviewseveral hundrednames for each of the peer rating items. In thesmaller schools, subjects might review as few as 60 names for each ofthe item s. As the interviewer read eachpeer rating item, the subject responded with the identification numbers ofany of the individuals on the roster of subjects to which that item applied. The aggressionscore for any sub- ject was then computed. For the nine aggressionitems the number of 52 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 45 people giving a patticular subject's name was counted.This sum was divided by nine (the number of items). This average was thendivided by the number of people who said they knew the particular subject.For example, if a subject received 45 nominations for the nine items, 45 was divided by 9 to yield an average of five nominations per item. If ten peo- ple said they knew the subject. this quotient of 5 was dividedby 10 to yield an aggression score of .50. The same general technique for computing peer rating scores was used for aggression-anxiety, popularity, activity level, andleadership. Peer-rated aggression-anxiety and popularity are compositecategories made up of two items each. Thus the numerator of eachof these scores was the average number ofnominations that the subject received on the two aggression-anxiety items (or the popularityitems). The numerators of peer-rated activity level and leadership were each based on oneitem and therefore were simply the number of nominations that thesubject received on each item. In addition to these percentage scores for aggression-anxiety, popu- larity. activity level, and leadership. there were two other peerrating scores. which were simply the averagenumber of nominations a subject received on the nine aggression items and the number of nominationshe received in answers to the question, "Whom do you know wellenough to rate?" The interview schedule Interview items were derived from five sources: (1) TheRip Van Win- kle Child Rearing Interview used with parents in the third-gradestudy (Eron et al., 1971); (2) an interview written by the staff of theTelevision and Social Behavior program of the NationalInstitute of Mental Health:6 (3) Project Talent (Flanagan, Davis, Dailey, Shaycoft,Orr, Goldberg. and Neyman, 1964): (4) Youth in Transition (Bachman,1967): and (5) The Teen Age Interview (Lange, Baker, and Ball,1969). In addi- tion, interview questions were formulated by the researchstaff. Consist- ing of 180 items, the interview was almost entirely precodedand re- quired about one hour for administration (see Appendix B). A pilot study of the interview was conducted to determinethe applica- bility, clarity of directions, and general meaningfulness for19-year-old individuals. This study was done at the University of Illinois atChicago Circle, and 15 introductory psychofru students served assubjects. Final revisions were made on the basis of experience with thefirst few Columbia County subjects. For the purpose of the present study, subsets of the 180 items were formed to build variables bearing on the hypotheses understudy. Like the Rip Van Winkle Child Rearing Survey, the presentinterview was regarded as a test consisting of a number of subscales. 33 46 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

The following variableswere derived from the interview schedule (see Appendix C for detailed derivations): I.Respondents aggression a. Respondent as object of aggression (VAG) b. Respondent as witness of aggression (WAG) c. Aggressive habitA (AH A) d. Aggressive habitB (A H B) e. Total aggressive habit (TA H) f. Antisocial behavior (ASB) g. Aggressive drive (AGD) h. Total aggressive environment (TAG) i.Personal opinion inventory (WAZ) j.Potential punishment for aggression (PUN-TOP) ").Social status factors a. Social status of family (1SS) (1) Number of books (NOB) (2) Occupation of father (FOC) (3) Total number of rooms (4) Composite social status index (CIS) b. Respondent's occupational status (CSR) (1) Occupation (ROC) (2) Education (EDR) c. Mobility aspirations (ASP) (1) Total aspiration (TAS) (2) Mobility orientation (M 00) d. Church attendance (REL-RAT) 3.Psychopathology a. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (M M PI) b. Z-Test (1) Hostility (Z-HOS-2) (2) Psychopathology (Z-SUM) 4. Television variables a. Hours of watching (TV-HW-A) b. TV violenceHudson (TV-VIOL-H) c. TV violenceGreenberg (TV-VIOL-G) d. Sports programs (TV-SPT) e. Realism of TV (ROT) 5. Height a. Height of subject (SHT) b. Discrepancy between subject's height andaverage of moth- er's and father's height (DA H) 6. School records a. Achievement (AC H) b. IQ c. Times tardy (TARDY) TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 47 7. Number of arrests

Data for varying numbers of subjects were collected from six sources: (1) peer rmings (N = 427): (2) individual face-to-face interviews (N = 427): (3) height (N = 427): (4) psychological tests, objective and projec- tive (N = 427): (5) IQ as measured in the twelfth grade and obtained from school records (N = 103). standardized achievement test scores obtained from twelfth grade school records (N = 120). tardiness ob- tained from twelfth grade school records (N = 49): (6) Number of ar- rests in New York State of boys in the upper and lower quartiles of ag- gression as measured in the third grade (N = 255). Data analyses A basic data set comprised of 49 third-grade variables, tive five-year followup variables. and 40 ten-year followup variables was stored on magnetic tape. A code manual listing the 94 variables is presented in Appendix E. The following types of analyses were performed for the above data set: descriptive statistics, histograms. correlations, partial correlations, multiple correlations. cross-lagged correlations, principal components factor analyses, regression analyses, analyses of variance, and contingency analyses. These analyses were applied where appropri- ate to the data and to the hypothesis in question. In the main, the data were analyzed for each sex group and for the total.

RESULTS Television and aggression Data bearing on the first hypothesisthat positive relations exist be- tween preference for violent television programs and aggressive behav- iorconfirmed previous findings of a contemporaneous relation be- tween the two variables (Eron, 1963). Table 3 presents these findings in a longitudinal contextthe intercorrelations7 among four variables: peer ratings of aggression at two stages (third grade (AGG3) and thirteenth grade (AGG13)8. and violence ratings of preferred television programs at these same two stages (TVVL3 and TVVL13). At the time of the third-grade study, each parent was asked what his child's three favorite television programs were. All programs mentioned were then categorized as violent or nonviolent by two independent rat- ers who were familiar with television programs. There was94 percent agreement in their ratings. Differences in the remaining six percent of the programs were resolved by discussion between the raters. Each sub- ject received a score according to the number of violent programs he 55 48 IH.F.VIsioN AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 3: Correlations among violence ratings of preferred TV programs and peer ratings of aggression at two different periods

Boys Girls

TVVL3AGG3 TVV13AGG13TVVL3AGG3 TVV13 AGG13

TVV L3b1.00 .21" .05 .31" 1.00 .02 .38 .08 -.13 AGG3 1.00 .01 1.00 -.08 .47" TVVL13 1.00 -.05 1.00 -.05 AGG13a 1.00 1.00 7.15 12.12 6.39 80.46 5.29 7.51 5.07 26.51 S.D. 5.05 12.73 6.09 96.98 4.72 9.87 4.71 37.68 184 211 211 211 175 216 216 216

aThe AGG13 score, when being calculated, was multiplied by 10. Otherwise itwas calcu- lated in the same manner as was AGG3.

I The number 3, 8, or 13 followinga variable indicates the period, 3rd, 8th, or 13th grades when the data were obtained. *indicates significance of r at or beyond .01 level of confidence.

was reported to favor. Scores ranged from 1 (for no violent programs) to 4 (for three violent programs). The same scoring procedure was used in the five-year followup study (eighth grade) except that the programswere rated for violence by two professional censors employed by NBC-withan equally high degree of agreement between them. These ratings were made approximately four years after the data were collected.9 In the tcn-year followup study (thirteenth grade), each subjectwas himself asked to mention his four current favorite televisionprograms. All programs were then categorized forpresence or absence of violence by two independent raters of differentsex and educational background who were only a few years older than the subjects themselves.Scores were assigned to each program on the basis of agreement between the raters. If they agreed a program was nonviolent, the program receiveda score of zero: if they agreed it was violent, the score was two: if they disagreed in categorization. the score was one. Therewas much agree- ment between the two raters. They agreed on 81 percent of 125pro- grams mentioned by the subjects. (The list of programs categorized ac- cording to violence rating is presented in Appendix D.) Their designation of violent and nonviolentprograms agreed very well with the assignment of programs by Feshbach and Singer (1971)to ag- gressive and nonaggressive diets in their field experiment. Furthermore, the judgments of these two raters were in close agreement with there- sults obtained by Greenberg and Gordon (1970). The latter research team did an intensive rating study. using as raters both established tele- vision critics (approximately 45) and 300 subjects randomly selected from the Detroit telephone book. Of the 20 programs which Greenberg 56 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 49 and Gordon indicated had the highest violence ratings. 19 were selected as violent by our raters.For the 427 cases in the ten-year followup study. there was a correlation of .94 between the Greenberg-Gordon average ratings and our ratings. There can be little doubt thatthese pro- grams were accurately rated for presence or absenceof violence. It is apparent. in Table 3, from the intercorrelations that a relation ex- ists between the violence of the television programs preferred by boys when they are in the third grade and their aggressive behavior ten years later. The relation between third-grade television and later behavior isin fact stronger than that between third-grade television and aggressive behavior at that time. Furthermore. there is no relation between later television preference and earlier aggressive behavior. Such a finding supports the notion that preference for violent programs inthe third grade is causally related to aggressive behavior ten yearslater.") This becomes clear when the pertinent correlations are viewed in a cross- lagged context as shown in Figure 1. The large and significant difference (Fisher's Z = 3.07. p = .002) between the cross-lagged correlations on the diagonals lends strong support to the hypothesis that watching vio- lent television causes aggressive habits." However, this hypothesis. which is diagrammed in Figure 2a. is not the only possible interpretation of the correlations, and several rival hypotheses deserve consideration.

TVVL3 .05 TVVL13

.01

.21 -.05

.31

AGG3 .38 AGG13 Figure 1: The correlations between television violence and aggressionfor 211 boys over a ten-year lag

The first alternative is that television violence has only a synchronous effect on aggression at the third-grade level and that this effect, coupled with the temporal reliability of the aggression measure. explains the cross-lagged correlation from television violen,ce to later aggression. The corresponding causal chain is diagrammed in Figure 2b. This inter- pretation can be rejected because, if it wet 'rue. the cross-lagged corre- lation would be less than the product of the synchronous correlation and 57 50 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS TVVL3 TVVL3i AGG3 AGG3/ AG 13 AGt13

(A) (B)

AGG3 AGG3

TVVL3

AGG13 AGG13 TVVL3

(C) (D)

Figure 2: Four feasible causal hypotheses for the correlations presented in Figure 1

the reliability. For the same reason, the causal chain in Figure 2c can be eliminated as a contending hypothesis. However, one cannot reject so easily the hypothesis (diagrammed in Figure 2d) that early aggression causes both concomitant watching of violent television and later aggres- sion. We can compare the partial correlation between AGG3 and AGG13 holding TVVL3 constant (r = .33) with the partial correlation between TVVL3 and AGG 13 holding AGG3 constant (r = .21). Since both partials remain substantial, we can conclude that neither of these possibilities provides a complete causal explanation. The higher partial correlation between AGG3 and AGG13 is not surprising, since that cor- relation is partly a reliability measure. It is more interesting to compare

the two relevant longitudinal correlations in Figure 1 (r = .38 and r = .31) with the synchronous correlation between AGG3 and TVVL3. Both relevant longitudinal correlations are larger than the synchronous corre- lation (r = .21). This is easily explainable for the correlation between A003 and AGG13 (r = .38) since (as indicated) that correlation contains a reliability component. However, it is difficult to explain why the cross- lagged correlation from TVVL3 to A0013 should be greater than the synchronous correlation between TVVL3 and AGG3 r.:cept in terms of the causal model in Figure 2a. Hence, on the basis of the cross-lagged 58 TV VIOLENCE AND CHII.I) AGGRESSION 51 correlations, the most plausible single causal hypothesis would appear to be that watching violent television in the third grade leads to the build- ing of aggressive habits. Another point to be considered. as Rozelle and Campbell (1969) have indicated, is that a cross-lagged correlation may be viewed as a deviation from the initial synchronous correlation. Under this view the correla- tions in Figure 1 could be interpreted as indicating that early aggression causes a decrease in the watching of violent television because .01 is far- below the .21 synchronous correlation than .31 is above it. Howev- er. following the method of Rozelle and Campbell. the most appropriate baselinether. is not the early synchronous correlation. but the a% erage of the two synchronous correlations attenuated for the reliability of the two variables. The higher the reliabilities, the less is the attenuation. With a conservative assumption of a very high tempoYal reliability of .70. the base line for Figure 1 would be 1.21 + (.05)1 V77(77 /2 = .06. With this correction, the hypothesis that aggression causes diminished watching of violent television becomes untenable. The relation between third-grade aggression of boys and eighth-grade television violence is not significantly different from zero (n = .16): nor is the relation between eighth-grade television violence and eighth- grade aggression (r = .10). although the relation between third- and eighth-grade aggression for boys is moderately positive (r = .48). For girls the relation between third- and eighth-grade aggression is also posi- tive (r = .30). while the relations between aggression and television vio- lence at the between the two age levels is not different from zero. It should be remembered, however, that in the pilot study done in Alexan- dria. Virginia. with eighth graders. the same findings were obtained as in the third grade in Columbia County. As has been pointed out (Eron et al., 1971), the Pearson product moment correlations may very likely be masking more marked relations in these data due to the kurtosis of the distributions. That this is so can be seen in the table of means (Table 4) and the resultant analyses of vari- ance. In Table 4 the independent variable is violence of preferred televi- sion programs in the third grade. There is a three-way partition of the sample of 184 boys into three groupslow, medium, and high television violence preferences. The breakdown into three parts was made by inspection of the frequency distribution at approximately the tenth and ninetieth percentiles. The relation of third-grade television habits to later behavior now appears even more impressive. Not only is violence of programs preferred in third grade related to peer-rated aggression in the third grade and ten years later, but it is also related positively to t elf-dis- closure of antisocial behavior ten years later and to the sum of T scores on scales 4 and 9 of the M M PI at that time. This pair of scales has been demonstrated to discriminate potential and actual delinquents from normal populations (Hathaway and Monachesi, 1963). Further data bearing on the sum of scales 4 and 9 are presented below. 59 I FLEVISION .AND ADOLESCFN I AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 4: Mean aggression scores asa function of TV violence ratings ol programs preferred by boysin third grade _ AGG3 AGG13 ASBa MMPI.49Sb TVVL3 N M SD to SD M SD M SD La 31 9,06 9.91 51.39 50.54 25.58 12.12 121.97 20.50 Med 139 11.1911.54 81.39 98.80 22.06 12.69 122.50 19.33 Hi 14 21.0013.79 -- 164.64 132.98 30.86 14.23135.86 18.14 Total 184 11.58 11.75 82.67 98.58 23.33 12.90123.424 19.67 F 5.43 6.82 t<4.04c 3.63 3.105 P <.005 <.001 <.01 <.03 <.05

a ASB Self.rating of frequency of antisocial behavior bMMPI-495 Sum of Tscores on scales 4 and 9 of MMPI Because of heterogeneity of variancea ttest, between the two most discrepant means for AGG13, was performed. The t was conservatively evaluated by using df equal to N forthe smallest group, i.e., 14. Means and variancesare significantly dif erent.

Although current television behavior,in terms both of violencescore of preferred programs and oftotal number of hours the subjectwatches television per week. are not relatedto current aggressive behavioras rated by peers. bothaspects of television viewingare related to other measures of current behavior, as seen in Table S.Especially meaningful are the relations between the number of hoursthe subject estimates he watches television and hisachievement test scores, educational attain- ment. social status as measured by father'soccupation. and peer-rated popularity and leadership. Themore the subject watches television, the lower are his measured abilities andhis social and educationalaccom- plishments. Realism of television(extent to which the subjectstates that life as depicted on televisionwesterns and crime stories is realistic) is

Table 5: Correlation between TV viewing habitsat grade 13 and other contemporaneous variables

Hours watched Variable TV violence N Boys N Girls N Boys N Girls Achievement 13 58 -.39 Judgment of TV realism 13 210 .28216 .34 211 .36216 .18 Social status 13 208 .24210 .22 Subject's education 13 210 -.26 Subject's mobility aspirations 13 211 .18 Personal opinion inventory -Walters-Zak 13 210 .19 POP 13 210 -.26 Leadership 13 210 -.19216 -.19 TVVL 13 210 .20216 .25 TV-SPT 13 210 .24 60 1 V VI01.1. NCI AND CHILD ..5(iGIOSSION 53 related moderately positiv:ly both to number of hours television is watched currently and to the violence score of preferred programs. It is ..lso interesting that the number of hours the subject watches tele- vision at this later period is negatively related to measures taktnten years earlier. These measures, presented in Table 6. included IQ at grade three, popularity, father's educational aspirations for the child at that time. mother's educational aspirations for the child, and father's and mother's achieved education.1- There is no relation between num- ber of hours the subject watched television in grade threeas reported by mother and thc number of hours he watches at grade thirteenas reported

Tatge 6: Correlationsa between amount of TV viewing at grade 13 and measures taken at grade 3 for boys

Measure Correlation

103 -.33 204 Father's ertocational aspiration for child 3 -.21 143 Mother's educational aspiration for child 3 -.41 185 Popularity 3 -.22 210 Father's education 3 -.25 143 Mother's education 3 -.27 185

°Only those correlations significant at or beyond .01 level of confidence are presented. by himself. As shown in Figure 3. the child's peer-rated popularity at grade three has the same order of relation to hours of television watched ten years later as does the later rating of popularity. Popularity at grade three is related to popularity ten years later (r = .38). Although IQ at grade three is not related to the number of hours television was watched

TV HW3 .13 .TVHW13

-.22

-.07 -.26 .06

POP3 .38 POP13

Figure 3: The correlations between television hours and popularity for 211 boys over a tenyear lag

61- III. VISION 1ND %DO! 1(i(1141SS1VFNI:SS at Ow time. it is inversely related to Frer of hours television is cur- rentl) watched. However, the relatonnetween tw elfth-grade IQ and number of hours television is watched in the thirteenthgrade. although in a negative direction. does not reach significanceat our accepted level tr - .20. N 53). This occurs despite the fact that thetwo IQ mea- sures are moderately positively related tr= .511. Since different mea- sures of IQ were used and the number of subjects in the later periodin- cluded only a subsample of the original number ofsubjects it is surpris- ing that the latter relation isas high as it is. The relation of judged real- ism of television to current IQ is negligible. althoughit is related to third- grade IQ (r = - .21). Again it should he pointedout that IQ measures at the later period were avail:ible for onlya small subsample of subjects: the true relation between the two variableswas probably not permitted to emerge. These correlations apply only to boys. As previouslyindicated, ag- gression in girls does not enter intoas many antecedent-consequent relations as does aggression in boys. Thesame is generally true for tele- vision violence and number of hoursper week during which the subject watches television. This is apparent in Table 5. Tables 7.8. and 9 represent the multiple correlationsof increasing sets of predictors to peer-rated aggression in the third,eighth, and.thirteenth grades. (These correlations again referto boys only.) The violence of the programs boys prefer at the third-grade levelenters into the multiple

Table 7: Multiple correlation of third-gradepredictor variabl.ts to third.grade peerrated aggression for boysa Xb Predictor F13 A Partialed r

1 103 .09 .29 -.29205 2 Social status 3 .12 .34 -.19 -.16144 3 Discrepancy in identification with .15 .39 .20 .25 132 Father 3 4 Father's lack of nurturance 3 .18 .43 -.18 -14144 5 TV hours watched 3 .21 .46 -.17 -.19186 6 TV violence 3 .24 .49 .20 .21 184 7 Father's punishment 3 .26 .51 .17 8 .13144 Father's occupational aspirations for .27 .52 -.13 -.16144 child 3 9 Mother's occupational aspirations for .28 .53 -.13 -.03186 child 3 10 Father's generational level 3 .30 .55 -.15 -.13144 11 Mother's educational aspirations for .31 .56 .13 -.04186 child 3 12 Father's mobility orientation 3 .32 .57 .12 .14 144

aThe number of variables entering into themultiple correlations in this and the two subse- quent tables is contingent upon the requirement thatany variable add at least 1% to the variance explained. bF inal standardized regressionequation: AGG3 -.232X,- .133X3 + .223X, - .176X4 - .190X5 + .188X, + 134X, - .146X,- .116X, - .155X10 + .132X + .105X12 62 IV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 55 correlations of predictor variables to aggression at each grade level. However, the number of hours boys watch television is included among the multiple predictions only for aggression in the third graae.

Table 8: Multiple correlations of third.grade predictor variables to eighthgrade peerrated aggression for boys

Xa Predictor R Partialed r

1 Discrepancy in identification .22 .46 .47 58 with mother 3 2 Father's occupational aspirations .30 .54 -.32 -.35 53 for child 3 3 Father's punishment 3 .38 .60 .31 .30 53 4 Father's lack of nurturance .42 .65 -.29 -.26 53 5 Father's generational level .46 .68 -.26 -.29 53 6 Mother's judgment of punishment .50 .71 .29 .24 64 harshness 3 7 Discrepancy in identification with .55 .74 .29 .45 47 father 3 8 Father's mobility aspirations .60 .78 -.36 -.20 53 for himself 3 9 Mother's educational aspirations .67 .82 .40 -.12 64 for child 3 10 Father' education 3 .69 .83 -.28 .15 53 11 TV violence 3 .72 .85 .29 .16 64 12 Residential mobility 3 .74 .86 -.28 -.03 53 13 10 3 .78 .87 -.25 -.30 68 14 Father's aggression on .77 .88 .25 .31 53 WaltersZak scale 3 15 Mother's education 3 .78 .88 -.24 .18 64 16 Parental disharmony 3 .79 .89 .20 .04 64 aFinal standardized regression equation: AGG8 -.134X, - .229X3 + .328X3- .272X4 - .341X1 + .423X1 + .655X7 - .326X, + .321X, - .151X,0 + .162X ,1 - .201X ,3 - .181X 1 3+.156X 1, - .168,, + .100X ,,

Table 9: Multiple correlations of third-grade predictors to 13th-grade peerrated aggression for boys

Xa Predictor Rz R Partialed r

1 TV violence 3 .09 .31 .31 184 2 Father's mobility 3 .13 .36 .20 .19 144 3 Discrepancy in identification .18 .39 .17 .17 167 with mother 3 4 Father's lack of nurturance 3 .17 .41 -.12 -.12 144 5 Parental disharmony 3 .18 .42 .12 .14 186 6 Father's punishment 3 .19 .44 .12 .14 144 7 Mother's church attendance 3 .20 .45 .09 .02 188

aFinal standardized regression equation: AGG13 .278X, + .193X2 + .130X,- .147X4 + .132X, + .112X6 + .087X,

These regressions were computed by a stepwise method that entered the variables into the equation in order of their utility in predicting the 63 5h 11'.1.F.VISION AND ADOI.F.SCEN1 MaittF.SSIVFNE.Sti

criterion aggression variable. Hence. the incrementin R2 when a varia- ble is entered reveals the proportion of the varianceit predicts when used in conjunction with the variables previouslyentered. From this fact and from the data in Table9. one can see that for pre- dicting aggression in the thirteenth grade, thethird-grade television vio- lence variable was the single most useful predictorof all third-gradecau- sal variables.It accounted for nine percent of thetotal variance and about 40 percent of the variance thatcould be explained by all 20 third- grade causal variables. In predicting third-and eighth-grade aggression. the third-grade television violence variablewas the sixth and eleventh best predictor respectively. but accountedfor three percent of the total variance in each case. This regression demonstrates thata substantial component of aggres- sion at all three grade !evels anda particularly large component at the thirteenth grade can be predicted better by theamount of television vio- lence the child watched in thc third grade than byany other causal varia- ble measured and reinforces the contention that thereis a cause and effect relation between the violence content of televisionand overt ag- gressive behavior. However, as shown in Table 7,the best predictor of third-grade aggression is third-grade IQ. As Darlington (1968) has pointed out. onecan treat the standardized coefficients in a multiple regression equationas measures of the causal contributions of the predictor variables to the criterionvariable. This approach, called "path analysis,"assumes that all causal variables not in the regression equationare uncorrelated with those that are. While it is clear that this assumption is violated, it is still worthwhileto examine the coefficients as approximatemeasures of causal contributions. Table 9 reveals that of all the third-grade causal variables,third-grade television violence is the major "cause" of thirteenth-gradeaggression with a beta weight of .276. Similarly.as indicated in Table 7, not watch- ing television and television violence watchedare the fifth and sixth most important "causes" of concurrent third-grade aggression with standardized betas of .190 and .188. respectively. On theother hand. third-grade television violence is not revealedto be a major "cause" of eighth-grade aggression as seen in Table 8. Relation of early to later aggression

The second hypothesis states that early aggression isa predictor of and a basis for later aggression. Table 10 contains theintercorrelations among the peer-rated aggression scores for boys obtained in thethird grade (AGG3), the eighth grade (AGG8), andat the thirteenth (AGG13). Table I I contains the same type of information about girls.The six cor- relations in Table 10 and II are all dependablynonzero (significant at the

64 "IV VIOLENC AND CHILD AGGRESSION 57

Table 10: Intercorrelations for boys among peer nominations of aggression obtained in 3rd, 8th, and 13th grades

AGG3 AGG8a AGG13

AGG3 1.00 .48 .38 AGG8 1.00 .65 AGG13 1.00 Mean 12.12 79.47 80.46 S.D. 12.73 89.05 96.98 N 211 71 211

aThe AGG8 and AGG13 scores presented here and in the next table were multiplied by 10. Otherwise they were calculated in the same manner as was AGG3.

Table 11: Intercorrelations for girls among peer nominations of aggression obtained in 3rd, 8th and 13th grades

AGG3 AGG8 AGG13

AGG3 1.00 .30 .47 AGG8 1.00 .52 AGG13 1.00 Mean 7.51 39.98 26.51 S.D. 9.87 49.85 37.68 N 216 79 216

.01 level). For each sex group the highest correlation was the one be- tween the aggression scores at eighth and thirteenth grades. All the scores are obviously skewed to the high end. This J curve phenomenon has been discussed in Walder et al. (1961) and Eron et al. (1971). Each of the correlations among these early and later aggression scores needs to be examined for its dependence upon some third variable such as IQ, social status, or aggressive milieu. Table 12 includes data relevant

Table 12: Correlations for boys between early and later aggression with selected controls

Control variables for partialed r s Father's occupa Father's Predictor Outcome r 103 tional status 3 aggression 3

AGG3 AGG8 .48 .43 .51 .50 71 AGG13 .38 .34 .39 .38 211 AGG8 AGG13 .65 .64 .65 .65 71 ASB13 .39 .37 .38 .38 70 TAH13a .48 .46 .47 Zi3 .47 71 TAG13b .48 .47 .47 .47 71

aTAH = Admission by the subject that he has displayed aggressive and antisocial behaviors. bTAG = Statements by the subject that he has expressed, witnessed, and experienced aggression. 65 58 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS to this type of cor cern. For the boys data each of the three intercorrela- tions has been partialed with respect to the boys' IQsin the third grade. the occupational (social) status of the boys' fathersas measured by the 1960 Bureau of the Census index (low numbers here referto high occu- pational status), and the fathers' aggression levels (thefathers' scores on Walters and Zak's Personal Opinion Inventory). These threethird-grade measures were used as controls since the interest herewas to predict from early aggression. The first three rows of Table 12 contain the three correlations which were presented in Table 10. The remaining three rows contain otherpre- dictor-outcome correlations. These are the survivors froma selection procedure which first looked for dependablynonzero correlations be- tween early aggression measures (AGG3 or AGG8) and any other mea- sures that were obtained from or about the children at any later time in the five- or ten-year followup studies. This involved examining the rele- vance of early aggression to later peer rating measures, school and occu- pational variables, television scores, test results (e.g., M M PI andZ-Test scores), self-descriptions, and attitude responses. A number of these later measures could, in fact, be predicted from aggressionscores which had been obtained five and ten years earlier. These substantial correla- tions over long periods of developmental time represent impressivepost hoc evidence of reliability for these measures of aggression and of other behaviors. (See, e.g., Nunnally, 1967, p. 172 ff. for his discussion of reli- ability as a necessary but not sufficient condition for validity.) The second aspect of this selection procedure required that each de- pendably nonzero predictive correlation be partialed with respect toone of the three control variables, as listed in Table 12. Predictor-outcome pairs whose correlations became markedly smaller whenany one of these three variables was controlled were not retained for presentation here. Outcome variables such as peer-rated aggression-anxiety, school achievement test scores, later IQ scores,some school behaviors such as tardiness and attainment in school, and selected television behaviors did not survive this test in the data for boys. Thus Table 12 contains predic- tor-outcome correlations which demonstrate validities over five- and ten- year spans of time and which are not functions of any of the control var- iables examined. Table 13 presents variables which were selectedon the same basis for girls. A third selection procedure was employed to determine which predic- tor-outcome pairs should be presented here. Predictive correlations which were dependably nonzero and whose partial correlationswere not lowered were subjected to this third requirement. Specifically,a one- way analysis of variance using unweighted means with the predictoras the independent variable and the outcomeas the dependent variable yielded a significant F. Tables 14 and 15 present the six surviving predic- tor-outcome relations for the boys, and Table 16 and 17 present the five 66 TV VIOLENCE: AND CHILD AGGRESSION 59

Table 13: Correlatioas for girls between early and later aggression with selected controls

Control variables for partialedL s Father's occupa Father's Predictor Outcome 103 tional status 3 aggression 3

AGG3 AGG13 .47 .44 .46 .47 216 TVSPT13 .22 .25 .22 .22 216 AGG8 AGG13 .52 .51 .51 .52 79 TAH13 .37 .38 .40 .37 79 TAG13 .41 .42 .45 .41 79

Table 14: Mean predicted scores as a function of earlier aggression in boys

AGG8 AGG13 AGG3 N M SO M SO

Low 44 not 24.61 35.14 Med 139 available 83.10 92.77 Hi 28 155.07 127.67

Total 211 80.46 96.98 F 18.195 t.6.50 P < .0009 surviving predictor-outcome relations for the girls. (An error in scoring has temporarily delayed the availability of the ANOVA for the AGG3 to AGG8 relation.) The boys' data indicated that one can predict from aggression in the third grade to aggression in the eighth and thirteenth grades. Also one can predict from aggression in the eighth grade not only to peer-rated aggression in the thirteenth grade but also to various self-ratings of ag- gression obtained in that grade. These self-ratings were Antisocial Be- havior, Total Aggressive Habit, and Total Aggressive Environment. Tables 14 and 15 show that heterogeneity of variance occurs for the AGGI3 variables. In each case a nest between means of the two groups differing most on variances was calculated. The t was evaluated con- servatively using as the number of degrees of freedom the N of the smaller group, 28 and II, respectively. The prediction from third to thir- teenth-grade aggression was unaffected but that from eighth to thirteenth (Table 15) was less certain, t = 2.63, p <.05. Similar t tests were per- formed for certain of the girls' data, as illustrated in Tables 16 and 17 where the variance was heterogeneous. Only television sports failed to survive this testt was not statistically significant. The girls' data indicated that one can predict from aggression in the third grade to aggression ten years later (AGG13). From aggression in the eighth grade (AGG8) predictions can be made not only to aggression in the thirteenth grade but to two self-descriptive aggression scores (TAH and TAG) which have alreVeen described for the boys. AGG8 N M Table 15:AGG13 Mean predicted scores as a function of earlier aggression in boys SD M ASB SD M TAH SD M TAG SD HiMedLo 471113 120.73 66.77 9.39 146.32 71.7212.39 21.7025.73 9.833 12.51 4.617.55 36.6429.5314.46 14.73 8.477.62 58.5545.8930.23 12.7919.1914.85 -PTOTALF 71 15.51275.47< .0009 t=2.6397.03< .05 20.30< .001 7.215 11.90 27.87< .00099.860 14.51 44.99< .001 7.838 19.31 aThis mean is based upon 12 cases; the mean of the total is therefore based upon 70 cases, df = 2 and 67. TV VIOLENCE ANDCHILD AGGRESSION 61

Table 16: Mean predicted scores asa function of earlier aggression in girls f AGG1 3 TV-SPT AGG3 N M SD M SD Lo 29 9.21 13.94 5.41 0.91 Med 1 65 22.78 28.71 5.62 1.65 Hi 22 - 77.27 67.95 6.73 2.12 Total 2 16 26.51 37.68 5.71 1.66 30.340 t=5.36 4.990 t=1.05 < .009 < .01 < .0 08 ns

Table 1 7: Mean predicted scores as a function of earlieraggression in girls

AGG13 TAH TAG AGG8 N M SD M SD M SD Lo 13 2.77 5.10 18.39 8.20 2 9.31 9.67 Med 53 19.94 27.9 6 19.51 10.17 30.42 11.89 Hi 13 56.15 65.38 28.23 9.44 42.62 12.21 - I Total 79 23.08 37.96 20.76 10.21 3d:.24 I 12.38 1 F 8.280t=4.12 4.621 6.250 P < .001 < .01 e.013 < .003

To be assured that one can reasonably predictlater scores from earlier aggression, the predictive relations with thegreatest heterogeneity of variance (A003 to AGG13)are presented in Table 18 for boys and 19

Table 18: Predictability of AGG13 from AGG3 for boys

AGG13

%ile 28 50 75 99 % of row Sum % of Col

28 % of row 40.929.5 15.9 13.6 99.9 N 18 13 7 6 44 20.9 50 % of row 2.535.035.027.5 100.0 N 1 14 14 11 40 19.0 A 75 G % of row 20.0 16.423.640.0 100.0 G N 11 9 13 22 55 26.1 3 99 % of row 2.8 9.730.656.9 100.0 N 2 7 22 41 72 34.1 SUM 32 43 56 80 211 100.1 % of row 15.220.426.539.7 100.0

Chisquare = 60.538' df = 9, P < 0.0009

for girls. For best prediction, the highest row percentage indicatedas the top number in each cell should be on the diagonal. The table should 69 61 1F.I.F.VISION :AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVFNESS

Table 19: Predictability of AGG13 from AGG3 for girls

AGG13

%-ile 28 50 7 5 99 %of row Sum % of Col.

28 %of row 56.2 25.016.2 2.5 99.9 N 45 20 1 3 2 8 0 37.0 5 0 %of row 41.524.524.5 9.4 99.9 N 22 13 1 3 5 5 3 24.5 A75 % of row 34.1 29.52 5.0 11.4 100.0 G N 15 13 11 5 44 20.4 G 39 9 %of row 23.1 10.328.2 38.5 100.1 N 9 4 11 15 39 18.1 SUM 91 50 48 27 21 6 100.0 % of row 42.1 23.1 2 2.2 12.5 99.9

Chisquare = 4 1.631' " df = 9, P < 0.0009 approximate a simplex with percentages high at the diagonal and decreas- ing in the cells farther from the diagonal. This monotonic relation isex- pressed by the correlations already provided for the AGG3 and AGG13 relation. By inspection one can see that for both sexgroups the best prediction is from the extreme categories of third-grade aggression. It should be noted that the percentile score groupings were established for each aggression score by examining each distribution of pooledscores of both sexes. The obvious relation between sex and aggression showsup in the resulting skew to the high scores in the two middlerows of the girls' table (Table 19) and a smaller skew to the low scores in the two middle rows of the boys' table (Table 18). In spite of these shortcotn- ings, inspection of these two tables gives assurance thatone can predict fairly well that a person's relative position on AGG3 will general,y be maintained ten years later in AGG 13. This prediction is possible in yite of the considerable skewness of both distributions. It seems fair to say that the second hypothesis is supported byour ability to predict from earlier aggression scores to aggression and other related scores obtained five and ten years later. These predictionsare not a function of the child's IQ or the social status or aggression level of the father. Coupled with the support of the first hypothesis, we may say from hypothesis one that precursors including television behaviorsare related to contemporary and later aggression. From hypothesis, twoone can say that aggression is related to subsequent peer- and self-rated ag- gression. The examination of the data so far has revealed, once more,a difference between boys and girls in the distribution of aggression scores and in the relations of these scores to other variables. TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 63 Aggression and psychopathology Hypothesis three deals with the relation of aggression to psychopath- ology. This hypothesis was tested by examining the correlations be- tween various measures of aggression and measures of maladjustment. These latter measures were composed of certain components of the MM PI, the psychopathology score of the Z-Test, school achievement, and arrest record. Table 20 presents the Pearson product moment correlation coeffi- cients for boys between various measures of aggression and the clinical scales of the MMPI. These measures were obtained from the subjects'

Table 20: Correlations for boys between various measures of aggression and psychopathology

MMPI MMP1 Clinical Scales MMPI scales 4+9 Scales Aggression (Psychopathology) Partialed 4+9

Frequency of home aggression Father 3 144 .14 .09 Frequency of home aggression mother 3 186 .06 .04 Recency of home aggression father 3 144 .22 .1 3 .19 Recency of home aggression mother 3 186 .16 .11 AGG3 211 .07 .07 .21" AGG8 71 .07 .20 AGG13 211 .23" .02 .39" Personal opinion inventory (Walters-Zak) 13 211 .11 .17 Antisocial behavior 13 209 .28' ' .00 .50" Total aggressive habit 13 211 .27** .04 .48** Total aggressive environment 13 211 .27** .03 .48" 103 205 .12 .16 Social status 1 3 209 .15 .15

parents or from the subjects them selves in the third, eighth. and/or thir- teenth grade. The criterion measures (e.g.. MMPI. achievement, ar- rests) were obtained only at the period of the thirteenth grade. Because M M PI scale 4 and scal,e 9 may be more a measure of delin- quent behavior (Hathaway and Monachesi, 1963) or an active hostility index (Dahlstrom and Welsh, 1960) than a measure of psychopathology, the effects of these scales were removed by partial correlation tech- nique. With this control, five if the variables in Table 20 entered into 71 1 64 1E1.1:VISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

significant relations with the M M PI clinical scales-the criterionmea- sure. As a result of the control for the 4 plus 9 scale. all of these correla- tions were reduced to zero order as seen in column 4. The correlations of aggression measures with the sum of scales 4 and 9are presented in column 5. The peer nominations of aggression obtained in the third grade (AGG3) and those obtained in the tcn-year followup (AGG 13) related significantly to this later putativemeasure of delinquency. Mod- erate correlations also occur between scales 4 plus 9 and the following self-ratings of aggression: Antisocial Behavior (ASB), Total Aggressive Habit (TAH) and Total Aggressive Environment (TAG). These three lat- ter measures were obtained concurrently with the M M PI. Neither IQ nor social status enters into a significant relation with the psychopathol- ogy measure or scales 4 plus 9. Since the Z-Test proved unrelated to any of the aggression measures, the results are not presented.

Table 21: Correlations for girls between various measures of aggression and psychopathology

MMP1 MMP1 Clinical Scales MMPI scales 4+9 Scales Aggression (Psychopathology) Partialed 4+9

Frequency of home aggression- father 3 157 .00 .0C Frequency of home aggression- mother 3 184 .07 .05 Recency of home aggression- father 3 157 -.07 .00 Recency of home aggression- 1 mother 3 184 -.04 .00 AGG3 216 .03 .12 AGG8 79 .12 .27 AGG13 216 .10 Personal opinion inventory 13 21 6 .15 .231* Antisocial behavior 13 211 .29** -.00 .451* Total aggressive habit 13 21 6 .271* -.03 .451' Total aggressive environment 13 21 6 .30" .01 .451 103 207 -.15 , -.07 -.14 Social status 13 210 .03 .07 Table 21 preents similar data for girls. The only significant relations between the measures of aggression and psychopathology occurred for those measures obtained concurrently: ASB, TAH, TAG. However, when the effect of scales 4 plus 9 was controlled by partial correlation, these relations were reduced to zero orderas shown in column 4. 72 IV VIOLENCE ANDCHILD AGGRESSION

Table 22: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 1 3 for boys rated high or low on aggression in grade 3

MMPI Low (74) High (34) Scale M SD SD

L 47.54 6.66 49.35 7.04 1.29 F 57.30 9.08 61.32 14.4 6 1.76 K 51.54 8.38 50.26 8.13 .74

1 53.38 1 1.57 53.62 6.66 .11 2 55.97 1 11.21 55.91 12.64 .03 3 55.36 11.30 56.62 7.48 .59 4 56.73 9.8 6 63.32 11.96 3.01" 5 60.80 10.29 58.94 8.73 .91 6 54.53 9.82 59.26 12.50 2.13 7 58.74 10.42 59.41 12.13 .29 8 59.58 1 2.33 64.79 16.10 1.85 9 61.68 1 1.82 66.21 12.64 1.81 0 53.38 9.9 7 52.26 9.37 .55 1 CI 111.31 12.39 99.18 11.5 5 4.83" Social status 4.23 .2.69 4.12 1.74 .22

Inspection of column 5 shows that scales 4 plus 9 relate to these three measures of aggression positively as they did for boys: allincrease con- siderably in magnitude. Consequently, the relation between the mea- sures of aggression and the M M PI clinical scales wasonly apparent; the variance in this relation was accounted for by scales 4 plus 9. Again, nei- tht-r IQ nor social status entered into significant relations with the M M PI measures of psychopathology and delinquency. Because some of the relations between aggression and the M M PI may have been obscured by compressing the 11 clinical scales into one mea- sure, a more extensive analysis of the M M PI wasconducte6. Presenter;

Table 23: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 1 3 for girls rated high or low on aggression in grade 3

MMPI Low (80) High (39) Scale M sD

L 49.29 6.84 50.59 7.18 .96 F 54.09 7.61 59.03 13.2 1 2.58 K 53.16 7.65 52.23 7.40 .63 .66 1 50.30 7.56 51.33 8.90 2 52.45 8.74 53.85 13.02 .69 3 54.61 7.51 54.72 6.4 7 .08 4 57.31 10.97 62.31 13.30 2.17 5 46.55 8.83 54.03 8.71 4.36" 6 56.72 9.27 57.51 11.07 .41 7 55.92 7.13 56.62 9.07 .45 8 55.56 8.4 3 59.82 13.14 2.14 9 59.05 1 1.11 62.74 9.97 1.76 0 ,53.10 9.32 51.21 9.5 5 1.03 1C1 108.61 12.56 97.28 1 3.4 5 4.52" Social status 3.79 1.65 4.13 1.88 1.01 .73 66 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 24: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 13for boys rated high or low in aggression in grade 8

MMPI Low (18) High (19) Scale M SD M SD -t L 47.33 6.65 45.21 8.27 .91 F 57.72 6.52 60.63 10.51 1.00 K 53.06 6.49 49.37 9.38 1.38 1 51.67 13.30 54.32 8.41 .73 2 57.22 14.97 56.26 9.83 .23 3 56.67 8.90 57.16 9.87 .16 4 55.28 10.12 62.63 11.49 2.06 5 61.00 7.81 59.21 12.09 .54 6 52.39 9.56 59.53 11.38 2.06 7 59.22 12.78 61.26 12.23 .50 8 57.17 14.81 63.58 15.49 1.29 9 54.94 12.53 64.47 11.93 2.37 0 58.72 11.06 51.63 6.49 2.39 ICI 118.39 13.28 105.05 11.52 3.27" Social status 3.39 1.46 3.37 1.61 .04

in Table 22 are the means and standarddeivations of the 13 basic MMPI scales for boys in theupper and lower quartiles of aggressionas deter- mined by peer nominations in thethird grade. Again, values for IQand social status serve as controlmeasures. Inspection of this table demon- strates that high aggressive boys havea significantly higher mean score on scale 4 as indicated by the t test betweenmeans. Furthermore the IQs differ significantly: the high aggressivegroup has a lower mean IQ. Also evident in Table 22 is the absenceof a significant relation betweenag- gression and social status. The same analysis is presented forgirls in Table 23. These data show that girls who received the highestpeer nominations for aggression Table 25: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 13 for girls rated high or low in aggression in grade 8

MMPI Low (20) High (21) Scale M SD M SD

L 50.85 7.95 46.71 4.92 2.01 F 50.85 5.75 55.33 9.60 1.80 K 68.10 5.99 52.52 7.78 2.56 1 49.40 3.50 49.24 6.50 .10 2 51.35 9.66 50.24 7.13 .42 3 64.05 5.83 55.76 6.22 .91 4 53.50 9.25 59.86 13.61 1.74 5 46.65 10.46 48.38 9.07 .57 6 56.10 9.30 56.81 8.32 .11 7 56.80 10.31 56.43 7.79 .13 8 57.20 10.07 56.52 7.87 .24 9 57.55 9.71 61.43 10.31 1.24 0 53.45 6.77 60.29 8.28 1.19 ICI 108.50 14.70 108,14 10.37 .09 Social status 3.35 2.01 3.38 1.56 .06 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 67 when they were in the third grade havesignificantly higher scores on scale 5 ten years later than girls categorizedin the low aggressive group. This scale is a measure of masculinity-femininityand a high score for girls is in the masculine direction. Again, IQ wasnegatively related to aggression whereas social status was unrelated toaggression. The relations between ratings of aggressionfor boys in the eighth grade and the MMPI administered in thethirteenth grade are presented in Table 24. Although several of the clinical!,cales are elevated in the predicted direction, none of the ts reachesthe stipulated level of signifi- cance. IQ was negativelyrelated to aggression for this group, andsocial status was unrelated.

Table 26: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 13for boys rated high or low in aggression at the OMR period

MMP1 Low IMO High 1531 Welch's t Scale M SD M SD t

L 49.26 7.45 47.79 6.71 1.09 F 56.02 7.66 66.58 14.38 4.89** 4.76" K 54.10 7.43 49.64 7.31 3.18** .99 1 53.09 10.33 64.96 9.45 2 55.67 11.29 58.04 12.79 1.03 3 55.G3 10.40 56.60 10.13 .55 4 55.66 8.71 66.13 10.81 5.64 5 60.00 10.05 68.53 9.73 .78 6 53.55 9.47 60.72 12.65 3.40" 7 59.45 11.18 62.68 13.05 1.40 8 58.74 11.41 69.60 16.53 4.06" 9 59.33 12.03 69.40 10,71 4.64" 0 54.64 10.30 51.68 8.07 1.67 IQ 111.48 13.14 106.30 . 12.94 2.49 Social status 3.83 1.74 4.34 1.53 1.64

Table 27: Comparison of MMPI mean scores in grade 13 for girls rated high or low in aggression at the same period

MMPI Low 1581 High (54) Scale SD M SD t Welch's t

L 49.41 7.24 60.30 7.13 .t ; F 50.88 6.17 59.17 11.91 4.84"4.73" K 53.91 7.11 52.44 7.77 1.04 1 48.88 6.61 50.98 8.18 1.76 2 51.68 8.58 52.85 9.65 .69 3 52.68 6.47 55.02 7.05 1.99 4 54.50 9.65 64.72 12.37 4.90" 6 46.38 9.37 62.80 9.13 3.66" 6 54.67 8.71 58.07 10.85 1.84 7 55.52 7.32 55.98 7.85 .32 8 54.60 8.54 58.98 11.25 2.33 9 57.60 10.32 63.30 10.57. 2.88" o 54.14 8.49 60.76 8.29 2.13 I Q 108.22 11.49 102.80 13.77 2.27 Social istatus 3.91 1.78 4.02 1.49 .34 68 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 28: Comparison of MMPImean scores in grade 13 (carboys rated high high or low.lowon ratings of aggressican in grades 3 and13 MMPI Low (31 ) High (17) Scale M rvi SD L 47.26 . -1. 6.63 48.12 F 7.11 .42 57)6 7.07 65.94 K 18.24 2.39 54.45 7.89 51.29 1 6.53 1.41 ... 53.87 11.95 52.59 2 7.59 .40 ,, 57.23 12.96 58.00 3 14.97 .19 I/ 56.03 13.15 57.47 4 9.27 .40 .1 56.55 8.79 68.06 5 11.56 3.88" I 63.55 10.21 59.88 9.95 6 54.32 1.20 9.56 60.76 14.55 1.85 7 60 .06 10.75 61.71 14.39 .45 8 59.77 11.53 9 70.41 19.33 2.39 60.55 13.04 71.71 9.78 3.08" 0 54.52 11.17 10 50.65 9.73 1.20 113.74 13.28 98.53 9,55 Social status 3.90 4.16" 1.58 4.82 1.33 2.04

For girls the same analysisis illustrated in Table25. No significant differences occurred betweenM M PI scalesor the cotitrol meastires of IQ and social status. Table 26 presents the analysisof mean differenceson M M PI scores for boys grouped into the lower and upper quartiles ofaggression ac- cording to peer nominations received synchronously. Thehigh aggres- sive boys have significantly higher meanscores on the F-scale andon scales 4, 6, 8, and 9. In addition, this group manifests significantlylower scores on the K-scale when compared withthe low aggressivegroup. IQ

Table 29: Comparison of MMPImean scores in grade 13 for girls rated highhigh or low-lowon ratings of aggression in grades 3 and13 MMPI Low (23) High (24) Scale M SD M SD t Welch's t L 50.85 7.97 53.13 . F 6.47 1.15 50.73 4.95 60.46 14.22 K 54.30 3.65"3.16" 6.56 53.50 7.71 .42 1 48.91 ( 5.30 52.00 8.41 2 1.70 51.39 8.40 54.171 3 11.14 1.07 52.85 5.15 54.50, 4 6.61 1.06 54.03 9.31 65.13 13.71 5 3.64* 47.48 10.10 54.92 7.98 6 54.58 2.99" 9.09 58.92 11.97 1.56 7 54.45 7.32 57.38 8 8.29 1.41 53.24 8.90 59.75 9 13.61 2.18 66.55 10.21 63.25 10.86 0 2.38 54.48 8.34 50.25 7.74 10 1.95 107.33 11.63 95.29 16.07 Social status 4.12 3.41" 1.83 4.00 1.62 ..26 IV VIOLENCE ANDCHILD AGGRESSION 69 and social status were not significantly related to aggression. In the case of the F-scale. the variance between groups proved to be heterogeneous (F = 3.508 for 52 and 57 degrees of freedom). This scale was reanalyzed using Welch's t (Winer. 1962). and both the mean and variability on the F-scale were found to be significantly greavr for the high aggressive group. The same data are presented for girls in the thirteenth grade in Table 27. High aggressive girls evinced significantly higher mean scores on the F-scale and on scales 4, 5, and 9. IQ and social status were not siPnifi-

Table 30: Correlations for boys between various measures of aggression and intellectual functioning

Social status Aggression Achievement 10 partialed partialed

Frequency of home aggression 34 .05 father 3 Frequency of home aggression 45 .09 mother 3 Recency of home aggression 34 .16 father 3 Recency of home aggression 45 .00 mother 3 AGG3 58 .32 i AGG8 23 .30 AGG13 58 .36" .31 .32 Personal opinion inventory 13 58 .59** .54** Antisocial behavior 13 57 .26 Total aggressive habit 13 58 .29 Total aggressive . 58 .25 environment 13 MMPI 4+9 13 58 .25 10.3 57 .53** .51" Social status 13 E7 .37" .33* cantly related to aggression for this sample. Again group variances on the F-scale were heterogeneous: F = 5.259, p <.01 for df = 53 and 57. According to Welch's t, both the means and variances on the F-scale appear to be related to the classification byaggression. Tables 28 and 29 present analyses for boys and girls who met the con- ditions of being consistently in either the upper or lower quartiles of ag- gression at both the third and thirteenth grades. Table 28 demonstrates that the high-high aggressive boys have significantly higher mean scores on scales 4 and 9. In addition IQ forthis group is significantly lower compared with the low-low aggressives: social status is unrelated to ag- gression. 70 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 31: Correlations for girls between variousmeasures of aggression and intellectual functioning

Social status Aggression Achievement 10 partialed partialed

Frequency of home aggression 43 .23 father 3

Frequency of home aggression 50 .17 mother 3

Recency of home aggression 43 .29 father 3 Recency of home aggression 50 .03 mother 3 AGG3 62 .30 AGG8 37 .44" .47" .45" AGG13 62 _39" .34* .39" Personal opinion inventory 13 62 .19 Antisocial behavior 13 60 Total aggressive habit 13 62 .08 Total aggressive 62 .04 environment 13 MMP1 4+9 13 61 .20 103 59 .60" Social status 13 61 .26

Table 29 shows that the high-highaggressive girls have significantly elevated scores on the F-scale andon scales 4 and 5. The low-low ag- gressives have significantly highermean IQs. A test of the variances of the F-scale yielded the following:F = 8.416, df = 23 and 72,p < .01. According to Welch's t for this scale,both the means and variances seem to have been directly affected by the aggressiongroupings. Again, IQ is significantly higher for the low-lowaggressives. Table 30 presents the correlations forboys between the variousmea- sures of aggression and intellectual functioning. Thelatter variable was measured by standardized achievementtest scores obtained when the subjects were in the twelfth grade.Because IQ and social statusare cor- related with achievement (r= .53 and .37, respectively) the effects of these variables on achievementwere controlled by partial correlation. Inspection of Table 30 illustratesthat none of the third-grademeasures of home aggression obtained fromthe parents' interview predicted later school functioning. Among thepeer nomination measures, aggressionas measured in the thirteenth gradewas apparently related to achievement but did not quite survive the control bypartial correlation for the effects of IQ and social status. The onlymeasure of aggression related to achievement irrespective of these controlswas the Personal Opinion TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 71 Inventory administered in the thirteenth grade. The higher the aggres- sion, the lower the achievement. Similar data for girls is presented in Table 31. The peer nominations of aggressionin the eighth (AGG8) and thirteenth grades (AGG13) were negatively related to achievement. These relations did withstand the controls for IQ and social status. Again, aggression was negatively related to achievement. Because of the nature of the measures, it is im- probable that method variance is contributing to these relations. The contingency relation between peer nominations of aggression in the third grade and arrest during adolescence is presented in Table 32.

Table 32: Chi-square analysis of arrests by levels of aggression

Boys

AGG3 Arrests Yes No Total

High 7 118 125 Low _2 128 130 9 246 255

Chisquare3.087 p .075

The classification of aggression is by upper and lower quartile. Only those arrests which occurred in New York State were available and only in aggregate form. Arrest charges were as follows: burglary, grand larce- ny, larceny 3rd, attempted grand larceny, sexual misconduct,criminal mischief, petty larceny. Although more than three times as many boys were arrested in the high as compared to the low aggression group,the chi-square of 3.087 does not reach statistical significance.

DISCUSSION

The above results would tend to indicate that television habits estab- lished by age eight influence aggressive and other behaviors at that time and at least through late adolescence. This is more true for boys than for girls, although many of the relations for girls are in the same direction as those for boys, though less strong. The more violent the programs pre- ferred by boys in the third grade, the more aggressive is their behavior both at that time and ten years later. This positive relation between early television habits and later behavior prevails both for peer-rated aggres- sion and for self-ratings of aggression. Actually these early television habits seem to be more influential than current viewing patterns,. since 79 Jle 72 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS the number of hours per week that the subject watches television inthe thirteenth grade and the violence ratings of his preferredprograms at that time are not at all related to current aggressivebehavior; nor are early television habits related to later television habits. Similarlythere is a stronger negative relation between IQ at grade three and number of hours television is watched at grade thirteen thanbetween IQ at grade twelve and extent of television watching at grade thirteen.However, the attenuation in relation to the two later obtainedmeasures may be a func- tion of the smaller number of subjects for whom theinvestigators could obtain IQ scores at the laterage (N = 53). It should be noted that the lack of relation betweenlater television habits and later aggression is nota result of inadequate measurement operations. The television ratings,as pointed out above, were reliably made and were closely correlated with ratings independentlymade in at least two other studies (Greenberg and Gordon, 1970; Feshbachand Singer, 1971). Furthermore, although unrelatedto current aggressive behavior, these violence ratingsare related significantly to other current and past behaviors, as noted above. It is unlikely that thefindings pre- sented are a result of unreliable and/or invalid ratings of either television violence or aggressive behavior. Wecan surmise that the absence of a concurrent relation between television violence and aggressive behavior in the thirteenth grade is due to the fact that this behaviorhas already been established and is no longer responsive to conditions whichinflu- ence such behavior in the young. The multiple R analyses provide further evidence that the violenceof preferred television programs at the third grade level retainsa strong predictive relation to aggression five andten years later. Of 20 different variables which entered significantly intoany of the three multiple R s, television violence is one of only three which madea significant contri- bution to all three levels. It can be assumed that children whoprefer vio- lent programs in the third grade will continueto watch such programs as they get older; since the relation with aggression isstronger in the thir- teenth grade than it is in the third grade,we can hypothesize that the effect of television violence on aggressive behavior is cumulative.It has been demonstrated that the more hoursa subject watches television and the more he prefers violentprograms, the more likely he is to judge that the situations depicted in television westerns and crime storiesare real- istic representationS of life. One might speculate that subjectscontinual- ly exposed to television violence would not perceive theirown aggres- sive behavior as deviant or unusualthis is theway life is and the way one goes about solving problems. Inhibitions against expressing overt aggression would thus be diminished. These findings showing a direct positive relation between the viewing of television violence and aggressive behavioron the part of the viewer corroborate in a field study what has been demonstrated in the labora- TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 73 tory (Bandura, Ross, and Ross, 1961, 1963a, 1963b; Berkowitz, 1964; Berkowitz and Rawlings, 1963; Berkowitz, Corwin, and Heironimus, 1960). Because the foregoing are manipulative studies in which system- atically varied treatments were administered to randomly selected sub- jects under controlled conditions, statements about cause and effect re- lations based on their findings can be made with more confidence per- haps than those based on findings of naturalistic studies. In such studies, many uncontrolled variables are unaccounted for, and observation and measurement cannot be as precise. The investigators feel that their re- cent finding relating television violence and behavior over a ten-year pe- riod strengthens the conviction that this is indeed a real relation. The direction indicated for the relation is that viewing violence regularly on television at age eight leads to more aggressive behavior on the part of the viewer at that time and also in subsequent years than does viewing nonviolent programs. Feshbach and Singer (1971), in a bold attempt to compromise between laboratory manipulation and a field setting, provided findings which would indicate that the viewing of aggressive programs on television lelds to a diminution of aggressive behavior on the part of the viewers, at least for some types of subjects. They regulated the amount of televi- sion aggression viewed by their subjects over a six-week period by pre- scribing an aggressive or nonaggressive diet of television programs to groups of subjects. (The authors believed they were able to do this suc- cessfully because the subjects were enrolledin residential private schools and homes for boys.) The researchers employed a number of behavioral, attitudinal, and fantasy measures, both self-ratings and rat- ings by peers and supervisors, before the experiment began and at var- ious times throughout the study. It was found in general that the boys who were exposed to an aggressive television diet decreased in manifes- tations of aggression over this period while the control subjects in- creased in aggression. Actually the research by Feshbach and Singer addressed itself to a different question than did the present research, which is more con- cerned with the long-range effects of viewing television violence and with more pervasive aggressive dispcsitions. In this regard it is of inter- est that Feshbach and Singer used the peer rating measure which we had developed for our studies (Walder et al., 1961) in order to assess preex- perimental aggressive levels of their subjects and thereby separate them into high and low aggressive subsamples. They found: "Regardless of experimental group, boys who score high on the peer aggression nomi- nation measure display about twice as much aggressive behavior to- wards peers as boys who score low on the peer nomination measure" (Feshbach and Singer, 1971,p.91). Thus any manipulationthat changes the peer nomination score would indeed have powerful effects. 81 74 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

It is this measure that is affected by television preference.The findings of Feshbach and Singer are smaller and perhapsmore transient. Feshbach and Singer criticize laboratory studies of aggressionin that they do not "...replicate real life viewing and behavioralconditions sufficiently to permit extrapolation to the effects ofthe depiction of vio- lence in the media on daily behavior" (p. 43). However,although these researchers have come a long way from the laboratory in settingup their research arena, they are still themselves far from approximatingnatural situations. The subjects watched television ingroups of ten to 18 boys at precise times either in their classroomsor in their headmaster's or teach- er's living rooms, for a minimum of six hoursa week. In two of the schools, participation by the subjectswas compulsory; in five it was voluntary. There was of course no voluntary choice oftelevision pro- grams since the boys were restricted to the television dietto which they were assigned according to the experimental design. There issome indi- cation that at least at first this lack of preferencewas resented by boys assigned to the nonviolent diet, since itmeant they could not watch their favorite programs. In a fewcases the experimenters, in order to retain the cooperation of the subjects, permitted the boys inthe nonaggressive diet to watch Batman even though itwas one of the aggressive pro- grams. Although all subjects were given thesame cover story, that the researchers were interested in the relation betweencertain personality factors and evaluation of televisionprograms, some (it is not stated how many and at v hich schools) guessed the real purpose. All of thesecondi- tions just described do not resemble those underwhich most youngsters in this culture watch television.Nor is their independentmeasure com- parable to our predictor measure, preference for specificprograms. Furthermore, their subjects are not really representativeof any given segment of the population. They ranged from elementarythrough high school level at seven different residential schoolsand institutions in Cal- ifornia and New York. These includeda military school, a coeducational boarding school, a school describedas "very similar to a better New England prep school," and four boys' homesfor "boys with inadequate home care facilitiesor with minor social adjustment prob- lems." What kind of population to which theirresults are generalizable is unclear. The findings holdup significantly only in the homes for boys, not in the private schools. It is not unlikely that conditionsin the private schools approximate more closely real-life viewingand behavioral con- ditions than those in the homes for boys. Theone instance in which the Feshbach and Singer findingswere reversed was at the junior high level in the private schools. The researchers preferredto ascribe this to chance occurrence, but it may indeed bea real effect, given those more natural conditions in the privatesc hool. In a recent review of the litera- ture pertaining to aggression and filmed material, Bryan andSchwartz (1971) conclude that scant supportcan be marshaled for a catharsis prin- ciple from experimental studies. The preponderanceof studies indicated 82 --,--..- --- - TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 75 that exposure to an aggressiw! model increases the viewer's aggressive behavior. Another important difference between the Feshbach and Singer study and the present research is that in the former, the viewing and perform- ance situations are the samethe boys watched televisionin the same setting in which their behavior was evaluated. In the current study tele- vision viewing took place in the home and the aggressive behavior was noted in the school setting. We submit that this is the more natural situa- tion, allowing for displacement and wider generalization of expression of the aggression. The investigators' criticism of the biased sampling represented by Feshbach and Singer's subject population behooves us to justify our own sampling representativeness. Our subjects represent a given popu- lation: 19-year-old boys and girls who had been in the third grade in Co- lumbia County ten years previously. The demographic, socioeconomic, educational, intellectual and other social characteristics of this group have been delineated.As was pointed out in the method section above, a greater proportion of low than high aggressive subjects from the third grade volunteered for the ten-year followup study. This bias in compo- sition of the subject pool was in part controlled statistically in the ANOVA (analysis of variance) procedure. A graphic example of how sampling bias can influence the direction of findings, especially when contemporaneous relations are being consid- ered without reference to longitudinal effects, is in the data presented

Table 33: Mean scores on selected variables as a function of violence of preferred TV programs in grade 8 TVVL3 AGG3 TVVL8 AGG8 TVVL13 AGG13 PID3a N

LOW TVVL8 4.46 16.25 16.5 85.5 2.42 70.2 5.70 12 MED TVVL8 7.45 11.11 98.0 87.2 5.62 82.5 5.94 42 HIGH TVV L8 6.50 9.50 176.2 51.7 6.90 88.1 5.25 10

apl D3 was a 3rd grade measure concerned with discrepancy in identification with mother (see Eron et al., 1971 for explanation of this measure). here. It appeared that in the eighth grade we had found a negative rela- tion between peer-rated aggression of boys and violence score of pre- ferred television programs. This correlation coefficient (r = .245, p = .05) missed the predetermined acceptable alpha level of .01. Table 33 shows the mean aggression and television violence scores of subjects separated according to their eighth-grade television violence scores. It appears that the negative correlation between concurrenttelevision vio- lence and aggression in the eighth grade is caused primarily by a small number of subjects with very high television violence scores and very low aggression scores. From Table 33 one can see that the preference of these children for violent television was not present in the third grade. 83 76 TELEVISION AND ADOI.ESCENTAGGRESSIVENESS

Hence, if the effect of violenttelevision is primarily longitudinal,one would not expect thesechildren to be highly aggressivein the eighth grade. Moreover, one would expect those subjects watchingviolent tel- evision to be more aggressiveby the thirteenth grade. In factthis is the case, as Table 33 reveals. By the thirteenthgrade, the ten high violence watchers have become highlyaggressive. But whywas there a negative correlation between televisionviolence and aggression ratherthan a zero order correlation in the eighth grade?This phenomenoncan be ex- plained by sampling bias.Apparently the ten crucial subjectswere ex- posed to a third-grade environment that made them low in aggressionin the eighth grade. This hypothesisis supported by the data.As indicated in Table 33, theseten subjects identifiedvery closely with their mothers and this variable turnsout to be the best predictor of lowaggression in thc cighth grade (see Table 8).This finding illustrates thenecessity for both longitudinal andmultivariate study when attemptingto specify cause and ettect in a correlation design. Thus far in this discussionthe investigators have dealtprimarily with the causal relation betweentelevision viewing and aggressivebehavior. However, television variablesalso enter into other interestingrelations both contemporaneousand longitudinal. Themore hours the young adult watches television, thelower is his school achievementand peer- rated leadership. However, thedirection of the antecedent-consequent relation cannot yet be stated,since the cross-lagged correlationsthat were available for the television-aggressionrelation are not at hand. For the peer nomination ofpopularity, however,some of the ingredi- ents for establishing sucha relation exist. Figure 3 representsthe corre- lations between peer-ratedpopularity and number of hourstelevision is watched over a ten-year period.While there isno relation between the number of hours a child watchestelevision in the third gradeand his popularity at that time,a negative relationship does existbetween his popularity in the third grade andthe number of hours hewatches televi- sion in the thirteenth grade.Furthermore, there isa negative relation between hours watched in thethirteenth grade and popularityat that time. Of course,a component of the relation betweenthe measures of popularity is probably attributable to the temporal reliability of themea- sure. However, this would not explainthe relations between popularity and television hours watched inthe third or thirteenth grades.The child who is unpopular in the thirdgrade tends to watch televisionmore as he gets older and continues to be unpopular.Although it is undeterminedat this time whether he isunpopular because he watchestelevision or watches television because he isunpopular, there isno doubt that popu- larity at grade three is negativelyrelated to television hourswatched at grade thirteen and positively relatedto popularity at this grade. In summary, therefore, itappears (for boys) that preference fora vio- lent television diet in the thirdgrade ;eads to aggressivebehavior at that time and also in late adolescence. Furthermore, the third-gradechild TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 77 who is unpopular with his peers tends to spend more time watching tele- vision during adolescence. During late adolescence, the more unpopular he is the more time he devotes to television. However, the antecendent- consequent direction of this relation is undeterminate. Similarly un- known is the cause and effect direction in the negative relations between amount of time spent watching television and achievement variables. Aggression in childhood and adolescence The relation of early aggression to aggression five and ten years later may appear to be a test reliability index rather than a stability of behav- ior index. The evidence available supports a stability of behavior inter- pretation of the correlations. The scores each time were based upon overlapping but different sets of raters, who were basing their ratings upon different observations. Some raters were rating the same class- mates on two or three of the three rating occasions. Perhaps these raters were indeed influenced by their own memory of how they answered the questions on a single day five andlor ten years before, or perhaps their ratings stem from descriptive labels about each child which were learned by each classmate from his social group (teacher and peers). The latter notion, reminiscent of reputation, seems more substantial than the for- mer. However, data from earlier studies of this peer rating measure (see Walder et al., 1961; Eron et al., 1971) suggest that, while reputation is a factor, the raters were responding to the rated child's behaviors in addi- tion to any response to other people's desc riptions of the rated child. Writers about psychological measurement have been concerned with such issues as the role of method compared with content. Method refers to the form of the test or measuring device and the procedures for ob- taining and calculating the score; content refers to the goal of the test makerthat, if measured, makes some call the test "valid." Feshbach (1970), for example, in his recent comprehensive review of aggression, states that peer nomination (or sociometric) techniques have adequate reliability and that initial reports concerning their validities as measures of overt aggressive tendencies have been encouraging. He then writes, "However, the predictive utility of sociometric, projective, and inven- tory measures is limited by the substantial method variance yielded by each procedure, and it is evident that the more dissimilar the test of ag- gression is to the aggression criterion, the weaker are the relationships obtained" (p. 181). We have reported elsewhere (Eron et al., 1971) data which bear upon the above concern: (1) The application of Campbell and Fiske's multi- trait, multimethod matrix to patterns of correlatious demonstrated that the measuring devices of the authors yielded sufficient content variance. (2) The factor analytic studies of the investigators' peer rating, parent rating, and self rating questionnaire measures of aggressive and of other behaviors showed essentially the same results. (3) The relation of peer 85 78 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

rating measures to performanceon the IowaAggression Machineto such other independent eventsas clinical referral and the content of the clinical request, and to teacher ratings,revealed that the investigators had developed a psychometrically soundmeasure of important aggres- sive behaviors. These data add support to claimswe made in the past and make here once again that one can avoid undue method variance incomparison to content variance if the measuresare built and/or selected with some care and sophistication. The present findings havedemonstrand the re- lations between a variety of differenttypes of measures representing different methods and differentcontents secured at different times. In this case the time spansare five and ten years during important develop- mental periods of childhood and adolescence.In point of fact, peer-rat- ed aggression scores from the eighthgrade related substantially topeer- rated aggression scores at the thirteenthgrade; they also related justas -substantially to self-ratings of aggressionfor both boys and girls. The pattern of relations does, of course, suggest thatmethod variance ac- counts for part of the size of the correlations; thepattern also suggests that relations obtain across method andacross content. This finding in the thirteenth grade thatself ratings relate to ratings by others was not found in the third grade(Walder et al., 1961). The study of third graders shows that self-ratings ofseveral behaviors are positive- ly intercorrelated in spite of differentcontents but are not related to scores on the same behaviors as rated by others. In thethirteenth grade at least, self-ratings of aggression relateto peer ratings of aggression. Perhaps the ei2ht-year-old school child hadnot learned to describe him- self as well as he had learnedto describe others. Another possibility is that the testing conditionsmay have changed in that in the third grade the investigators were perhaps identifiedas being in league with the school; in the thirteenth grade thiswas obviously not so. A third possi- bility is that the youngster today, irrespectiveof his age or the testing conditions, is less inhibited in dcscribinghimself than even thesame children were ten years ago. These possibilitiesrequire further tests. Aggression and psychopathology

Support for the hypothesis that aggressionis a facet of psychopathol- ogy was partially afforded by the data. Whena global measure of the MMPIthe number of clinical scaleselevated above a T score of 70 was used as the criterion, aggressionwas unrelated. The partial correla- tion technique demonstrated that whenaggression does appear to relate to the clinical scales it is really thecomponent of scales 4 plus 9 which accounts for this relation. Since these scalesare typically peaked or re- ceive the highest codes in the profiles ofdelinquents (Dahlstrom and Welsh, 1960; Hathaway and Monachesi,1961 and 1963), it seems that aggression as measured in its variousaspects in the present study isa 86 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 79

fair predictor of potential delinquency or socially maladaptive behavior. The synchronous correlations between measures of aggression and MMPI scales 4 plus 9 are fairly substantial. Although a small portion of the relation may be attributed to method variance between some of the measures, the nature of other measures makes this explanation less like- ly. For example, method variance would be less likely involved in the relation between peer nominations and scales 4 plus 9 or between scales 4 plus 9 and Total Aggressive Environment. The latter variable is one in which the subject reports on the extent of his experience with aggres- sion. Because of the pronounced sex differences in aggression which have been found in the present study as well as in other studies (Feshbach, 1970), the moderate correlations for girls between various concurrent measures of aggression and scales 4 plus 9 deserve further comment. Sex differences in aggression have been variously attributed to causes such as biological (Lorenz. 1966), biochemical-hormonal (Scott, 1963; Hamburg and Lunde. 1966; Harris and Levine, 1962), and child rearing- cultural (Sears, 1961; Montagu, 1968). These broad reasons are obvious- ly not mutually exclusive, and the topic is highly polemical. The present findings strongly suggest that attitude also plays a role in the difference in aggression between males and femaks. The high as compared to the low aggressive girls have significantly higher scores on scale 5 of the MMPI, the measure of masculinity-feminity. A high score for girls sug- gests a masculine pattern of interests in work, sports, and hobbies (Pear- son and Swenson, 1967). Significantly, peer nominations of aggression for girls also relate positively to the watching of contact sportsfoot- ball, boxing, wrestling, hockeyon television. Thus, aggressive girls seem to have acquired masculine attitudes and interests. It seems perti- nent, in this regard, to note that when scale 5 is high for males (i.e., in the feminine direction), the inhibition of manifest delinquent behavior is indicated (Dahlstrom and Welsh, 1960). Femininity and aggression thus appear to be antipodal qualities. Previous studies concerning aggression and M M PI personality differ- ences show no clear pattern. Magee (1964) found nodifference between high and average aggressive boys on items selected from scale 4. Butch- er (1965), on the other hand, found that high and low aggressive boys were more disturbed than those in the middle range of aggression. Lef- kowitz (1966) found that delinquents who failed to adjust to institution- alization had significantly higher scores on scale 9 than delinquents who succeeded in adjusting. Scale 9 has been found to relate to delinquency in conjunction with scale 4. In the present study, analyses of the 13 MMPI scales for subjects in the upper and lower quartiles of aggression at three age periods demon- strated that, although scales 4 and 9 have the largest relation to aggres- sion, other scales were also involved. Generally, as the aggression and MMPI measures tend towards synchrony, the differences between high 8 1ELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

and low aggression groups become more pronounced. These findingsare qualified by the marked negative relation of IQto aggression. From the present analyses. therefore,it cannot be determined unequivocally whot her M M PI differences area function of differences in aggression or differences in IQ. However, the dataare presented in this report as an empirical finding with the understanding that further analysis is required to control the possible effect of IQ on the M MPI scales. The literature addressed to this question (e.g.. Dahlstrom and Welsh, 1960)suggests that the basic clinical scales are generally negatively relatedto IQ. An exception. but for males only, is the positive relation of scale 5to IQ. Beyond an IQ of 65, the reliability of the M M PIseems to be unaffected by the intelligence of the subjects. In the current study, the relation between IQ and the MMPI scales was examinee for certain of thecom- parisons. For the eighth-grade peer nominations, IQ is significantly high- er for the low aggressive boys, but no differences at the acceptable sta- tistical level emerge between M M PI scales. For girls,mean IQ for both groups is not different and again no M M PI differences occur. The great- est number and largest differences between groups occur when therat- ings of aggression and MMPI were obtained concurrently, which istrue for both boys and girls. Evidence for the hypothesis that aggression is relatedto psychopa- thology is obtained from these data. For highas compared to low aggres- sive boys, scales 4, 6, 8, and 9 are significantly elevated. Scales 6, 8,and 9 are three of the four scales of the so-called psychotic tetrad designed to measure psychoticism. In addition, the level of elevation of the F- scale for this group is likely to be produced by psychotic and severely neurotic individuals (Dahlstrom and Welsh, 1960). Computation of the first two high points in the composite profile results ina 98 combination for the high aggressives. This code is often found in psychiatric popula- tions. The 57 high points in the profile of the low aggressive boysseem clinkally to be benign. For the highas compared to the low aggressive girls, the significantly elevated scales suggest that these subjects might behave in an antisocial aggressive or a sociopathicmanner rather than in a psychotic fashion. The MM PI differences between subjects Whowere rated consistently high or consistently low in aggression at grades three and thirteenwere in the direction of the highs being significantly elevatedon The scales in- dicating delinquent behavior or antisocial maladaptive behavior. Yet inspection of profiles within groups shows that the high aggressive boys evince a highly elevated 98 profile code suggestive of psychoticism,as compared to the 95 code of the low group. Themean and standard devia- tion of the F-scale for the high group suggests the presence in thisgroup of individuals who are prone to admit to bizarre behavior. For thecon- sistently high aggressive girls the results are similar. However the eleva- tion of scores within profile is more within the normal range than is the TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 81 case for high aggressive boys. These results are merely tentative and must be interpreted with caution because of the attenuation of the high aggressive subjects due to sampling bias (discussed in the section on method) and because of the differences in IQ. When intellectual function as measured by school achievement is examined in relation to aggression, some evidence emerges that aggres- sion is a maladaptive kind of behavior. This finding seems to be more salient for girls and may occur hecause girls are better school achievers than boys. Thus any variable which relates to achievement does so with greater strength. Although the measures of aggression were obtained prior to and after the measures of school achievement, the antecedent- consequent relationship between these two variablesisdifficult to determine. Obviously, high aggression could lead to low school achieve- ment; or low achievement,as frustration,could produce high aggression. Other studies cited in Feshbach (1970) also have found that aggression is positively related to reading disability and underachievement for school children. When the data are examined for aggressive behavior which is tangibly socially maladaptive, the evidence is only barely suggestive. Although more high aggressive boys were arrested on criminal charges than those in the low group, the colt litions determining arrest are so confounded with social status, sex, population density and other variables that inter- pretation of these findings as support for the hypothesis is largely un- warranted. Yet it should be noted that a relation exists between third- grade aggre.ision and residential mobility. Since the arrest data reported are exclusively for New York State, a tenable assumption is that the number of arrests in the high aggression group would be larger if data could have been obtained for those subjects who may have moved out- side of New York State. Within the limits of the restrictions on the interpretation of the data, aggression does seem to be related to maladaptive behavior. Notwith- s.anding the conservative evaluation of the findings, significant relation- ships occurred between ,various measures of aggression and indicators of psychopathology. The relations discussed have appeared out of the context of a theoreti- cally oriented study. Measures appropriate to sets of rationally selected variables were used at the three points in the subjects' lives which the investigators were able to study. In addiction to making a deliberate search for relations of interest (e.g., television violence and aggresSive behavicr), the data speak for themselves through multivariate analytic techniques. In the current phase of this longitudinal study, as in the ear- lier phases, the approach which has used both deductive and inductive methods has supported and explicated the theory underlying this study. The relevance of experience at the third grade, such as exposure to television violence, on aggressive behavior is noted.not only at the age of 8,9 82 TELEVISION AND ADOL ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS exposureas previously reportedbut evenmore strongly at later ages. As a child develops his aggressive behaviorsover the years, exposure to television violence seems to affect hisexperiences as victim and witness of aggression as well as his expression ofaggression and antisocial acts. Aggression even seems to affect indices ofpsychopathology; e.g., MMPI scores. It alsoappears to relate to the subjects' educational and vocational achievements. As Berkowitznotes (1970), the expression of aggression appears to be self-stimulating:aggression may well beget fur- ther aggression.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

The present study is the third and final phase ofa longitudinal investi- gation of the psychosocial developmentof aggressive behavior. The original study, begun in 1959-60, focusedon 875 school children, the en- tire third grade in Columbia County,New York. The childrenwere test- ed in groups in their classrooms; 713 of theirmothers and 570 of their fathers were interviewed face-to-face,independently and in individual sessions. The primary goai of the studywas to determine the extent of aggressive behavior at school andat home and to attempt to account for the variation in aggression by social, geographic,economic, and cultural factors. A second goal was to gainan understanding of how aggressive behavior is learned. This goalwas approached by relating the ways in which children act out their aggressionto the kinds of training in its ex- pression and control they receive from varioussocializing agents, such as parents, teachers, and peers. A third aimwas to determine the con- sistency of aggressive behavioracross time by studying the same sub- jects at ages 13 and 19 in the eighth and twelfthgrades, respectively. The major dependent variablewas aggression as observed in the school situ- ation, and the major class of independentvariables was defined bypar- ents' socializaton practices. This study is.presentedin detail in Eron et al. (1971). In accordance with the design, 252 of theoriginal 875 children were tested in their eighth-grade classrooms in 1964-65. Emerging from the study of the third graderswas the somewhat unex- pected finding that theexposure of boys to a violent television diet was related to peer nominations of aggression. A similarfinding occurred for eighth graders using the same methods in anothergeographical region. Further exploration of these findingswas a pivotal aim of the design of the ten-year followup phase. The current project had three specific aims:to complete the longitudi- nal study of the psychosocial development of aggressivebehavior for which data were collected in 1960 and 1965;to investigate the synchron- ous and longitudinal relationships between violence content in television and aggressive behavior on the part of the vievier;to examine the conse- TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 83 quences of childhood aggression for functioningin late adolescence and young adulthood. Three hypotheses wereinvestigated: (I) When examined longitudinally, positive relationships obtain be- tween violent television preferences and aggressivebehavior; (2) Early aggressive behavior is positively related to later aggressive behavior; (3) Aggressive behavior is positively related to psychopathology. Survey and assessment techniques were used to obtain data from as many as possible of the original 875 third-gradesubjects. Current ad- dresses were culled from various sources, and letters were sent to 735 (or 84 percent) of the original 875 subjects, inviting them to be interviewed. The letter explained the nature of the research, and each subject was offered $20 as an incentive to participate. Subjects were required to give two hours of their time: the first hour in an individualface-to-face inter- view and the secona responding to written psychological tests. Willingness to be interviewed was indicated by 460 former subjects. Of this number, 427 contributed data which could be compared across the ten-year span. Thus the ten-year followup sample was comprised of 211 boys and 216 girls. The modal age of this group was 19 years and the mean number of years of school completed was12.57 + .82. Based on current test scores the mean IQ was 109.12 + 11.57.According to their father's occupation, the sample may be described as predominantly middle class. Sampling bias occurred in the ten-year followup phase, resulting in the attenuation of subjects in the upper quartile of third- grade aggression. Approximately twice as many subjectswho were low in aggression as measured in the third grade consented to beinterviewed in the thirteenth grade, as compared to subjects who were high in aggres- sion in the third grade. In part the effect of suchsampling bias was con- trolled statistically. Peer nominations provided the primary measure ofaggression for these thirteenth-grade subjects. With few changes, the peernomination items were the same as used in the third- and eighth-grade studies.Self ratings and psychological tests provided secondary measures of aggres- sion. Television diet was obtained from the interview, and the programs were rated for violence content by twojudges, independently. A second violence rating developed in another study was also employed. Data on these 427 subjects were collected from six sources: peernominations; individual face-to-face interviews; subjects' heights;psychological tests; school records yielding measures of IQ andstandardized achieve- ment test scores; and number of boysarrested in New York State who were in the upper and lowerquartiles of aggression as measured in the third grade. A basic data set, comprised of 94 variables, wascompiled. Included were 49 variables from the third-gradeitudy, five variables from the eighth-giade study, and 40 variables from the currentstudy. The data were analyzed within and across timeperiods by a variety of 84 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

statistical procedures. Basic analyses were performed for eachsex group and for the total. The first hypothesis relating violent television programs to aggressive behavior was largely confirmed. The data suggest that a violent televi- sion diet is in fact a longitudinal antecedent of such behavior. Thesec- ond hypothesis which related aggression at one stage of development to aggression at later stages was also largely confirmed. The data substanti- ated the hypothesis that aggression in childhood was a predictor ofag- gression in early and late adolescence. The third hypothesis, whichstat- ed that aggression was related to psychopathology,was only partially substantiated. High aggression impedes functioning andappears to be maladaptive. However, certain of the results were confounded with IQ and were therefore inconclusive. Further analyses of these dataare re- quired.

Conclusions

The analyses performed on the data of the present study lead to these findings: Aggressive behavior in early and late adolescence is predicatable from aggressive behavior in childhood. Peer nominations provide a highlyre- liable and valid measure of this behavior. Boys are substantially more aggressive than girls throughout the range of development. Highlyag- gressive girls possess "masculine" attitudes and interests. High aggres- sion appears to be an intellectually, socially, and emotionally maladap- tive form of behavior. Early television preferences are unrelated to television preferences five and ten years later. Amount of television viewing is inversely relat- ed to measured intelligence and social educational accomplishments, whereas preferences for violent televison are unrelated to these varia- bles. Judgment of the content of television as realistic is inversely relat- ed to measured intelligence. Preference of boys for violent television fare at grade three, as report- ed by their parents, is related to the expression of aggressive behavior at that age. The relationship between aggressive behavior and preference at grade three for violent television fare is stronger longitudinally than synchronously. The relationship between violent television preference at grade three and aggressive behavior at grade 13 is moderately positive (.31), while the relationship between aggressive behavior at grade three and preference for violent television at grade 13 is near zero (.01), and the difference between these correlations is statistically significant. On the basis of these cross-lagged correlations, the most plausible single causal hypothesis would appear to be that preferring violent television fare in the third grade leads to the building of aggressive habits. 92 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 85 ( FOOTNOTES

1. The investigators are indebted to the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Com mittee on Television and Social Behavior and to the New York State Department of Mental Hygiene for their support. Thanks are also due to Anne Karabin and Victor Pompa, New York State Department of Mental Hygiene, research assistants. Dr. Eron is on the faculty at the University of Illinois, Chicago Circle. Dr. Walder works with Behavioral Service Consultants, Inc., Greenbelt, Maryland. Dr. Huesmann is on the faculty at Yale University. 2. The original plan for followup in the twelfth grade was delayed be- cause the investigators were unable to obtainfunding until the sub- jects were one year older than planned. 3. The major findings of this survey of third-grade children and their parents are reported in the following papers: Eron, 1956, 1960,1963; Eron, Banta, Walder, and Laulicht, 1961; Eron, Laulicht, Walder, Farber, and Spiegel, 1961; Eron and Walder, 1961; Eron, Walder, Toigo, and Lefkowitz, 1963; Eron et al., 1971; Lefkowitz, Walder, and Eron, 1963; Lefkowitz, 1962, 1964; Toigo, 1962, 1965; Toigo, Walder, Eron, and Lefkowitz, 1962; Walder et al., 1961; Walder, Eron, and Laulicht, 1957. 4. The investigators are indebted to Bert Pekowsky of NBC for obtain- ing these ratings. 5. Temporary staff consisted of two interviewers, one research assist- ant, and one secretary. The investigators are indebted toMarjorie Kline and Ann McAleer for their assistance in this study. 6. Thanks are due John Robinson. 7. Because of the large number of intercorrelations resulting from the analyses, only those coefficients reaching statistical significance at the .01 level of confidence or beyond for a two-tailed test were ac- cepted as non-zero. 8. For convenience of presentation, the group of subjects tested in the ten-year follow up study will be designated as thirteenth grade. 9. An attempt had been made to have the programs rated by local raters (school librarians) immediately after the data collection period. However, because of lack of resources and support previously not- ed, it was impossible to communicate personally with these raters or train them in the rating task. Thus we had little confidence in their ratings; indeed, superficial inspection of the ratings indicated that these raters were not familiar with the programs they rated. 10.It should be pointed out that these relations held only for boys. They do not apply to the girls in this sample, except for the relation be- tween third-grade aggression and aggression ten years later.This cor- relation is even' higher for girls than it is for boys. P3 86 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

I I.The correlations reported in Table 3 and Figure 1 did not vary signifi- cantly when IQ and social status were held constant by partial corre- lation technique. 12. The splcifics of the scores are available in Eron et al. (1971), which describes their construction in detail.

REFERENCES

Bachman, J. G. Youth in transition. Institute for Social Research, Uni- versity of Michigan, 1967. Bandura, A., Ross, D., and Ross, S. A. Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 575-82. Bandura, A., Ross, D., and Ross, S. A. Imitation of film mediated ag- gressive models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1963a, 66, 3-11. Bandura, A., Ross, D., and Ross, S. Vicarious reinforcement and imita- tive learning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, I963b, 67, 601-07. Berkowitz, L. Aggressive cues in aggressive behavior and hostility ca- tharis, Psychological Review, 1964, 71, 104-22. Berkowitz, L. Experimental investigations of hostility catharsis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1970, 35, 1-7. Berkowitz, L., Corwin, R., and Heironimus, M. Film violence and sub- sequent aggressive tendencies. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1963, 27, 217-29. Berkowitz, L., and Rawlings, E. Effects of film violence on inhibitions against subsequent aggression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 66, 405-12. Bryan, J. H., and Schwartz, T. Effects of film material upon children's behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1971, 75 (1), 50-59. Butcher, J. Manifest aggression: MMPI correlates in normal boys. Jour- nal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 446-54. Cattell, R. B. The scientific analysis of personality. Baltimore: Penguin Books, 1965. Dahlstrom, W. G., and Welsh, G. S. An MMPI handbook. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1960. Darlington, R. D. Multiple regression in psychological research and practice. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 69, 161-82. Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, 0. H., and Sears, R. R. Frustration and aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1939. Eron, L. D. Social and cultural factors in mental illness. Proceedings of the Rip Van Winkle Clinic, 1956, 7, No. 2, 16-38. Eron, L. D. Psychosocial development of aggressive behavior. Progress Report, Project M 1726, U.S.P.H.S., May 15,1960. '94 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 87 Eron, L. D. Relationship of TV viewing habits and aggressive behavior in children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 193-96. Eron, L. D., Banta, T. J., Walder, L. 0., and Laulicht, J. H. Compari- son of data obtained from mothers and fathers on child rearing prac- tices and their relation to child aggression. Child Development, 1961, 32,457-72. Eron, L. D., Laulicht, J. H., Walder, L. 0., Farber, I. E., and Spiegel, J. P. Application of role and learning theories to the study of the development of aggression in children. Psychological Reports, 1961, 9, 291-334, (Monograph Supplement 2-V 9 1961). Eron, L. D., and Walder, L. 0. Test burning: 11. American Psycholo- gist, 1963, 166, 237-44. Eron, L. D., Walder, L. 0., and Lefkowitz, M . M. The learning of ag- gression in children. Boston: Little, Brown, 1971. Eron, L. D., Walder, L. 0., Tiogo, R., and Lefkowitz, M. M. Social class, parental punishment for aggression and child aggression. Child Development, 1963, 34, 849-67. Feshbach, S. Aggression. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael's Manual of Child Psychology (3rd Ed.), Vol. 2, New York: Wiley, 1970, Pp. 159-250. Feshbach, S., and Singer, R. D. Television and Aggression. San Fran- cisco: Jossey-Bass, 1971. Flanagan, J. C., Davis, F. B., Dailey, J. T., Shaycoft, M. F., Orr, D. B., Goldberg, I., and Neym an, C. A. Project Talent: The identification, development and utilization of human talents. The American high school student (Final Report to U. S. Office of Education, Project No. 635). Pittsburgh: Universk if Pittsburgh Project Talent Office, 1964. Goodenough, F., and Ty!er, L. Developmental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1959. Greenberg, B. S., and Gordon, T. F. Critics and public perceptions of violence in TV programs. Michigan State University: Department of Communication, 1970. Hamburg, D. A., and Lunde, D. T. Sex hormones in the development of sex differences in human behavior. In E. E. Maccoby (ed.), The development of sex differences. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford Univer- sity Press, 1966. Hathaway, S. R., and McKinley, J. C. The Minnesota Multiphasic Per- sonality Inventory. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1969. Hathaway, S. R., and Monachesi, E. D. An atlas of juvenile MMPI pro- files. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1961. Hathaway, S. R., and Monachesi, E. D. Adolescent personality and behavior.Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1963. 88 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Holtzman, W. H., Thorpe, J. S., Swartz. J. D., and Herron, W. E. Ink- blot perception and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press, 1961. Lange, D. L., Baker, R. K.. and Ball, S. J. Sampling procedures used in the Harris Poll (Appendix 1114). In Violence and the media, Vol. 9. A Report to the National Commision on the Causes and Prevention of Violence. Washington, D.C., United States Government Printing Office, 1969. Lefkowitz, M.M. Some relationships between sex role preference of children and other parent child variables. Psychological Reports, 1962. 10, 43-53. Lefkowitz, M.M. Aggression and size of human figure drawings. Psy- chology in the Schools, 1964, 3. 312-14. Lefkowitz, M. M. MM PI scores of juvenile delinquents adjusting to in- stitutionalization. Psychological Reports, 1966, 19, 911-14. Lefkowitz, M. M. Screening juvenile delinquents for psychopathology by use of the Z-test. Journal of Projective Techniques & Personality Assessment, 1968, 32, 5, 475-78. Lefkowitz, M.M., Walder, L. O., and Eron, L. D. Punishment, identifi- cation and aggression. Merrill Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 1963, 9, 159-74. Lorenz, K. On aggression. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1966. Magee, R. D. Correlates of aggressive-defiant classroom behavior in elementary school boys: A factor analytic study. Dissertation Ab- stracts, 1964, 2 (25), 1340-41. Montagu, M.F.A. The new litany of "innate depravity", or original sin revisited. In M.F. Ashley Montagu (Ed.), Man and Aggression, New York: Oxford University Press, 1968, 3-17. Nunnally, J. C. Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw Hill, 1967. Pearson, J. S., and Swenson, W. M. A user's guide to the Mayo Clinic Automated MMPI Program. New York: The Psychological Corpo- ration, 1967. Rozelle, R. M.,and Campbell, D. T. More plausible rival hypotheses in the cross-lagged pause correlation technique. Psychological Bulle- tin, 1969, 71, 74-79. Scott, J. P. Animal behavior. Garden City, New York: Doubleday, 1963. Sears, R. R. Relation of early socialization experiences to aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 461-92. Semler, I. J., and Eron, L. D. Replication Report: Relationship of ag- gression in third grade children to certain pupil characteristics. Psy- chology in the Schools, 1967, 4, 356-58. Semler, I. J., Eron, L. D., Meyerson, L. J., and Williams, J. F. Rela- tionship of aggression in third grade children to certain pupil charac- teristics. Psychology in the Schools, 1967, 4, 85-88. 96 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 89

Stroo, A.A. De relatie tussen agressie bij kinderen van + 8 jaar en op- voedingsmethoden van moeders. Doctoraaiscriptie, Vrije Universi- teit, Amsterdam, 1970. Toigo, R. Parental social status as a contextual and individual determi- nant of aggressive behavior among third-grade children in the class- room situation. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia Uni- versity, 1962. Toigo, R. Social status and schoolroom aggression in third-grade chil- dren. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 1965, 71, 221-63. Toigo, R., Walder, L. 0., Eron, L. D., and Lefkowitz, M. M. Examiner effect in the use of a near sociometric procedure in the third grade classroom. Psychological Reports, 1962, 11, 785-90. Walder, L. 0., Abelson, R., Eron, L. D., Banta, T. J., and Laulicht, J. H. Development of a peer-rating measure of aggression. Psycholog- ical Reports, 1961, 9, 497-556, (Monographs Supplement, 4-49). Walder, L. 0., Eron, L. D., and Laulicht, J. H. Manual of procedures in study of aggression. Proceedings of the Rip Van Winkle Clinic, 1957, 8, 1-30. Walters, R. H., and Zak, M. S. Validation studies of an aggression scale. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 209-18. Warner, W. L., Meeker, M., and Eells, K. Social class in America. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1960. Williams, J. F., Meyerson, L. J., Eron,L. D., and Semler, I. J. Peer- rated aggression and aggressive responses elicited in an experimen- tal situation. Child Development, 1967, 38, 181-90. Winer, B. J.Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. Zulliger, H. The Zulliger Individual and Group Test. New York: Inter- national Universitiespress, 1969.

97 90 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Appendix A: Invitational Letters

1. Letter to superintendents

May 1970

Dear You may recall that in 1960 the Rip Van Winkle Foundation,then lo- cated in Hudson, New York, begana longitudinal study .'of the psycho- social development of aggressive behavior of all thirdgrade school chil- dren in Columbia County. (The results of this researchwill shortly be published by Little, Brown & Co. ina book entitled "The Learning of Aggression in Children.") A tenyear follow-up in two phases was antici- pated and in 1965 the research team again collecteddata from these chil- dren in a number of the schools in the county. Completionof the final phase of the study is planned for thisyear. The subjects of this study are now aprioximately 19years old and no longer in the public school system. The researchteam, comprised of Leonard Eron, Leopold Walder, and myselfas Principal Investigator, plans to interview as many of the original subjectsas possible. Carried out under the aegis of the New York State Department of MentalHy- giene, this last phase of the project is supportedby the Surgeon Gener- al's Scientific Advisory Committeeon Television and Social Behavior of the United States Department of Health Education andWelfare. Appreciative of your full cooperation in thepast, we would like, once again, to enlist your aid in bringing this longitudinalstudy to completion. Our immediate needs are for add,ess lists of these former students, class rosters, and certain information from school records. As inour past re- search, the anonymity of all subjects is guaranteedoursole interest being the statistical relationshipsamong groups. I would like to plan a visit with you in thenear future to discuss the final phase of this project in greater detail and will call yousoon to arrange for an appointment.

Sincerely,

Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Ph.D. Principal Research Scientist

St3 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 91 2.Initial letter to subjects

May 1970

Dear About ten years ago you were in the third grade in Columbia County. At that time you participated in a study on the developr ent of behavior conducted by the Rip Van Winkle Foundation. The study was intended as a ten year follow-up and it is now time forcompletion. An interview with you is required to complete the study. We estimate the interview will take about two hours of your time. Within a week after the inter- view you will receive a check for $20 from our business office. The $20 will pay for the time you have devoted to this important scientific study. The interview will be conducted at our field office in Hudson, New York, at 414 Union Street (across from Court Square and next to the post office). Briefly the interview will deal with background material on yourself, television preferences, and questions about your behavior and attitudes. This is a study of developing behavior in young people. It has been deemed of vital importance by the United States Surgeon General's Office and is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health. We are asking for your continued participation. We would greatly appreciate it if you would complete the enclosed form and return it in the stamped addressed envelope indicating your intention to participate. Please return the form even if you do not wish to participate. Should you have any further questions, please call or write to me at the numbers or address below.

Sincerely,

Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Ph. D. Principal Investigator 92 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 3.Followup letter to subjects

July 1970

Dear Several weeks ago we wrote to you about a study in which you partici- pated when you were in the third grade. In that letter we stated that this study was intended as a ten-year follow-up of how behavior develops and that an interview with you was required to complete the study. Since we have not heard from youperhaps the original letter did not co ne to your attentionwe would like to offer you a second invitation to appear for an interview. We estimate the interview will take about two hours of your time. Within a week after the interview, you will re- ceive a check for $20 from our business office. The $20 will pay for the time you have devoted to this important scientific study. The interview will be conducted at our Field Office in Hudson, New York, at 414 Un- ion Street (across from Court House Square and next to the Post Office). We would greatly appreciate it if you would complete the enclosed form and return it in the stamped, addressed envelope ind42.ating your intention to participate. It is important that you return the form even if you do not wish to participate so that we can close our records.

Sincerely yours,

Monroe M. Lefkowitz, Ph.D. Principal Investigator

100 dO TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 93 4.Subject's response form

I am interested in participating

Yes No

Please Print

Name:

Address:

Phone:

Today's date:

Best time to be called for appointment

PLEASE RETURN THIS FORM EVEN IF YOU DO NOT WISH TO PARTICIPATE

Hudson Field Unit Office 414 Union Street Hudson, New York 12534 Tel: 828-3707 or 828-3860 94 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Second request

I am interested in participating

Yes No

Please Print

Name:

Address:

Phone:

Today's date:

Best time to be called for appointment

PLEASE RETURN THIS FORM EVEN IF YOUDO NOT WISH TO PARTICIPATE

Hudson Field Unit Office 414 Union Street Hudson, New York12534 Tel: 828-3707 or 828-3860 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 95 Appendix B: Interview

1970 Questionnaire: Follow Up Study of Behavior in Columbia County, New York FACE SHEET

Name Last First Middle

If married woman, maiden name

Address

Phone

Date of Interview Month Day Year

Interviewer

Time started 96 TELEVISION AND ADQLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Peer Rating Questions

What schools did you attend for each of the grades after grade 3?

Grade School

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11 -

12

Any other schooling?

GIVE S THE LISTS (FROM FEEDER SCHOOLS) OF THE LAST COLUMBIA COUNTY SCHOOL HE ATTENDED TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 97 "Ten years ago, when you were in the third grade, you answered a series of questions about yourself and about your classmates. I would like your cooperation in answering a similar series of questions. "I have lists of people who might have gone to school with you. GIVE FEEDER SCHOOL LISTS TO S Put a check in the box next to the, names of the people you know well enough to answer some questions about. Generally,,these would be people who had been in class with you. You might remember the way they acted in school. "S MARKS NAMES. IF S SAYS "I DON'T KNOW ANYONE," CHECK ANSWERS TO PAGE 2 A ND THE APPROPRIATENESS OF THE PACKET OF FEEDER SCHOOLS "I shall ask you questions, one at a time. For each question, tell me the identification numbers of all those people who fit that question. Do not name yourself for any of these questions. You may name any num- ber of people for each question. Notice that to these questions there are no answers that are right for everybody. Base your answers on what you last knew of each person from personal observation and contact. "START WITH 03. IF S RESPONDS WITH ONLY ONE NAME, SAY You may give any number of answers. You may check more names if you think of them as I ask the questions."

103 98 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Peer Rating Record Form 00. Whom do you know"

03. Whom did you like to sit next to in class?

04. Who did not listen to the teacher?

09. Who was very quiet?

1 I. Who did things that bothered others"

19. Who used to say "excuse me" even when they did not do anything bad9

.12. Who started fights over nothing"

21. Who would never fight even when picked on?

14. Who was always getting into trouble? 82. Who made up stories and lies to get other students into trouble?.

16. Who used to say mean things?

35. Who pushed or shoved students?

36. Who took other students' things without asking?

87. Who gave dirty looks or made unfriendly gestures to other students?

88. Who was a leader of a club or a group?

37. Who were the students that you would like to have had foryour best friends"

4 106 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 99

PR 1.Who are the students who have moved out of the School Dis- trict? For each, tell me where they have moved to.

SUBJECT NUMBER TO WHERE MOVED

IND.3.What is your birthdate? month day year

FAS4.What is your marital status? I. single 2. married 3. separated 4. annulled 5. divorced 6. widowed 7. other (specify)

FAS5.(IF MARRIED) How Long 0. less than 1 year have you been married? I.1 to 2 years 2. 2 to 3 years 3. more than 3 years 99. DNA

FAS6.(IF MARRIED) Do you have 0. none any children? I. one 2. two 3. three 4. more than 3 99. DNA

ASP7.What is the greatest amount of I. less than high school education you expect to have 2. completionofhigh during your life? school 3. vocational or business school 4. junior college 5. 4 year college degree 6. graduate education 100 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS ASP8.How well off financially do yoU I. barely able to make a expect to.be in your lifetime? living READ ALTERNATIVES 2. abletoprovidethe necessities 3. comfortable 4. well-to-do 5. wealthy 6. extremely wealthy

ASP 9,. What kind of work do you expect to be doing ten years from now? OBTAIN SPECIF-, IC OCCUPATION, E.G., TEACHER, TRUCKDRIV- ER, SMALL BUSINESS OWNER, ETC. HOC 10.How many people live in the 2. two home in which you grew up?3. three Includeyourself,brothers, 4. four sisters,,parents,relatives, 5. five boarders, roomers, servants, 6. six etc. 7. seven 8. eight 9. nine O. ten II. eleven 12. twelve 13. thirteen or more

1SS 12.What kind of work do you do in your full time occupation? , (Do not include summer-only jobs.) OBTAIN SPECIFIC OCCUPATION,E.G.,TEACHER, TRUCKDR1VER, SMALL BUSINESS OWNER, ETC. E WRITES DETAILS OF OCCUPATION AND CODES LATER

CODE

1SS 13.What kind of work does your father (or male head of house- hold) do in his major occupation? If he works (or has worked) on more than one job, tell me the oneon which he spends (or spent) most of his time? OBTAIN SPECIFIC OCCUPATION,E.G.,TEACHER, TRUCKDRIVER, ib8 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 101 SMALL BUSINESS OWNER, ETC. EWRITES DETAILS f OF OCCUPATION AND CODES LATER.

CODE

ISS 14. What kind of work does your mother(or female head of household) do in her major occupation? If sheis a housewife in addition to outside work, tell me abouther outside work. IN THIS CASE, E CODES ONLY THEOUTSIDE WORK. If she works on more than one outsidejob, tell me the most important one. If she is now out of work, or if sheis retired or not living, tell me the oneshe did last. OBTAIN SPECI- FIC OCCUPATION. E.G., TEACHER, TRUCKDRIVER, SM ALL BUSINESS OWNER, ETC. E WRITESDETAILS OF OCCUPATION AND CODES LATER.

CODE

ISS 16.Which of the following best 1. barely able to make a describes yourfamily'sfi- living nances? 2. have the necessities READ ALTERNATIVES 3. com fortable 4. well-to-do 5. wealthy 6. extremely wealthy

ISS 17. How many books are in your 1. none, or very few (0- home? 10) 2. a few books (11-25) READ ALTERNATIVES 3. one bookcase full (26- 100) 4. twobookcasesfull (101-250) 5. three or four bookcas- es full(251-500) 6. a room full - a library (501 or more) ISS 18. How many rooms are in your home? Count all rooms; bed- rooms, bathrooms,kitchen, living room, dining room, rec- reation room, enclosed porch, etc. (please write in) 109 102 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

REL 19. How often do you attend church? 0. never I. a few times a year 2. about once a month 3. few times a month 4. once a week 5. more than once a week

REL 20. Would you mind telling me your religion? I. Protestant 2. Catholic (write response) 3. Jewish 4. Greek Orthodox 5. Greek Catholic 6. Other

INPUN ITEMS SEX OF ADULT AND OF CHILD IS SAMEAS SEX OF S.

"We would like to learn something aboutyour ideas for raising chil- dren. Imagine that you are the father (mother) ofan eight year old boy (girl) and try to answer the following questions accordingly."

PUN21.If you saw your son (daugh- 0. no ter) grab things from another I. yes child, would youtellhim 98.DK (her) that young men (ladies) don't do this sort of thing?

PUN22.Ifyou saw your son 0. no (daughter) grab things from I. yes anotherchild,wouldyou 98. DK say, "I would liketo be proud of you."?

PUN23.Would you make your son 0. no (daughter) apologizeifhe I. yes (she) grabbed things from 98. DK another child?

: 4 110 -

TV VIOLENCE ANDCHILD AGGRESSION 103

PUN24.Would youtellyour son 0. no (daughter)you don't love I. yes him (her) for grabbing things 98. DK from another child?

PUN25.Would you point out how 0. no some closefriendofhis I. yes (hers) behaves better than 98. DK your son (daughter) does,if he (she) grabbed things from another child?

PUN26.If you saw your son (daugh- 0. no ter) grab things from another 1. yes child, would you not let him 98. DK (her)playwithhis(her) friends for two days?

INSTRUCTIONS TO RESPONDENT: "I am going to read a numberof statements to you. For each one I would like you totell me if you agree or disagree."

RAG27.There are two kinds of peo- 1. disagree ple in this world: the weak 2. agree and the strong.

RAG28.Dealills with policemen and 1. disagree governmentofficialsisal- 2. agree ways unpleasant.

RAG29.Most people get killed in ac- 1. disagree cidents because of their own 2. agree reckless driving.

RAG30.Horses that don't pull should 1. disagree be beaten and kicked. 2. agree 104 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

RAG31. At times we enjoy being hurt I. disagree bi/ those we love. 2. agree

RAG32. Many a decent fellow be- I. disagree comes a crook or a criminal 2. agree because he can't stand to be pushed around so much.

RAG 33. Ieasilylose patience with I. disagree people. 2. agree

RAG 34. I often do things that Iregret I. disagree afterwards. 2. agree

RAG 35. It makes me mad when I I. disagree can't do things for myself the 2. agree way I like to.

RAG 36. Occasionally I was in trouble 1. disagree with the police or law. 2. agree

RAG 37. I almost never dare to ex- 1. disagree press anger toward people 2. agree for fear 1 may lose their love or approval.

RAG 38. As a young kid I often mixed 1. disagree with the wrong crowd. 2. agree

VAG 39. Have youever been slapped 0. No or not or kicked by another person? sure

IF YES: How many times 1. once would you estimate that this 2. twice has happened to you? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. not sure 112 . __.....--*.w.won7fritYCI14.1701?,f1T.rntr.6

TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 105 VAG 40.Have you ever been punched 0. No or not or beaten by another person? sure

IF YES: How many times I. once wou:d you estimate that this 2. twice has happened to you? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. Not sure

VAG41.Have you ever been choked 0. No or not by another person? sure

IF YES: How many times I. once would you estimate that this 2. twice has happened to you? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. Not sure

VAG 42.Have you ever been threat- 0. No or not ened or actually cut by some- sure body using a knife?

IF YES: How many times I. once would you estimate that this 2. twice has happened to you? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. not sure

VAG43.Have you ever been threat- 0. No or not ened with a gun or shot at? sure

IF YES: How many times I. once would you estimate that this 2. twice has happened to you? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. not sure

113 106 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS WAG 44.Have you ever seen another 0. No or not sure person slapped or kicked?

IF YES: How many times 1. once would you estimate that you 2. twice have seen another person 3. three times slapped or kicked? 4. four or more times 98. not sure

WAG45.Have you ever seen another 0. No or not sure person punched or beaten?

IF YES: How many times I. once would you estimate that you 2. twice have seen another person 3. three times punched or beaten? 4. four or more times 98. not sure WAG46.Have you eve: seen another 0. No or not sure person choked:

IF YES: How many times I. once would you estimate that you 2. twice have seen another person 3. three times choked? 4. four or more times 98. not sure WAG47.Have you ever seen another 0. No or not sure person threatened or actual- ly cut with a knife?

IF YES: How many times 1. once would you estimate that you 2. twice have seen another threat- 3. three times ened or actually cut with a 4. four or more times knife? 98. not sure

WAG 48.Have you ever seen another 0. No or not sure person threatened with a gun or shot at?

IF YES: How many times 1. once would you estimate that you 2. twice have seen another person 3. three times threatened by a gun or shot 4. four or more times at? 98. not sure 114 IV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 107 IN PUN ITEMS SEX OF ADULT AND OFCHILD IS SAME AS SEX OF S. "Imagine again that you are the father (mother)of an eight year old boy (girl) and try to answer the followingquestions accordingly."

PUN 49.Ifyouheardyourson 0. no (daughter) say mean things to 1. yes another child, would you tell 98. DK him (her) in a nice way to act differently?

PUN50.Ifyouheardyourson (daughter) say mean things to 0. no anotherchild,wouldyou 1. yes say, "Get on thatchair and 98. DK don't move until you apolo- gize."?

PUN 51.Would you not let your son 0. no (daughter) play with his (her) 1. yes friends for two days if you 98. DK heard him (her) say mean things to another child?

PUN 52.Ifyouheardyourson (daughter) say mean things to 0. no another child,wouldyou 1. yes point out how some close 98. DK friends of his (her) behave better than he (she) does?

PUN 53.Ifyouheardyourson 0. no (daughter) say mean things to 1. yes another child,wouldyou 98. DK wash out his (her) mouth with soap? 108 M.U.VISMN AND M)01.1.SWi1A(GRESSIVI:NMS PUN54. If you heard your son 0. no (daughter) say mean thingsto yes another child,wouldyou 98. DK say. "I wouldlike to he proud of you."?

RAG 55. HMV you ever spankeda 0. No or not sure child?

IF YES: Howmany times I. once would you estimate thatyou 2. twice have done this? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. not sure

RAG56. Have you ever slappedor 0. No or not sure kicked another person?

IF YES: Howmany times I. once would you estimate thatyou 2. twice have done this? 3. three times 4. four ormore times 98. not sure

RAG 57.Have you ever punchedor 0. No or not sure beaten another person?

IF YES: Howmany times 1. once would you estimate thatyou 2. twice have done this? 3. three times 4. four or more times 98. not sure

Getting ahead in yout job or place in the communitysometimes means that you have to do certain things you may not like. Howwilling would you be to do each of the followingthings in order to get ahead? 116 1V VIOLENCE AND CHII.D AGGRESSION 109

ASP 58.How willing would you be to I. not at all willing learn new skills in order to get 2. a little willing ahead? 3. somewhat willing READ ALTERNATIVES TO S 4. very willing 98. DK

ASP 59.How willing would you be to I. not at all willing leaveyourfriend% toget 2. a little willing ahead? 3. somewhat willing 4. very willing READ ALTERNATIVES TO S 98. DK

ASP 60.How willing would you be to I.not at all willing move around the country alot to get 2. a little willing 3. somewhat willing ahead? 4. very willing 98. DK READ ALTERNATIVES TO S

ASP 61.How willing would you be to I. not at all willing take on more responsibility in 2. a link willing order to get ahead? 3. somewhat willing 4. very willing 98. DK READ ALTERNATIVES TO S

ASP 62.How willing would you be to 1. pot at all willing give up spare time in order to 2. a Mae willing get ahead? 3. somewhat willing 4. very Willing 98. DK READ ALTERNATIVES TO S

IN PUN ITEMS SEX OF ADULT AND OrCHILD IS SAME AS SEX OF S.

"Imagine again that you ate the father (mother) of an eight yearold boy (gitl) and try to answer the following questionsaccordingly."' 117 110 ff1 IVISION AND ADOLLSCENIAGGRI:SSIVI.NESS PUN O.If your son (daughter)were 0. no rude to you. w oukl you tell 1. yes him (hrr). "I will giveyou 98. DK something you like if you act differently."?

PUN 64.If your son (daughter)were 0. no rude to you. would you wash -. yes outhis(her) mouth with 98. DK soap?

PUN 65.Would you remind your son 0. no (daughter) of whatothers 1. yes will think of him (her) if he 98. DK (she) were rude to you?

PUN 66. If your son (daughter)were 0. no rude to you. wouldyou say. I. yes "Get on that chair and don't 98. DK move until you apologize."?

PUN 67. Would youtellyour son O. no (daughter) that young men I. yes (ladies) don't do this son of 98. DK thingif he (she) were rude to you?

PUN 6E.Would you spank your son O. no (daughter) until he (she) cries I. yes if he (she) were rude to 98. DK you?

118 IV VIOI.ENCE ANDCHILI) AGGRESSION ill PROFILE IDENTIFICATION - PID - INSTRUCTIONS

REMOVE THIS PAGE AND HAND QUESTIONNAIRE TO S

We are interested in finding out how you do certain things such as t4 alking. talking. and so forth. Onthe sheet of paper before you is a list of things described by sth of opposite words. Between each pair of words are five steps. or grades. ranging from one way of doing some- thing to its opposite. I would I;Ise you to rate yourself for each typeof activity, such as walking. talking. etc.. hy placing a check mark on one of thc steps on each line. Notice that thc closer you place your check mark to either of the opposite wc rds. the more it means you act the way the word says. Please try the example. If I were to ask you how you like yourcoffee. how would you place your check marks?

IF S GIVES ONLY EXTREME RESPONSES ON EXAM- PLES. SAY:

You showed that you liked your coffee very hot/cold. How would you place your check mark if you wanted your coffee just a little less hot/cold?

(AFTER EXAMPLE IS COMPLETED) Thank you. would you please complete the list. 112 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS SCAN PID FOR COMPLETIONOF EACH ITEM AND THEN RE- TRIEVE QUESTIONNAIRE.

PI D I walk fast slow loud soft often not often I talk

slow MOM fast soft loud not often 4111mOINOMI.1.111. 11111. often I stand straight 41=11.111M11 ______lean forward at ease .1---firm leaf much _____ ------little fast ------_slow 1 write slow ------fast small ------______large heavy ------light My body is

light 11111=110.11.0. /1. .1.10.161001. . dark tallalMMO.MO ------short thick domomommi thin hard 411111M.0.111111. 4111.0.111111111MIMED soft

11.°11g am000mmom almOMMONOM. OMMOMMOO weak I like coffee hot --- -- , cold light ------dark sweet OMMOMM. AMOOMOMON...... not sweet TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 113 PRESENT TO S

TEL 69.Here is a list of possible reasons for watching television. When you watch T. V.. how often does each of these rea- sons apply to you? Is it USUALLY oneof your reasons. OCCASIONALLY a reason. RARELY. or NEVER?

Check the appropriate box for each reason

.., .. c ,. .., .. REASON ... -4 ...:,,,- -: .:,... .,;.., ... 4.c ..-c z

, , .01 1 watch to see a special program I've heard a lot about

.02 1 watch because there is nothing else to do at the time

.03 1 watch to get away from the ordinary cares and problems of the day

.04 1 turn on the set just to "keep me company" when I'm alone

.05 1 watch because I think 1 can learn something

.06 1 watch because I'm afraid 1 might be missing something good

.07 1 watch so I can talk later to my friends about the show

.08 I start on one show and can't leave the T.V. for the rest of the evening .09 1 watch just for "background"

while I am doing something else .

.10 1 watch mp.inly to be sociable when others are watching . . .11 1 watch to see a specific program that 1 enjoy very much 121 114 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AOGRESSIVENESS PRESENT TO S

TEL70. Here is a list of weeklyprograms that are on network televi- sion during the evening.

Circle each program that you haveseen all the way through TEN times or more since January 1. 1970. (Each program has beenon about 15 weeks./

Monday

Gunsmokc My World/WelcomeIt Takes a Thief Here's Lucy Laugh-In ABC Monday Movie Mayberry RFD Monday Movies Doris Day Carol Burnett

Tuesday

Lancer I Dream of JeannieMod Squad Red Skelton Debbie Reynolds Movie of the Week Governor and J.J.Julia Marcus Welby. M.D. Sixty Minutes Tuesday Movies

Wednesday

Hee Haw The Virginian Nanny and the Professor Beverly HillbilliesKraft Music Hall Courtship of Eddie's Father Medical Center Then Came BronsonR00171 222 Hawaii Five-0 Johnny Cash Engelbert Humperdinck Thursday

Family Affair Daniel Boone Pat Paulson 1/2 Comedy Hour Jim Nabors Ironside That Girl Thursday Movie Dragnet Betwitched Dean Martin Tom Jones Friday Paris NM

Get Smart High Chaparral Flying Nun Tim Conway Name of the Game Brady Bunch Hogan's Heroes Bracken's World Ghost and Mrs. Muir Friday Movie Here Come the Brides Love. American Style 122 lv VIOI.ENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 115

Saturday

Jackie Gleason Andy Williams Let's Make a Deal My Three Sons Adam-12 Newlywed Gamc Green Acres Saturday Movie Lawrence Welk Petticoat Junction Hollywood Palace Mannix Cesar's World

Sunday

Lassie Marlin Perkins Land of the Giants To Rome with Love Walt Disney The FBI Ed Sullivan Bill Cosby Sunday Movie Glen Campbell Bonanza Mission: Impossible Bold thies

TEL71. How many hours altogether on Saturday and (PLUS) Sun- day do you watch TV?

hours

TEL72. What is the total number of hours during the rest of the week (Monday through Friday) do you watch TV:

hours

TEL 73. How many hours did you watch yesterday?

hours

TEL74. How many T.V. sets (that work) do you have in your home? black &hite -color TEL75. if in real life you could be like some T.V. character,which one would you like to be?

TEL 75.2 How many hours do you personally spendwatching TV per day? 116 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS TEL 76.Would you agree or disagree with these complaints about T.V.?

1. too much sex I. agree 2. disagree 98. DK

2. Not enough information I. agree program s? 2. disagree 98. DK

3. Too many commercials I. agree 2. disagree 98. DK

4. Too much violence I.agree 2. disagree 98. DK

S. Not enough comedy pro- I. agree grams 2. disagree 98. DK

6. Too many news programs I. agree 2. disagree 98. DK

7. Not enough prop ams 1. agree showing life as it really is 2. disagree 98. DK

8. Not enough violence 1. agree 2. disagree 98. DK TEL 77.All things considered. would 1. from T.V. you say you have learned 2. in school more things 3. both equally 98. DK READ ALTERNATIVES

124 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 117

TEL78.Do you ever watch the net- 0. no work news programs.like Walter Cronkite.Huntley- Brinkley.orFrankRey- nolds? IF YES. How often do you 4. every night watch these network news 3. two or three/week programs? 2. once/week 1. less often than once/week TEL 79.Some people say that these 3. too much shows have giventoo much 2. right amount attention to violence, while I. not enough others say there isnot enough attention to violence in news shows. How do you feel?

We're also interested in sports you watch on television. Of the following sports events which have been on television, how many have you watched this last year?

TEL80.Football games 4. most 3. about half 2. some I. none

TEL81.Hockey 4. most 3. about half 2. some I. none

TEL82.Boxing 4. most 3. about half 2. some I. none

TEL83.Wrestling 4. most 3. about half 2. some I. none

125 118 inl.FVISION ND ADOITSCEN : MaiRLSSR

83a. What are your four favorite programs (in television. the ones you try to watch every time they are on the air?

a.

h.

C.

d.

MEI) 84. And whataboutmovies? 0. none (go to next) Since January I. about how I. one plany movies would you say 2. two to four you've gone to see? 3. five to ten 4. more than ten IF YES: Which movie was yourfavorite?(IF ONE. which movie did you see?)

We're also interested in things you like in the other mass media: newspa- per.. magazines, comics, and radio.

MED 85.What newspapers do you read?

MED 86.How often do you read the 4. every day newspapers you read most? 3. 3 or 4 times/week 2. once a week I. less than that

What kinds of things interest you in the paper? Do you usually read each of these parts of the paper all the way through. just read a little of it. or skip over it?

MED 87.Sports S. all I.little 0. skip over

MED 88.Stories about politics 2. all I.little O. skip over 126 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 119

M ED 89. Stories about murders and 2. all robberies I.little 0. skip over

MED90. Stories about the fighting in 2. all Vietnam I.little 0. skip over

MED91. Stories aboutriots and 2. all bombings I.little 0. skip over

MED92. Comics 2. all I.little 0. skip over

MED93. What are your four favorite magazines?

Most favorite: .1 .2 3 4th favorite: 4

MED 94.We'd like you to tell us some things about different kindsof T.V. shows. First of all, here is a list of Westerns.How realistic do you think these programs are in tellingabout how life in the West really was? (Put an X in the correct position.)

3 2 98 Exactly like More real More fake it was than fake than real Don't know

Gunsmoke 1.111111.111, Bonanza Lancer High Chaparral

The Virginian .1111111611ey. 127 120 EVISIM AND ADOI.ES('ENI AGGRESSIVENESS M ED 9.We'd like to ask you the same questions hut about shows that tell about police and government w ork. How realistic would you say these programs are in showing w hat police work is really like? (Put an X in the correct position.)

1 3 _ 1 98 Exactly like More real More fake it is than fake than real Don't know

Mod Squad Hawaii Five-0 Dragnet It Takes a Thief Name of the (lame Mannix Adam-12 The FBI Mission: Impossible

MEI) 96.Now I would like to get your judgment on some questions concerning the possible effect of television violence (RE- PEAT BEFORE EACH STATEMENT BELOW: "HOW LIKELY IS IT.THAT T.V. VIOLENCE: IS IT LIKELY, POSSIBLE, OR UNLIKELY?")

.1Plays a partin making 3. likely America a violent society? 2. possible I. unlikely

.2 Allows viewers to blow 3. likely off steam by watching viol- 2. possible ence,thus decreasingthe I. unlikely likelihood of their being vio- lent?

.3 Makes people insensitive 3. likely to real acts of violence that 2. possible they hear about or see? I. unlikely

.4 Provides entertainment 3. likely and relaxation without harm- 2. possible ful or bad effects? I. unlikely 123 lv VIOLENCE AND (1111.1).ACIGHESSION 121

.5 Triggers violent acts from 3. likely people who are maladjusted 2. possible or mentally unstable? I. unlikely

.6 Supports and strengthens 3. likely traditional A merkan values? 2. possible I. unlikely

MED 97.How do you feel about thc 3. too much amountofviolence por- 2. a reasonable trayed in television programs amount today. not including news I. very little programsdo you think that 98. not sure there is too much, a reasona- ble amount. or verylittle violence?

MED 98.Apart from the amount of I. approve violence, do you generally 2. disapprove approve or disapprove of the 98. not sure kind of violence that is por- trayed on T.V.?

IN PUN ITEMS SEX OF ADULT AND OF CHILD IS SAME AS SEX OF S

"Imagine again that you are the father (mother) of an eight year old boy (girl) and try to answer the following questions accordingly."

PUN 99.If your son (daughter) got 0. no very mad at you. would you I. yes get angry with him (her)? 98. IT/K

PUN 100. If your son (daughter) got 0. no very mad at you. would you 1. yes slap him (her) in the face? 98. DK

PUN 101.Would you say. "That isn't 0. no a nice thing to do." if your 1. yes son (daughter) got very mad 98. DK at you? 129 122 1 El. EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

PUN 102. Would youtellyour son 0. no (daughter) you don't lovc I. yes him (her) for getting very 98. DK mad at you?

PUN 103. Would youtellyour son 0. no (daughter) inanice way I. yes how to act differently if he 98. DK (she) got very mad at

PUN 104. If your son (daughter) got 0. no very mad at you. would you I. yes send him (her) to another 98. DK room where he (she) would be alone and without toys?

IAG 105. Please put a checkmark for each statement in the box which best expresses your feeling.

4 3 2 1 0 Almost Some- AlwaysOften timesSeldomNever true true true true true

.1I feel like swearing

.2I feel like losingmy temper at people

.3I feel like being a little rude to people. .4Ifeel like pickinga fight or arguing with people

OAG 106. Please put a checkmark for each statement in the box which best describes how you act.

4 3 2 1 0 Almost Some- AlwaysOften timesSeldomNever true true true true true .1I get angry and smash things .2I am a little rude to people .3 Hose my temper at people 111 iv VIOLLNuE AND (um) GGREssurs 123 ND THIS AND NEXT PAGE TO S.

BF H 107. Here are a number of things which you might do that could get you into trouble. Please tell us how many times you have done these things in the last three years. For each question. put a check in the box next to the answer that is true. Number of times In the last three years how many times 5 or 3 have you done this? more4 2 1 0 .01 Stayed out later than parents said you should .02 Got into a serious fight with a student in school .03 Run away from home

.04 Taken something not belonging to you worth under $50 .05 Went onto someone's land or into some house or building when you weren't supposed to be there .06 Set fire to someone else's property on , purpose _ .07 Been suspended orexpelledfrom school 08 Got something by telling a person Nomething bad would happen to him if you did not get what you wanted , .09 Argued or had a fight with either of your parents .10 Got into trouble with the police be- cause of something you did 11 Hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or a doctor .12 Damaged school property on purpose 13 Taken something from a store without paying for it 124 nil VISION ND ADM LtiCf A(iCIREStik'ENU:Sti Nlimher of times

5 or

more4 3 2 1 0

.14Hit a teacher

.15 Drunk beer or liquor without parents' permission .16 Smoked in school (against the rules)

.17 Hit your father

.18 Taken a car that didn't belong to someone in yourfamily without per- mission of the owner

.19 Taken an expensive part of a car with- out the permission of the owner

.20 Taken part in a fight wherc a bunch of yourfriendsareagainstanother hunch

.21Hit your mother

.22 Taken something not belonging to you worth over S50

.23 Had to bring your parents to school because of something you did

.24 Taken an inexpensive part of a car without permission of the ow ner

.25 Skipped a day of school without a real excuse .26 Used a knife or gun or some other thing (like a club) to get something from a person

132 IV VIOLENCE AND CHILI) AGGRESSION 125

ZIN. ADMINISTER THE Z TEST

There are a few other things to do before we are finished. One is a measure of your height. Another is a True-False questionnaire about your attitude% and feelings. The last is to give you your voucher for $20 when you complete the questionnaire. Let's go to the office so that the secretary can measure your height. hut before we do I would like to have the height of your parents. (Did you %ay they are both alive?)

ANT 109. How tall is (was) your father? feet inc.hes

ANT 110. How tall is (was) your mother? feet inches

TAKE S TO SECRETARY

ANT 1 1 1. S's HEIGHT feet inches

MMP 112. TAKE S TO M M PI AND GIVE S MMP1 INSTRUC- TIONS.

If you have :Iv questions after I leave, ask the secretary or some oth- er stall' person who's in this room. When finished with this test, give it to the secretary and she will give you youi voucher.

SECRETARY CHECKS FOR THE NUMBER OF UNANSWERED QUESTIONS(?)TO DETERM INE TH ATTHE MMPI IS SCORABLE BEFORE GIVING VOUCHER. IF TOO MANY UNANSWERED QUESTIONS (?) THE SECRETARY ASKS S TO ANSWER MORE OF THE QUESTIONS BEFORE GIVING VOUCHER.

FATHER ALIVE? I. YES 0. NO MOTHER ALIVE?I. YES 0. NO 133. 126 EL v!SION AND AIMI.I..tilTN.1 MiGKESSIVLNESS

INT 113. WAS THE INTERVIEW AN EASY J013? I. YES 2. NO

INT 114. WAS THERE MUCH DISSIMULATION? I. YES 2. NO

INT 115. ANYTHING UNUSUAL ABOUT THE I. YES INTERVIEW? 2. NO

INT 116. IF YES. EXPLAIN

NOTES:

Time ended 1.34

'.11m .11. IV vioLENcE AND CHILD AGGREssIoN 127 Appendix C: Variables derived from interview schedule

I. Respondents' Aggression a. Respomknt as object of aggression (VA(;). This score is the sum of five items, questions 39 through 43. The higher the score, the more frequently respondent says he has been the vietim of aggression. b. Respondent as witness of aggression (WAG). This score is the sum of five items. questions 44 through 48. The higher the score, the more frequentl the respondent says he has observed aggression towards oth- ers c, Aggressive habit - A (A HA). This score is the sum of three items. questions 55 through 57. The higher the score, the more often the re- spondent says he carried out physical aggression against another person. d. Aggressive habit - HB). This score is the sum of three items. questions 106.1 through 106.3. The higher the score, the more frequently does the respondent say he vents his hostility. e. Total aggressive habit (TAH). This score is the sum of three other scores: AHA + AHB + ASB. The higher the score, the more the re- spondent admits thats that he displays aggressive behavior. f.Antisocial Ikhavior (ASHIAM is score is the sum of 26 items, ques- tions 107.1 through 107.26. The higher the score, the more the subject admits to past delinquent behaviors. g. Aggressive Drive (AGD): This score is the sum of four items, ques- tions 105.1 through 105.4. The higher the score, the more often the sub- ject says he would like to express aggression. h. Toth/ Aggressive Environment (TAG). This score is the sum of five other scores: VAG + WAG + A HA + ASB + AGD. The higher the score, the more often has the subject been exposed to the expression or experience of aggression. i.Personal Opinion Inventory (WAZ). This score is the sum of 12 items. questions 27 through 38 with1 being the item score for "disa- gree and 2 being the item score for "agree". This scale was adopted from Walters and Zak (1959) and is identical to the one administered to the subjects' parents ten years earlier. The higher the score, the more aggression. j.Potential Punishment for Aggression (PUN-TOP). This score is the weighted sum of 24 items. question 21 through 26, 49 through 54, 63 through 68, and 99 through 104. The weights are as follows:

135 128 1 ELF visION AND ADOLEscENTGGRESSIVENEss Response

Question No or Don't Know Yes

21 0

1, 0

0

24 0

15 0

26 0

49 0

50 0

51 0

5' 0

53

54 0

63

64 0

65 0

66 0

67 0

68 0

99 0

100 0

101 0

102 0

103 0

104 136 '1V VIOLEN(E Als.1)(1111 .1) AGGRESSION 129

The higher the score. the more punishment the respondent would admin- ister were he the parent of an eight-year-old child. These items were identical to those asked of the subjects' parents ten years earlier.

'. Social status factors

a. Social Status of Family (ISS). This category of variables is a set of 4 scores. They are: (1)Number of hooks (NOB).question 17. The higher the number. the higher the social status. (2)Occupation offaalkr(FOC).question 13 (or head of household) or 14. coded into 7 categories (Warner et al..1960). The higher the score. the lower the social status. (3)Total number of room.s. (4) COM poshe social status index (CIS). These responses were converted into 4 standard scores and combined as follows: questions 16 + 17 -13 18. The higher the score. the higher the social status. h.Current occupational status of respondent (CSR ). This category is a set of two scores. They are: (I) Occupation of respondent (ROC). Question 12 coded into 7 cat- egories (Warner et al.. 1960). 'The higher the score. the lower the social status. (2) Education of respondent (EDR). The higher the score, the high- er the social status. These two scores were not combinedinto a composite. c.Mobility aspirations (ASP). This category is a set of two scores. They are: (I) Total aspiration (TAS). Questions 7. 8, and 9 are combined in the following manner:

(7-Q7) + (7-Q8) + Q9. The higher the score. the higher the aspi- ration. (2) Mobility orientation (MOO). The sum of 5 items. questions 58 through 62. The higher the score, the higher the aspiration. d.Church attendance (R Question 19 yields the stated frequency of church attendance. The higher the score, the more fre- [ quent the stated church attendance.

1 3. Psychopathology a. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory(M MM. Form R (Hathaway & McKinley. 1969). This pencil-and-paper test provided three scores: (I)MMP1-49.Elevation of scales 4 and 9 scored in the following manner: 137 130 TELEVISION AND ADomscENT AGGRESSIVENESS if 4 and 9 not the two highest of 11 "clinical" scales.* score = 0: if both 4 and 9>70. score = 4:

if only one of the two270 or if both 4 and 9 >60. score = 3:

if only one of the two >60. score = 2:

if neither 4 nor 9>60. score = I. (2)MMPI-49S.Sum of T scores on scales 4 and 9. For both of the foregoing M M PI scores, the higher the score, the more likely is the person to act out in an antisocial manner. (3)MMPI-T.This score is the number of clinical scales (11 scales. K corrected) greater than T = 70. The greater the number, the more the likelihood of psychopathology. b. The Z-Test (Zulliger. 1969). This is a 3-card inkblot technique for which scoring categories were adopted from Holtzman. Thorpe. Swartz. and Herron (1961) as modified by Lefkewitz (1968). Two scores were computed: (1)Hostility (Z-HOS-2).This is a binary score in which zero signi- fies the absence and 1 the presence of hostility. (2)Psychopathology (Z-SUM).This score is computed by summing four binary scores: hostility, anxiety, movement, and pathog- nomonic verbalization. The higher the score, the greater the psychopathology.

4.TV Variables a. Hours of watching TV (TV-HW-A).This score is the sum of questions 71 and 72. total number of hours TV is watched by subject per week. If one of the questions was not answered. then the response to the other question was multiplied by 2. b. TV ViolenceHudson (TV-VIOL-H).Ratings of violence by two judges were computed from question 83a. Each program (see appendix C) was categorized as nonviolent = 0. uncertain = 1, violent = 2. A subject's score was the average rating of the programs mentioned multiplied by 10. The ffigher the score the more violent the mentioned programs. c. TV ViolenceGreenberg(TV-VIOL-G).Ratings of violence on question 83a. The aVerage violence ratings for the subject's four favorite TV programs In ,:d on the "Public" violence rat- ings as determined by Greenberg and Gordon (1970). If the

*The IC-Scale was inadvertently included in the clinical scales by the computer program. 136 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 131

program mentioned had not beennoted by these authors. it was assigned a ratingof 1.5 which approximated the mean rat- ing. The higher the score, the more violentthe programs. d. Sports Programs on TV (TV-SPT).This score was comprised of questions 80-83. The higher the score,the more contact sports watched (football, hockey.boxing, and w re stling). e. Realism of TV (ROT). The sumof ratings I. 2, or 3, for each of 14 programs contained in questions 94 and95. The lower the score, the more realistic isthe subject. i.e.. the more he states that these TV programs are "fake" or"phony".. 5. Height a. Height of subject (SHT).Question 111. Standing height in inches obtained by reading from a tape measurefixed to the wall. Girls removed their shoes. Subject'sheight in inches was multiplied by 10. b. Discrepancy between Subject's Height and Averageof Moth- er's and Father's Height (DA H).The average of questions 109 and 110 minus subject's height, plus 100. When scoreis above 100, subject is shorter than average of his parents;when score is below 100, subject is taller than average ofhis parents. 6. School Records a. Achievement (ACH).Mean of all 12th grade achievement tests for which the subject had percentile scores.The mean was multiplied by 10. b. IQ.Obtained from 12th grade school records. c. Times Tardy (TARDY).Obtained from 12th grade school re- cords. Does not include subjects who were nevertardy. 7. Number of arrests Boys in the third grade study wereclassified within the upper and lower quartiles of aggression. The NewYork State Identification and Intelligence Service was asked to furnish aggregatedata on the number of arrests in New York State within each group.The criterion data were collected when modal age of subjects was 19 years.

4.30 132 1 II.IVISIoN AND ADOLESCEN1 A(;GRESSIVI.NEtis Appendix D: Classification according to violence of TV programs in ten-year followup

Violent

Avengers It Takes a Thief Bold Ones Lancer Bonanza Land of the Giants Daniel Boone Man From Uncle Dark Shadows Mannix Dragnet Mission: Impossible FBI Mod Squad Get Smart Name of the Game Gunsmokc Prisoner Hawaii Five-0 Superman High Chapparal Then Came Bronson I Spy Three Stooges Invaders Virginian Ironsides Wild. Wild West

Nonviolent

Adam 12 Coinedy Tonight Adams Family Courtship of Eddie's Father American Bandstand David Frost Andy Williams David Susskind Another World Days of our Lives Art Link letter Dean Martin Auto Racing Debbie Reynolds Banana Splits Dick Cavett Basketball Doris Day Beverly Hillbillies Ed Sullivan Bewitched Engelbert Humperdinck Big Valley Evening With the Pops Bill Cosby Family Affair Bracken's World First Tue.sday Brady Bunch Football Bul !winkle Fugitive Burke's Law Galloping Gourmet Cartoons Ghost and Mrs. Muir Carol Burnett Glen Campbell Comedy Shows Cotner Pyle, U.S. M.C. 140 TV VIOLENCE AND CHILI) AGGRESSION 133 N onviolent

Governor and J.J. Mike Douglas Green Acres Monkeys Hullaballoo My Favorite Martian He & She My Three Sons Hee Haw N.E.T. Playhouse Here Come the Brides News Programs Hogan's Heroes Pat Paulsen Show Honeymooners Perry Mason Huntley-Brinkley Petticoat Junction I Dream of Jeannie Quiz Shows I Love Lucy Ray Stevens Jack Benny Red Skelton Jeopardy Room 222 Jim Nabors Science Shows Johnny Carson Sesame Street Johnny Cash Sixty Minutes Julia Soap Operas Kraft Music Hall Smothers Brothers Lassie Star Trek Laugh-In Stock Car Races Leonard Bernstein That Girl Let's Make a Deal The Doctors Love, American Style The Show Love Is a Many Splendored ThingTo Rome With Love Love Stories Tom Jones Marcus Welby, M.D. Undersea World of Jaques Cousteau Mayberry, R.F.D. Walt Disney Medical Center Walter Cronkite Meet the Press White Paper Mery Griffin Wide World of Sports

Cannot be codeddon't know

Laredo Specials Outer Limits T.V. Movies

141 134 1 F.1. I'. V MON AN D A 1)01.1:SCENT AGGR ESSIV N F.SS Appendix E: Code manual for data set of 95 variables

Matrix 7tVariable Mnemonic Descr ip tion CardField Grade

1 1 N51 3 aggression 1 1 3

2 2 N533 aggression anxiety 1 2 3

3 3 N543 popularity 1 3 3

4 6 MBGA boys games 1 6 3

5 7 MGGA girls games 1 7 3

6 8 LFFR frequency aggression 1 8 3

7 9 LMFR frequency aggression 1 9 3

8 12 LFEA educational aspiration 1 12 3

9 13 LMEA educational aspiration 1 13 3

10 14 LFWZ Walters-Zaks 1 14 3

11 15 LMWZ WaltersZaks 1 15 3

12 16 LFRO respondents occupation 1 16 3

13 18 LFRJ rejection 1 18 3 14 19 LMRJ rejection 1 19 3 15 20 LFRA recency aggression 1 20 3

16 21 LMRA recency aggression 1 21 3

17 22 LFNU nurturance 2 1 3 18 23 LMNU nurturance 2 2 3 19 24 LFPD parental disharmony 2 3 3 20 25 LMPD parental disharmony 2 4 3 21 27 LMCI confessing 2 6 3 22 36 LFPU punishment 2 16 3 23 37 LMPU punishment 2 17 3 24 38 LFRM residential mobility 2 17 3 25 42 LFGL ganerational level 2 21 3 26 SU subject number 3 27 44 LFJP judgrnent punishment 3 2 3 28 45 LMJP judgment punishment 3 3 3 29 46 LFPI profile identification 3 4 3 30 47 LMPI profile identification 3 5 3 31 58 LFGI guilt 3 16 3 32 59 LMGI guilt 3 17 3 33 97 LFOA occupational aspiration 5 13 3 34 98 LMOA occupational aspiration 5 14 3 35 109 LFRG religiosity 6 4 3 36 110 LMRG religiosity 6 5 3 37 121 LFM0 mobility orientation 6 16 3 38 122 LMMO mobility orientation 6 17 3 39 136 LFFP F-scale acquiescence 7 10 3 40 137 LMFP F-scale acquiescence 7 11 3 41 139 LMEN enuresis 7 13 3 42 141 LFED parent education 7 15 3 43 142 LMED parent education 7 16 3 44 150 LFTV hours TV watched 8 3 3 45 151 LMTV hours TV watched 8 4 3 46 201 MDAP first picture drawn 10 12 3

47 8 I Q I 0 16 1 3 48 9 TVVLF TV violence 16 2 3 49 10 TV V LM TV violence 16 3 3 50 11 SEX sex of subject 16 4 3 51 12 ROT TV realism 16 5 13 52 13 JET effect TV violence 16 6 13 53 6 ISS-NOB number of books 17 6 13 142

4AIN , 111.,1,...... TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 135

CardField Grade MatrixtsVariable # Mnemonic Description 8 13 54 8 ISS-FOC fathers occupation 17 1 7 9 13 55 9 ISS-CIS composite social status 10 13 56 10 CSR-ROC respondents occupation 17 1 7 11 13 57 11 CSR-E DR respondents education 17 16 13 58 16 ASP-TAS total aspiration 17 17 13 59 17 ASP-MOO mobility orientation 18 13 60 18 REL-RAT religiosity 1 7 1 7 20 13 61 20 PUN-TOP punishment total 18 4 13 62 25 RAG-WAZ Walters-Zaks 13 63 26 RAG-AHA spank, slap, punch 18 5 18 6 13 64 27 RAG-AHB smash, rude, temper 18 7 13 65 28 RAG-ASB delinquent behavior 18 8 13 66 29 RAG-TAH Total aggressive habit 18 9 13 67 30 RAG-AGD aggressive drive 18 10 13 68 31 VAG victim-aggression 18 11 13 69 32 WAG witness-aggression 12 13 70 33 TAG Total aggressive environ- 18 ment 18 13 13 71 34 PER -AGG numerator-aggression 18 14 13 72 35 PER-ANX anxiety-aggression 13 73 36 PER-POP popularity 18 15 13 74 38 HGT-SHT subjects height 18 17 18 19 13 75 40 HGT-DAH height deviation 18 21 13 76 42 Z-HOS-2 hostility 13 77 46 Z-SUM psychopathology 19 4 13 78 47 MMPI-49 4, 9 above 70 T 19 5 6 13 79 48 AC H achievement 19 13 80 49 I 0 I CI 19 7 8 13 81 50 TARDY TARDY 19 19 9 13 82 51 TV-HW-A TV hours watched 13 83 55 TV-SPT TV sports 19 13 13 84 56 TV-VIOL-H Violence-Hudson raters 19 14 13 85 57 TV-VIOL-G Violence-Greenberg 19 15 13 86 58 MMPI-T Clin. scales > 70 T 19 16 17 13 87 59 MMP I-495 sum 4 + 9 19 19 19 13 88 61 PER-LED peer leader 20 13 89 62 PER -WKY who knows you 19 13 90 63 PE R-AGG/WKY aggression 19 21 20 1 8 91 1 PER-AGG 9 items 2 8 92 2 PER-AGG 2 item aggression score 20 20 3 8 9 3 3 PER-POP popularity 8 94 4 TV-VIOL-1 violence rating 20 4 20 5 8 95 5 TV-VI OL-JH violence rating

143 Two Commentson Cross- lagged Correlation

Threats to the Internal Validity of Gross-Lagged Panel Inference,as Related to "Television Violence and Child Aggression: A Followup Study"

David A. Ken ny Northwestern University

Panel or longitudinal studies allow the researcher to study important variables in their naturalistic setting. However, since panel studies lack both the experimental and statistical controls present in experimental research, there are greater threats to the validity of an inference made from a panel study. A major threat to the validity of an inference ina panel study is the problem of differential reliability ofr measurement. This problem has been repeatedly emphasized by Campbell and hisstu- dents (Campbell and Clayton, 1961; Campbell, 1963; Rozelle and Camp- bell, 1969; Crano, Kenny, and Campbell, 1971).

136

114 CROSS-LAGGED CORRELATION 137

In its simplest form cross-laggedpanel correlation (CLPC) involves a simple comparison of cross-laggedcorrelations. However, such a model assumes that the reliabilityof each variable stays constant over time.As Campbell (1963) points out: "Avariable which increases in reliability from Time 1 to Time 2 will. coedsparibus. show up as an effectrather than a cause" (p. 240). A secondand related threat to the validityof an inference from panel data refers to truefactorial structure of the varia- bles. As Kenny (1971) points out,what distinguishes CLPC modelfrom path models elucidated by Duncan(1969) and Heise (1970)is the as- sumption that there are factors that causeboth %variables in the panel. The logic of CLPC assumes that thefactorial structure of each variable is stationary over time. Rozelle andCampbell (1969) suggest a test of this assumption by comparing the twosynchronous correlations over time. If the two are not equal, thenit is not plausible to assume a station- ary factor structure.Kenny has devised a more general testof the sta- tionarity assumption for the multivariate case. Keeping these threats to the validity of aCLPC inference in mind, let us turn our attention to"Television violence and child aggression: afol- lowup study" (1971). Using CLPC theauthors conclude that viewing television violence (TVVL) causesaggression (AGG). The relevant cross-lagged correlations are reproducedfrom the study in Figure I. The authors base their argument thatTVVL3 causes AGG13 on:

1.rTVVI..3 AGGI3 rTVVI-I3 AGG3

2. 1TVVI.3 AGGI3 rTVVI.3 AGG3. An examination of FigureI reveals that none of thevariables signifi- cantly correlates with the TVVL13 measure.Given this failure to corre- late, one or both of the followingis true:I) The TVVL I 3 is very unre- liable. This seems plausible sinceit has near-zero correlations withall variables. including TV VL3.2) A common factor that causesthe other three variables causes TVVL13 in the opposite direction.Since either one of the two ofthe above are true, and sinceboth are ,nreats to the validity of a CLPC inference, asimple comparison of cross-lagged cor- relations is not valid in this case.Given unreliability or unstationarity. the cross-lagged asymmetry canbe explained without resorting to causa- (ion. (The author attempted tofind a variable that caused TVVL13 nega- tively and TVVL3, AGG3, andAGG 13 positively, but no suchvariable was found. In fact,intelligence, achievement, andsubjects' mobility aspirations correlated in the samedirection with all four variables.) The results would be completelyuninterpretable if it were not for the fact that rTVVL3 AGG 13 is greaterthan rTVVL3 AGG3. If TVVLdid not cause AGG, thenthe cross-lagged correlationshould be smaller than the synchronous correlation,ceteris paribas. There are twoimportant aspects that determineexactly how much difference the twocorrelations should be:1) the relationship of thereliability of AGG3 to AGGI3; and 145

_.,44,04 a', I. 4, 138 TELEVISION AN D ADOLESCENT AG(;RESSIVENESS

TVVL3 .05

.21 -.05

AGG3 .38 AGG13

Figure 1: The correlations between television violence and aggression for 211 boys over a ten/ear lag

2) the temporal erosion or attenuation rate of the common causes of AGG and TV VL. If the reliability of the AGG3 measure was lower than the AGGI3 measure, the inequality of the cross-lagged and synchronous correla- tions could be partly explained. Differential reliability over time is an omnipresent threat to the validity of panel inferences. There are no measures of internal consistency of peer aggression, but it is possible to indirectly obtain evidence of the reliability of the AGG3 and AGGI3 measures by the test-retest correlations and the correlation of aggres- sion with other variables. Since there are three waves, it is possible to estimate reliability coeffi- cients for the middle measure (cf. Humphreys. 1960: Wells, Threskog, and Linn, 1971). Such an estimateis simply rAGG3 AGG8rAGG8 AGG13I'AGG3AGG13. The obtained reliability isa respectable .82. The reliability for the first or last measure is not determined or identified. However, we can get a rough idea of the reliability of the variables by examining the test-retest correlations with the eighth grade measure. The 3-8 correlation is .48 and the 8-13 correlation is .65. At first glance this suggests that the 13 measure is more reliable, but other considera- tions lessen such a possibility. It is reasonable to expect aggression to be more stable over the 8-13 time period than 3-8. If such is the case, the correlations are not equal because of differential temporal erosion of the aggression trait and not because of reliability. Also the grade 13 measure is not actually a thirteenth grade measure but a retrospective measure. It is actually a twelfth or eleventh grade measure. Thus we should expect a '8 11.5 correlation to be greater than r3 8. So the test-retest correlations fail to indicate that the third grade measure is less reliable than the thir- teenth grade measure of aggression. 139 CROSS-LAGGED CORRELATION

A second and stronger test ofreliability stability has been suggested (Kenny, 1971; Crano, Kenny. and Campbell.1971). This "communality adjustment" involves an examination of theratio of time one synchro- nous correlations of a variablewith all other variables over the compara- ble time two synchronous correlations.Unfortunately for the present study, few variables were repeatedly measuredin the same manner. Of those that were, the correlations are rather small.However, it is possi- ble to use correlations involving measuresof intelligence at grade 3 (1Q3) and achievement at grade 13 (ACH 13). Let us as?....methat some factor common to both IQ and ACH causes AGGsynchronously (or vice versa). If such is the case, then the ratioof the reliability of AGG3 over the reliability AGGI3 equalsrAGG3 IQ3r AGG3 ACH131('AGG 1 3 IQ3 rAGG 13 ACH 13). The obtained value is1.09. Thus, it appears that the reliability of AGG3 is just as large if notlarger than AGGI3, so the fact that the AGG3-TVVL3 correlation is smallerthan the AGG13- TVVL3 correlation cannot be attributed to the factof lower reliability of the AGG3 measure. The problem now is to infer how muchsmaller the .21 correlation would have eroded if TVVL did not cause AGG.Basically the reasoning is like this:If AGG and TVVL are not causally related, thenthe .21 synchronous correlation between them is due to thefact that both are caused by some common cause. The common causechanges over time and so, ceteris paribus, the lagged correlationsshould be smaller than the synchronous correlation. The magnitudeof the difference is a func- tion of the test-retest correlation of the common cause.Rozelle and Campbell (1969) refer to this correlation as theattenuation constant, and Kenny (1971) as the temporal erosion rate. Otherauthors have suggest- ed partialing AGG3 out of the correlationbetween TVVL3 and AGG13. Such a procedure assumes that the test-retestcorrelation is the temporal erosion rate, but the test-retest correlation is a grossunderestimate of the rate of erosion since it is affected byboth unreliability and temporal erosion (Bohrnstedt,. 1969; Brewer, Crano, andCampbell, 1970). Partial correlations and multiple regression usuallyoverestimate the strength of a relationship if the controlvariable is measured with error. One estimate of the common factor erosion rateis the unattenuated test-retest correlation of the aggression measure.Earlier we stated that an estimate of reliabilityof the A008 measure is .82. If we assume that AGG3 and AGG13 have the same reliability,the unattenuated test-retest correlation is .46. But such a value is a weighted sumof all the erosion rates that make up the aggression me;Isure.For instance, it is reasonable to assume that intelligence causesaggression. An estimate of the erosion rate due to intelligence can beobtained by rAGGI3 IQ3kAGG3 IQ3. This equals .75. A rough ad hoc estimate of the commonfactor erosion rate can be obtained by averaging .46and .75. The common factor tem- poral erosion rate equals .605. Multiplying .605times .21 equals .127.

. 147 140 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Testing to see whether .127 is significantly different from .31 usinga Fisher's z yields a z of 1.97 which is significant at the .05 level. It should be kept in mind that this difference is a function of the erosion rate that we have chosen and the assumption of equal reliability in the two AGG measures. Thus, the strongly significant results that the authors originally ob- tained were biased by unstationarity and the use of partial correlations. The data still indicates that TVVL causes violence, but the results, though statistically significant, are somewhat marginal. Nonexperimen- tal inference is a risky business, and a sample of 211 boys will almost undoubtedly never present us with a final answer. However, the data do suggest that watching violent television shows does cause later aggres- sion. BIBLIOGRAPHY

Bohrnstedt, G.W. Observations on the measurement of change. In Ed- gar F. Borgatta (Ed.) Sociological Methodology, 1969. San Francis- co: Jossey-Bass. 1969. Brewer, M., Crano, W., and Campbell, D.T. The use of partial correla- tions to test hypotheses. Sociometry, 1970, 33, 1-11. Campbell, D.T. From descriptionto experimentation: Interpreting trends as quasi-experiments. In C.W. Harris (Ed.) Problems inmeas- uring change. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1963. Crano, W.D., Kenny, D.A., and Campbell, D.T. Does intelligence cause achievement? A cross-lagged panel analysis. Accepted for publication, Journal of Educational Psychology, 1971. Duncan, O.D. Some linear models for two-wave, two-variable panel analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 1969, 70, 177-82. Heise, D. Causal inference from panel data. In E. Borgatta and G. Bohrnstedt(Eds.) SociologicalMethodology 1970. Jossey- Bass: San Francisco, 1971. Humphreys, L.G. Investigations of a simplex. Psychometrics, 1960, 25, 313-23. Lefkowitz, M., Eron, I.., Walder, L., and Huesmann, L.R. Televi- sion violence and child aggression: a followup study. In Television and Social Behavior, Vol. 3 (this volume). Kenny, D.A. Cross-lagged and synchronous common factors in panel data. To be published in A. Goldberger and 0.D, Duncan (Eds.) Structural Equations Models. Rozelle, R.M., and Campbell, D.T. More plausible rival hypotheses in the cross-lagged panel correlation technique. Psychological Bulle- tin, 1969, 71, 74-80. Werts, C., Jiireskog, K., and Linn, R. Comment on "The estimation of measurement error in panel data." American Sociological Review, 1971, 36, 110-12. 148 Comment on "Television violence and child aggression: afollowup study"

John M. Neale

State University of New York at StonyBrook

Correlational investigations have always been prevalentin behavioral research and forsome purposes can provideimportant information. However, two problems of interpreting an obtainedcorrelational rela- tionship have been viewed as major limitations on theutility of the ap- proach. One, the problem of directionality, refers to thefact that a cor- relation between two variables tells us only that they are related or tend to covary with one another, but does not tell uswhether one is caused by the other. Consider, for example, the correlation between attendancein class and obtained grades. One possible interpretationof this relation- ship is that greater class attendance increases the amount learned and 141

149 142 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

thus produces higher grades. However,a second interpretation is also available: good grades might lead the studentswho obtain them to at- tend class more frequently. Thus, inmany instances the problem of directionality cannot be resolved: hence, theoft-cited dictum "correla- tion does not imply causation." The second major problem in interpretingcorrelational data is the so- called third variable problem. That is.two variables may be correlated without being causally related to each other.Rather, some unknown variable may be mediating the obtainedrelationship. For instance,re- turning to the previous example, the correlationbetween grades and at- tendance may reflect the operation ofa third variable"conscien- tiousness." That is, students whoare high in conscientiousness may both attend class regularly and work diligentlyto obtain high grades. The previous example illustratesa fairly obvious instance of a rela- tionship being produced by the operation ofa third variable. However, in many instances unsuspected third variablesmay be operative, thus making the problem considerablymore difficult to solve. The cross-lagged panel design

The problem of the directionality ofa correlational relationship may be answered in some circumstances byemploying a design in which measurements are repeated on the same individualsat two points in time the cross-lagged panel design (Campbell,1963). As an example, con- sider the study mentioned previouslyon attendance and grades. This time, however, the measurements of attendanceand grades are made twice, say at the end of the first and secondsemesters. The two compet- ing hypotheses of primary interest 1are: Hypothesis 1: higher gradescause higher attendance; Hypothesis 2: greater attendancecauses higher grades. The design and a hypotheticalpattern of results are shown in figure 1.

First semester Second semester

Figure 1: Cross-lagged panel design to assess the effect of attendanceon grades TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 143

Note that the most relevant correlations are those onthe diagonals. Hypothesis1 states thatgrades "cause" attendance and thus implies that the correlation between first-semestergrades and second-semester attendance should be high. Conversely, Hypothesis2 states that attend- ance "causes" higher grades(assuming that the subject matter of the course is cumulative over the twosemesters), and thus implies that the correlations between attendance in the first semesterand grades in the second semester should be relatively high.Generally, the two diagonal correlations are compared and a decision between the twohypotheses is then based on which correlation is higher. The cross-lagged panel techniqueemploying the diagonal correlations rests onthe assumption that "causes" must precede"effects." Thus, the correlation of an effect with aprior "cause" is compared with the correlation of an effect with a "cause" whichtemporally came after it. An example of a pattern of results favoringthe hypothesis that good attendance causes high grades is illustrated inFigure 1. The correlation between first-semester attendance and second-semestergrades is .80, while the correlation between first-semestergrade s and second-semester attendance is .10. Since the former correlationis significantly larger than the latter, hypothesis two is selected. The recent study of Lefkowitz, Eron,Walder, and Huesmann (1971), a correlational study of the relationshipbetween watching vio- lent television fare and subsequentaggression, used the cross-lagged panel technique. In the Lefkowitz et al. study, allthe children in a par- ticular county were examined while in the third grade,and were subse- quently followed up ten years later. While manyvariables were investi- gated, for the purposes of these commentsonly the measures relating to the viewing of violent television and ratedaggressiveness need be consi- dered. The measure of aggression in theclassroom was a peer nomina- tion technique in which the children in aparticular classroom selected other children who fit particular descriptionssuch as: Who is always getting into trouble? Who starts a fight overnothing? The measure of the degree of violenftelevision watched by the child wasobtained by having the parents provide a list of the child's favorite programs.Ratings of the violence contained in these programs were then madeindependently by two raters so that the child'sputative television diet could be assigned a violence rating. Of the initial 875 subjects in the study, 427 wereable to be contacted successfully ten years later. The followup sample wascomposed of 211 boys and 216 girls. These subjects wereinterviewed by a member of the research team, and during this interview data wascollected both on stat- ed preferences for television programs and onratings of aggression. As was done with the third gradesample, the subjects' preferred television programs were independently ratedfor their violent content, so that a score could be assigned reflectingthe degree of violence in the preferred 144 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS television shows of each subject. To obtain:a rating of degree of aggres- siveness, subjects were presented with lists of names of other students and were asked which of them might fit into the various categories of aggressive responding. The categories of aggressive responding repre- sented a slightly modified version of the form which had been used for the third graders. For the male subjects the major outcome of the Lef- kowitz et al. investigation is presented in Figure 2.

Third Ode Thirteenth grade

TV Violence .05 TV Violence

.21 -.05

.01 .31 I Aggression .38 Aggression Figure 2: Results of the Lefkowitz, Eron, Walder, and Huesmann study

Within the cross-lagged technique, the critical correlations to be ex- amined are those on the diagonals. Based on the assumption that a "cause" must be temporally prior to an "effect," two rival hypotheses may be pitted against each other. If aggression during the third grade was causally related to the amount of television violence preferred ten years later, then the correlation between these two variables should be high. Conversely, if the amount of television violence preferred during the third grade were causally related to the level of aggressive behavior ten years later, then this correlation should be high. As may be seen from Figure 2, the Lefkowitz et al. cross-lagged (on the diagonals) data may be taken as support for the hypothesis that higher levels of violence on preferred television programs in the third grade is causally related to the amount of aggressive responding ten,years later. That is, the correla- tion between preference for violent television programs in the third grade and peer-rated aggression ten years later (r = .31) was significant- ly higher than the correlation between peer-rated aggression in the third grade and preference for violent television fare in the thirteenth grade ( r = .01). Path analysis There may be some problems involved in utilizing the raw correlation coefficients in attempting to interpret the results of a panel study, some of which may be circumvented by employing the procedures of path analysis. Path analysis was developed to explicate the causal paths ac- counting for a set of observed correlations. With the variables expressed 152 TV VIOLENCE ANE) CHILD AGGRESSION 145 in standardized form, path coefficients are obtained from a multiple re- gression equation between the predictor and criterion variables.While the technique has been used more often in cases where alarge number of variables are under study, Heise (1970) has applied thetechnique to the cross-lagged panel design. He has noted that in thisapplication: different measumments on the same variable are treated here as hypothetically different variables. For example, Xi andX3are construed as distinct variables even though they actually are only measurements at different times on the same variable (X odd). Therefore, in the path analysis, the two-variable, two- wave situation is treated as afour-variable problem (1970, p.4). An illustration of a four-variable problem is presented inFigure 3. xl x3

X2 X4

Figure 3: Path diagram for a two-variable, two-wave panel design

Of the twelve possible paths or connecting lineswhich could be drawn in such a four-variable problem, four can beimmediately eliminated as not implicating causality. The elimination of these four paths isbased on the assumption that later states can not "determine"prior states; thus, the pathsX3-4-X X3 --0-X2, X4 X,.andX4---.X1can all be elimi- nated. In such a design there are four causalpaths of primary interest: X X----4`Xand X That is, the primary X1-4-X3 , I 4 ' 2 3 2-1X4' interest is in the possible causalrelationships between prior an-d later states (the synchronouscorrelation, e.g. rx1x,, provides aconteIxt in which the causal paths may be interpreted).In contrast to the usual pro- cedures iithe cross-lagiged panel design.however, path analysis does notem ploy raw correlationcoefficients to estimate the strength of these various possible causal rellitionships.Rather, path coefficients are em- ployed. With the variables expressedin standardized form, pathcoefficients equation between the predictor are obtainedfrom a multiple regression In the simple case withwhich we are now deal- and criterion variables. variables holding ing, the regression equationis merely between two constant the possibleinfluence of third variables.Thus, the path coeffi- cients for each of the fourimportant paths in such astudy may be writ- ten as follows: p 13 P12P23 P3 I p 2 153 12

1. . -.- 146 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

P32 P 23 PI2P I 3t d 2/ I 12

P41 P 14 P12 P24

1 P I 2 P42 P 24 P 12 P14 I P Z 12

Observed correlations among variables, rij, are used to estimate the true correlations, Pij. Each of these path coefficients is equivalent to a 'stand- p31 = ardized partial regression coefficient; that is, 31.2'P32= 032.1 P4I andp42 = 41.1. The use of path analyiis controls successfully for one important prob- lem which might ensue were the raw correlations employed. That is, con- sider the path from X1 to X4. In addition to the direct path between these two variables, there is a less direct but nevertheless plausible means by which X1 could indirectly influence X4. This possibility is diagrammed in Figure 4. For example, a high correlation between first-semester attend- ance and second-semester grades could be mediated by: 1) a high corre- lation between grades and attendance in the first semester, and 2) a high correlation between first and second semester grades.

xl X3

X2 X4

Figure 4: Alternate path for the X, X4 relationship

More formally, the synchronous correlation between X1 andX2plus a high X, X4correlation could "explain" the X1 toX4relationship. Path analysis circumvents this problem by employing standard partialregres- sion coefficients rather than the raw correlations. Heise (1970) has also examined some important properties of path analysis of panel data by employing computer simulation procedures. In one important series, he investigated the influence of low reliabilities of the measurements (i.e., reliabilities of .50 to .64). His conclusionwas as follows: There appears to be a close parallelism between the estimated values and the true values of the system parameters, and, in fact 154 - 4,",..7 vv.

TV VIOLENCE AND CHILD AGGRESSION 147 the product-moment correlation between the two setsof coeffi- cients is .99. This suggests that even though the two-wave mod- el does not yield the actual values of the system parameters when measurements are imprecise, it might give a setof num- bers which could be used for causal inference (p.15). For the aforementioned reasons, it seems both important andappropri- ate to apply the procedures of path analysis tothe Lefkowitz et al. data generated. The results of this analysis are displayed in Figure 5.

Third grade Thirteenth grade

TV Violence .05 TV Violence

. 0 .24 Aggression .33 Aggression

Figure 5:Path analysis of the Lefkowitz et al. data

As may be seen, the results of the path analysis do not contradictand, in fact, add further support to the conclusion of Lefkowitz et al. con- cerning the influence of violent television in,the third grade upon subse- quent aggressive behavior in early adulthood. Thepath coefficient for the relationship between television violence in the third grade and subse- quent aggressive behavior was .24, while the pathcoefficient for the re- lationship between aggression in the third grade and subsequent prefer- ence for violent television fare was 0.00. Themost plausible interpreta- tion of these results is that a preference for violenttelevision in the third grade is causally related to aggressive behavior ten years later. Two further points merit discussion. First, while thepath analysis does not require that the same variables be measured at eachof the two times, the introduction of different measures does raisethe issue of pos- sible changes in reliability and thus provides a possiblechallenge to the interpretation of the results of the cross-lagged design. As Campbell (1963) has noted, a variable which increases in reliability fromtime one to time two will, ceteris paribus, show up as an"effect" rather than as a cause. However, a case can bemade that both the measure of prefer- ence for violent television programsand peer-rated aggressiveness may have been more reliable in the thirteenth grade (seeChaffee, 1971). Thus, since no compelling evidence is available that only oneof the vari- ables increased in reliability, the change-in-reliabilityinterpretation of the Lefkowitz et al. data becomes less plausible.Second, within the 148 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS context of the logic of panel designs. the possibility remains that the re- lationship between viewing violent television fare and subsequent levels of aggression is mediated by a third variable. However, other correla- tional and experimental research has ruled out many plausible third vari- ables. Thus, the most parsimonious interpretation of the Lefkowitz et al. data seems to be that favored by the investigators themselves namely, that preference ifor violent television programs in the third grade is causally related to subsequent aggressive behavior.

FOOTNOTES

1. As noted by Rozelle and Campbell (1969), there are actually four possible hypotheses, but only two need be considered for illustrative purposes.

REFERENCES

Campbell, D. T. From description to experimentation: interpreting trends as vasi-experiments. In C. W. Haris (Ed.) Problems in meas- uring change. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press, 1963. Chaffee, S. J. Television and adolescent aggressiveness. In Television and social behavior. Vol. 3 (this volume). Heise, D. R. Causal inference from panel data. In E. F. Borgatta and G. W. Bohrnstedt (Eds.) Sociological methodology 1970. San Francis- co: Jossey-Bass, 1970. Lefkowitz, M. M., Eron, L. D., Walder, L. 0., and Huesmann, L. R. Television violence and aggression: A followup study. In Televi- sion and social behavior, Vol. 3 (this volume). Rozelle, R. M., and Campbell, D. T. More plausible rival hypotheses in the cross-lagged panel correlation technique. Psychological Bulle- tin. 1969,71,74-80. Adolescent Television Use in the Family Context

Steven H. Chaffee and Jack M. McLeod University of Wisconsin

This study consists of a secondary analysis of data originally collected in a field survey of parent-adolescent communication and political so- cialization, under a grant to the authors by the National Science Founda- tion.1 The data were gathered in 1968 in five eastern Wisconsin school districts, which had been selected to provide socioeconomic and politi- cal diversity. Since details of sampling and data collection have been described elsewhere (Chaffee, Ward, and Tipton, 1970), only those par- ticulars that are pertinent to this study will be described here. Adolescent viewing of two kinds of television programs that often feature violent actionspy-adventure shows and westernsis the

149

1.5'7 150 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

central dependent variable in this stucly. Adolescents'self-reported fre- quency of viewing such programs is analyzed in relationto a series of individual and parent-child interaction factors. Individualcorrelates in- clude the youngster's grade level,sex , 1(), socioeconomic status (SES), dogmatism, and total use of television. Parentaland interaction varia- bles include the parent's sex, viewing of violentprograms, and total use of television; parent-child communicationstructure; and parental sanc- tions in terms of affection, punishment, and restrictivenesstoward the child.

PROCEDURES AND MEASURES The sample

The five communities are located in the Fox RiverValley and subur- ban Milwaukee regions of Wisconsin and ranged inpopulation from about 18,000 to 68,000 in the 1960census. On the basis of census occu- pational and income data, three communitieswere classified as predomi- nantly white-collar and twoas blue-collar in preliminary planning for the study. In May 1968, questionnaires were administered allseventh- and tenth-grade students in the smaller communities, and incertain schools (selected in collaboration with local school officials) in thelarger cities. These questionnaires provided what will be referredto hereafter as our "spring-child" data. They also formed the basic universefor drawing the final sample. In all, some 8,000 questionnaires were filled out in May.From these a subsample was drawn according to three criteria: If more than a few questionnaire items hadnot been answered, the student was eliminated from the study. Those who gave rural-route addresses,or who indicated they would not be in the same school district the following fall semester,were elimi- nated. A sampling list was compiled randomly from the remainingquestion- naires, designed to provide roughly equal Ns for each district,grade lev- el, and sex. This sampling list was given to interviewers of theWisconsin Survey Research Laboratory, who interviewedone parent of each student at home in September and October. Fathers andmothers were sampled alternately, according to a predetermined schedule.These interviews provide the "parent" measures referred to in thisreport. At the end of each interview, the parent was asked for permissionto administer a sec- ond questionnaire to the child at school. In November 1968,those chil- dren whose parents had given permissionwere remeasured, providing what is called "fall-child" data here. The eventual sample of1,292 com- prises more than 90 per cent of those whoseparents had been inter- viewed in early fall. 15$ Prvs",..'"^7.,...-""("'"

ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 151 Throughout this report, we will refer to "junior high" and "senior high" subsamples, although those labels do not necessarily coincide with the precise titles of the schools. The junior high sample consists of 641 who were seventh graders in May and eighth graders in November. The 651 senior high respondents were in the tenth grade in May and the eleventh in November. This sample is obviously not representative of the schools, districts, or cities involved, since several nonrandom factors were involved in the sampling. The two grade-level subsamples are only roughly comparable, because junior high and middle school district boundaries are not always coterminous with senior high boundaries, and because Roman Catholic families are more likely to send their children to public senior high schools than to public junior highs. There is nothing "typical" about these five districts, any more than any other five. Their populations are overwhelmingly white, and they are suburban and industrial communities concentrated in an agrarian region of the upper Midwest. These biases in the sample will affect the absolute level on at least some of our variables. Therefore we will con- centrate on relationships between variables in our analysis. The extent to which these leationships are peculiar to this sample can be checked independently, by replication in other settings. Measures This section describes the indices used in this report, beginning with the dependent variables and following with the independent variables in the order in which they are introduced in the analysis. Child's violence viewing. In both spring and fall, the student was asked to indicate which kinds of television programs he watched at least "pretty often." Among the program types listed were "westerns" and "spy-adventure shows." The spring and fall data have been combined into a single three-level index for each type of program, and these in- dices have been further summed into a six-level measure that we will call "violence viewing" on the assumption that the programs included were fairly likely in 1968 to present scenes involving violent action. For convenience throughout the paper, we will use the label VV to refer to this violence viewing index and to the corresponding measure for the parent (see below). Some important inadequacies of the VV measures should be men- tioned. Few items, not finely graded, are involved; this means that we cannot expect better than minimal reliability of measurement. Further, the questions are not very specific; in 1968 a great variety of programs could have been loosely described by the labels "spy-adventure" and "western," including some comedy shows. No better measures are available for this study, simply because television violence was not con- sidered a factor of importance when the original study of family

....sevrat.40P100.401WM .. _, 152 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

communication and political socialization was planned and carried out. To compensate somewhat for unreliability, the sample size is large, and there is a rather rich variety of other variables for analysis. Rather small correlations will be statistically significant (albeit numerically unimpres- sive), and we will be able to control simultaneously fora number of po- tentially important factors. One might reason that there is an important difference between watching television a great deal (and therefore seeing considerable vio- lent action) and the more specific behavior of selecting violentprograms to the relative exclusion of other television fare. Thereforewe have ana- lyzed not only theabsolute VVlevel, but also therelative VVlevel by controlling for the child's total television time. Items in the springand fall questionnaires asked, respectively, "About how long didyou watch television yesterday?" and "Onan average day, about how many hours do you usually spend watching television in the evening after5 p.m.?" These items were summed intoa single index of television time. This index bears an obvious part-whole relationshipto VV (with which it cor- relates .29). By controlling for television time,we can examine the spe- cific effects on VV of our independent variables, separately from their general influence on the child's overalluse of television. Parent's television use.The parent questionnaire included single-item measures of television time and of western and spy program viewing that were identical to the spring-child questions. These itemswere summed and analyzed in the same fashionas were the corresponding child items. Dogmatism.Rokeach (1960) introduced this concept as an indicator of a "close-minded" personality. Four items were drawn from the Ro- keach battery and included in both the parent and child (fall) question- naires; agreement scores were summed to providean estimate of dog- matism for each individual. The items: a. In this complicated world, the only way wecan know what is going on is to rely on leaders or experts whocan be trusted. (+) b.It's often good to reserve judgment about what's goingon until you have heard all the opinions about it. () c.Of all the different beliefs that exist in the world, there is proba- bly only one that is correct. (+) d.In most political disagreements, there is something good to be said on both sides of the question. () Socioeconomic status.A three-item SES index was constructed from questions (asked of the parent) concerning occupation of mainearner, family income, and parent's education. Intelligence quotient.For approximately 90 per cent of theyoung- sters, school records provided estimates of IQ. Therefore analyses in- volving this measure will have somewhat reduced Ns. The most recent IQ score was used, regardless of the particular test involved. 160 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 153 Family communication pattern. The present data constitute the most extensive attempt to date to assess the structure of parent-child commu- nication in a large sample of families. This line of researchbegan in a southern Wisconsin community in 1965 (see McLeod,Chaffee, and Eswara, 1966; Chaffee, McLeod, and Wackman, 1966;McLeod, Chaf- fee, and Wackman, 1967); fragments of relatedfindings have been pub- lished (McLeod, Rush, and Friederich, 1968-69; Stone andChaffee, 1970; Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). These earlystudies provide the empirical base for a general model ofcoorientation (Chaffee and McLeod, 1970; McLeod and Chaffee, in press). Since the measures con- stitute the most original aspect of this report, we willdigress somewhat here to describe their background. In a half-dozen studies in a variety of social settings, wehave consist- ently found by factor analysis that there are at least twodimensions of variation in the structure of parent-child communication.Although these dimensions are in some respects conceptuallycontradictory, we have found that empirically they are either uncorrelated orslightly posi- tively associated. We have called these dimensionssocio-oriented and concept-oriented patterns of constraint on the developingchild. (In the present sample, these two measurescorrelate .11, which is the strongest association we have found between them in any study to date.) The socio-oriented relation, which may be pressed on a very young child, is typified by encouraging the youngster tomaintain harmonious interpersonal relations, avoid controversy, and repress hisinner feelings on extrapersonal topics.Following are some sample items that were asked, with appropriate variations in wording, of both parent andchild; in some cases the question asked about the frequency of the act,and in other question parental emphasis was estimated by the respondent: (Parent) urges (child) to give in on arguments rather than risk antagonizingothers. (Parent) answers (child's) arguments by saying something like, "You'llknow bet- ter when you grow up." (Parent) lets (child) know that (child) should not show anger in a group. (Parent) stresses that there are some things in life that are either right or wrong. (Parent) says that the best way to stay out of trouble is to keep away from it. (Parent) says that discussions are better if you keep them pleasant. Concept-oriented communication was measured on a similar set of items, dealing with the emphasis or frequency of parental constraints on the child to express his own ideas, become exposed to controversy, and challenge the views of others. Some sample items: (Parent) encourages (child) to challenge (parent's) ideas and beliefs. (Parent) asks (child's) opinion when family is discussing something. (Parent and child) have family talks about topics like politics or religion, where some persons take different sides from others. (Parent) says that (child) should always look at both sides of an issue before making up (child's) mind. (Parent) argues about things like politics or religion when visiting with friends or relatives, when (child) is present. The socio-orientation decreases during adolescence. Concept-orien- tation changes very little from junior to senior high school. At both .161 ....1 154 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

grade levels, about as many families stress both or neither orientationas stress one or the other (Chaffee, McLeod and Atkin, 1971). Although it is possible to treat each of these dimensions as a separate independent variable, we have found in some studies that the two inter- act, producing structural patterns that are not simply the sum s of their two constituent functions. Therefore we will treat family communica- tion at later points in this report in term s of four types: a. Laissez fake families, which emphasize neither type of relation. Children are not prohibited from challenging parental views but neither are they exposed to the world of independent and contending ideas. b. Protective families, which stress socio-relations only. The child is encouraged to get along with others, at the expense of concept-relations that would expose him to the controversial world of ideas. Not only is he prohibited from expressing dissent, but he is given little chance toen- counter information on which he might base his own views. c.Pluralistic families, which emphasize the development of strong and varied concept-relations in an environment comparatively free of social restraints. The child is encouraged to explore new ideas and is exposed to controversial material; he can make up his own mind without fear of endangering social relations with his parents. d. Consensual families, which attempt to stress both orientations. While the child is exposed to controversy, and told he should enter into it, he is (paradoxically) constrained to develop concepts that are conso- nant with existing socio-relations. That is, he is in effect encouraged to learn his parents' ideas and adopt their values. Affection, punishment, and restrictiveness. In addition to socializing their children via family communication patterns, parents can exercisea number of direct sanctions over their children's behavior. We construct- ed four indices representing different kinds of sanctions: parental affec- tion, physical punishment, verbal punishment, and restrictiveness. Affection is based on three items in the spring-child questionnaire. The first two were separate indicators for father and mother, in response to the question, "How often do you feel your parents show they love you?" The third item consisted of the number of different ways of dem- onstrating affection that the child indicated in response to the question, "How do you think your parents show you they love you?" Physical Punishment is a four-item index, consisting of two questions, each asked separately of parent and child (spring). The first question asked how often (parent) punished (child) physically when (child) was younger. The second asked how often (parent) punishes child "now" by "spanking." (Appropriate variations in wording were used in the respective questionnaires.) Verbal Punishment is a six-item index, consisting of three questions, each asked separately of parent and child (spring). The items asked how often (parent) punishes (child) by three methods: by yelling at (child);

lt".3 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 155 by trying to make (child) feel bad: and by lecturing (child) or talking to (child) about it. Restrictiveness is a six-item sum based on three items, each of which was asked of both parent and child (spring). Two of these items asked how often (parent) punishes (child) by taking away privileges and/or by "grounding" (child). The third item asked how many different kinds of (child's) activities the parents had "definite rules" for; a checklist of eight possible areas for rules was offered. On the assumption that the youngster's perception of a rule was functionally more important for our purposes than the parent's perception, the parental response to this item was given only half the weight of the child's response in our index.

FINDINGS Although the selection of variables and models of analysis implies cer- tain theoretical assumptions, the presentation of data here will not pro- ceed on the basis of formal hypotheses. Rather, we will examine a series of complex tables, each of which shows a number of relationships, and discuss the possible theoretical import of those relationships that we find to be non-zero. Statistical significance tests are not an important element of the analysis, except as rough indicators of which relationships are strong enough to warrant further consideration. We therefore will use asterisks to indicate three conventional significance levels: * (p <.05), ** (p <.01) and *** (p <.001). For difference comparisons, these aster- isks indicate the significance of the z-ratio (two-tailed). For correlations, they indicate the significance of the difference of the correlation from zero, in either direction. All correlations are Pearson product-moment coefficients (r), unless otherwise indicated. Overall analyses

Taking the entire sample (N=1292) first, Tables 1-3 show various rela- tionships between child's VVand other variables. Since the sample size is large, some quite modest correlations are asterisked as statistically significant in these tables. Table 1 shows the raw correlations between child's VV and 12 varia- bles, each of which is related to the criterion variable to at least some extent. The first two correlated measures are the parent's estimate of his own viewing. Clearly the specific VV measures correlate more strongly with child's VV than does the general measure of the parent's television time. However, the parent's television time measure correlates .19*** with child's television time. Correlations between parent and child on similar measures have led other authors to conclude that a process of "modeling," wherein the child fashions his viewing behavior in line with "parental example," can explain why youngsters watch television as 163 156 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS they do (Himmelweit et al., 1958; Schramm et al., 1961). In Schramm's rather strong words, "example is the best persuader" and exercises "a very potent kind of influence" over a child's viewing (Schramm et al., 1961: 182). On the basis of several kinds of evidence, we have concluded elsewhere (Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971) that the case for the modeling hypothesis is fairly weak, at least at the adolescent age level.

Table 1: Correlates of child's violence viewing, total sample

Child's viewing index Correlated measure Westerns Spy-adventure VV sum

Parent's Vt/a .22*** .10"* .13*** Parent's TV time .04 .05 .06* .23*** .24* .29*** Child's TV time ....21** Child's I.Q. -.24*** -.10" SES -.15*** .04 Child's dogmatism .12*** .03 AO*" Concept-oriented FCP -.07** .02 -.04 Socio-oriented FCP .11** .11.* .13** Affection .06* .05 .07* Physical punishment .09** .05 Verbal punishment .06* .08" Restrictiveness .10* .12*** .14***

(N=1292) aCorresponding indices were correlated (parent-western with childwestern, etc.). < .05 ""p < .01 *p < .001

Table 1 also shows correlations between VV and several properties of the family or the child. Unsurprisingly, the child's TV time is,1 good predictor of his VV ; this can be thought of as a part-whole correlation. The fact that this is the largest correlation in Table 1 is trivial substan- tively. (Indeed, the fact that it is no larger than .30 is evidence that both measures are little more than minimally reliable.) The child's IQ is clearly negatively associated with VV , which repli- cates the Schramm et al. (1961) finding among adolescents. For dogma- tism and SES, the correlations with VV are restricted to western pro- grams; these variables are not significantly associated with spy-adven- ture viewing, a pattern that we find for many other predictors as well (see below). Generally, however, these variables, which are exogenous to the parent-child interaction, form a coherent pattern. The high VV child is more likely to be from a low SES family, to record low scores on IQ tests, and to endorse dogmatic statements on a "personality" inven- tory. These factors are theInselves interrelated, of course; dogmatism is associated with lower IQ (r = .26) and lower SES (r = .11). In our later analyses, we have controlled for IQ and SES, but have dropped 14 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 157 dogmatism from the partialing analysis because it is a minor factor and redundant to the more obvious measures of intelligence and status. (We continue to show the raw correlations between dogmatism and VV in various subsamples, however.) We retain SES in our partialing (below), even though it is weakly related to VV , because earlier studies found that SES interacts with parent-child interaction variables similar to those in this study (Maccoby, 1954; Schramm et al., 1961). Looking next in Table 1at family communication patterns, there is a tendency toward higher VV in socio-oriented homes; in the concept-ori- ented families the youngsters are less likely to view westerns. As indi- cated in our earlier discussion, we expect these two dimensions to inter- act somewhat; we will defer detailed comparisons among the four family types to a later point in this report. Finally, Table 1 shows positive correlations between VV and the four indices of affection, punishment, and restrictiveness. The positive asso- ciation with affection runs counter both to suggestions from earlier stud- ies with younger children (e.g., Maccoby, 1954), and perhaps to "com- mon sense." On the other hand, the general thrust of the literature would incline one to expect, if anything, stronger correlations between VV and the punishment and restrictiveness measures. This hypothesis has not been specifically stated, but it is certainly suggested by the em- phasis on primary group and family influences on children's television use (Him melweit et al., 1958; Schramm et al., 1961; Hess and Goldman, 1962; Maccoby, 1954; Riley and Riley, 1951, 1959; Tannenbaum and Greenberg, 1968; Ward and Wackm an, 1971). Moreover, the rather low raw correlations of these c.ur measures with VV could be due to re- sponse-set on the part of the child; inferences about the relationships should be deferred pending partialed analyses that would control for this factor (see below). Table 2 presents three multivariate analyses, with and without con- trols for child's television time. The first of these shows that parent- child VV correlations stand up rather well under partialing and are inde- pendent of the time spent with television by either parent or child. The small raw correlation between parent's television time and child's VV (see Table 1) is accounted for by the specific correlation between the two VV measures. A similar situation is found for family communication patterns in Ta- ble 2. The relationship between socio-orientation and a portion of the VV variance is not strengthened by adding the concept-orientation measure and using the multiple correlation. Finally. Table 2 shows that the positive raw correlation between VV and the two punishment indices vanishes when the affection and restric- tiveness measures are controlled. The latter two measures remain posi- tively associated with VV even when child's television time is partialed out. The direction of causality (if any) remains an open issue, of course. -165 158 TE.UVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 2: Multiple regression analyses of child's VV, total sample

Child's viewing index

Independent variable Westerns Spy-adventure VV sum abs.a rel.a abs. rel. abs. rel.

Parental TV use Parent's VV .2310 .22 .10' .09 .12.0 .11 .4.* Parent's TV time -.01 -.04 .02 -.01 .01 -.04 Multiple correlationb .23 .11 .1344

Family communication patterns

Concept-orientation -.08-.06" .00 .03 -.05 -.02 Socio-orientation .12*".08 .1 1 .07* .14*..10... Multiple correlation .140 .11. .14

Parental sanctions

Affection .07* .06* .06` .05 .08** .07* Physical punishment .05 .05 -.02 -.02 .03 .03 Verbal punishment .01 .02 .04 .04 .03 .04 Restrictiveness .06* .04 .10...08** .10.07" Multiple correlation .13" .14.4* .1800*

(N=1292) Note: Cell entries are partial correlations between the independent variable and the view- ing index, controlling for all other independent variables in the group. a Absolute viewing index (abs.) is the raw sum score. For the relative viewing index (rel.), child's TV time is controlled. bMultiple correlation represents the total relationship with the entire group of independent variables. Table 3 shows the relative predictive power of each of these inde- pendent variables for VV,when all the other variables are held con- stant. Only three independent variables statistically "survive" this strin- gent partialing: parent's VV,affection, and IQ. When child's television time remains uncontrolled, two other variables are of borderline signifi- cance: restrictiveness and the suppressing influence of concept-orient- ed FCP. Analyses by grade level

Tables 4-7 examine the junior high and senior high subsamples sepa- rately. Table 4 replicates the Schramm et al. (1961) finding of a decline in television time during adolescence, and shows that this trend includes both western and spy-adventure programs. An earlier study from this same body of data shows that this trenddoes not extend to news program viewing, which increases gradually through adolescence and into adult- hood (Chaffee, Ward, and Tipton, 1970). ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 159

Table 3: Grand multiple regression analysis of violence viewing, total sample

Child's violence viewing Independent variable Absolute Relative

Parent's VV .11.. Parent's TV time .00 .04 Child's TV time .24' Child's I.Q. .18' .16*" SES .01 .03 Concept-oriented FCP .07* .04 Socio-oriented FCP .05 .03 Affection .09.. .08" Physical punishment .02 .02 Verbal punishment .03 .04 Restrictiveness .06* .05 Multiple correlation .29"*

(N=1178) Note: Cell entries are partial correlations between the independent variable and child's VV, controlling for all other independent variables in the table. Total N is reduced because IQ data were not available for all students.

Table 4: Child's TV use, by grade level

Index Junior high Senior high

Child's TV time 5.20 3.93 12.6*" Child's western viewing 3.01 2.69 4.9*" Child's spy viewing 3.87 3.49 6.3*" Child's VV 6.88 6.18 6.9*"

(N) (641) (651)

* p < .001

Table 5 examines the parent-child "modeling" correlations for VV separately for each grade level. These correlations are stronger at the junior high level, particularly when various control variables are par- tialed out. It is comparatively impressive that the junior high raw corre- lation between parent and child VV is not reduced when variance due to IQ, SES, parent and child television time, family communication pat- terns, affection, punishment, and restrictiveness are all controlled. At the senior high level, these controls reduce the parent-child correlation to a nonsignificant figure. The importance of these latter factors at each grade level is examined in Tables 6 and 7. IQ, SES, and dogmatism are associated with VV to about the same extent regardless of grade. This is not true of the parent- child interaction variables, however. There appears to be a decline with age in the importance of family communication patternsand physical 167 160 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS'

Table 5: Parent-child viewing correlations, by grade level

Junior high Senior high Total sample I ndex uncon. cont.d uncon. cont. uncon. cont. TV time: Raw correlationa .17*" .20*" .19*** Western viewing: Raw correlation .24*** .23** .21*** .18"* .22***.201" Partial correlationb .16*** .18*** .10* .09* .13***.13*** Relative partial corr.c .15* * .16** :10* .09* .12***.13*** Spy-adventure viewing: Raw correlation .13*".15" .07 .04 .10.".09** Partial correlation .10** .10* .00 .00 .05 .05 Relative partial corr. .09* .08 .00 .00 .04 .03 Violence viewing (VV) Raw correlation .16.20** .10** .07 .13***.12*** Partial correlation .17*** .18" .06 .06 .11*".11*** Relative partial corr. .16**.16** .06 .06 .10**.10' (N) (641) (543) (651) (635) (1292)(1178) aRaw cbrrelation is the Pearson r between parent's viewing and child's viewing, on the index listed. bPartial correlation is the raw r, controlled for FCP, affectionpunishment indices, and parent's TV time. clielative partial correlation is controlled for child's TV time. dControlled ("cont.") correlations are controlled for SES, IQ, and parent's TV time. Ns are somewhat smaller due to missing IQ data.

Table 6: Correlates of child's Y.V. by grade level

Child's viewing index

Correlated measure Grade Westerns Spy-adv. VV (N)

Child's IQ jr. hi -.23* -.05 -.18*** (543) sr. hi -.23*" -.11 -.21 (635) SES jr. hi 1 -.18** .05 -.09* (641) sr. hi -.15 .01 -.09* (651) Child's dogmatism jr. hi .12** -.01 .07 (641) sr. hi .09* .02 .07 (651) Conceptorientation jr. hi -.13** .00 -.094 (641) sr. h i -.02 .02 .00 (651) Socio-orientation jr. hi .13** .11* .16" (641) sr. hi .05 .05 .06 (651) Affection jr. hi .01 -.02 -.01 (641) sr. hi .06 .06 .07 (651) Phys. punishment jr. hi .10** .03 .09* (641) sr. hi .03 .00 .02 (651) Verbal punishment jr. hi .03 .07 .06 (641) sr. hi .06 .05 .07 (651) Restrictiveness jr. hi .02 .07 .05 (641) sr. hi .11" .08 .11** (651) ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 161 punishment, and contrarily an increase for affection, verbal punishment, and restrictiveness. The data in Table 7, where each of these variablesis examined with the others held constant, are consistent with thoseinferences-to the extent that any relationships hold up underthis, extensive partialing. An interesting note, in passing, is that a negative partial correlationbetween parent's television time and child's VV emerges in Table7, at the junior high level. This finding is, of course, relative to many otherfactors in Table 7: it simply underscores the meaninglessness ofthe parent's total viewing, in contrast with his specific viewing of violent programs.The latter is the best predictor of child's VV in the juniorhigh subsample.

Table 7: Grand multiple regression analysis of violence viewing, by gradelevel

Child's violence vi-wing

Independent variable Ju nior high Senior high

abs.a rel.a abs. rel.

Parent's I/V .18*** .16*" .06 .06 Parenes TV time -.08 -.09* .05 .01 Child's TV time .14" Child's I 0 -.15*** -.14** -.19*** -.16' SES .00 .03 -.01 .00 Concept-oriented PCP -.08 -.08 -.03 -.01 Socio-oriented FCP .11* .10* -.01 -.01 , .04 .08* .07 Affection , .03 Physical punishment .07 .07 -.04 -.02 Verbal punishment .00 .01 .07 .06 Restrictiveness -.01 -.01 .06 .06 Multiple correlation .31*** .27*** (N) (543) (543) (635) (635)

Note: Cell entries are partial correlations between child's VV and the listed independent variable, controlling for all other variables in this table. aAbsolute viewing index (abs.) is the raw sum score. For the relative viewing index(rel.), child's TV time is controlled. Analyses by sex Tables 8 through 12 break the analysis into subsamples according to the respondent's sex. Table 8 shows that boys spend more timewith tel- evision, and watch more violent programs, than girls. The differences are stronger at the senior high level.The decline in VV during adoles- cence (see Table 4) is more marked amongthe girls in Table 8. In effect, girls develop more rapidly during adolescence, toward the lower adult levelS of VV. All these trends are similar to those reported by Schramm et al. (1961).

169. 162 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 8: Child's TV use, by sex and grade level

Index Grade Females Males

Child's TV time jr. hi 5.08 5.29 1.6 sr. hi 3.78 4.13 3.0" Child's western viewing jr. hi 2.91 3.10 2.0* sr. hi 2.57 2.83 2.7" Child's spy viewing jr. hi 3.76 3.96 2.5* sr. hi 3.35 3.66 1914 Child's VV jr. hi 6.67 7.06 3.3.* sr. hi 5.92 6.49 4.1*** (N) jr. hi (298) (343) sr. hi (364) (287)

Sex differences in relative violence viewingwere analyzed by special correlational techniques. The point biserial correlationbetween child's sex and VV is .11** for junior high and .15*** for senior high, using the means in Table 8 as base scores. When these figuresare controlled for child's television time, the partial point biserial correlationsare .10** (junior high) and .13** (senior high), respectively. Thismeans that the tendency for males to view violencemore, particularly at the senior high level, holds true for relative asw ell as absolute viewing. Parent-child sex comparisons are shown in Table 9. One salientfea- ture stands out: the child's viewing correlatesmore strongly with the mother' s than with the father's, regardless of the child'ssex. This is true at both grade levels, and for each viewing index, and holdsup consist- ently regardless of other controls introduced in the partialinganalyses. There is no VV correlation in the father-daughtersubsample. The tend- ency toward greater parent-child correlations for westerns than forspy- adventure programs holds up consistentlyacross all four subsamples in Table 9 and under all statistical controls. Table 10 shows the raw correlations within each sex-grade subsample, between VV and IQ, SES and dogmatism. There isa tendency toward more VV among low-SES boys, at both grade levels. The subsample partialing makes little difference withrespect to either IQ or dogmatism. Correlations of parent-child interaction variables with VVare shown separately for each sex-combination subsample in Table 11. Thepattern is quite clear: each of the six variables predicts VVmost strongly within the mother-son subsample. In fact, aside from restrictiveness, themoth- er-son data provide the only significant relationships in Table 11. It isas if all forms of maternal controlover the son increase the likelihood of VV to some extent: affection, punishment, restrictiveness,and socio- oriented corn m,unication. The same-sex (i.e., father-son, mother-daughter) dataare quite weak overall, and the father-daughter relationshipappears to mean nothing aside from a positive correlation between VVand restrictiveness.

170 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 163

Table 9: Parent-child viewing correlations, by sex of parentand child

Fathers Mothers Index Sons Daughters Sons Daughters

TV time: Raw correlation .14* .15" .23" .22'

Western viewing: .25*** Raw correlation .19** .19" .29' Parti.il correlation .18" .19" .27' .24"* Relative partial r .18" .17" .26' .23' Spy-adventure viewing: .13* Raw correlation .11 .02 .13* .12* Partial correlation .09 .02 .12* Relative partial r .08 -.01 .10 .11* Violence viewing (VV): Raw correlation .13* .02 .19' .19' Partial correlation .1 1 .02 .15" .16" Relative partial r .10 -.02 .12* .15" (N) (297) (301) (333) (361)

Note: For explanation of the various partialingtechniques in this table, see notes to Table 5. There are no controls for IQ or SES in thistable.

Table 10: Correlates of child's VV, by child's sex and gradelevel

Sons Daughters Correlated measure jr. hi sr. hi jr. hi sr'. hi

Child's IQ -.1 1 -.25"* -.25' -.20" (35Z) (N) (291) (282) (252) -.05 SES -.13* -.19" -.08 Child's dogmatism .04 .12 .10 .02 (N) (343) (287) (298) (364)

In Table 12 these variables areexamined simultaneously, within each subsample. With the other independentvariables controlled, VV ap- pears to be a functionof the following: Mother's VV. Mother's affection. Father's restrictiveness toward daughter. Mother-son family communication pattern,both as a positive func- tion of socio-orientation and as a negativefunction of concept-orienta- tion. The father-child variables predictVV less well as a group than do the mother-child variables, to judge fromthe multiple correlations in Table 12. The weakest data are in thefather-son column, where none of the interaction variables is significantly related tochild's VV. This is some- what a surprising nonfinding onits face, and it contrasts with Clarke's 171

.wwwisemmagmws..vggioweSOMewilh 41414101.111. 11. 164 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 11: Correlates of child's VV, by sex of parent and child

Fathers Mothers Correlated measure Sons Daughters Sons Daughters

Concept-oriented FCP -.02 .0:? -.10 ' -.06 Sociokariented FCP .10 .04 .30** .08 Af fection .09 .05 . .11* 09 Physical punishment .08 -.03 .11* 18 Verbal punishment .10 -.03 .17* .08 Restrictiveness .08 .12* .18** :10* (N) (297) (301) (333) 13631

(1969) finding that the father-son relationship isa sensitive factor in ac- counting for teenage magazine reading. Other analyses (not shown)indi- cate that child's VV ic asstociated with mother's punishment'and reStric- tiveness, in the junior high sample, only when coupled witha high de- gree of affection.

Table 12: Grand multiple regression analysis of child's VV, by sex of paient and child Fathers Mothers

Independent variable partial r Sons Daughters Sons Daughtbrs Parent's VV abs. .11 .02 .15** .16" rel. .10 -.02 .12* C' did's TV time rel. .38** .27*** .18***

Concept-oriented FCP abs. -.07 -.01 ' -.08 rel. '-.04 .05 -.05 Sotio-oriented FCP abs. .05 .02 .23*" .03 rel. .03 -.04 .21*** .00 Affection abs. .10 .04 .14* .13* rel. .07 -.04 .13* .13* Physical punishment abs. .04 -.08 -.06 .02 rel. .04 -.07 -.05 .02

Verbal punishment abs. .05 -.07 .05 . .04 rel. .05 -.06 .05 .06 Restrictiveness abs. .00 .15*. .08 .06 rel. .01 .12* .07 aDS

I 39*** Multiple r . abs. .20**.17" .25*** (297) (301) (333) 13631 Note: For explanations of the various entries in this table, see notes to Table 7.

Our data also shim that The mothers spendmore time' watching televi- sion daily than the fathers, but that the fathers report higher VVlevels, The absence of significant father-child "modeling" correlationsthus cannot be ascribed to a low level of VV by the father;nor can the signifi- 172 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 165 cant mother-child "modeling" data beaccounted for by her high general viewingsince parent's television iime is uncorrelated with child'sVV. All of this suggests that it is at least plausible tohypothesize that the youngsters influence their mothers to watchviolent programs, rather than vice versa. We have discussed this "reverse modeling"hypothesis elsewhere (Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971), andit is supported by studies showing that parents more frequently ask their teenagersfor tel- evision program advice than vice versa (Clarke, 1963; Bottorff,1970). Analyses by fam i ly co m m unication patterns For our last set of detailed analyses, we turn to- thefourfold break- down by family communication types, in Tables 13-15. Table13 shows differences among the:four FCP types in terms of standard scores.For parents,these scores.are standardized on the overall mean across the four types. For the youngsters, the scores are standardized acrosseight categoriestwo grade levels for each of four FCP types. Thus thechild data show developmental differences within, as well asdifferences be- tween,,the four family types. Looking first in Table 13 at the television time measures, the highest scores within each line, for both parentand child, are lound in tile pro- tective families, and the iowest in the pluralistic homes. For VVthe pro- tectives are again highest, but the laissez-faire scores aresomewhat lower than the pluralistics.

Table 13: Parent's and TV use (standard scores), by family commynication pattern

Index Laissez-faire Pluralistic Protective Consensual Parent's TV use

Parent's TV time 09 16* +22** +06 Parent's VV 14* 04 +16* +05 (N) (345) (317) (277) (353) Child'sTV use

Child's TV time jr. hi +34*** 02 +71*** +36*** 15 . sr. hi 27*** --51*** Chilcrs VV jr. hi +09 +09 +36*** +24** sr. hi _29*** I-25** 02 15 (N) jr. hi (160) (138) (146) (197) sr. hi (185) (179) (131) (156) Note: Standard scores represent each cell mean, calculated as a deviation from the over- all Mean on the listed index, divided bv the overall standard deviation. Scores havebeen multiplied by 100 to eliminate decimnls..A negative score indicates the cell mean isbelotto the overall mean for the index. Asterisks indicate the cell mean issignificantly different from the overall mean for remaining cells. 173

...la Id.711110 4.11. IF 166 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

In terms of junior-to-senior high net change, the pluralisticyoungsters show the least shift during adolescence in the reduction of their totaltel- evision time, but this k mainly because they donot watch TV a great deal even in junior high school. Conversely, the protectives show the greatest net change, but mostly because in junior high they spendso much more time with television than the othergroups. (If more age lev- els were included in the sample, they might well show that the adoles- cent trend toward less television occurs first for pluralistics and latest among protectives.) For V V,however, the four types of children are about equal in the degree to which they reduce this behavior duringado- lescence. (These inferences would be much more solid if theywere sup- ported by longitudinal data from the same cohort, rather than sem;-om- parable samples from different cohorts as is thecase here.)

Table 14: Parent-child viewing correlations, by family communicationpattern

Index Grade Laissezfaire Pluralistic Protective Consensual TV time jr. hi .13 .11 .01 .27' sr. hi .13 .22** .29' IP Western viewing jr. hi .26" .11 .23" .28" sr. hi .27** .23** .13 .18*

Spy viewing jr. hi .14 .09 .11 sr. hi .07 .06 .06 .04 Violence jr. hi .20* .03 .15* .25* viewing (Vt/i sr. hi .11 .11 .07 .08

(N) jr. hi (160) (138) (146) (197) sr. hi (185) (179) (131) (156)

Table 14 presents the parent-child "modeling" correlations for each FCP type. They tend to be stronger at the junior high level for VV andat the senior high level for television time. The most consistent evidence for the modeling hypothesis is found in the consensual families, forthe junior high subsample; this finding is consistent with the generalpropo- sition that within-family agreementon values is most likely to be found in the consensual home. The pluralistic homesappear to provide the least parent-child similarity in violence viewing, at least at the junior high level; similar findings have been reported elsewhere for television news and for news reading (Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). Parental control variables associated with violence viewing, within FCP types, are examined in Table 15. Generally speaking, thereare few significant correlations, and they followno obvious pattern. This is per- haps to be expected, since earlier (Table 7) we found that these indices show little relationship with VV when grade level and FCP orientations are controlled.

117& ...... *InNWV*W.VReVAPV"?

ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 167

Table 15: Correlates of child's VV, by family communication pattern

Correlated measure Grade Laissezfaire Pluralistic Protective Consensual Affection jr. hi .08 .10 .08 sr. hi .01 .04 .20' .11

Physical ir. hi .01 .10 .08 .07 punishment sr. hi .02 .02 .03 .03 Verbal jr. hi .11 .19* .08 .09 .05 Punishment Sr. hi .07 .04 .03 .02 Restrictiveness jr. hi .02 .06 .01 sr. hi .19" .07 .06 .03

(197) (N) jr. hi (160) (138) (146) sr. hi (185) (179) (131) (156)

CONCLUSIONS With the full range of evidence before us, we can nowconsider the possible roles played by various elements of parent-childinteraction in fostering VV by adolescents. Our inferences can be madewithout being challenged on the basis of the following variables, whichhave been con- trolled in our analyses: a.IQ, which is strongly, and negatively, related toVV viewing. b. Socioeconomic status, which has a mildnegative relationship with VV that vanishes when other variables arecontrolled. c.Age, which in adolescence is negatively related toVV. d. Sex, which is related to VV in that boys watch programsof this type more than girls do. e.Child's television time, which has apart-whole relationship with VV. We will consider three classes of independentvariables: parent's tel- evision use, parent-child communication patterns,and parental controls and sanctions regarding the child's behavior. Parent's television use. The amount of time spentwatching television by the parent is unrelated to the adolescent'sviolence viewing. Specific viewing of westerns by the parent is associatedwith similar viewing by the child; for spy-adventure programs thereis a similar, though weaker, correlation. The correlations are stronger inearly adolescence and where the parent involved is the mother. Is this evidence that adolescents "model"their viewing of violent programs after "parentalexample"? Undoubtedly there are somein- stances in which parent viewingof violence encourages an adolescent to watch the program too. But a generalhypothesis of child-to-parent modeling is scarcely necessary to account forthe correlations we find. 175 -- 168 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

To begin with, it should be noted that "parental example"can be either positive or negative. It seems more likely thatparents, who watch vio- lent programs far less than their adolescent children, wouldset a "nega- tive example" by not watching. (Further, bynot watching, the parents are more likely to involve the family in alternative activities.) Thisnega- tive-effect inference is strengthened by the finding thatthe mother's viewing correlates more strongly with the child's than does thefather's. If "modeling" were truly operating withany frequency, we should ex- pect a teenage son to emulate his father. Moreover, the father tendsto watch violent programs more often than the mother, althoughhe spends less total time with television; if "modeling"were occurring, he would seem the more likely model, insofar as violence viewing is concerned. Perhaps an even more plausible hypothesis,as we have mentioned earlier, is that the correlations are caused by "reverse modeling,"from child to parent. Adolescents are, after all, the "TV experts"in the home, and we have cited evidence that parents often ask them forview- ing advice (see also Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). Another explanation for the parent-child correlations couldbe that two people living in the same house will be exposed to thesame televi- sion shows to some extent, simply because it is noteasy for one resident to avoid seeing a show that another has tuned in. Elsewherewe have rejected this "opportunity" hypothesis at least tentatively,because we were unable to find any difference between one-set and multiple-set homes in the degree of parent-child viewing correlation (Chaffee,Mc- Leod, and Atkin, 1970); we reasoned that forcedexposure to another's program selection would be more likely in single-set homes. Finally, of course, there remains the possibility thatexogenous fac- tors that we have not incorporated into this study might independently "cause" members of the same family touse television in similar ways. We have controlled for some factorson which parent and child will be similar (IQ, television time, family communication pattern)or identical (SES). These controls have not eliminated the parent-childcorrelations, but it is conceivable' that other variables might. During adolescence the youngster's televisionuse, including his view- inp. of violent programs, gradually decreases, approachingan adult level. But the similarity of his viewing to that of his parents decreases in this maturing period. It is quite unlikely, then, that "modeling" playsa ma- jor role in this phase of the developing person's life. Earlierauthors, who were perhaps more concerned withyounger children, appear to have relied too much on raw correlations without consideringother hypotheses and external variables. Family communication patterns.One factor that appears to account for a portion of the parent-child similarity in violence viewing is the ha- bitual structure of family communication. The socio-orientation inpar- ticular accounts for large differences among the youngsters, and these 176 ADOLESCENT TELEVISION USE 169 differences rem ain fairly constant duringadolescence. Similar, though less pronounced, patterns ofdifference are found among the parents in these families. Violence viewing isespecially high in the "protective" home, where the parents stress thesocio-orientation but not the con- cept-orientation in their child rearing cornmunication. These findings are roughly consistentwith earlier studies, in which we found that the use of mass media for publicaffairs content is mainly as- sociated with the concept-orientation, butentertainment media use fol- lows the socio-orientation (Chaffee,McLeod, and Wackman, 1966; McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman, 1967;Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). We do not find here, however, that partialing outthe family communi- cation typology eliminates the parent-childcorrelations; nor does it help us much in specifying theconditions under which these correlations will be stronger. An earlier report showed greaterparent-child television use correlations in socio-oriented homes (Chaffee,McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). This finding does not appear to holdfor violence viewing specifi- cally. Parental sanctions.The most disappointing set of predictorvariables in this study are the indices of parentalaffection, physical and verbal punishment, and restrictiveness. Neither punishment measurecorre- lates with violence viewing at all. The"influence" of restrictiveness appears to be restricted to thefather-daughter relationship and the sen- ior high level. Only parental affection (asperceived by the child) is asso- ciated with violence viewing, and this findingholds mainly for the moth- er and senior highchildren. Our measures are scarcely optimal, of course,and the question of a relationship between punishment and violenceviewing should remain open pending more elaborate attemptsto investigate it. Physicalpunish- ment is, after all, rare foradolescents, and verbal punishment may be something of a "constant" that is common to allparent-adolescent rela- tionships. With a younger sample, or morereliable measures, the ex- pectable correlations might yet be found. The association we find between maternalaffection and violence viewing by the child is neither intuitivelysatisfying nor consistent with what little "literature" exists on the topic.Because this correlation has withstood som e rather stringent statisticalpartialing tests here, howev- er, it is difficult to ignore.Maccoby (1954) found that upper-middle-class children who are not treated warmly at home spent moretime watching television. Schramm et al. (1961) concludedthat the Maccoby inference was basically sound, afterexamination of data based on a measure of parent-child conflict over aspirations for the child.The measures, time frames, ages, and locales of those studies are notnecessarily compara- ble to ours, but in a loose conceptualfashion there is some discrepancy between the conclusions invited by our dataand theirs. 177 170 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

The positive correlation between restrictiveness and violence viewing is clearly more in line with earlier inferences. It isnot decreased appreci- ably by controlling for the child's television time,so it cannot be attrib- uted to the fact that a "grounded"or restricted adolescent has more opportunity to stay home and watch television. On the otherhand, it is not a strong correlation, all things considered. As withour other paren- tal sanction variables, conclusive inferences (nullor otherwise) should be withheld pending much fuller investigation than hasbeen possible here. Violence viewing in the family context. Ifthere is one general conclu- sion to be reached from this study, it would be that familycontext varia- bles do not make as much difference in adolescent violenceviewing as earlier writers have suggested. Watching television (violentand other- wise) appears to be a "cultural universal" in earlyadolescence, and the period in which the developing child withdraws from heavytelevision use is also the period in which he becomes progressively less influenced by his parents. Parental viewing preferencesare probably a minor, mostly negative, factor. Sex role differences account forsome diver- gence among the adolescents, but do not account for similarities be- tween them and their parents. Parental controls on the child make little difference, and neither does the family's socioeconomicstatus. There is evidence that long-range patterns of family communicationhelp to shape the use the adolescent will make of television (andother media), but it is likely that these patternsare firmly established by early adoles- cence. All of this begs the pressing policy question of effects oftelevision violence on the youngster. Does the adolescentwho watches violent programming in an affectionateor concept-oriented home environment react differently to it than, say, another youngster ina punitive or socio- oriented home? Is a boy's possible "modeling" of violent televisionac- tion in any way related to the viewingor other behavior of his parents? We cannot deal with such questions with the limited data here.Hopeful- ly, however, this study can help future researchers selectimportant var- iables and fruitful hypotheses formore detailed study of television vio- lence and the family environment.

FOOTNOTES I.This technical report describes research pursuant to Contract No. HSM 42-70-30 with the National Institute of Mental Health, Health Services and Mental Health Administration, U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. Charles K. Atkin (nowat Michi- gan State University) was the primary project assistant. Others aid- ing in the data analysis included George Pasdirtz, William Elliott, Garrett O'Keefe Jr., and Selwyn Edwards. 178 TMeM1,1,1,

ADOLESCENTTELEVISION USE 171 REFERENCES

Bottorff, A. Television, respect, and the olderadolescent. Unpublished master's thesis, University of Wisconsin, 1970. Chaffee, S.H., and McLeod, J.M. Coorientationand the structure of family communication. Paper presented at themeeting of the Inter- national Communication Association , Minneapolis,May, 1970. Chaffee, S.H., McLeod, J.M., and Atkin, C.K.Parent-adolescent simi- larities in television use. Paper presented atthe meeting of the Asso- ciation for Education in Journalism,Washington, D.C., August, 1970. Chaffee, S.H., McLeod, J.M., and Atkin, C.K.Parental influences on adolescent media use. American BehavioralScientist, 1971, 14, 323- 40. Chaffee, S.H., McLeod, J.M., and Wackman, D.B.Family communica- tion and political socialization. Paper presented atthe meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism, IowaCity, Ia., August, 1966. Chaffee,. S.H., Ward, L.S., and Tipton, L.P.Mass communication and political socialization. Journalism Quarterly, 1970,47,647-59. Clarke, P. An experiment to increase theaudience for educational tele- vision. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,University of Minnesota, 1963. Clarke, P. Identification with father and father-sonsimilarities in read- ing behavior. Paper presented at the meetingof the Association for Education in Journalism, Berkeley, Calif., August,1969. Hess, R., and Goldman, H. Parents' views of theeffects of TV on their children. Child Development, 1962, 33, 411-26. Himmelweit, H., Oppenheim, A., and Vince, P.Television and the child. London: Oxford University Pre ss, 1958. McLeod, J.M., and Chaffee, S.H. The constructionof social reality. In J. Tedeschi (Ed.) The social influence processes.Chicago: Aldine, in press. McLeod, J.M., Chaffee, S.H., and Eswara, H.Family communication patterns and communication research.Paper presented at the meet- ing of the Association for Education inJournalism, Iowa City, Ia., August, 1966. McLeod, J.M., Chaffee, S.H., and Wackman, D.B.Family communica- tion: An updated report. Paper presented at themeeting of the As- sociation for Education in Journalism, Boulder,Colo., August, 1967. McLeod, J.M., Rush, R.R., and Friederich, K.H.The mass media and political information in Quito, Ecuador. PublicOpinion Quarterly, 1968-69, 32, 575-87.

. >179 172 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Riley, J.W., and Riley, M.W. Mass communication and the social sys- tem. In R.K. Merton, L. Broom, L.S. Cottrell (Eds.) Sociology Today. New York: Basic Books, 1959. Riley, M.W., and Riley, J.W. A sociological approach to communica- tions research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1951, 15, 445-60. Rokeach, M. The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books, 1960. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B. Television in the lives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961. Stone, V.A., and Chaffee, S.H. Family communication 'patterns and source-message orientation. Journalism Quarterly, 1970,47,239-46. Tannenbaum, P.H., and Greenberg, B.S. Mass communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 1968, 19, 351-86. Ward, S., and Wackman, D. Family and media influenceson adolescent consumer learning. American Behavioral Scientist, 1971,14,415-27.

180 -.001TIS.

Adolescents, Parents, and Television Use: Adolescent Self-report Measures from Maryland and Wisconsin Samples

Jack M. McLeod, Charles K. Atkin, and Steven H. Chaffee University of Wisconsin

This is a report of two nonexperimental studies of the relationships among three sets of variables: adolescent aggression, television viewing behavior, and structural attributes of family social environment.1 The first study examines questionnaire data obtained from 473 adolescents in Prince Georges County, Maryland. Data for the second study were gath- ered from 151 adolescents in Midd!eton, Wisconsin. This report con- tains data comparable between the two samples: a final report will in- clude additional data available only from the Wisconsin sample. Various self-report measures of aggression are treated as the ultimate dependent variable, although specification of the direction of causality is

173

181 174 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS precluded by the lack of experimental control and panel design. The aggression measures are related to indices of self-reported frequency of viewing specific television programs and to cognitive reactions to violent television content. The aggression and viewing variablesare studied in associationwithvariousaspectsofthefamilysocialenviron- ment: parental punishment, affection. control over television viewing, nonaggression training, and social status. Our analysis begins with an examination of each of the major sets of variables taken singly. We then take pairs of variable sets in their bivar- iate relationships, and conclude by investigating various multivariate com binations.

PROCEDURES AND MEASURES Sampling

During April of 1970, questionnaires were completed by 229 seventh graders and 244 tenth graders in eight public schools in Prince Georges County. Maryland. This county, contiguous with the eastern half of Washington. D.C., was included to provide geographical and socioeco- nomic balance to our ongoing Wisconsin research. The sample selection, supervised by Dr. Jennie McIntyre of the University of Maryland, was coordinated with other studies conducted for the Television and Social Behavior program. The Wisconsin data were gathered in Middleton, a community of ap- proximately 7,000 that serves both as a bedroom suburb of Madison and as a trading center for the surrounding area. In October of 1969, 225 ado- lescents, comprising the entire school population in two grades,com- pleted questionnaires in the city's two schools. During the same month, personal interviews were conducted with the mothers of these children. One year later, interviewers returned to the homes to reinterview 151 of the mothers and to administer questionnaires to 68 of the then seventh graders and to 83 adolescents now in the tenth grade. Move-outs ac- counted for the largest portion of the attrition from the original popula- tion. Neither sample should be thought of as "typical" of the larger U.S. populations because opportunity was involved in the selection of both research sites. The Maryland sample was closer to the national average in some respectsas, for example, in its having 15 percent black stu- dents, contrasted with the all-white Wisconsin group. In other ways, however, both samples are atypical in being somewhat higher than aver- age on various measures of socioeconomic status. For example, the proportion of mothers attending college was 33 percent for Maryland and 37 percent for Wisconsin. On all such measures, the Wisconsin fam- ilies are slightly higher in social status. While the effect of bias in each ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 175 sample cannot be known, it is likely that these effects would alter the absolute level on at least some of the variables. For that reason,stand- ard scores rather than means are shown in our tables. We will also con- centrate on relationships between variables in our analysis. Thegeneral- ility of these relationships can be known with certainty only through replication in other settings. In keeping with our otherresearchon adolescents, throughout this report we refer to "junior high" and "senior high" subsamples, al- though those labels do not necessarily coincide with the precise titles of schools. The Maryland junior high students were in seventh grade and those in senior high were in tenth grade. The Wisconsin groups were a grade lower in each case when the first questionnaire was administered and at those grade levels when they were sludied for the second time. The two grade levels are only roughly comparable, because the junior and senior high boundaries are not always coterminous and because Roman Catholic families are more likely to send their children to public senior high schools than to public junior highs. Measures: adolescent aggression

This section describes the various indices of self-reported adolescent aggression. The sections following describe our measures of television viewing behavior, structural attributes of the family social environment, and cognitive reactions to violent television programming. The descrip- tion of each index will include a limited number of examples of items; a full listing for each index is included in Appendix A. Manifest physical aggression.The adolescents were asked to judge a battery of 17 items and to indicate whether each was "not like me,""a little like me," or "a lot like me." This "like me" scale is takenfrom Greenberg and Dominick (1968). Item analysis and factor analysisindi- cated a grouping of six of these items having a common emphasis onthe display of physical aggression. Three of the items are adapted from the assault aggression subscale of the Buss-Durkee (1957)aggression-hos- tility inventory, although these authors use a different underlying re- sponse scale. The items: a. Whoever insults me or myfamily is asking for a fight. b. If somebody hits me first, I let him have it. c. When I lose my temper at someone, oncein a while I actually hit them. Three other items were devised for this research: d. When I am mad at someone, I sometimes fight with them instead of talking about the problem. e. When I was younger, I used to actlike a bully sometimes. f.I don't feel it is wrong for me to hit other kids who deserve it. 183 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Aggressive behavioral delinquency.Three items that involve overt aggressive acts were taken from the 23-item delinquencyscale of Short and Nye (1957-58,1958; Nye and Short, 1957). Thestudent was asked to indicate the frequency with which he, hadbeen involved in each on a five-step scale ranging from "never"to "more than five times." Be- cause of time limitations, these items were among the questions elimi- nated from the questionnaire given Wisconsin junior highstudents. The items: a. Been in fights with several people on each side. b. Hurt someone on purpose to get back for somethingthey had done to you. c. Got into a serious fight with another student at school. Zaks-Walters aggression. Seven itemswere chosen from the Zaks- Walters 12-item inventory of generalized aggression.Although the mani- fest content of their items less clearly dealswith overt aggressiveness, Walters and Zaks (1959) report predictive validityin discriminating be- tween assaultive and nonassaultive prisonersto be greater than other more obvious self-report measures. They show similar validation results for other criterion groups. While the originalscale involves a simple agree-disagree dichotomy, for certain itemswe used the three-choice "a lot like me" category system described abovefor manifest physical ag- gression. For most items,we used a five-step scale ranging from "agree strongly" to "disagree strongly" through themiddle "no opinion." A sample of items: a.I often do things which I regret after. ("a lot like me"scale) b.I am very patient with people. ("a lot likeme" scale, reversed scoring) c. There are two kinds of people in th:s world: the weak and the strong. (agreement scale) The combination of two underlying scaleswas justified by a satisfac- tory.level of internal consistency and discriminant validity. Hypothetical aggressive reactions. Adolescentswere presented with four hypothetical conflict situations and askedto choose among three or four alternatives the thing theywere most likely to do. The chosen re- sponses were coded according to their degree of aggressiveness and summed across the four items. Two sample items: a. What if someone cut in front of you in a long line. What wouldyou do to them? (Shove them out, yellat them, just let it go) b. Suppose someone playeda real dirty trick on you. What would you do? (Hit them, yell at them, ignore them, laughat them) In the above examples, shoving and hittingwere coded as highest in aggression, with yelling asan intermediate aggressive response. Letting it go, ignoring, and laughing at themwere scored as equally low. Overall aggression sum. By following the assumptionthat self-report measurement of adolescent aggression is best approached bysurround- ing the concept with a variety of items and scales,we combined the four 184 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 177 indices described above into a single "best" measure of overall aggres- sion. The combination of the 20 items makes a potential rangeof 64 scale positions for this "best estimate". For convenience, wewill refer to this sumas OASthroughout this report. Buss-DurkeP assault aggression.This measure is comprised of the three Buss-Durkee items 'among the six described abovefor manifest physical aggression plus the following: a.I can't think of any good reason for hitting anyone. (reversed scor- ing) AU items use the "a lot like me" three-category systemfor measure- ment. Buss-Durkee irritability.More covert internal responses were meas- ured by grouping three Buss-Durkee itemswith the "a lot like me" underlying scale: a.I lose my temper easily. b. It really makes me mad when somebody makes fun of me. c.If someone doesn't treat me right, I don't let it bother me (reversed scoring). Approval of aggression.Two items, rated on a five-step agree strong- ly-disagree strongly scale, comprised the approval ofaggression meas- ure. Neither item refers tothe adolescents' overt aggression, but rather they tap the sanctioning of aggression as a means of solvingconflict. The measures: a.It's all right to hurt an enemy if you are mad at him. b. In order to get revenge, it's all right to hurt an enemy. Both are judged on a five-point agree-disagree'scale.

Measures: adolescent television viewing levels Adolescents were given a list of 65 prime time programs organized by night of the week shown. Each show was checked according to frequen- cy of viewing:almost always(nearly every week);often (atleast half the time);sometimes (atleast once or twice); andnever.The shows were then grouped according to theirmanifest content into six catego- ries. Frequency ratings for each adolescent were then summed across all shows in each category. Program categories.The six program categories, the most popular shows in each, and the number of shows in the category were: a. Crime-detective:Mod Squad, Adam 12, Mannix (11shows). b. Westerns:Here Come the Brides, Bonanza, Daniel Boone (7 shows). c.Adventure-drama: Hogan's Heroes, Land of the Giants, Marcus Welby(6 shows). d. Comedy-variety:Laugh-in, Tom Jones, Glen Campbell (14 shows). 185 178 TELEVISION AND ADO ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

e. Situation comedy: Bill Cosby. Room 222.Eddie's Father (25 shows). f. Game shows: Let's Makea Deal. Newlywed Game (2 shows). Overall violence viewing. Although the first threeprogram types are associated with violent content and the last three withlesser levels. there is considerable variation within categories. Asa result. it was de- sirable to index the level of violent content for each showindividually to produce the best indicator of total exposureoverall violenceviewing. or OVV as we shall call it for the remainder of the report. This index was constructed fr9m the ratings of theamount of violent content in individual television programs. obtained from three different samples: Minneapolis high school students (Murray. Cole.and Fedler. 1970): a probability sample of the Detroit adult public(Greenberg and Gordon. 1970). and a selected sample of television critics(Greenberg and Gordon. 1970). Each judge rated eachprogram along a five-step scale ranging from 1 (least violent) to 5 (most violent).For each set of judges. the programs were rank ordered into twelve groupings.using natural breaks in the distribution of mean ratings. These rankswere then summed across the three sets of judges to providean overall rank order of programs. An overall mean ratingacross all judges was also comput- ed for each program, yielding thesame ordering of programs. Actually, there was a very high level of agreementamong the three sets of judges across the 65 programs (see Appendix B). Natural bre3ks in the mean/rank order datawere used to cut the pro- grams into eight levels of violence, with weighting ranging from 0 (low violence) to 7 (high violence). Amount of viewing for eachprogram for each adolescent was given a weight ranging from0 (never watch) to 4 (almost always watch). Thus, the OVV for each individualis the sum of the products of the violence andexposure weights across 65 programs. News-public affairs programs. The frequency of viewingof four types of showsnational news, localnews, current event shows, and inter- iew showswere summed to geta news-public affairs index. Response categories for these shows were "never." "sometimes."and "often." Saturday morning programs. Adolescentswere asked how many hours they spent watching television beforenoon on an average Satur- day. They filled in estimates of hours and minutes. Total viewing time. Estimates of the adolescents'total television viewing were obtained by combinirt theresponses to three questions regarding time spent viewing yesterday, the day beforeyesterday, and on an average day after 5 p.m. It is particularly important to devisea good index of viewing time because it does doubleduty as a control vari- able and as an independent variable. Itserves to control our exposure in analyzing inferences about the associations of specificprogram-type viewing and the aggression measures. It will also beconsidered an inde- pendent variable because much of the previousliterature on effects of 186 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 179 television has used simple exposure time ratherthan specific nrogram- m ing. Violent television movies.Five movies shown on television during the weeks preceding our data collection wereused to determine the viewing of violent movies. The students wereasked to indicate whether or not they had seen each of the five moviesthat varied in level of violent con- tent according to analysis byjudges. Their exposure to the more violent films relative to the less violent ones constitutedthe index.

Measures: family environment A wide variety of family environment indicators wereused, ranging from specific parental regulation of televisionviewing to more general treatment variables of affection andpunishment. Also included were structural aspects of the environment, number ofsiblings, and social status. Parental control over television.The extent to which the parents con- trol the extent of adolescent television viewing wasmeasured by sum- ming responses to six items with a variety of underlyingscales. Included were: a. Who has the most to sayabout what you watch on television? (self or sibling, either self or parent,parent) b. Do your parents always know what programs you arewatching on TV? (no, yes) c. Are there certain programs that your parentssometimes do not let you watch? (no. yes) The "parent" and the two "yes" responses are scored ashigh con- trol. Additional points were added if the parentspecifically prohibited either "crime shows." "westerns," or "violent shows." Parental emphasis on nonaggression.Teaching of nonviolence was indexed by four items: a. Do your parents punish you if you are meanto other kids? (no, I'm never mean, yes) b. Do your parents want you to fight back if other kidspick on you? (yes, no) c. How important does your motherthink it is for you to learn to de- fend yourself? (very important, somewhat important notimpor- tant) d. How often did your parents say you shouldn't dothe bad things people do on TV? (never, sometimes, often) In each case, the last response indicated is scored as nonaggressive. Parental interpretation of television violence.Parental attempts to immunize their children against television violence were indexed by ask- ing the adolescent how frequently (never, sometimes, often)their par-

. 187 180 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS ents said each of five things to them, interpreting violent television con- tent. Some of these are: a. Told you that things are not like this in real life. h. Said that these stories are "just pretend." c.Explained that there are better ways than violence to solve prob- lems. Parental punishment: physical. verbal, and restrictive. Adolescents were given a list of parental behaviors an asked to indicate whether their parents did each "very often." "fairly often." "not too often."or "never. The single item indicating physical punishmentwas, "punish you physically when you were younger." Verbal punishment was indexed by two items: a) Punishyol! by yell- ing at you: b) Punish you by lecturing you. Restrictive punishment was measured by: a) Punishyou by "ground- ing" you: 1)) Punish you by taking awayyour privileges. The intercorrelation of items justified the separation of parentalpun- ishment into these three components. Parental affection. On the same frequency scale used for the punish- ment items Parental Affection was indexed by the single item: "show that the y love you." Staxtural aspects. The number of siblings and twomeasures of social status. father's occupation and mother's education, provided three structural indicators. Duncan's socioeconomic status scalewas used to classif y the occupation of the father. In addition. school performance will be usedas a control variable in the later sections of this report. Measures: cognitive reactionsto television violence

Cognitive reactions of adolescents to violent televisioncontent may he thought of as qualitative dimensions parallelto the quantity of con- tent to which they are exposed. Five types of responses are examined. Perceived learning of aggression. It is reasonableto expect that any association between violent television viewing and aggressivereactions would be higher among adolescents who feel that televisionpresents opportunities for learning of antisocial behavior. A five-item indexwas constructed to tap this dimension using the three-step "a lot like me" scale. Some items: a. These program% show me how to get back at people who make me angry. h. Sometimes I copy the things I see people doingon these shows. c. Some programs give me ideas on how to get away with something 1 without getting caught.

185 ADOLESCENTS. PAR ENTS. TELEVISION USE 181 Linkage of violent television to real life. Perception of a close similari- ty between the world portrayed on violent television programs and every- day reality should also tend to facilitate the relation between the extent of violent viewing and aggression. Following the work of Greenberg and Dominick (1968) and Berkowitz (1962). four items were developed using the "a lot like me" scale. The items: a. Action and adventure shows tell aboutlife the way it really is. b. The people I see in adventure stories are just like the people I meet in real life. c. Some stories remind me of frustrating thingsthat have happened to me. d. Some ,:haracters remind me of people who have made me mad. Involvement in violent programming. To measure the degree of psy- chological involvement with the characters and stories of action-adven- ture shows, a six-item index was developed. Once more,the "a lot. like me" scale was used. An examination of item intercorrelations provided justification for separating involvement and the two preceding cognitive indices. Some examples of involvement items: a.I am so involved in some programs that I get carried away with the story. b.I get upset when my favorite star is yelled at or threatened. c.I sometimes forget that characters in these shows are just actors. Identification with violent characters. Adriescents were asked to name the one person on television they would mostlike to be. They were also given a list of six male actors and asked topick the one they most like to see at the movies. The characters chosen in each case were rated by the amount of violent action typically involved in the actor's portrayal. The two ratings were combined to form an index of identifica- tion with violent characters. Perceived efficacy of violent characters. A series of descriptive state- ments about what happens on action and adventure shows werelisted. The students indicated the frequency (often. sometimes. never) with which each happens on shows they watch. Three highly correlated items were grouped because each indicates theeffectiveness of violence for the aggressing character: a. The hero's friends think it is OKif he hurts the bad guy. h. The guy who gets rough gets his way. c. The bad guy deserves the beating he gets.

FINDINGS Our analysis is divided into three segments: levels and internal con- sistency of the major sets of variables: bivariate relationships between sets of variables: and multivariate predictors of adolescentaggression. 189 182 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS For each part of the analysis. there will be successive parallel tables. the first for Maryland and the second for Wisconsin. Although the selection of variables and modes of analysis imply cer- tain theoretical perspectives, the presentation of data will not formally test hypotheses. Rather we will examine a series of complex tables, each of which shows a number of relationships, and discuss the possible theo- retical import of those relationships that we find to be non-zero. Statisti- cal significance tests are not an important element of the analysis. ex- cept as rough indicators of which relationships are strong enough to warrant further consideration. We therefore use asterisks to indicate two conventional significance levels:* (p <.05). ** (p <.01). For com- parison of levels of standardized means across se). and age categories, these asterisks indicate the significance of the particular cell mean from the mean of all other cells combined. For correlations. they indicate the significance of the difference of the correlation from zero, in either direction. All correlations are Pearson product-moment correlations (r). Single variable analyses The standardized levels and interindex correlations are shown for each of four sets of variables in Tables 1-16: adolescent aggression, tel- evision viewing, family environment, and cognitive reactions to televi- sion. (MI tables are in Appendix C.) Standardized levels of adolescent aggression.Two very clear general- izations can be made from the data in Tables 1 and 2: that boys show considerably higher levels on most aggression measures than do girls. and that there is a considerable decline in aggression level from junior to senior high on most indices. The lowered senior high levels are found for both boys and girls. While the sex differences should surprise nobody. the magnitude of the age differences across the three-year period is per- haps less obvious. This developmental pattern is not well recognized in the research literature. The Zaks-Walters scales show no definite pattern, the lack of differ- ence between sexes being consistent with previous work (Walters and Zaks, 1959). The mixture of attitudinal with behavioral items in the Zaks-Walters scales may partially account for the lack of difference between groups. An exception to the general pattern is the nonbehavioral Buss-Durkee irritability index, which deals with covert internal responses. Girlsare higher than boys, and there are inconsistent findings for age groups. The reversal is perhaps not surprising since four of the seven studies cited by Buss (1961) show females with higher irritability levels than males. The conceptual definition offered by Buss, "a readiness to explode at the slightest provocation...including quick ter:1,6er, grouchiness, exaspera- tion, and rudeness" (p. 169). certainly fits the femak stereotype better than does the ovett aggression implied in other of our indices. 190 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 183 Two other findings regarding adolescent aggression levels should be mentioned. (Data for neither finding are shown in our tables.) The distri- bution on all indices proved to be satisfactory. with the raw means on all eight for each sample falling not further than one-half of a standard de- viation from the mean that would have been obtained if all respondents had checked the middle position on a given scale (e.g.. "somewhat" on the "like me" scale). Although all means except those on Zaks-Walters Aggression are somewhat below this "theoretical mean." the degree of skew ness on no index is so severe as to seriously attenuate the correla- tions in subsequent analyses. Another comforting finding is that the means of our two samples are remarkably similar. Only in the hypotheti cal aggressive reactions index. where Maryk nd is significantly higher.. do differences occur. Internal consistency of adolescent aggression indices. Tables 3 and 4 contain the intercorrelations of the aggressions in the two samples. It should be noted that the overall sum (OAS) is a combination of the first four indices, producing several strong part-whole correlations. In addi- tion, assault has three items in common with physical aggression: since these two indices are quite similar, only the physical aggression index will be subjected to further analysis. Of the remaining 36 independent cells in the two matrices, all correlations except two are positive, al- though most are low to moderate in magnitude. The two indices lowest in internal consistency are the same two that showed deviant patterns on the sex and grade level comparisons. Zaks- Walters and Buss-Durkee Irritability have the lowest average correla- tion with other independent indices (+.20 and +.15 respectively), per- haps reflecting the lesser concern with overt behavior in these measures than elsewhere. Standardized levels of adolescent television viewing. Adolescent boys watch considerably more violent television fare than girls as evidenced by higher levels on crime-detective programs. overall violence viewing (OVV). and violent television movies. These differences are shown for the two samples in Tables 5 and 6. These findings are consistent with the data reported by Chaffee. McLeod, and Atkin (1970) and Chaffee and McLeod (1971), which show sex differences for westerns and spy-ad- ven.ure programs. The Maryland data also show greater male viewingof westerns and adventure-drama, although the Wisconsin pattern is some- what inconsistent. The present data also show Wisconsin boys higher on viewing time among seniors only. while Maryland sex differences are negligible. Finally, Chaffee et al. found that boys exceeded girls in public affairs viewing at both grade levels. In varying degrees. our data support this finding. For both our Maryland and Wisconsin samples, girls watch more situation comedy and game programs than do boys. A sharp decline in adolescent viewing from junior to senior high for virtually every program category except news-public affairs and violent 191 184 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVEN ESS television movies is shown in Tables 5 and 6. The drop is particularly sharp for situation comedy and Saturday morning shows and includes all three high violence categories and the 0 VV index. The violence viewing results are consistent with Chaffee and McLeod. who found similar trends for both western and spy-adventure programs: using the same data. Chaffee. Ward. and Tipton (1970) also found that viewing of news and public affairs programming increases through adolescence and into adulthood. Table 5 for Maryland also replicates findings of a general decrease in time spent with television during adolescence. reported by Schramm. Lyle. and Parker (1961). Chaffee and Mc7Leod. and Chaffee et al. The Wisconsin girls' subsample also follows this trend, but the boys show a surprising increase. One possible explanation is that the Wisconsin questionnaires were given out during October at a time when football (including the nearby Green Bay Packers) was occupying some 12 hours of air time per week. A good share oi the senior high boys' television time may have been devoted to this programming. since they are the subgroup most interested in sports. The Maryland data, having been gathered during April. showed no such bulge for the senior boys. lntercorrelation among television viewing indices. Some justification for using the label "violence viewing" is found in Tables 7 and 8. With- out evidence that crime, westerns, and adventure shows correlate as an actual syndrome of adolescent viewing behavior, it might be argued that such a label is merely a fiction of the researcher. Across the two sam- ples, the three "violent" program types do have a higher average inter- correlation with each other (+.59 and + .34 for the two samples) than with the "less violent" categories of comedy-variety, situation comedy. game shows. and news (+.40 and + .22). The fit is less than perfect, however, for in the Maryland sample the comedy-variety and situation comedy indices are more highly associated with the three violent catego- ries than with game shows or news. News does not correlate highly with anything in either sample. Additional evidence for violence viewing as a syndrome is shown in the correlation of overall violence ( OVV ) with the specific program types. It correlates much more highly with the three violent program types (average r of +.82 and +.75 for the two samples) than with the four less violent categories (+ .49 and + .23). Of course, the raters of vio- lence may have been aware of these subject matter categories wben they rated the shows and thereby unconsciously biased their judgments. Standardized levels of family environment variables. Tables 9 and 10 show considerable variation between subgroups and inconsistency among samples on the family environment standardized lock. In terms of parental attempts to influence the adolescents' viewing behavior. there is a consistent pattern for parents to interpret (e.g., to say things

1(z2 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 185 on television are not like that in real life) more forboys and for younger children generally than for girls and older children. Younger children also reported more parental zontrol over viewing and more emphasis on nonaggression. Punishment and alection levels showed considerable inconsistency between samples. but there was a nonsurprising pattern of boys report- ing more physical punishment and less affection than girls. Between the junior and senior high age groups. there is a trend toward greater paren- tal use of verbal punishment and less restrictive punishment and display of affection. Senior high students in both samples also report adecidedly lower level of education for their mothers. It cannot be ascertained if this is a real difference or merely misreporting among the younger chil- dren. lntercorrelation among family environment indices.Tables 11 and 12 reveal moderate correlations within areas (attempts to influence view- ing. punishment. and social structure) and very low correlations across these areas. Attempts to control are more closely related to affection than to punishment. although neither association is large. Affection is negatively related to punishment, but again the correlations are low. Perhaps the most interesting finding in these tables is the independence of measures of punishment and attempted influence from the social sta- tus measures. Mothers' education and affection show only a slight posi- tive relation in both samples. Standardized levels of cognitive reactions to television.Boys are more likely to see opportunities for learning antisocial behavior and to identify with violent characters while girls report higher levels of in- volvement in program ming. according to Tables 13 and 14. This finding seems compatible with our findings in Tables I and 2. where boys were higher in the measures of overt aggression and lower on the more inter- nalized responses of the irritability index. Table 13 and 14 also show a pronounced decline from junior to senior high for all cognitive reaction indices except perceived efficacy of vio- lent characters which has a slight increase. The drop is quite pronounced for perceived learning of aggression and for identification with violent characters, particularly among girls. The lessening of most types of cog- nitive reactions parallels the general decline of viewing among the older children shown for most specific program types in Tables 5 and 6. Intercorrelation among cognitive reactionstotelevision.Some suggestion that there is a general dimension of reactivity to violent tele- vision is found in the data in Tables 15 and 16. All indices except identi- fication with violent characters show moderate to high correlations with the other reactivity measures. Identification was perhaps the weakest index in terms of reliability, perhaps accounting for its lower level of association.

1F.1, 3 186 TELEVISION AND A DOLESCENT AGGR ESSIVEN ESS Bivariate analyses

Our analysis of bivariate relationships between sets of variablesis based on the data in Tables 17-28. found in Appendix C. Thereare five subsections with two tables within each: aggression by violence view- ing: violence viewing by family environment: aggression by family envi- ronment: cognitive reactions to television by aggression and violence viewing, and all variables by socioeconomic status and school perform- ance. Violence viewing and aggressive behavior. Any purported linkbe- tween childrens' viewing of media violence and their level of aggressive behavior requires evidence that showsa positive association betWeen these two variables. To show causalitythat violence viewing leadsto aggressiveness, for examplewould also require evidence about the direction of influence and eliminate other alternative explanations. For the present. however, we will simply examine the evidence regarding the required posiiive association. Tables 17 and 18 show the correlations between Overall Violence Viewing ( OVV) and the Overall Aggressive Sum ( OAS) and its four component parts. In general. there is cvidence of a positive association between viewing of television violence and the indices of agpression. The key OVV by OAS correlations are substantial (+.32 and+ .30, both significant at .01 level) for all respondents combined (N=472. N=151). and for each of the sex-age subgroups except junior high boys (+ .14 and +.12). The strongest association is among the junior girls in each sample (+.28 and + .38). followed by senior boys (+.31 and +.23) and senior girls (+.21 and +.23). On the individual indices of aggressive behavior, the overall correla- tions are significantly positive in each case except for Zaks-Waltersag- gression in Maryland. Violence viewing relates most strongly with hypo- thetical aggressive reactions (+.32 and + .22). followcd by manifest physical aggression (+ .28and + .17). aggressive behavioral delinquency (+ .22 and +.20). and Zaks-Walters aggression (+ .08 and+ .24). Within age-sex subgroups, 31 of 38 correlationsare greater than + .10, and 12 correlations are statistically significant. On the other hand. the relationship of violence viewing with irritability aggression is uni- formly nonsignificant across all respondents (.03 and +.02), with all subgroup correlations falling between .10 to +.10 (datanot shown). Thus, this index of internal aggressiveness personalityappears to be unrelated to viewing of violent programs. The consistently positive association between behavioral aggression and violence viewing in both samples stands in contrast to the apparent- ly inconclusive pattern of findings from experimental and field investiga- tions of the impact of media violence on adolescent antisocial behavior. Most experimental evidence substantiates the proposition that under 1S4 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TEL.EVISION USE 187 certain conditions the viewing of violent media content makes an aggres- sive response by the child more likely, but permits no more than specu- lation about the learning of aggressiveness as a relatively enduring dis- position. For reviews of these studies. see Berkowitz (1962). Hartley (1964), Walters (1966). Flanders (1968), Goranson (1969a, 1969b). and Atkin, Murray, and Nayman (1971). While field studies are often cited as refuting any causal link between television and aggressive behavior, a careful reading of the survey litera- ture reveals little convincing evidence for or against the proposition that television bears some guilt for adolescent aggression. Himmelweit. Oppenheim, and Vince (1958) concluded that well-adjusted children will learn to adjust to television violence, but they presented no evidence directly bearing on this inference. They do suggest that violence viewing can precipitate aggressive behavior among those who are emotionally disturbed and predisposed to act aggressively. In addition, mothers' dia- ries indicated that young children often displayed aggressive play after watching television. Teacher ratings of students along an aggressive- submissive continuum were not different between samples with and without television available. Schramm et al. found that tenth graders who preferred television to print media were significantly higher than others on an antisocial aggres- sion scale, but no significant differences were found among sixth grad- ers. On the other hand, sixth graders in a Canadian community with tele- vision were significantly lower on antisocial aggression than sixth grad- ers in a comparison community without television: there were no differ- ences between tenth graders in these two towns, however. Eron (1963) reported a strong positive relationship between peer rat- ings of aggression and viewing of violent programs among third grade boys but there was a negative association between aggression and over- all time spent with television for these boys. There were no significant relationships for girls. Cowden. Bassett, and Cohen (1969) found that a high level of expo- sure to violence portrayals was associated with emotional instability and getting into arguments and fights among institutionalized adolescent boys, but there was no relationship between violence viewing and more serious assaultive offenses. Our research shows that among both boys and girls at two grade lev- els, the more the child watches violent television fare, the more aggres- sive he is likely to be as measured by a variety of self-report measures. Since the data are correlational, the reverse also holds: the more aggres- sive the child is, the more likely he is to watch high levels of violent tele- vision programming. Since much of the writing in the field refers to the amount of time the child spends with television rather than with the specifics of violent cor tent, it is useful to relate out measure of total viewing time to the indices 195 188 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS of aggression. Tables 19 and 20 show these correlations. Once again. predominantly positive correlations are shown with significant associa- tions between viewing time and OAS across all subjects in each sample. The magnitude of the correlations are considerably smaller than those for 0 VV. however, except for the junior boys. where the viewing time by OAS correlations are a respectable +.20 and +.25. Later in this re- port we will control viewing time in studying the OV V by aggression indices. Family environment and violence viewing. Tables 21 and 22 show the relationship of violence viewing (VV ) and ten measures of the family environment. There is an overall positive relationship between OVVand interpretation (e.g.. things are not like that in real life) and a mild posi- tive relationship between OVV and control of the child's viewing. While these findings suggest the ineffectiveness of such techniques to actually inflyence the adolescent's television viewing behavior, perhaps a more lik:ly inference is that of reverse causation: the heavy violence viewing c'tild influences the parent to do something to control and discount this behavior. Bassett. Cowden. and Cohen (1968) found that among institutional- ized delinquent boys. those who reported a high incidence of physical punishment at home were more likely to prefer violent television con- tent. Others have stated theories with implied hypotheses regarding the relationship of violence viewing and various forms of punishment. such that we would expect a strong positive relationship (Himmelweit et al.. 1958: Schramm et al.. 1961: Hess and Goldman. 1962: Maccoby. 1954: Riley and Riley. 1951. 1959: Tannenbaum and Greenberg. 1968: Ward and Wackman. 1971). On the other hand. Chaffee and McLeod (1971). using almost identical punishment measures to ours with a different sample. found only low correlations with violence viewing (+.09 with physical punishment. +.08 with verbal punishment. and +.14 with re- strictive punishment). The present data in Tables 21 and 22 are close to those of Chaffee and McLeod in showing a larger positive correlation for restrictive punish- ment overall (+ .14 and +.24) than for physical or verbal punishment. The data for physical and verbal punishment show no relationship to OVV in Maryland and mild positive correlations in Wisconsin. Contrary to Maccoby (1954). who found a negative relationship be- tween affection and "escapist" television viewing, we find essentially no relationship in either sample using the violence viewing index. In fact. the direction is slightly positive (+.03 and +.04). This is also in keeping with the Chaffee and McLeod +.07 correlation with similar measures. Tables 21 and 22 also show a slight tendency for children in larger families to watch more violent programming. In parts this may be due to a negative relationship between size of family and social status. The measures of social status. fathers' occupations. and mothers' educa- 196 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 189 tions do show the expected negative association with OVV. but the edu- cation relationship is not consistently large. It does indicate, however. that social status controls are desirable in our later multivariate tables. Family environment and aggressive behavior. It is apparent from Ta- bles 23 and 24 that parental attempts to influence the child's violence viewing behavior and aggressive behavior are not associated with lower levek of adolescent aggression. The correlations are inconsistent and generally low. Of course. this does not mean that such parental attempts are ineffective. The possibility of interactive effects will be examined in the multivariate section of this report. There is an extensive though not totally consistent literature suggest- ing that we should expect a positive relationship between punishment and aggressive behavior. Sears et al. (1953) found a positive relationship between maternal punitiveness and overt aggression at school among boys and a curvilinear function for girls. Becker et al. (1962) replicated Sears et al. and also found that aggression in the home was positively correlated with mothers' punitiveness for both sexes. Similar supportive findings for various measures and samples are shown by Eron et al. (1961). Sears et al. (1957). and Lefkowitz et al. (1963). Other data sug- gest that use of power-assertive techniques over time may inhibit the more overt forms of .aggression. Sears (1961) found in a panel design that early punishment by the mother lost its positive association with aggression between ages flve and twelve and tended to relate to inhibited or deflected forms of aggression. McCord et al. (1959) found that crimi- nal acts were less likely when both mother and father were consistently punitive. Our data show the expected positive re/ationship between all three forms of punishment and the OAS measure for both boys and pirls in senior high school only (Tables 23 and 24). The junior high correlations are low and inconsistent. The interaction of punishment and age-grade for OAS contrasts with earlier results for punishment and violence view- ing where the correlations were almost equal in size for each grade level. Likewise. all forms of punishment predict aggression about equally while restrictive punishment was clearly the best predictor of violence viewing. The stronger findings for senior high also carries over to the affection variable where we obtain the expected negative relationship for the old- er group only. Both the punishment and affection interaction findings are surprising because of the purported decline of family influence during 1-1.e adolescence. Social status does not seem to be related to aggression in any clear and consistent way. This does not support the popular and stereotype of high levels of aggressive behavior among working-class children. Cognitive reactions to television and aggressive behavior. A close inspection of the data from the two samples in Table 25 reveals consid- erable discrepancy. In general. there ate stronger ties between cognitive 197 I 90 'I I: 1.1A1SION AND AIVIITSCEN1 MiGRFSSIVFNESS

reactions and aggressive behavior in the Maryland sample. andamong girls in both groups. The strongest association is found for the first in- dex. where those perceiving learning from the violent contentare clearly the most aggressive adolescents. Among girls at least those seeinga tie between television violence and real life and those reporting the highest levels of involvement are also high in aggressive behavior. While the logical status of these variables is in question because they in a sense presume viewing of violence, they will he usefulas additional variables in our later regression analyses. Cognitive reactions to television and violence viewing. Table 26 shows a generally positive correlation between cognitive reactionsto television and the level of violence viewing. The magnitude of thesecor- relations are somewhat lower than in the previous table, perhaps indicat- ing that reactivity is more closely tied to the adolescent's aggressive behavior than to his viewing habits. Tbe two reactions with thestrongest relationships to violence viewing arc perceived learning and linkageto real life. The causal question can be raised whether frequent viewing leads to perceiving learning and reality in television violenceor whether such perceptions lead the adolescent to seek out themore violent con- tent Social status, school performance, and other variables. Tables 27 and 28 show the correlation of two control variables, socioeconomicstatus and school performance, with all other variables. As shown in partear- lier, social status is generally unrelated to adolescent aggression. Only for approval of aggression do we find a semblance ofa relationship, and these correlations are quite small. As also has been discussed earlier, the fathers' occupational status is related negatively toevery program cate- gory except news-public affairs and violent television movies, and is unrelated or inconsistently related to other variables inour analysis. It appears likely that school performance hasa somewhat different meaning in each of our samples. While it is negatively related to allag- gression measures except irritability in our Maryland sample,our Wis- consin group shows substantial negative correlations only for the Zaks- Walters and approval of aggression measures. The OAS correlations show a wide discrepancy ( .29 and +.07) between samples. It should be noted that the measures differed slightly between the two schoolsys- tems; while the students self-reported school grades on an identical item, school reports of student performance were basedon teacher rat- ings along a four-step scale in Wisconsin anda three-level track assign- ment in Maryland. While the two samples show greater consistency in violence viewing in that the low school performers tend to watchmore violence, the cor- relations are higher for Maryland. The low performers in each sample also tend to be low on news-public affairs viewing, and, consistent with previous research (Schramm et al., 1961; Himmelweit et al., 1958; Scott. 198 ADUI.I.S(FNI S. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 191

1956). they tend to spend more time with television. For entertainment of various types, however, the high users tend to be adolescents with low grades in Maryland and with high grades in Wisconsin. Parents who emphasize nonaggression and those displaying affection tend to have high performance children. For other forms of parental influence and punishment, there appears to be no consistent difference in performance. Although the correlations arc not large. low school performance tends to he associated with the adolescents' perceiving learning fromviolent content, with seeing a linkage to real life, and in identifying with violent television characters. M u ltivari ate analyses

Adolescent aggressive behavior will be the key criterion variable for our multivariate analysis. We will begin by examining thebasic relation- ships between violence viewing and aggression, partialing out three con- trol variables, and then examine more complex regression analyses. Violence viewing and aggressive behavior: partial correlations. Ta- bles 29 and 30 show the raw zero-order correlations of violence viewing ( OVV) by various aggression measures, together with the partial corre- lations removing the effects of total television viewing time. The partials represent the associations for viewing of violent content per se, apart from thc sheer time spent with television. In this way, it represents choice behavior rather than simple exposure. Partialing out viewing time slightly reduces the positive correlations of violence viewing and aggressive behavior in most cases, but the basic result is the same as for the raw correlations. With only one exception. the statistically raw correlations for all students within each sample remain significant for the partials. The key OVV by OAS correlation for all respondents drops from + .32 to +.28 for the partial in Maryland and from + .30 to +.24 in Wisconsin. Similarly, the partialing out of socioeconomic status and school per- formance does not alter the basic pattern of the raw correlations (Tables 31 and 32). There was essentially no effect of partialing out SES and only a minor decline (from + .32 to +.26) in one sample for the school performance partial. We may conclude, then, that adolescents viewing high levels of vio- lent content on television tend to have high levels of aggressive behav- ior. regardless of television viewing time, socioeconomic status or school performance. These partials appear to rule out as alternative ex- planations simple television exposure, social status, and general com- petence as a student. Viewing of program types by aggressive behavior. Table 33 attempts to measure thc contribution of each of seven specific program types to 199 I.,%'ISMN ND DOLESUFN1 .A(ifikl.SSIVI NI,Ss OAS by partialing out the effects of each of the other six types. Data for both samples are presented in the same table. Since Tables 7 and 8 re- vealed that virtually eacl program type is related positively with all oth- er program types. it is obvious that the partial here should act to reduce all positive raw correlations. Crime-detective and Saturday morning programs retain their signifi- cant positive associations with aggression level after the viewing of oth- er program types is removed. The adventure-drama correlations, though considerably diminished. remain low positive and statistically significant in the M aryland sample. Perhaps the most interesting feature of Table 33 is the finding that the moderate to low positive associations for wJsterns by ()AS (+ .20 and + .12) become negative ( .01 and .07) after removing the effects of viewing of other shows. Situation comedy viewing becomes rather strongly negative through a similar process in the Wisconsin sample. Multiple predictors of aggressive behavior. Thc objective of the anal- yses shown in Tables 34 and 35 is to sec to what degree the different variables can improve on the prediction of aggressive behavior ( OAS by our violence viewing index ( OVV). Thc key to understanding the table, then, is to compare the zero-order OAS by OVV correlations shown in the top rows to the multiple correlations producc d by each of thc third variables. The larger the difference from the zero-order corre- lation, the more the third variable contributes to predicting aggressive behavior over and above that of violence viewing. Across the two sam ples, perceived learning of aggression from violent television programs makes the largest independent contribution to the OVV-OAS relationship (from + .32 to +.56 and from + .30 to +.40). Thus, thc quantitative and qualitative aspects of violence viewing com- bine to produce a substantially larger multiple correlation with aggres- sive behavior than either singly. Similarly, irritability combines with vio- lence viewing to augment the correlation (to + .42 and + .41). Thc other factors are less consistent or relatively unimportant in accounting for additional aggressive behavior. Among senior high students, higher lev- els of physical punishment and lower levels of affection tend to make mild contributions to the relationship, but the junior high pattern is mixed. Multiple regression: aggressive behavior by violence viewing and family predictors. Tables 36 and 37 show. the results of a multiple regres- sion analysis using the OAS index as thc criterion variable and the OVV index, punishment, and affection as the three predictor variables. We felt the relatively high intercorrelation of punishment items and thc con- sistency of direction in their predictions justified the combination of all three punishment types (physical, verbal, and restrictive) into a single index for regression purposes. Admittedly, this is a post hoc procedure, but our efforts here are not hypothesis-testing, but hypothesis-building for future research. 200 AIMLESUEN1S. PARFN I S. 1E1 EVISION USE 191

rhe cell entriesifor each of the three predictorvariables represent the correlation of that variable and OAS. partialing outthe effects of the other two predictor variables. In both samples.the violence viewing index for all subjects combined has the highestpartial correlation coeffi- cient. This appears to he. largely a functionof a relatively weak predic- tion by punishment in the junior high sample. Bothviolence viewing and punishment contribute considerably more than doesaffection. which shows a weak negative ox erall partial andpositive coefficients for three of the Light sub-group cells. The multiple correlations of thevariables on OAS is shown in the fourth row of Tables 36 and 37. The threevariables together account for 17.6 percent of the variance in OAS in Marylandand for 12.2 percent in Wisconsin. The multiple correlation coefficientstend to be somewhat larger for senior high than for juniorhigh students. Multiple regression: aggressive behavior by five key predictor varia- bles. Two more strong correlates of aggressive behaviorperceived aggression learning and Buss-Dnrkee irritability. are added in Tables 38 and 39. These two variables, one a cognitive reaction to violent televi- sion content and the other a presumed internal response. become impor- tant indepengent predictors of OAS. with partials equal to or greater than those of OVV in most comparisons. Nevertheless, the addition of these two variables does not decrease the partial coefficients of OVV by OAS to any marked degree. The par- tials for the punishment index are diminished, and its predictive power (as well as that of affection) is largely confined to the senior high re- spondents. The combination of these five variables provides for a substantial mul- tiple correlation with the overall ggression sum in each sample (.62 and .51. both significant at .01 level). These factors account for 38.5 percent of the OAS variance in Maryland and 26.0 percent in Wisconsin. Within the age-sex subgroups. the multiple correlations range from .46 to .69: se ..,en out of eight are statistically significant. We may conclude that the level of violence viewing remains an impor- tant predictor of aggression when a series of other variables are con- trolled. but we may also say that by taking into account the adolescent's perceptions of learning and his level of irritability, we can increase our power topredict considerably. Violence viewing hy aggressive behavior Within levels of nonaggres- sive emphasis. The parental attempts at emphasizing nonaggression was shown to have little consistent association with either violence viewing (Tables 21 and 22) or aggressive behavior (Tables 23 and 24). In our post hoc probing. we treated this variable as a possible factor interacting with level of violence viewing. To do this, we divided the nonaggressive teaching attempts into high and lowroups and ran OV V by OAS corre- lations within the two levels. The resuits lire shown in.Tables 40 and 41. 201 194 IIIEVIS1ON .1) ADOI FSCENT MiCIRFSSIVENESS There k a distinct tendency for the correlations to be lower for those reporting that their parents emphasize nonaggression and higher where less parental concern is expressed. although the pattern of results is somewhat inconsistent in thc Wisconsin cells with few respondents. This tendency holds for the physical aggression and hypotheticalaggres- sion indices as well as for OAS. The differencesare more uniform across measures and across sex and age categories for the larger Mary- land sample than in Wisconsin. An analysis of variance on OAS using high vs. low nonaggression emphasis and high vs. low violence viewing yields a significant interaction for all Maryland respondents (p <.01. F test). To some degree. then. there is evidence that parental emphasis on nonaggressive behavior has some effect, not directly either on violence viewing or on aggressive behavior, but indirectly in reducing the rela- tionship between these two factors. An examination of interactive relationships with other third variables did not show any significant conditional differences in the basic OVV- OAS correlations.

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings for the internal consistency of various indices ofaggres- sion (Tables 3 and 4) and television viewing behavior (Tables 7 and 8) provide some justification for our use of the terms "aggressive behav- ior" and "violence viewing." With the exception of the Buss-Durkee Irritability scale, all other indices of self-reported aggression showed moderate to high intercorrelations. Since irritability is the only measure which involves internal feelings rather than overt behavior,we grouped four of the remaining indices using the term aggressive behavior." The sum of the four indices became our key criterion variable, the overall aggression sum ( OAS). Similarly. the three "violent" program types (crime-detective, west- erns, and adventure-drama) among the seven categories, tended to form a highly intercorrelated cluster of adolescent viewing preferences. Each correlated more highly than did the less violent program types with the sum of violence viewing based on our ratings of the amount of violent content in each of 65 programs. This helped to justify our use of the lat- ter measure, overall violence viewing ( OVV), as a key variable in our

One comforting conclusion that can be drawn from our analysis of standardized means is that both of our key measures. the OAS and the OVV indices, along with adolescents' reactivity to violent television, decline in level from junior to senior high school. This may bea function of the general maturation of the child and also of the specific competi- tion from other more socially approved activities in later adolescence. 202 ADOLESCENTS. PARLNIS. TELEVISION USE 19f: The cruciallfinding in this report. however, is that there is a clear if moderate positive association between the adolescents' level of violence viewing and the:r level of self.reported aggression. This finding holds across most indices of aggression, and for both sexes and age levelsin two samples of adolescents. The average correlation between the over- all aggression ( OAS iand violence viewing ( OVV ) indices is +.24 with- in sex-age groups and + .31 for all respondents combined. A variety of causal inferences could be drawn from these bivariate findings. First, the long-term viewing of violence may lead the adoles- cent to perform aggressive acts. Of at least equal plausibility is the sec- ond possible inferencethat the aggressive child may seek out the more violent programi while viewing television. Finally, some third variable or set of variables may be causing the level of both aggressivebehavior and violence viewing to be high or low. Without further evidence, we cannot choose among these inferences. We have at least shown that there is concomitant variation between the two variables, to the extent the correlations are very unlikely to be simply chance fluctuations. Two other factors are necessary to move beyond the statement of a positive relationship: some evidence must be shown regarding time order of the variables in order to rule out reverse causation. and other alternative explanations must be eliminated. While our research design did not contain the desirable attribute of having a panel with a long inter- val intervening between successive measurement. we will present some limited analyses of the time-order question in our next report. In this report. we present evidence only for the last of the requirements. elimi- nation of alternative explanations through control. Our nonexperimental design. lacking in the ability to randomly assign people to manipuked conditions, implies that we would have to check an infinite list of alternative explanations, whereas only alimited num- ber of such checks are available with the data we have gathered. We are able to say that we have tested out several dozen variables as potential alternatives. This was accomplished by partialing their effects from the basic violence viewing-aggressive behavior correlations. Although these zero-order correlations are not overwhelmingly large, none of the third variables taken singly or in combination with other variables reduces the magnitude of the basic correlations in a fundamental way. Prior to examinigg our data, it might have been expected that several of our control variables would be likely to be highly correlated with both violence viewing and aggressive behavior and thus. when partialed out would markedly lower the key OVV by OAS correlations. For example. socioeconomic status is such a potential alternative explanation. Pre- vious research has indicated that lower-status adolescents watch more entertainment television, and the conventional wisdom attributes higher levels of aggression to them as well. While our data show the expected relationship with violent content, socioeconomic status is unrelated to 203 191) ANI) IGKIISSIvI.NFSS aggressive behavior. Thus, the status control doesnot affect the vio- lence viewing-aggressive behavior correlation. Similarly..various tables in thk report show little or no effect on the basic OVV by OAS relation- ship when these variables arc controlled:sex. age. school performance. punishment. affection, and irritability. Many other variablesnot shown in the tables produce a similar result. Somewhatgreater reduction re- sults from controlling television viewing time and the various cognitive reaction measures. but the basic finding remains intact. Wemust con- clude that, after introducing a wide variety of variablesas controls. the relationship between violence viewing and aggressive behavior isro- bust. Correlates of violence viewing and aggressive behavior. Conclusions about the correlates of aggressive behavior and violence viewingmust be tempered by the admission that the results for family environment variables arc characterized more bya lack of relationship than by strong findings. Parental attempts to influence produceno clear and consistent association with either variable, and other parentaltreatment variables of punishment and affection show relationships only for certain sub- groups. The situation is disappointing from both a theoretical and a practical standpoint. A summary of all variables found to be related to either aggressive behavior or to violence viewing is shown below:

Type of relationship to:

Variable Aggressive behavior (OAS) Violence viewing (OVV)

Social structural

Sex of child Boys higher Boys higher

Age of child Younger higher Younger higher

Socioeconomic status No relationship Negative

School performance Negative, but inconsistentNegative

Parental treatinent

Physical punishment Positive, older children only No relationship

204 PARENTS..IELEVISION USE 197

Variable Aggressive behavior (OAS) ViolenceViewing (OVV)

Verbal punishment Positive, older children only No relationship

Restrictive punishment Positive, older children only Positive

Affection Negative, older children only Norelationship

Cognitive reactions

Learning of aggression Positive Positive

I inkage to real lifePositive Positive.Tirls only

Involvement Positive Positive, girls only

Identification Positive Slight, boys only

Person a lit y

Irritability Positive No relationship

The emergence of cognitive reactions totelevision as the most con- sistent set of variables related to both OAS and OVVraises questions about the logical status of these variables. In a sense,they imply both aggression and television use. Perhaps they are bestconceptualized as intervening variables operating as contributory conditionsincreasing the strength of the violence viewing-aggressive behaviorrelationship when reactivity is high and decreasing it under lower activity. Despite the disappointing results for the parental treatmentvariables. we cannot conclude that parentshave little influence on their childrens' television viewing and the aggressiveness of behavior.We did find rela- tively low levels of violence viewing amongchildren reporting less fre- quent use of restrictive punishmentand, among senior high school ado- lescents at least, those less oftenpunished and more often given affec- tion were lower on self-reports of aggressivebehavior. We also found a considerably lower violence viewing-aggressivebehavior correlations among adolescents who reportedthat their parents stress nonaggression by such techniques as telling the child not tofight back, not to copy things on television, and so forth. Greaterattention should be paid to the effectiveness of these "immunizing" techniquesin future research. 198 .1[1.1AlsloN AND.Apol U.SCENWaikl SI\' EN FSS

We should ado that we have by no means exhausted the possibilities for studying family variables. In our next report.we shall consider the role of the child's modeling of his mother's television viewing and her self-reported aggressive behavior. Dimensions of parent-child interac- tion will also he considered. Doubtless thereare other variables that other investigators will examine. Our main advice to them basedon the studies reported here concerns the measurement of television behavior. We have found that it improves the precision of prediction ofmeasure- ment to go from simple amount of viewing time to watching of specific shows, and finally to the level of violence in those specificprograms. We also suggest that the child's reaction to violent television isas important an attribute to consider as his exposure to programs.

CONTENTS OF FORTHCOMING REPORT

The present report describes data gathered from both the Maryland and the Wisconsin samples. Additional findings will be presented ina second report (McLeod. Atkin. and Chaffee, also in this volume) that will deal with the two-year Wisconsin investigation. The reports have been kept separate because rather different kinds of dataare involved. For example. the second report will examine longitudinal data from both adolescents and their mothers across a greater number of variables, with more extensive measurement of variables from the first report. First, we will examine mother-child modeling of television viewing behavior and of aggressive behavior and values. Both mother and child separately completed identical scales of program viewing and 20aggres- sion items indexing five types of aggression. The mother also reported the level of aggressive behavior of the child: teacher and peer reports of the child's aggressive behaviorwere also obtained to provide additional independentsources of aggression data. The second report will describe the relationships among the child's self- report and the three external sources, and relate them to his television viewing behavior. Both the mother and child completed ratings on the interpersonal communication environment within the home: these family communica- tion patterns will be examined in their relation to the viewing andaggres- sion measures. Parent-child coorientational agreement. accuracy, and congruency on various values will also be studied. Another key inde- pendent variable is the child's peer relations, as reported by himself and his mother. Longitudinal data on 1969 to 1970 differences on certain aggression measures, television viewing, and attitudes about television will be ana- lyzed. For many variables in the first report, the two-wave Wisconsin study obtained additional measures, providing improved indices. Twice 206

..10.1111. ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 199 as many items will enter intothe Buss-Durkee and behavioraldelinquen- cy indices. Supplementarydata from the mother is availablefor the in- dices of punishment, affection and other measures. New data from the child will include aviolence viewing index relating to viewing behavior three tofour years earlier, selectivity in television viewing choices and differences in involvement,reality linkage, and ag- gression learning from westerns vs.crime-detective programs. The re- port will also includemultivariate analyses of violence viewingand cog- nitive reactions to television as criterionvariables, examining the rela- tive contribution of various familyvariables to these behaviors.

FOOTNOTES

1. This technical report describes research pursuant toContract No. HSM 42-70-77 with the National Institute ofMental Health, Health Services and Mental Health Administration,U.S. Department of Health. Education, and Welfare. Jack McLeod andSteven Chaffee were co-principal investigators.Charles Atkin, now at Michigan State University, was the study director. Othersaiding in the data analysis were William Elliott. William Engels.Kenneth Sheinkopf. and Catherine Willette.

REFERENCES

Atkin. C.K.. Murray. J.P.. and Nayman.O.B. (Eds.)Television and social behavior: an annotated bibliographyof research focusing on television's impact on children.Rockville, Md.: Public Health Serv- ice Publication No. 2099. NationalInstitute of Mental Health, 1971. Bassett, T., Cowden. J., and Cohen. M. Theaudio-visual viewing habits of selected subgroups of delinquents.Journal of Genetic Psycholo- gy.1968, 112, 37-41. Becker, W.C., Peterson, D.R., Luria,Z.. Shoemaker. D.J.. and Hell- mer. L.A. Relations offactors derived from parent-interview rat- ings to behavior problems offive-year-olds. Child Development. 1962, 33, 509-35. Berkowitz,L.Aggression: asocial psychological analysis.New York: McGraw-Hill. 1962. Buss, A.H. Aggression and hostilityinventories.The psychology of aggression.New York: Wiley, 1961. Buss, A.H.. and Durkee. A. Aninventory for assessing different kinds of hostility.Journal of Consulting Psychology.1957, 21, 343-49. Chaffee, S.H., and McLeod, J.M.Adolescent television use in the fami- ly context. InTelevision and social behaviorVol. 4.Television in day-to-day life.Washington. D.C.: U.S. GovernmentPrinting Office. 1971. 207 200 TEITVISION AND ADOLLSCENT AGGRESSIVENF.SS Chaffee. S.H.. McLeod. J.M.. and Atkin. C.K. Parent-adolescent simi- larities in television use. Paper presented at the meeting of the Ao- elation for Education in Journalism. Washington. D.C.. August. 1970. Chaffee, S.H.. McLeod. J.M.. and Atkin. C.K. Parental influences on adolescent media use.American Behavioral Scientist.1971, 14. 323- 40. Chaffee. S.H.. Ward. L.S.. and Tipton. L.P. Mass communication and political socialization.Journalism Quarterly.1970, 47. 647-59. Cowden. J.. Bassett. H.T.. and Cohen, M. An analysis of some relation- ships between fantasy-aggressive and aggressive behavior among institutionalized delinquents.Journal of Genetic Psychology.1969. 114. 179-83. Eron. L. Relationship of TV viewing habits and aggressive behavior in children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,1963. 67 (2), 193-96. Eron, L.. Banta. T., Walder. L.. and Laulicht. J. Comparison of data obtained from mothers and fathers on childrearing practices and their relation to child aggression.Child Development,1961, 32, 457- 72. Flanders. J.P. A review of imitative behavior.Psychological Bulletin. 1968, 69. 316-37. Goran son, R.E. The catharsis effect: two opposing views. In Baker, R., and Ball, S.Violence and the media: A report to the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence.Washington D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1969. Goran son, R.E. A review of recent literature on psychological effects of media portrayals of violence. In Baker and Ball,Violence and the media. Greenberg, B.S.. and Dominick, J.R. Television usage, attitude, and functions for low-income and middle-class teenagers. Report #4, Communicationamong theurbanpoor. East Lansing, Mich,: Department of Communication, Michigan State University, 1968. Greenberg, B.S., and Gordon, T.F. Perceptions of violence in television programs: critics and the public. InTelevision and social behavior, Vol. 1. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Hartley.R.I,. The impact of viewing "aggression": studies and prob- lems of extrapolation.New York: Columbia Broadcasting System Office of Social Research, 1964, Hess, R., and Goldman, H. Parents' views of the effects of TV on their children. Child Development,1962,33,411-26. Himmelweit, H., Oppenheim, A., and Vince, P.Television and the child.London: Oxford University Press, 1958. Lefkowitz, M.M., Walder, L.O., and Eron, L.D. Punishment, identifi- , cation, and aggression.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,1963, 9, 159-74. '208 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 201 Maccoby. E.E. Why do children watchtelevision? Public Opinion Quarterly, 1954, 18, 239-44. McCord. W.. McCord. J.. and Zola,I.Origins of crime. New York: Columbia University Press, 1959. Murray. R.. Cole, R.. and Fedler, F.Teenagers and TV violence: how they rate and view it. JournalismQuarterly. 1970, 47, 247-55. Nye. F.L. and Short, J. Scaling delinquentbehavior. American Socio- logical Review, 1957, 22, 326-31. Riley, J.W., and Riley, M.W. Masscommunication and the social sys- tem. In R.K. Merton, L. Broomand L.S. Cottrell (Eds.) Sociology today. New York: Basic Books, 1959. Riley, M.W., and Riley, J.W. A sociologicalapproach to communica- tions research. Public Opinion Quarterly,1951, 14, 445-60. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B.Television in the lives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford UniversityPress, 1961. Scott, L.F. Television and schoolachievement. Phi Delta Kappa, 1956, 38, 25-28. Sears, R. The relation of earlysocialization experiences to aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormaland Social Psychology. 1961, 63,466-92. Sears, R., Whiting, J., Nowlis, V., andSears, P. Some child-rearing antecedents of aggression and dependencyin young children. Ge- netic Psychological Monographs, 1953, 47,135-236. Short, J., and F. Nye. Extent of unrecordeddelinquency, tentative con- clusions. Journal of Criminal Law.Criminology. and Police Sci- ence, 1958,49. 196-302. Short, J., and F. Nye. Reported behavior as acriterion of deviant be- havior. Social Problems, 1957-58, 207-13. Tannenbaum, P.11., and Greenberg, B.S.Mass communication. Annual Review of Psychology, 1968, 19, 351-86. Walters, R. Implications of laboratorystudies of aggression for the con- trol and regulation of violence.Annals of the American Academyof Political and Social Science, 1966, 364,60-72. Walters, R., and Zaks, M. Validationstudies on an aggression scale. Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 209-18. Ward. L.S., and Wackman, D. Familyand media influences on adoles- cent consumerlearning. American Behavioral Scientist,1971, 14, 415-27. Zaks, M., and Walters, R. First stepsin the construction of a scale for the measurement of aggression.Journal of Psychology, 1959, 47, 199-208. 202 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Appendix A: Complete listing of questionnaire items comprising each index

Aggression indices

Manifest physical aggression: "Hereare some things other students say about getting along with people. How much is each statement like you?" Whoever insults me or my family is asking fora fight. If somebody hits me first. I let him have it. When I lose my temper at someone. once ina while I actually hit them. When I am mad at someone, I sometimes fight with them instead of talking about the problem. When I was younger. I used to act likea bully sometimes. I don't feel it is wrong for me to hit other kids who deserve it. Scoring"a lot like me" (2). "a little like me" (I). "not like me" (0)

Aggressive behavioral delinquency:"Here is a list of things that kids at other schools say they have done. How often haveyou done these things in the last three years?" Been in fights with several people on each side. Hurt someone on purpose to get back for somethingthey had done to you. Got into a serious fight with another studentat school.

Scoring"more than 5 times" (4), "4or 5 times" (3), "2 or 3 times" (2), "one time" (I), "never" (0)

Zaks-Walters aggression:"How m uch do you agree or disagree with these statements?" There are two kinds of people in this world: the weak and thestrong. Dealings with policemen and government officialsare usually pleasant. (reversed) Many good people become crooks or criminals because they can'tstand to be pushed around so much.

"Here are some things other studentssay about getting along with peo- ple. How much is each statement like you?" I often do things which I regret after. I am very patient with people. (reversed) It makes me mad when I can't do things for myself theway I like to. When I was younger, I often hung around with thewrong kind of kids. Scoring"a lot like me" (2), "a little like me" (I), "not like me"(0) ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISIONUSE 203 Hypothetical aggressive reactions: Whatif someone cut in front of you in a longline. What would you do to them? "shove them out" (4), "yell at them"(2), "just let it go" (0) Supposesomeone played a realdirty trick on you. What would you do? "hit them" (4), "yell at them" (2),"laugh at them" (0), "ignore them" (0) What if somebody picks a fight with you onthe way home from school. What would you do about this? "fight" (3),"back out of it" (I), "try to discuss the problem" (0) Suppose you saw some guys fighting eachother after school one day. What do you think you would do in thissituation? "cheer on the fighters" (2), "watch the fight" (1), "breakit up" (1), "ignore it" (0)

Buss-Durkee assault aggression: "Here are somethings other students say about getting alongwith people. How much is each statementlike you?" Whoever insults me or my family is askingfor a fight. If somebody hits me first, I let him haveit. When I lose my temper at someone, oncein a while I actually hit them. I can't think of any good reason forhitting anyone. (reversed)

Scoring"a lot like me" (2), "a little like me"(I), "not like me" (0)

Buss-Durkee irritability: "Here are some things otherstudents say about getting along with people. How much is each statementlike you?" I lose my temper easily. It really makes me mad when somebodymakes fun of me. If someone doesn't treat me right, Idon't let it bother me. (reversed) Scoring"a.lot like me" (2), "a little likeme" (I), "not like me" (0) Approval of aggression: "How much do you agree ordisagree with these statements?" It's all right to hurt an enemy if you aremad at him. In order to get revenge, it's all right tohurt an enem y. Scoring"agree strongly" (5), "agreesomewhat" (4), "no opinion" (3). "disagree somewhat" (2),"disagree strongly" (I)

Overall aggression sum: Unweighted sumof first 20 items, including Manifest physical aggression, Aggressivebehavioral delinquency, Zaks- Walters aggression, and Hypotheticalaggressive reactions, ranging from low (0) to high (50).

211 204 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Television viewing indices

"Here is a list of some programs that have been on television thisyear. About how often have you really watched each of these shows? For each program. make onc check showing whether you watched it: Almost always (nearly every week) or Often (at least half the time)or Some- times (at least once or twice) or Never.-

Scoring--"almost always" (4). "often- (3), "sometimes" (2), "never" (0)

Crime-detective Western programs: Adventure-drama programs: program s: Mod Squad Marcus Welby. M.D. Hawaii Five-0 Lancer Me dica I Center Ironside Virginian Then Came Bronson Dragnet Daniel Boone Hogan's Heroes It Takes a Thief High Chapparal World of Disney Get Smart Here Come the Brides Land of the Giants Name of the Game Bonanza Mission Impossible The FBI Adam-I2 Mannix Comedy-variety program s: Situation comedy programs: Red Skelton Here's Lucy Debbie Reynolds Laugh-ir Mayberry RFD Governor and J.J. Carol Burnett Doris Day Tim Conway Pat Paulsen Beverly Hillbillies Petticoat Junction Hee Haw Green Acres Brady Bunch Johnny Cash Julia Nanny and Professor Glen Campbell To Rome with Love That Girl Jim Nabors I Dream of Jeannie Love American Style Dean Martin Flying Nun Ghost and Mrs. Muir Lawrence Welk Eddie's Father My Three Sons Tom Jones My World Ed Sullivan Room 222 Game programs: Jackie Gleason Bill Cosby Andy Williams Family Affair Let's Make a Deal Bewitched Newlywed Game

Overall violence viewing: Weighted sum of previous 65 programs,rang- ing from most violent (7) to least violent (0), as indicated in Appendix B. The sum across all 65 programs ranges from low violence viewing (0) to high violence viewing (436). ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 205 News-public affairs programs: "How often do youwatch news and pub- lic affairs shows on televn?" National news broadcasts (like Walter Cronkite) Current events shows (like 60 Minutes) Local news broadcasts (like Six O'Clockreports) Interview shows (like Meet the Press) Scoring"often" (2), "sometimes"(1), "never" (0)

Saturday morning programs: "On an averageSaturday morning, about how many hours do you usually spendwatching TV before noon?" _hours __minutes Scoring(8) 4 hours 40 minutes or more (7) 3 hours 40 minutes 4 hours 39 minutes (6) 2 hours 40 minutes 3 hours 39 minutes (5)2 hours 10 minutes 2 hours 39 minutes (4)1 hour 40 minutes 2 hours 9 minutes (3)1 hour 10 minutes 1 hour 39 minutes (2)1 hour 40 minutes 1 hour 9 minutes (1) less than 40 minutes (0) None Total viewing time: sum of three items,using same scoring system as apove.

On an average weekday, about how manyhou do you personally spend watching TV?

hours m inutes

Now we would like to find out about yourtelevision viewing in the last few days. Think of all the programs you sawyesterday and the day be- fore. and figure out exactly how muchtime you spent watching TV pro- grams each day.

Yesterday. hours minutes (what day was it)

Day Before minutes Yesterday: hours (what day was it)

Violent television movies: "Did youwatch any of these movies shown on TV this year?" 213

1_A_a.l..1.k..c.1. 1!,* 206 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Maryland Wisconsin San Francisco International Countess from Hong Kong Tony Rome* Guns of Navarone* Casino Royale* Spy Who Came in from the Cold* The Family Jewels Georgy Girl The Dirty Dozen* Fall of the Roman Empire* ScoringOne point eachfor starred movies; ifall five watchod, 2 points.

Family Environment Indices

Parental control over television: Who has the most to say about whatyou watch on television? "mother" (2), "father" (2), "either motheror me" (I), "brothers or sisters" (0), "me" (0) Do your parents always know whatprograms you are watching on TV? "yes" (I), "no" (0) "Are there certain programs thatyour parents sometimes do not let you watch?

No Yes: (mark as many as you have to) Westerns* TV movies Scary shows Crime shows* Cartoons Violent shows* Sexy shows Adult shows

Scoring"yes" (1), "no" (0), plus I point each for starredprogram types.

Parentalemphasis on non-aggression:

Do your parents punish you if youare mean to other kids? "yes" (2), "I'm never mean" (1), "no" (0) Do your parents want you to fight back if other kids pickon you? "no" (1), "yes" (0) How important does your mother think it is foryou to learn to defend yourself? "not important" (2), "somewhat important"(I), "very important" (0) How often did your parents sayyou shouldn't do the bad things people do on TV? "often" (2), "sometimes" (1), "never" (0)

Parental interpretation of TV violence:"When you watched action-ad- venture shows with your parents, how often did they usedto say these things if someone in the story was hurt badly, duringwesterns and crime shows?" 214

10,-yr..ftAer.,, 46, ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 207 Told you that things are not like thisin real life. Said that these stories are "justpretend." Explained that there are better waysthan violence to solve problems. Said you shouldn't do the bad thingspeople do on TV. Reminded you that the people on TV arejust actors and not really get- ting hurt. Scoring"often" (2), "sometimes" (I),"never" (0)

Parental punishment: "How often do yourparents do these things with you?" Punish you by grounding you. Punish you by taking away yourprivileges. Punish you by yelling at you. Punish you by lecturing you. Punish you physically when you were younger. Scoring"very often" (3), "fairlyoften" (2), "not too often" (I), "never" (0)

Parental affection: "How often do your parentsdo these things with your Show that they love you. "veryoften" (3), "fairly often" (2), "not too often" (I), "never" (0)

Number of siblings:

How many brothers do you have? How many are older than How many sisters do you have? How many are older than you?____

Father occupation:

What kind of work does your father (orstepfather) do for a living? What is his job called, what kind ofbusiness or industry does he work in and what does he do? For example: "Salesclerk, waits on customers in a department store" or "Weaver, operates aloom in a cotton textile mill." ScoringCoded according to DuncanSocio-Economic Index, ranging from "Osteopath" (96) to "Textile MillLaborer" (01)

Mother education:

How much education did your motherhave? "college graduate" (5), "some college" (4), "high schoolgraduate" (3), "some high school" (2), "grade school only" (I)

. 215 208 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Cognitive Reactions

Perceived learning of aggression: "Here are some things other students say about TV programs with lots of action and adventure. like Westerns and crime shows. We want to know how much each statement is like you: how well does each statement describe your feelings about this kind of program?" These programs show me how to get back at people who make me an- gry. ! Sometimes I copy the things I see people doing on these shows. Some programs give me ideas on how to get away with something with- out getting caught. When someone attacks another person and isn't punished. I sometimes feel I can get away with it too. When the bad guy gets a beating he deserves, I sometimes feel like get- ting even with people who have bothered me. Scoring"a lot like me" (2), "a little like me" (I), "not like me" (0) Linkage of TV violence to real life: "Here are some things other stu- dents say about TV programs with lots of action and adventure, like Westerns and crimt.: shows. We want to know how much each statement is like you: how well does each statement describeyour feelings about this kind of program?" Action and adventure shows tell about life the way it really is. The people I see in adventure stories are just like the people I meet in real life. Some stories remind me of frustrating things that have happened to me. Some characters remind me of people who have made me mad.

Involvement in violent TV programming: "Here are some things other students say about TV programs with lots of action and adventure, like Westerns and crime shows. We want to know how much each statement is like you: how well does each statement describe your feelings about this kind of program?" I am so involved in some programs that I get carried away with the sto- ry. I get upset when my favorite star is yelled at or threatened. I sometimes forget that characters in these shows are just actors playing roles. I get excited when I watch these programs. Once in a while I feel like things that happen to my hero are really hap- pening to me. I pay close attention to these shows. Scoring"a lot like me" (2), "a little like me" (I), "not like me" (0) ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 209 Identification with violent characters:

Name the One rerson on television who youw1ould most like to be, Men- tioned violent male (2), mentioned aggressive female (I),mentioned other character (0)

Which actors do you most like to see at the movies? "John Wayne"(2), "Jim Brov'n" (2), "Clint Eastwood" (2), "Sidney Poitier" (I),"Paul New man" (I), "Dustin Hoff man" (0).

Perceived efficacy of violent characters: "Here are some descriptionsof what happens on action and adventure programs. Howoften does each of these things happen on the shows that you watch?" The hero's friends think it is O.K. if he hurts the bad guy. The guy who gets rough gets his way. The bad guy deserves the beating he gets.

Scoring"often" (2), "sometimes" (I), "never" (0)

School Performance:

What are your average grades in school? "A's" (5), "A's" andB's" (4), "B's" (3), "B's and C's" (2), "C's" (I), "less than C's" (0)

Maryland: Track in school (Teacher report). "above average"(3), "av- erage" (2), "below average" (I)

Wisconsin: Ability estimate (Teacher report). Please estimate the over- all scholastic ability of this student. "superior" (4), "above average(3), "average" (2), "below average" (I), "poor" (0) 210 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Appendix B: Mean violence ratings of individual television programs by three sets of judges Assigned weight* Violent programs Public Students Critics (N=303)(N=41) (11=37) 7 Manna 3.37 3.26 3.91 7 The F. B. I. 3.19 3.41 3.79 7 Mod Squad 3.56 3.05 3.65 7 Hawaii Five-0 3.24 3.31 3.81

6 Mission Impossible 3.35 3.06 3.55 6 Gunsmoke 3.16 3.09 3.49 6 It Takes a Thief 3.23 2.94 3.39

5 The Virginian 2.88 3.13 3.19 5 Ironside 2.95 3.00 3.00 5 Bonanza 2.90 2.91 3.11 5 The Name of the Game 2.77 3.00 3.05 5 Land of the Giants 2.69 2.83 3.47 5 High Chapparal 2.98 2.78 3.46 5 Lancer 2.67 2.82 3.32 5 Dragnet 2.98 2.83 2.78

4 Daniel Boone 2.43 2.82 2.91 4 Adam 12 2.72 4 Then Came Bronson 2.51 2.50 2.65

3 Get Smart 2.27 2.76 2.24

2 Here Come the Brides 1.85 2.03 2.32 2 Hogan's Heroes 1.81 2.19 2.07

1 Room 222 1.87 1.61 1.49 1 Marcus Welby. M.D. 1.61 1.87 1.91 1 World of Disney 1.56 1.62 2.04 1 Medical Center 2.27 1 Laugh-In 1.33 1.79 1.79 Note: Table values are mean ratings of eachprogram along a five-step scale, ranging from 5 (a lot of violence) to 1 (none at all). * Assigned weight: In computing the Overall Violence Viewing Index, each of these programs was assigned the indicated weighting of violent content, based on the average weighting across the three sets of judges. -218 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 211

Nonviolent programs Public Students Critics (N=303)(N=41) (N=37) Love, American Style 1.40 1.41 1.39 Lassie 1.34 - 1.88 Red Skelton 1.24 1.50 1.51 Jackie Gleason 1.22 1.56 1.38 Let's Make a Deal - l.38 - The Newlywed Game - 1.59 - MyWorld and Welcome to It 1.36 1.38 1.09 Bill Cosby 1.33 1.52 1.12 Dean Martin 1.30 1.46 1.28 The Ghost and Mrs. Muir 1.28 1.53 1.20 Tim Conway 1.26 - 1.24 The Governor and J.J. 1.24 - 1.11 Beverly Hillbillies 1.23 1 .70 1.25 Pat Paulsen 1.22 - 1.26 Nanny and the Professor 1.20 - 1.12 Here's Lucy 1.18 1.46 l.30 I Dream of Jeannie 1.16 1.46 1.46 To Rome With Love 1.35 1.24 1.07 The Flying Nun 1.18 1.50 1.17 Debbie Reynolds 1.18 1.24 1.39 Bewitched 1.17 1.42 1.15 Tom Jones 1.17 1.43 1.07 Brady Bunch 1.17 1.50 1.14 ThatGirl 1.1 1.44 1.14 Mayberry R.F.D. 1.27 1.26 1.08 Petticoat Junction 1.13 1.44 1.16 Hee Haw 1.07 - 1.31 Julia 1.18 1.28 1.14 Green Acres 1.14 1.45 1.14 Johnny Cash 1.12 - 1.07 Jim Nabors 1.11 1.28 1.13 Glen Campbell 1.10 1.35 1.05 Eddie's Father 1.09 1.25 1.02 Carol Burnett 1.09 1.26 1.29 My Three Sons 1.07 1 .26 1.02 AndyWilliams 1.07 1 .36 1.19 Family Affair 1.06 1.21 1.02

Note: Above programs are all assigned a weighting of 0.

g1.9 212 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Appendix C: Tables

Table 1: Standardized adolescent aggression levels, by age and sex: Maryland data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Manifest physical aggression jr. hi +45** 02 (6 item index) sr. hi +13 44" Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hi +50" 29" (3 item index) sr. hi +26" 37** Zaks-Walters aggression jr. hi +01 +02 (7 item index) sr. hi 08 +05 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi +69" 16 (4 item index) sr. hi +27" 63" Overall aggression sum jr. hi +60** 16 (20 item index) sr. hi +21* 51" Buss-Durkee assault aggression jr. hi +37" +01 (4 item index) sr. hi +19* 40" Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi 20* +08 (3 item index) sr. hi 02 +12 Approval of aggression jr. hi +27" +18 (2 item index) sr. hi +12 44" (N) jr. hi (122) (108) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Standard scores represent each cell mean, calculated as a positive or negative devia- tion from the overall mean on the listed index, divided by the overall standard deviation. Scores have been multiplied by 100 to eliminate decimals. Asterisks indicate the cell mean is significantly different from the overall mean for the remaining cells. *p < .05 "p < .01 7 V,

ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 213

Table 2: Standardized adolercent aggression levels, by age and sex: Wisconsin data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Manifest physical aggression jr. hi +30* +21 (6 item index) sr. hi +24 70" Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hia (3 item index) sr. hi +21 23 Zaks-Walters aggression jr. hi +11 06 (7 item index) sr. hi +09 16 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi +62** +04 (4 item index) sr. hi +18 82" Overall aggression sum jr. hi +52" +11 (20 item index) sr. hi +24 Buss-Durkee assault aggression jr. hi +19 +13 (4 item index) sr. hi +38** Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi +05 +44" (3 item index) sr. hi 19 17 +39** Approval of aggression jr. hi +02 (2 item index) sr. hi +24 65" (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi 1431 1401

allot measured for junior high students.

Table 3: Intercorrelations among aggression indices: Maryland data

Aggression measure Correlations, all respondents

Physical Delinquency .56 Zaks-Walters .31 .22 Hypothetical .59 .54 .17 Overall sum .84 .79 .50 .83 Assault .87 .49 .24 .53 .73 Irritability ,30 .16 .31 .07 .27 .28 Approval .42 29 .14 .40 .38 .42 .12

(N = 473)

Note: Cell entries are zero-order Pearsonian r correlation coefficients. For r.r.?._ .09, p < .05 r p < .01 214 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 4:I ntercorrelations among aggression indices: Wisconsin data Aggression measure Correlations, all respondents Physical Delinquency .26 - - Zaks-Wal ters .13 .32 - - Hypothetical .33 .25 .10 - - Overall sum .74 .62 .46 .76 - - Assault .83 .30 .10 .38 .69 I rritability .34 -.02 .05 .22 .29 .29 Approval .27 .12 .29 .36 .44 .24 -.06

(N = 151)

Note: Cell entries are zero-order Pearsonian r correlation coefficients. For r .16, p < .05 r .? .21, p < .01

Table 5: Standardized adolescent television viewing levels, by age and sex: Maryland data

TV viewing measure Grade Boys Girls Crime-detective programs jr. hi +40** -05 (11 show index) sr. hi +05 -36** Western programs jr. hi +33** +09 (7 show index) sr. hi - 14 -29** Adventure-drama programs jr. hi +34" +03 (6 show index) sr. hi - 08 -27" Comedy-variety programs jr. hi +29" +18 (14 show index) sr. hi - 11 -30" Situation comedy programs jr. hi +20* +63" (25 show index) sr. hi - 66" -15 Game programs Jr. hi +17 +51" (2 show index) sr. hi - 46" -20* Overall violence viewing jr. hi +38" +01 (65 show index) sr. hi - 00 News-public affairs programs jr. hi +31" -33" (4 item index) sr. hi +15 -13 Saturday morning programs jr. hi +50" +47" (number of hours) sr. hi -40" -47" Total viewing time jr. hi +17 +17 (3 item index) sr. hi -13 -17* Violent TV movies jr. hi +34** -35" (5 item index) sr. hi 4.38**

(N) jr. hi (122) (108) sr. hi (107) (136)

22 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 215

Table 6: Standardized adolescent television viewing levels, by age and sex: Wisconsin data TV viewing measure Grade Boys Girls Crime-detective programs jr. hi +24 +13 (11 show index) sr. hi +12 -45" Western programs jr. hi +21 +06 (7 show index) sr. hi -15 -08 Adventure-drama programs jr. hi +06 +19 (6 show index) sr. hi -10 -10 Comedy-variety programs jr. hi -00 +36* (14 show index) sr. hi -07 -19 Situation comedy programs jr. hi +04 +74** (25 show index) sr. hi -46** -14 4.48** Game programs jr. hi -13 (2 show index) sr. hi -21 -01 Overall violence viewing jr. hi +22 +09 (65 show index) sr. hi +05 -33* Newspublic affairs programs jr. hi -07 -14 (4 item index) sr. hi +16 00 Saturday morning programs jr. hi +28" +77** (number of hours) sr. hi -24 -59** Total viewing time jr. hi +06 +29 (3 item index) sr. hi +24 -54*" Violent TV movies jr. hi -18 -32* (5 item index) sr. hi +57" -18 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40)

223

_ 216 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 7: Intercorrelations among television viewing indices: Maryland data TV viewing measure Correlations, all respondents Crime-detective Western .56 - - Adventure-drama .69 .52 - - Comedy-variety .53 .43 .54 - - Situation comedy .47 .44 .58 .48 - - Game .33 .30 .34 .31 .57 - - Overall violence .94 .77 .76 .56 .53 .37 - - News .17 .09 .15 .22 .01 .03 .16 - - Saturday morning .30 .34 .32 .28 .42 .32 .35-.02 - - Total time .34 .27 .39 .33 .41 .29 .37-.02 .33 TV movies .31 .23 .25 .16 .01 .01 .32 .18 .07 .18 > 0) 'a c a) c > ... ..7. cE) u o o (N = 473) .a) u E c ? E 0 cii a) 7.) 12 ... co P. E m To ? E .., cuw g: E C.) u) (.1 Zu u) I- For r = .09, p < .05

r ?* .12, p < .01

Table 8: Intercorrelations among television viewing indices: Wisconsin data TV viewing measure Correlations, all tespondents Crime-detective - - Western .45 - - Adventure-drama .48 .38 - - Comedy-variety .23 .20 .37 - - Situation comedy .31 .23 .49 .54 - - Game .15 .07 .40 .32 .55 -- Overall violence .92 .73 .61 .29 .36 .18 - - News .06 .10 .07 .19 .08-.10 .08 - - Saturday morning .19 .26 .29 .18 .31 .11 .25 .09 Total time .25 .30 .24 .37 .22 .07 .32 .15 .34 TV movies .18 -.02 .06 .04-.06-.03 .12 .20-.03 .16

a)

(N = 151) E w 2 For r = .16, p < .05 r?.21,p<.01

224 .:

DOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION usE 217

Table 9: Standardized adolescent family variable levels, by age and sex: Maryland data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +29" +29" (6 item index) sr. hi 23* 29" Parental emphasis on non-aggression jr. hi +19* +09 (4 item index) sr. hi 08 16 Parental interpretation of TV viotence jr. hi +21* +26" (5 item index) sr. hi 31" 14 Parental physical punishment when jr. hi +04 19* younger (1 item) sr. hi +ve. 14 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi 20* 14 (2 item index) sr. hi +22* +10 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +35** 10 (2 item index) sr. hi 02 18* Parental affection jr. hi +26" +27" (1 item) sr. hi 55" +05 Number of siblings jr. hi +01 +25" sr. hi 11 12 Father occupation jr. hi +07 21* (Duncan SES scale) sr. hi +15 03 Mother education jr. hi +21* +30" (Number of years) sr. hi 25" 21* (N) jr. hi (122) (108) sr. hi (107) (136)

24.4 218 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Table 10: Standardized adolescent family variable levels, by age and sex: Wisconsin data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +20 +44" (6 item index) sr. hi -43** -09 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi +16 +35* (4 item index) sr. hi -23 -12 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi +17 +39* (5 item index) sr. hi -37** -06 Parental physical punishment when jr. hi +47** -41* younger (1 item) sr. hi +12 -25 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi -15 +22 (2 item index) sr. hi 00 -03 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +09 +16 (2 item index) sr. hi +11 -31* Parental affection jr. hi -04 +28 (1 item) sr. hi -13 -03 Number of siblings jr. hi -13 +04 sr. hi +20 -13 Father occupation jr. hi +14 +08 (Duncan SES scale) sr. hi -12 -07 Mother education jr. hi +28* +27 (Number of years) sr. hi -19 -26* (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40)

Table 11: Intercorrelations among family variable indices: Maryland data Family variable measure Correlations, all respondents Control over TV - - Nonaggression emphasis .12 - - TV interpretation .09 .39 - - Physical punishment .11 .04-.04 - - Verbal punishment .04 .02 .02 .32 - - Restrictive punishment .13 .16 .15 .27 .25 - - Affection .11 .11 .14-.18-.17-.12 - - Number of siblings .09 .01 .07-.01-.04-.09-.05 Father occupation -.04 .04-.08 .03 .02 .04 .03-.11 Mother education .07 .18 .05 .02 .01 .02 .13-.03 .18

(N = 473)

For r .09, p < .05 r .12, p < .01 226 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 219

Table 12: Intercorrelations among family variable indices: Wisconsin data f

Family variable measure Correlations, all respondents Control over TV Nonaggression emphasis .23 - - TV interpretation .29 .45 - - Physical punishment -.03-.02-.14 - - Verbal punishment -.06-.02-.05 .24 - - Restri ctive punishment .05 .02 00 .24 .40 - - Affection .17 .04 .21 -.14-.07-.06 - - Number of siblings .10 .10-.07-.11-.04 .05-.15- - Father occupation - .05 .08 .07-.03 .06-.07 .14-.11 Mother education -.03 .14 .04 .06 .16 .12 .13-.29 .41

= 151) I. a a .0 8 0 0 u CL w

LL

For r .16, p < .05

r>.21, p < .01

Table 13: Standardized adolescent levels of cognitive reaction to television violence, by age and sex: Maryland data

Reaction measure Grade Boys Girls

Perceived learning of aggression +31** +17 (5=item index) sr. hi +04 -42" Linkage of violent TV content to real life jr. hi +08 +14 (4=item index) sr. hi -05 -14 Involvement in violent programming jr. hi -14 +27** (6=item index) sr. hi -36** +18* jr. hi +61** -17 Identification with violent characters _60 (2=item index) sr. hi +45** Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi -05 -07 (3=item index) sr. hi +18* -04 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) sr. hi (107) (136) For r ? .09, p.05 r ?.12, p<.01 220 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Table 14: Standardized adolescent levels of cognitive reaction to television violence, by age and sex: Wisconsin data

Reaction measure Grade Boys Girls

Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +52" +25 _68 (5=item index) sr. hi 00 Linkage of violent TV content to real life jr. hi +33* +05 (4=item index) sr. hi -07 -27* Involvement in violent programming jr. hi +05 +25 (6=item index) sr. hi -21 00 Identification with violent characters jr. hi +47** -17 (2=item index) sr. hi +20 Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi +01 -06 (3=item index) sr. hi -01 +06 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40)

For r .16, p<.05

r = .21 , p<.01

Table 15: Intercorrelations among cognitive reaction indices: Maryland data

Reaction measure Correlations, all respondents

Learning - - Linkage .50 - - Involvement .24 .38 Identification .17 .01 .06 Efficacy .16 .22 .1 5 .06 ...c g a) '4 ol a) E 8 (N = 473) c o) a) :g co > 7. .F... ..w O C m c > m Ja) C '0 For r.09, p<.05

r .12, p<.01

Table 16: Intercorrelations among cognitive reaction indices: Wisconsin data

Reaction measure Correlations, all respondents

Learning - \ Linkage .45 Involvement .36 .39 Identification .29 .18 .10 Efficacy .30 .19 .35 .00

(N = 151)

E. .59'w

c> For r.16, p<.05

r p<.01

A

OMR& 41 Atloo II, 1..../.1Pao. ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 221 f

Table 17: Correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior: Maryland data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Manifest physical aggression jr. hi +.17* +.33 +.28 sr. hi +.23* +.15 Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hi +.08 +.13 +.22** sr. hi +.23* +.16

Zaks-Walters aggression jr. hi -.02 +.15 +.08 sr. hi +.15 +.04 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi +.12 +.20* +.32 sr. hi +.33 +.26 OVERALL AGGRESSION SUM jr. hi +.14 +.28" +.32 sr. hi +.31** +.21** (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each aggression variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

Table 18: Correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior: Wisconsin data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall

Manifest phYsical aggression jr. hi +.13 +.29 +.17* sr. hi +.06 -.06 Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hi - - - - +.20* - sr. hi +.12 +.21 Zaks-Walters aggression jr. hi +.19 +.14 +.24 sr. hi +.11 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi -.02 +.24 +.22** sr. hi +.28 +.06 +.30 OVERALL AGGRESSION SUM jr. hi +.12 +.38* sr. hi +.23 +.23 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each aggression variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

229 , TY) TEI.F.VISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRF.SSIVENF.SS

Table 19: Correlations between total viewing time and level of aggressive behavior: Maryland data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Manifest physical aggression jr. hi +.16 -.02 +.14 sr. hi +.14 +.18* Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hi +.12 -.07 +.08 sr. hi .00 +.20* ZaksWalters aggression jr. hi -.06 +.11 +.04 sr. hi +.04 +.03 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi +.26" +.10 +.21" sr. hi +.22* +.20* OVERALL AGGRESSION SUM jr. hi +.20* +.04 +.17** sr. hi +.14 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each aggression variable and the Total Viewing Time I ndex.

Table 20: Correlations between total viewing time and level of aggressive behavior: Wisconsin data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Manifest physical aggression jr. hi +.18 -.15 +.16* sr. hi +17 -.08 Aggressive behavioral delinquency jr. hi - - - - +.19* sr. hi +.15 +.11 Zaks-Walters aggression jr. hi +.55** +.27 +.27" sr. hi +.15 +.08 Hypothetical aggressive reactions jr. hi -.07 -.17 +.09 sr. hi -.06 +.04 OVERALL AGGRESSION SUM jr. hi +.25 -.13 +.23" sr. hi +.14 +.04 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) 140) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each aggression variable and the Total Viewing Time I ndex. tn*trflflnt ¶flIr`r,

ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 223 1

Table 21: Correlations between level of violence viewing and family variables: Maryland data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi -.08 -.06 +.05 sr. h I +.16 .00 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -.21* -.15 -.07 sr. hi -.04 -.04 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi +.13 +.09 +.15" sr. hi +.06 +.23" Parental physical punishment jr. hi -.04 -.09 -.02 sr. hi +.02 -.06 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi -.02 -.04 -.01 sr. hi .00 +.11 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi -.04 +.08 +.14 sr. hi +.15 +.09 Parental affection jr. hi +.04 +.07 +.03 sr. hi +.13 -.14 Number of siblings jr. hi +.06 +.21' +.12" sr. hi +.24* -.01 Father occupation jr. hi -.15 -.03 -.17 sr. hi -.37" -.16 Mother education jr. hi -.19 +.03 -.03 sr. hi -.06 -.05 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each family variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

231 114 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 22: Correlations between level of violence viewing and . family variables: Wisconsin data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +.09 -.08 +.10 sr. hi +.29 +.04 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -.17 +.08 +.01 sr. hi -.03 +.11 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi -.08 +.23 +.09 sr. hi +.12 +.08 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +.18 +.21 sr. hi +.15 +.06 I Parental verbal punishment jr. hi +.11 +.07 +.11 sr. hi +.02 +.20 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +.29 +.02 +.24" sr. hi +.05 +.41** Parental affection jr. hi +.04 -.05 +.04 sr. hi +.12 +.01 Number of siblings, jr. hi -.17 +.27 +.10 sr. hi +.11 +.20 Father occupation jr. hi -.46" -.38* -.25** sr. hi -.29 .00 Mother education jr. hi -.47" -.25 -.11 sr. hi -.04 -.04 MI jr. hi (38) (30) (1511 sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entires are zero-order correlation coefficients between each family variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

23" ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 225

Table 23: Correlations between level of overall aggression and family variables: Maryland data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Overall Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi -.15 +.04 +.02 si. hi +.02 -.0 5 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -.23** -.1 6 -.09* sr. hi -.01 -.21* Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi +.05 +.15 +.07 sr. hi -.07 +.1 3 Parental physical punishment jr. hi -.08 -.0 3 +.12" sr. hi +.22* +.19 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi +.12 -.01 +.17" sr. hi +.36** +.38*. Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +.22* +.20* +.26" sr. hi +.12 Parental affection jr. hi -.15 +.0 6 -.17" sr. hi -.21* -.36" Number of siblings jr. hi +.02 +.0 3 +.07 sr. hi +.16 +.10 Father occupation jr. hi +.01 +.0 3 +.02 sr. hi .00 -.1 2 Mother education jr. hi -.04 +.04 .00 sr. hi -.14 -.01 (N) jr. hi (122) (1 08) 14731 sr. hi 11071 11 361 Note: Cell entires are zero-order correlation coefficients between each family variable and the Overall Aggression Index.

. 233 226 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 24: Correlations between level of overall aggression and family variables: Wisconsin data

Family variable measure Grade Boys Girls Overall

Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +.15 +.08 +.02 sr. hi -.01 -.24 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -.02 +.16 +.08 sr. hi +.03 +.11 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi -.08 +.08 -.08 sr. hi -.28 -.08 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +.11 +.21 +.30`" sr. hi +.26 +.31' Parental verbal punishment jr. hi +.13 -.07 +.09 sr. hi +14 +.25 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +.09 -.08 +IV sr. hi +.25 +.17 Parental affection jr. hi +.22 -.14 -.09 sr. hi -.384". -.06. Number of siblings jr. hi -.33* +.23 +.08 sr. hi +.21 +.12 Father occupation jr. hi +.09 -.20 .00 sr. hi -.09 +.04 Mother education jr. hi +.03 -.29 +.09 sr. hi -.04 +.13 (N) Ir. hi (38) OM (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zeroorder correlation coefficient!' between each family variable and the Overall Aggressign Index.

234 INIVOCI

ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 227

Table 25: Correlations between level of overall aggression and cognitive reaction variables: Maryland and Wisconsin data

Cognitive reaction measure: Maryland Grade Boys Girls Overall Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.40" +.52 +.53 sr. hi +.56" +.411 Linkage of TV violence to real life jr. hi +.32 +.31* +.31** sr. hi +.38" +.30* Involvement in violent TV programming jr. hi +.271 +.28 +.12" sr. hi +.13 +.24* Identification with violent characters jr. hi -.17 -.11 +.22" sr. hi +.25" +.05 Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi -.09 +.10 +.13" sr. hi +.22* (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136)

Cognitive reaction measure: Wisconsin Grade Boys Girls Overall

Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.01 +.31 sr. hi +.14 +.14 Linkage of TV violence to real life jr. hi -.03 +.11 +.13 sr. hi +.02 +.12 Involvement in violent TV programming jr. hi +.12 +.29 +.08 sr. hi -.15 Identification with vic.....at characters jr. hi -.01 +.33 +.31" sr. hi +.04 +.31* Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi +.06 +.34 +.12 sr. hi +.04 +.28 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note:. Cell entires are zero-order correlation coefficients between each cognitive reaction variable and the Overall Aggression Sum.

. 235 228 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 2 6: Correlations between level of violence viewing and cognitive reaction variables: Maryland and Wisconsin data

Cognitive reaction measure: Maryland Grade Boys Girls Overall Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi 4.19" +.25" +.24" sr. hi +.09 +.18* Linkage of TV violence to real life jr. hi 4.34" +.30 sr. hi +.08 , +.31** Involvement in violent TV programming jr. hi +.16 +.19* 1-.14" sr. hi +.13 +.25" Identification with violent characters jr. hi +.16 +.01 sr. hi +.16 +.1 7* Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi +.09 +.03 sr. hi +.11 +.1 0 (N) jr. hi (122) (1 08) (473) sr. hi (107) (1 36)

Cognitive reaction measure: Wisconsin Grade Boys Girls Overall Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.12 +.29 sr. hi +.08 +.10 Linkage of TV violence to real life jr. hi +.17 +.18 sr. hi +.12 +.2 5 Involvement in violent TV programming jr. hi -.05 +.31 +.10 sr. hi +.07 . +.08 Identification with violent characters jr. hi 4.06 +.07 +.15 sr. hi +.23 -.03 Perceived efficacy of violent characters jr. hi +.16 +.16 +.03 sr. hi .00 -.1 5 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entires are zero-order correlation coefficients betweeneach cognitive reaction variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

236 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 229

Table 27: Correlations with socioeconomic status and school performance: Maryland data

Measure r with SES r with SP

Manifest physical aggression -.01 -.30!* Aggressive behavioral delinquency -.01 -.26** Zaks-Walters aggression -.03 -.34.-.11* Hypothetical aggressive reactions -.01 Overall aggression sum -.02 -.35" Buss-Durkee assault aggression +.01 Buss-Durkee irritability +.06 +.02 Approval of aggression -.09* -.35** Crime-detective programs -.12** Western prograMs -.22** Advenluredrama programs -.13" -.15 Comedyvariety programs -.16" Situation comedy programs -.21 Game programs. -.17** -.17 Overall violence viewing index -.17" -.24* L Newspublic affairs programs +.12" +.13" Saturday morning programs -.12 -.21" Total viewing time -.12 -.14**

Violent TV movies 1 +.06 -.03 Parental control over TV viewing -.04 +.01 Parental emphasis on nonaggression +.04 +.15 Parental interpretation of TV violence -.08 r.15 Parental physical punishment +.03 +.06 Parental verbal punishment +.02 +.02 Parental restrictive punishment +.04 ' ' -.06 Parental affection +.03 +.05 Number of siblings -.11* -.20" Mother education +.18" +.02 Perceived learning of aggression -.14" -.25 Linkage of TV violence to real life -.08 -.14 Involvement in violent programming I -.04 +.09* Identification with violent characters +.09* -.15 Perceived efficacy of violent characters -.04 .00 (N = 473)

237 230 'TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 28: Correlations with socioeconomic status and school performance: Wisconsin data

Measure r with SES r with SP Manifest physical aggression -.04 +.11 Aggressive behavioral delinquency +.08 -.02 Zaks-Walters aggression -.15 -.16* Hypothetical aggressive reactions +.08 +.12 Overall aggression sum .00 +.07 Buss-Durkee assault aggression -.01 +.13 BussDurkee irritability +.05 +.05 Approval of aggression -.13 -.17* Crime-detective programs -.16* -.08 Western programs -.26** -.13 Adventure-drama programs -.22* -.03 Comedy-variety programs -.29" +.06 Situation comedy programs -.04 +.17* Game programs -.09 +.04 Overall violence viewing index -.25" -.12 News-public affairs programs +.04 4.16* Saturday morning programs -.10 +.15 Total viewing time -.29' -.13 Violent TV movies +.06 -.14 Parental control over TV viewing -.05 +.18* Parental emphasis on nonaggression +.08 +.23** Parental interpretation of TV violence +.07 +.02 Parental physical punishment -.03 +.02 Parentrl verbal punishment +.06 -.04 Parental restrictive punishment -.07 -.06 Parental affection +.14 +.26" Number of siblings -.11 -.02 Mother education +.41" +.26" Perceived learning of aggression -.02 -.18* Linkage of TV violence to real life -.14 -.18* Involvement in violent programming +.01 +.02 Identification with violent characters -.16* -.21" Perceived efficacy of violent character +.03 .00 (N = 151)

2 as ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 231 f

Table 29: Partial correlations between level of violenceviewing and level of aggressive behavior, controlling total televisionviewing time: Maryland data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: r TV time r TV time r TV time

Physical jr. hi +.17 +.13 +.33 +.33** +.28+.26** sr. hi +.23+.20* +.15 +.08 Delinquency jr. hi +.08 +.04 +.13 +.14 +.22+.21** sr. hi +.23 +.25 +.16 +.08 Zaks-Walters jr. hi -.02-.02 +.15 +.14 +.08+.08 sr. hi +.15+.15 +.04+.03 Hypothetical jr. hi +.12 +.03 +.20+.19* +.32+.26** sr. hi +.33+.28* +.26 +.20 OVERALL SUM jr. hi +.18+.12 +.28 +.28** +.32+.28** sr. hi +.31 +.29* +.21 +.14 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlationcoefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Cell entriesin partial: TV time columns are partial r correlation coefficients between theViolence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for the TotalTV Time Index. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

Table 30: Partial correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior, controlling total television viewing time: Wisconsin data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: r TV time r TV time r TV time

Physical jr. hi +.13+.08 +.29 +.37* +.17+.12 sr. hi +.06 -.01 -.06-.05 Delinquency jr. hi - - - - _ - _ - +.20+.17 sr. hi +.12 +.06 +.21 +.20 ZaksWalters jr. hi +.19+.06 +.14 +.05 +.24+.17* sr. hi +.11 +.06 +.45 +.44` Hypothetical jr. hi -.02 .00 +.24 +.33 +.22 +.20* sr. hi +.28+.33* +.06 +.05 OVERALL SUM jr. hi +.12 +.06 +.38 +.47" +.30+.24** sr. hi +.23 +.19 +.23 +.23 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Cell entries in partial: TVtime columns are partial r correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for the Total TV Time Index. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

. 239

0...11.111RtION10.1~6..4~ .. . im.m..11... .Aiilhudl---- 11 232 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Table 31: Partial correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior, controlling socioeconomic status and school performance: Maryland data

Aggression Boys Girls Overall measure Grade raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: r SES SP r SES SP r SES SP

Physical jr. hi+.17+.17*+.17* +.33+.33**+.31**+.28+.28"+.23" sr. hi+23+.27-+.16 +.15+.13 +.10 Delinquency jr. hi+.08+.08 +.06 +.13+.13 +.11 +.22+.22**+.17" sr. hi+.23+.21+.13 +.16+.14 +.12 Zaks-Walters jr. hi-.02-.02 .00 +.15+.16 +.15 +.08+.07 +.06 sr. hi+.15+.15 +.09 +.04+.04 +.02 Hypothetical jr. hi+.12+.11 +.10 +.20+.20* +.19 +.32+.32**+.26" sr. hi+.33+.35**+.25**+.26+.25**+.20* OVERALL SUM jr. hi+.14+.14 +.11 +.28+.28"+.26**+.32+.32*+.26** SUM sr. hi+.31+.33**+.21* +.21+.19* +.16 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Cell entries in partial : SES columns are partial r correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for Father Occupation (Duncan SES Scale). Cell entries in partial: SPcolumns are partial r correlation coefficients controlling for the School Performance Index. Each control variable is partialled out separately in this analysis. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

Table 32: Partial correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior, controlling socioeconomic status and school performance: Wisconsin data

Aggression Boys Girls Overall measure Grade raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: SES SP r SES SP r SES SP

Physical jr. hi+.13+.17 +.21 +.29+.29 +.32 +.17+.16+.18* sr. hi+.06-.02 +.06 -.06-.06 -.08 Delinquency jr. hi - - +.20+.23"+.20* sr. hi+.12+.21 +.11 +.21+.21 +.20 Zaks-Walters jr. hi+.19+.10 +.14 +.14-.03 +.11 +.24+.22*+.23" sr. hi+.11+.09 +.12 +.45+.45"+.44 Hypothetical jr. hi-.02+.10 +.04 +.24+.23 +.25 +.22+.25"+.24* sr. hi+.18+.28 +.28 +.06+.06 +.04 OVERALL jr. hi+.12+.18 +.18 +.38+.34 +.40 +.30+.31"+.31" SUM sr. hi+.23+.21 +.23 +.23+.23 +.21 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Cell entries in partial: SES columns are partial r correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for Father Occupation (Duncan SES Scale). Cell entries inpartial: SPcolumns are partial r correlation coefficients controlling for the School Performance Index. Each control variable Is partialled out separately in this analysis. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only. 4

vanen*,,, r.,7-3,ww.a4,7re,nnt.

ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 233

Table 33: Partial correlations between level of aggressive behavior and viewing of seven types of television programming

Viewing measure All respondents, Maryland All respondents, Wisconsin raw r partial r raw r partial r

Crime-detective programs +.32 +.16"" +.33 +.30* Adventure-drama programs +.30 +.11" +19 +.08 Saturday morning programs +.19 +.09* +.22 +.23" Game programs +.18 +.07 +.02 +.06 Western programs +.20 .01 +.12 .07 Comedy-variety programs +.17 .03 +.08 +.09 Situation comedy programs +.18 .05 .05 .26" (N) (473) (151) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and each television viewing variable, controlling for all other television viewing vari- ables in the set. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

241 234 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 34: Multiple predictors of aggressive behavior: violence viewing and selected third variables: Maryland data

I ndependent variables Grade Boys Girls Overall

Violence viewing (OVV) jr. hi +.14 +.28 +.32 (raw r) sr. hi +.31 +.21 OVV and Parental emphasis on jr. hi 22 .30 .32 nonaggression (multiple r) sr. hi .31 .29 OVV and BussDurkee irritability jr. hi .31 .50 .42 sr. hi .52 .33 OVV and Parental physical punishment jr. hi .15 .28 .34 sr. hi .38 .29 OVV and Parental verbal punishment jr. hi .18 .28 .36 sr. hi .48 .41 OVV and Parental restrictive jr. hi .24 .33 .39 punishment sr. hi .32 .35 OVV and Parental affection jr. hi .30 .28 .36 sr. hi .39 .40 OVV and Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi .37 .54 .56 sr. hi .62 .43 OVV and linkage of TV violence to real jr. h 1 .29 .36 .39 life sr. hi .48 .33 OVV and Involvement in violent TV jr. hi .26 .37 .33 programming sr. hi .32 .29 OVV and Identification with violent jr. hi .14 .30 .35 characters sr. hi .38 .21 OVV and Father occupation SES jr. hi .14 .28 .32 sr. hi .31 .11 OVV and School performance jr. hi .32 .34 .43 sr. hi .45 .38 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are multiple correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and the Violence Viewing Index and each third variable. one at a time. Multiple correlations greater than the raw correlation indicate the added contribution of the family and TV reaction variables to the relationship between aggression and viewing. Significance levels era not indicated. ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 235

Table 35: Multiple predictors of aggressive behavior: violence viewing and selected third variables: Wisconsin data

I ndependent variables Grade Boys Girls Overall Violence viewing (OVV) jr. hi +.12 +.38 +.30 (raw r) sr. hi +.23 +.23 OVV and Parental emphasis on jr. hi .12 .40 .31 nonaggression (multiple r) sr. hi .23 .24 OVV and Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi .65 .41 .41 sr. hi .23 .48 OVV and Parental physical punishment jr. hi .15 .41 .39 sr. hi .33 .38 OVV and Parental verbal punishment jr. hi .17 .40 .30 sr. hi .27 .31 OVV and Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi .13 .39 .32 sr. hi .33 .24 OVV and Parental affection jr. hi .25 .40 .31 sr. hi .47 .24 OVV and Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi .12 .43 .40 sr. hi .26 .26 OVV and Linkage of TV violence to jr. hi .13 .39 .30 real life sr. hi .23 .24 OVV and Involvement in violent TV jr. hi .18 .42 .30 programming sr. hi .28 .38 OVV and Identification with violent jr. hi .12 .49 .40 characters sr. hi .23 .39 OVV and Father occupation SES jr. hi .20 .39 .31 sr. hi .23 .23 OVV and School performance jr. hi .26 .40 .31 sr. hi .24 .24 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are multiple correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and the Violence Viewing Index and each third variable, one at a time. Multiple correlations greater than the raw correlation indicate the added contribution of the family and TV reaction variables to the relationship between aggression and viewing. Significance levels are not indicated.

243 236 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 36: Multiple regression analysis of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental punishment and affection: Maryland data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Violence viewing index jr. hi +.19* +.27** +.32" sr. hi +.30" +17* Parental punishment sum jr. hi +.14 +.10 sr. hi +.29** Parental affection jr. hi .15 +.05 .14" sr. hi .23* .24" Multiple correlation jr. hi .27 .30* .42" sr. hi .46" .49** (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients betweenthe Overall Aggression Sum and each family and viewing variable, controlling for all other independent variables. The Multiple correlation entries represent the total relationshipwith the entire set of independent variables.

Table 37: Multiple regression analysis of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental punishment and affection: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall

Violence viewing index jr. hi +.05 +.37 sr. hi +.27 +.16 Parental punishment sum jr. hi +.24 +.04 sr. hi +.29 +.30 Parental affection jr. hi +.30 .13 .07 sr. hi .42" +.09 Multiple correlation jr. hi .34 .40 35" sr. hi .53" .38 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151 ) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and each family and viewing variable, controlling for all other independent variables. The Multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship withthe entire set of independent variables.

244

_ vemnsew sevewarc.KOIZCCMSCIWT37,1

ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 237

Table 38: Grand multiple regression analysis of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing parental affection and punishment, child's irritability, and child's perceived ( learning of aggression from television: Maryland data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Violence viewing index jr. hi +.14 +.22* +.26* sr. hi +.29** +.15 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.29** +.46" +45 sr. hi +.46** +.34" BussDurkee irritability jr. hi +.21* +.41" +.22** sr. hi +.34" +.16 Parental punishment sum jr. hi +.11 +.03 +.18 sr. hi +.16 +.27" Parental affection jr. hi -.09 .00 sr. hi -.18 -.18* Multiple correlation jr. hi .48** .64** sr. hi .69** (N) jr. hi (122) (107) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and each independent variable, controlling all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with ihe entire set of independent variables.

Table 39: Grand multiple regression analysis of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental affection and punishment, child's irritability, and child's perceived learning of aggression from television: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Violence viewing index jr. hi +.16 +.33 +.24** sr. hi +.27 +.20 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.06 +.21 +.28 sr. hi +.07 +.12 Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi +.60** +.12 +.28 sr. hi -.02 +44** Parental punishment sum jr. hi +.09 -.01 +.11 sr. hi +.29 +.27 Parental affection jr. hi -.01 -.09 -.11 sr. hi -.40* +.11 Multiple correlation jr. hi .66 .46 .51 sr. hi .53 .56* (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Overall Aggression Sum and each independent variable, controlling all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables.

. _245 238 TEl.EV ISION AN D A DO I. ESCENT AGGRESSI V EN ESS

Table 40: Correlations between level of violence viewing and level ofaggressive behavior, at high vs. tow parental emphasis on nonaggressive behavior: Maryland data

Boys Girls Aggression measure Grade Overall Low High Low High LowHigh Physical jr. hi +.16 +.21 +.52+.05 sr. hi +.34+.10 +.07+.19 +.37 +.19 Delinquency jr. hi +.094..04 +.20+.02 +.30 sr. hi +.46+.13 +.19+.11 +.12 Zaks-Walters jr. hi +.09-.01 +.31-.07 sr. hi +.42-.02 +.24-.09 +.16-.05 Hypothetical jr. hi +.25+.09 +.23+.08 +.44 sr. hi +.40+.28 +.35+.11 +.18 OVERALL SUM jr. hi +.21 +.11 +A5+.03 sr. hi +.54+.17 +.30+.11 +.43+.17 (N) jr. h i (45) (50) (49) (42) (185) sr. hi (38) (64) (53) (82) (238) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between theViolence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Respondents were sorted into Highvs. Low groups on the Parental Emphasis on Nonaggression Index by cutting at the midpoint within each grade level. For the junior high group, Low = 0-3 and High= 4-8; for the senior high group. Low = 0-2 and High = 3-8. Higherscores indicate a greater parental stress on non-violent responses to social conflict situations.

Table 41: Correlations between level of violence viewing and level of aggressive behavior, at high vs. low parental emphasis on nonaggressive behavior: Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Aggression measure Grade Low High Low High LowHigh Physical jr. hi +.22-.17 +.25+.35 sr. hi -.10+.22 +.11-.33 +.22 +.07 Delinquency jr. hi ------.09 sr. hi -.44+.64 +.13+.41 +.49 Zaks-Walters jr. hi +.09+.32 +.30-.22 sr. hi -.10+.32 +.26 +.66 +.19+.33 Hypothetical jr. hi +.02-.08 +.39+.05 +.33 sr. hi +.19+.40 +.32-.21 +.02 OVERALL SUM jr. hi +.15+.03 +.42+.31 +.34 sr. hi -.12+.56 +.30 +.10 +.23 (N) jr. hi (22) (15) (15) (14) (82) sr. hi (23) (19) (22) (17) (65) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Respondents were sorted into Highvs. Low groups on the Parental Emphasis on Nonaggression Index by cutting at the midpoint within each grade level. For the junior high group, Low = 0-3 and High = 4-8; for the senior high group, Low = 0-2 and High = 3-8. Higher scores indicatea greater parental stress on non-violent responses to social conflict situations.

246 Adolescents, Parents, and f Television Use: Self-report and Other-report Measures from the Wisconsin Sample

Jack M. McLeod, Charles K. Atkin, and Steven H. Chaffee University of Wisconsin

This is the second and final report of research into the relationships among three sets of variables: adolescent aggression, television viewing behavior, and structural attributes of family social environment) This report contains data gathered from 151 adolescents and their mothers in Middleton, Wisconsin. Our previous report contained data comparable between the Wisconsin sample and a second sample of 473 adolescents in Prince Georges County, Maryland. The following topics are included in this final report: an analysis of modeling or mother-child similarity in both aggressive behavior and vio- lence viewing: the relationship of self-report measures of aggression to

239

. 247 240 TELEVISION AND A DOLESCENT AGG R ESSI V EN ESS

the ratings by mothers. peers and teachers.. mother-child communication as a family environment variable: the association of violence viewing and self-report and other-reportmeasures of aggressive behavior; and the relationship of the family environmentto *other variables. The report goes on to examine the role ofpeers; mother-child coorien- tation and its connection with aggressive behavior and violenceviewing: the time-order problem and causal inferences; andthe differential asso- ciations of westerns and other types of violent programming.The report concludes with a series of multivariate analyses. using violenceviewing. cognitive reactivity to television violence, approval ofaggression, and aggressive behavior as criteria. This second report introducesmany variables that were measured only in the Wisconsin surveys. In addition,a number of the previously discussed indices have been expanded to include data fromthe parental interviews. In all other cases, the original child self-report indicesfrom the Maryland-Wisconsin study are carriedover into the present analyses to provide for maximum comparability to the earlier findings.

PROCEDURES AND MEASURES Sampling

Since the details of the sampling procedureswere given in the first report (McLeod, Atkin, and Chaffee. 1971). we shall only briefly sketch the Wisconsin sampling here. In October of 1969, 225 adolescentscom- pleted questionnaires in two schools in Middleton, Wisconsin.During the same month. personal interviewswere conducted with the mothers of these children. One year later,a different set of interviewers returned to reinterview 151 of the mothers and to administer questionnairesto 68 seventh graders and 83 tenth graders. We will referto these groups as the "junior high" and "senior high" subsamples. Measures: adolescent self-report of aggression

Most measures of self-reported adolescent aggressionwere the same for both our Wisconsin and Maryland samples.These measures were discussed in our first report (this volume) and listed infull in Appendix A of that paper. Here we will simply mentionthe measures used earlier and concentrate on indices unique to the Wisconsinsample and intro- duced here for the first time. Overall aggression sum. Our best singlemeasure of adolescent aggres- sive behavior is the overall aggressionsum(OAS).It is a 20-item index made up of the following subindices: manifest physicalaggression (six items); aggressive behavibral delinquency (three items);Zaks-Walters 248 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 241

aggression (seven items); and hypothetical aggressive reactions (four items). The same items were used in the first report. Buss-Durkee assault aggression. The four-item index of assault ag- gression, adapted from Buss and Durkee (1957), was also used in our earlier report. Buss-Durkee verbal aggression. Data from our measures of verbal aggression have not been reported previously. Four Buss-Durkee items were adapted by using the "a lot like me" response scale: a.I demand that people respect my rights. b. When people yell at me, I yell back. c. When people disagree with me, I can't help getting into arguments. d.I would rather give in than argue about something. (reversed scor- ing). Buss-Durkee irritability. The three items indexing irritability were the same Buss-Durkee items as used previously. Buss-Durkee overall sum. The three indices using the 11 Buss-Durkee items were combined in this report for the mother-child modeling ana- lyses. Approval of aggression. Two items relating to the s4nctioning of ag- gression as a means of resolving conflict are repeated from our first re- port. Aggressive attitudes. An index extending the approval of aggression index is used here for the first time. Five new items are added, all rated along a five-steP agreement scale: a. The most successful people are the ones who use violence. b.It upsets me when I see someone beating up ariother kid. (reversed scoring). c. The rising amount of crime and violence in this country really doesn't bother me very much. d.If a student is fed up with his government these days, he is some- what justified if he sets off a bomb in an empty Army building: e. During the American Revolution, blowing up British buildings was completely justified.

Measures: mother self-report of aggression

It is obvious that it would be inappropriate to ask the mother ques- tions from such indices as behavioral delinquency and the hypothetical situations. In all, 20 of the 31 adolescent aggression items were used for the mother. The following indices were formed; (a) Buss-Durkee As- sault Aggression (4 items); (b) Buss-Durkee Verbal Aggression (4 items); (c) Buss-Durkee Irritability (3 items); (d) Buss-Durkee Overall Sum (11 items, sum of three indices above); (e) Approval of Aggres- sion (2 items); and (f) Zaks-Walters Aggression (7 items).

249 242 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENF.SS Measures: other-ratings of adolescent aggression In order to cross-validate our adolescent self-report measures. we obtained ratings of aggression for each child from three sets of others: peers, teachers, and the child's mother. While none of the three is a very precise measurement source. we feel the three other-reports combined produces a more solid basis of comparison to the self-report measures. Peers.Each adolescent in the Wisconsin sample was given a list of ten classmates and was asked to rate each on three items representing the Buss-Durkee irritability, assault, and verbal dimensions: a.Is patient with others. (reverse-scored for irritability) b. When loses temper. hits other people. (assault aggression) c. Yells back when yelled at. (verbal aggression) The rating was done on a three-point scale of "often," "sometimes," and "never." The total score for each adolescent was averaged for each dimension of aggression by dividing by the number of people rating him (approximately ten raters). Most children were known by almost all rat- ers; however, anyone not well known enough to be rated received "?" and this was not counted in averaging the ratings. Teachers.Because only the sixth-grade teachers taught a given stu- dent more than one period each day. teacher ratings of aggression were not obtained for the tenth graders. Each student was rated on a four- point scale with these anchors: highly aggressive-hostile, troublemaker; more aggressive-hostile than average; average; and very passive, gets along well, submissive. Mothers.A four-item index is used for the mother's report of her child's aggressive behavior. All questions were asked in the 1969 inter- view with the mother. The items: a. Does (name of child) seem to get into more fights than other chil- dren his age, fewer fights, or about the same amount of fights? (fewer= 0, same= I, more = 2) b. When (name of child) was younger, how often did (he, she) do mean things to other children while playing? Would you say often, sometimes, or never? (never= 0, sometimes= 1, often =2) c. When (he, she) was younger, how often did (he, she) show aggres- sive behavior toward other children? Would you say often, some- times, or never? (never= 0, sometimes= 1, often= 2) d.If your child had an argument with (his,her) best friend, how would (he,she) normally go about settling it? (nonaggressive response=0, physical aggression=2) Measures: adolescent television viewing levels Both the Wisconsin mothers and their children were asked to indicate how frequently each watched various prime time television shows. Each

k 50 Pelre.):171,...,,...V.-^711 I

ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 243

show was checked according to a four-step scale offrequency. The47 shows on the list in the 1969 survey for both mother and child were .grouped into six manifest content categories for the mother-child model- ing analyses... The categories and number of shows in each were: (a) Crime-detective (eight shows); (b) Westerns (seven shows); (c) Adven- ture-drama (eight shows); (d) Comedy-variety (nine shows); (e)Situa- tion comedy (ten shows); (f) News (four shows). Overall violence viewing.The same 65 program index of violence viewing(OVV)that was used in the first report is also used for the ado- lescents' television behavior here. The violence weighting for each show is based on the ratings of samples of high school students (Murray, Cole, and Fed ler, 1970) and of adults and .television critics(Greenberg and Gordon, 1970). Total viewing time.The earlier estimates of adolescents' total televi- sion viewing time were supplemented by mother reports of the number of hours the child viewed television on an average day and on the day preceding the interview. The child provided data regarding time spent yesterday, the day before yesterday, and on an average day. Television violence viewing when younger.In the 1970 questionnaire, the Wisconsin adolescents were asked how frequently they had watched each of 13 shows that were on television three or four years ago. Noneis still on the air. The shows were weighted according to their level of vio- lence by the research team. A violence viewing index for this era was obtained by summing the child's viewing level for each show weighted by the show's level of violent content. The violent content groupings:

Very high violence High violence Low yiolence

Combat I SO Gilligan's Island Manfrom UNCLE Felony Squad Dr. Kildare Rat Patrol Big Valley Gentle Ben Rawhide The Fugitive Donna Reed Patty Duke

Measures: mothers' television viewing levels In addition to giving the frequency with which they viewed the 47 spe- cific television programs, the mothers also estimated their total view.ing time on an average day and the day before the interview. Measures: family environment By including data from the mother, rpuch more reliable and probably valid assessment of the family environment was made possible for the

251: 244 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Wisconsin sample. This also allowed us to compare the views of the ad- olescents and their mothers through the coorientational measures of accuracy and agreement. Parental control over television. The six items used in the first report to index the degree of parental control over the child's viewing werein- creased to 28 with the repeating of items, inclusion of new items, and data from the mother on most items. Some new items: a. Do (your parents, you) set limits on the amountof time (you, he) watches television? (yes, no) b. (Mother only) How strict are you in controlling what your child is viewing (very lenient, fairly lenient, average, fairly strict, very strict) Parental emphasis on nonaggression. Repetition of items in both years, new items, and responses from mothers served toincrease the measurement of parental nonaggression emphasis from four to 18 items. Included as new items were the mothers' desired behavior for their chil- dren in two of the situations used in the hypothetical aggressive reaction index for the child. Parental interpretation of television violence. The five items referring to parental interpretation of television's "unreality.' were alsoasked of the mother, thus doubling the number of items in that index. Parental punishment. The five items indexing physical, verbal, and restrictive punishment were also asked of the mother. Thus, physical punishment has two items in this sample, while verbal and restrictive punishment have four each. Parental affection. The single measure of affection used earlier was considerably improved by adding two items and adding in the mothers' responses to all three items. Again, a four-step scale offrequency was used. The new items: a. Show (their, your) affection by hugging andkissing (you, him). b. Tell (you, him) that (they, you) love (you, him). Family communication patterns. The measurement of the structure of parent-child communication, which was not included in the first report, requires a more extensive explanation because it differs markedly in conceptualization and complexity of measurement from other variables. The measures have been developed in a series of studies where the de- pendent variables were various aspects of political socialization (Mc- Leod. Chaffee, and Eswara, 1966; Chaffee, McLeod, and Wackman, 1966; McLeod, Chaffee, and Wackman, 1967; McLeod, Rush, and Friederich, 1968-69; Stone and Chaffee, 1970; and Chaffee, McLeod, and Atkin, 1971). Their relation to violence viewing has been explored by Chaffee and McLeod (1971). In these various studies in a varietY of social settings, we have found that there are at least two dimensions in the structure of parent-child communication. The two dimensions, which are either.uncorrelated or 252 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 245

slightly positively related, are calledsocio-orientedandconcept-orient- edconstraints on the developing child. The socio-oriented emphasis is typified by encouraging the youngster to maintain harmonious interpersonal relations, avoid controversy, and suppress his inner feelings. Five items, asked of both the mother and her child, index this dimension. Some of the questions ask about thefre- quencyof the act, while others ask for an estimate of the degree ofem- phasis.The questions. with slight variations in wording for mother and child: a. (Parent) says (child) shouldn't argue with adults. b. (Parent) says (parent's) ideas are correct, and (child) shouldn't argue with them. c. (Parent) answers (child's) arguments by saying, "You'll know bet- ter when you grow up." d. (Parent) says that you should give in on arguments, rather than making people angry. e. (Parent) says there are some things that just shouldn't be talked about. Concept-oriented communication items deal with the frequency or emphasis of parental constraints on the child to express his own ideas, become exposed to controversy, and challenge others. The five items: a. (Parent) says that every member of your family should have some say in family decisions. b. (Parent) admits that kids know more about some things than adults do. c. (Parent) says (child) should always look at both sides of an issue. d. (Parent) says that getting (child's) ideas across is important, even if others don't like it. e. (Parent) asks for (child's) opinion when the family is discussing something. Although it is possible to treat each of these dimensions as a separate independent variable, we have found in some studies that the two inter- act, producing structural patterns that are not simply the sums of their two constituent functions. Therefore we treat family communication in terms of the following four types: a.Laissez-fairefamilies, which emphasize neither type of relation. Children are not prohibited from challenging parental views but neither are they exposed to the world of independent and contend- ing ideas. b.Protectivefamilies, which stress socio-relations only. The child is encouraged to get along with others, at the expense of concept-re- lations that would expose him to the controversial world of ideas. Not only is he prohibited from expressing dissent, but he is given little chance to enCounter information on which he might base his own views. 253 246 TEI.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVEN ESS Pluralistic families, which emphasize the development of strong and varied concept-relations in an environment comparatively free of social restraints. The child is encouraged to explore new ideas and is exposed to controversial material: he can make up his own mind without fear of endangering social relations with his parents. d Consensual families, which attempt to stress both orientations. While the child is exposed to controversy, and told he should enter into it. he is (paradoxically) constrained to develop con- cepts that are consonant with existing socio-relations. That is, he is in effect encouraged to learn his parents' ideas and adopt their values. Socioeconomic status. While the same measures of fathers' occupa- tion and mothers' education were used for both reports, a more complex scale of socioeconomic status has been added. The occupational and education measures are combined in approximately equal weighting to two other items. family income, and subjective social class. Measures: mother-child coorientational variables Our studies of family communication have also provided an empirical basis for a general model of coorientation (Chaffee and McLeod, 1970; McLeod and Chaffee, in press). This model also implies that we gather data about topics of mutual importance from both the mother and her child. The data include not only the measurement of the person's own view (perceptions, attitudes, values, etc.), but also the mother's esti- mate of the child's view and vice versa. Thus, four sets of views are involved, two for the mother and two for the child. The four sets of vieWs are combined into three coorientational measures: Agreement. A comparison of the difference between the child's own view and the mother's own view is called agreement. Obviously, there is only a single measure of agreement common to the mother and child. The smaller the difference, the greater is the agreement. Accuracy. The mother's estimate of the child's view compared with the child's own view forms the index of mother accuracy. The second measure, child accuracy, is taken from the child's estimate of mother's view compared to the mother's actual position. The smaller the discrep- ancy, the greater is the accuracy. Congruency. Each person's own view compared with his estimate of that of the other family member is called congruency. Independent meas- ures for the child and the mother are available; once more, the smaller the discrepancy, the greater the congruency. Strictly speaking, con- gruency is not a coorientational measure since it involves the views and 251 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 247 estimates of only one person at a time. Since it is part of the general coorientational model, we will keep congruency under the rubric of coorientation. Ideally, we would use a wide variety of important topics to obtain the judgments and estimates needed for the coorientational measures. In this case, limited time and space on our questionnaires forced us to set- tle on a single topicthe relative importance of various attributes the child might derive from his school experience. Fortunately, we were able to increase the reliability of measurement by using the same items in both years of the Wisconsin study. The mother and child were inde- pendently asked to rank four attributes to their importance (for your child to do, for yourself): (a) making new friends who share (child's) in- terests; (b) being part of school activities and social life; (c) learning new ideas and different ways of thinking; and (d) preparing to earn a living. After giving their own views, the mother and child estimated how the other would rank these same attributes. For each pair of measures, a difference score was computed for each year. The resulting sum over two years could range from zero (high) to 16 (low). Measures: cognitive reactions to television violence Three indices of cognitive reactions of adolescents to television vio- lence are included in this report. While all are measured identically to our explanation in our first report, we have added a new combined sum of these three measures. Perceived learning of aggression.The degree to which the adolescent feels television offers opportunity for him to learn antisocial behavior is again indexed by five item s using the "a lot like me" underlying scale. Linkage of violent television to real life.Four items are used to meas- ure the perception of similarity between the world portrayed on violent television programs and everyday reality. Involvement in violent programming.The final cognitive reaction in- dex used here is a six-item index of psychological involvement with the characters and stories of action-adventure shows. Total cognitive reactivity.Perceived learning, reality linkage, and involvement are combined into a I5-item index of total cognitive reactiv- ity. It will be used as a criterion variable in the last section of this report dealing with multiple regression analyses. Measure: peer integration. Riley and Riley (195U found that the child's lack of primary group ties with his peers is associated with a preference for violent media content. We have measured this with a five-item index of peer integration: a. Number of dose friends (none. one or two, three to five, six to ten, more than ten) 255

...... 111411, 248 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVEN ESS

h. Do you have a hunch of friends thatyou usually hang around with? (no. yes) c. Compared with the rest of the kids you know, wouldyou say you have more friends, less friends,or about the same number of friends? (less. same number, more) d. How do you prefer to spendyour time: alone, or in the company of friends? (usually like to be by myself, sometimes withfriends and sometimes by myself, usually like to he with friends) e. Does your child have a group of friends he usuallygoes around with? (Mother report: no. yes)

FINDINGS

In keeping with our first report. the presentation ofdata here will not attempt to test a set of formal hypotheses. Statisticaltests should be taken as rough indicators of which relationshipslook most promising and warrant further consideration. Caution shouldbe used in general- izing from these findings, becauseour Wisconsin sample is small and we lack the Maryland sample for the replication thatwas present in our first study. We will use asterisks to indicate significancelevels: * (p<.05). ** (p<.0 l). For correlations, these asterisks indicate the differenceof the correlation from zero, in either direction. All correlationsare Pearson product-moment coefficients (r). For tables showing standardizedlevels. the asterisks indicate the cellmean is significantly different from the overall mean for the remaining cells. Modeling analyses

Among the many possible explanations of children's behavior,model- ing. or the child's imitation of the parent's behavior, isthe most direct and has apparent simplicity. The simplicity ismore apparent than real. however. As we shall discuss later, thepresence of a correlation be- tween the behavior of mother and child allows fora variety of alterna- tive inferences. For the present. we will takeup the question of the de- gree of mother-child similarity in aggressive behavior and televisionuse. Modeling of aggression. Varying degrees ofev fence for modeling are shown in Table I. (All tables are in Appendix H.) Consistentsimilarity betw een mother and child is. shown for assault and verbalaggression. while irritability, a more covert attribute, showsno evidence for model- ing. Assuming that irritability ismore closely tied with physiological functioning, then we have little evidence that this attribute ispassed on through genetic transmission. We do have evidence thatmore overt behavioral manifestations of aggressionare passed on, presumably through environmental processes. 256 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 249 If modeling involves the learning of appropriate adult sex role, by the adolescent, then we would expect considerably higher mother-daughter correlations than those for mother and son. This is the case for almost all comparisons in Table I . clearly reversing only for approval of aggres- sion among the junior high group. Among boys. the similarity to the ag- gression level of the mother tends to lessen from junior to senior high. The sharp increase in mother-daughter similarity on assault aggression. from +.17 in junior high to a remarkable +.57 in senior high. suggests a possible inference that nonaggression rather than aggression is being modeled. As will be shown in Table 4. assault aggression declines very sharply for girls during the same year the level of "modeling" is going up. It might be argued. then, that the most likely changeis that of the daughter learning the relatively nonaggressive role of the mother. Some additional evidence that the modeling correlations of Table I. though mostly modest in magnitude. are not spurious comes from exam- ination of the full correlation matrix of mother and child indices. These data are not shown in our tables, but they do indicate generally higher mother-child correlations on the corresponding indices (e.g.. mothers' verbal aggression by daughters' verbal aggression) than on the cross- index comparisons (e.g.. mothers' verbal aggression by daughters' as- sault aggression). Overall, the average cross-index comlation is +.04. compared to an average correlation of +.14 for corresponding indices. In particular. there is a much stronger association between mother as- sault and child assault (+ .23) and mother verbal and child verbal ( +.23) than between mother assault and child verbal (+ .01) and mother verbal and child assault (+ .09). This reduces the likelihood that the modeling correlations are simply an artifact of their joint association with somethird variable. If this were the case, then a given aggression measure for mother would have corre- lations more equal in magnitude for all child aggression measures. Modeling of television behavior. Table 2 shows that the mother-child correlations for television viewing behavior are almost all positive. It is clear that the modeling correlations for types of shows are generally greater than those for time spent viewing television.Substantial correla- tions are also shown for the seven shows with the highestviolence rat- ings. The pattern of modeling correlations parallels those from our previous research (Chaffee. McLeod. and Atkin. 1971: Chaffee and McLeod. 1971) using a different and considerably larger sample of adolescents. Such correlation! have led other researchers to infer that this isevi- dence for "positive modeling." wherein the child imitates theviewing behavior of the example set by the parent (Himmelweit et al.. 1958: Schramm et al.. 1961). We have argued in our earlier work (Chaffee et al.: Chaffee and McLeod) that this inference is notnecessarily implied by the data. a point w e shall return to in our conclusions. 247 250 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 2 also shows that. while girls modeling correlations for the var- ious program types remains substantial for both age comparisons, there is a noticeable decline in mother-son similarity in the older group for viewing time as well as for several program types. The considerably higher modeling of western viewing for girls. a result expected from sex- role learning, is somewhat in contrast to the Chaffee and McLeod find- ings of no difference between sexes for this type of programming. Table 3 tests a possible alternative inference that the modeling corre- lations are a spurious artifact of the fact that mother and child are simi- lar in general television viewing time. The within-category figures are the average correlations within programs of a given types. The across- category figures are the average correlations of programs of the given type with all programs of other types. The difference between the 'two sets of correlations. then. is the "pure or specific modeling of that type of program with general viewing removed. It appears that modeling of westerns remains high after itmoving the effects of general viewing. All program types remain positive, although the within-category across-category differences are rather small for adventure-drama and situation comedies. Of course. this analysis re- moves only one alternative explanation and leaves a variety of other possible inferences open. Self-report vs. other-report of aggression

In all analyses up to this point, aggression findings have been based upon self-report measures. Our previous report analyzed the sex and age patterns for these measures and came to the conclusions that boys show considerably higher levels than girls on most aggression measures and that there is a marked decline in aggression from junior to senior high. Table 4 shows the sex and age category standardized levels for reports from peers. teachers. and the mothers of the adolescents. to- gether with some corresponding self-report measures. The peer report data show a pattern rather similar to the self-report results. with boys rated mote aggressive than girls and with the decline for older children on the assault aggression measure. Peer-reported ver- bal aggtession. however, departs somewhat from this pattern with a slightly higher level for girls at the junior high level. Both self- and oth- er-reports of verbal aggression show a slight increase at the senior high level for boys. The two reports of irritability resemble each other for girls, but peer reprts diverge, with a higher level for older boys. Teacher reports. available only for the juniot high sample. indicate an inconsistency u ith self-reports in that girls show slightly, though nonsig- nificamly. higher levels of aggression. The parent data arc also incon- sistent. with senior high mothers reporting more aggression for their sons than did the junior high mothers. 258 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 251

Table 5 shows the correlations among the various aggression meas- ures. Low positive correlations are shown among the first three suppos- edly parallel other-report measures (average r = + .18). Even less corre- spondence is shown for the teacher and mother reports with the overall self-report measure (+ .15 and +.09. respectively). Perhaps the lack of correlation of the teacher ratings with any other aggression index is un- derstandable becPuse it is only a one-item measure: however, the disap- pointing results for the mother reports with four items is less under- standable. Some encouragement is provided by the substantial correlation be- tween peer reports and self-reports for the assault aggression index (+ .40). The association for verbal aggression is more modest (+ .17). and peer report of irritability is unrelated to self-report of irritability. How- ever, the peer report of irritability is positively related to self-reports of assault and verbal aggression; perhaps the judgment of irritability is so difficult to make that it is made indirectly through an individual's more observable traits such as verbal and physical aggression. The adoles- cent's attitude toward aggression presents an anomaly. in that it corre- lates more highly with the peer measures than with the self-report measures (except for approval of aggression with which it has a part- whole relationship and hence is meaningless). Measures of family environment

Table 6 shows standardized levels of family environment indices im- proved for the second report by the addition of mothers' reports on these items and from some new items. New measures of mother-child communication are also introduced. The addition of the mothers' view reveals sharpened results for paren- tal contro' over adolescent television viewing, with considerably more restriction for the junior high families and ,1ightly more for girls than for boys. A somewhat similar pattern is shm, ii for parental affection. while physical punishment reverses with higher levels for boys. Restrictive punishment is largely characterized by very low levels for senior high girls. Socio-orientation. as shown in Table 6, declines sharply with age and is greater among the families with girls. Concept-orientation has a less clear pattern, with the highest levels for junior boys and senior girls. The socio-orientation findings follow our theory and previous research. but we would have anticipated an increase in concept-orientation for boys as well as for girls. The improved socioeconomic status measure used here evens out from the earlier patterns although the junior high families with boys retain their unexplained high status. The cortelations among the various family environment indices used in the first report are rather similar to those resulting in Table 7 from the 259 151 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVEN ESS introduction of more reliable measures. The two newmeasures, socio- orientation and concept-orientation, are unrelated. Socio-orientation shows positive correlations with all forms of parental control, withver- bal and restrictive punishment, and with affection. Concept-orientation has positive associations with control over television, with interpreta- tion of television violence, and with affection. Socioeconomic status is related to both new measures. .25 with socio-orientation and + .41 with concept-orientation. Television viewing and aggression The crucial correlations between adolescent television viewing and the new other-reports of aggression are shown in Tabi:s 8 and 9. The level of violence viewing ( OVV) used in the earlier report and the ex- panded measure of total viewing time are used as predictor variables. Three peer measuresa teacher reports a mother reports and an overall sum of the three other-reportsare used as criterion variables. Buss- Du rkee verbal aggression. approval of aggression, and aggressive atti- tudes are self-reports also used as criterion variables here. Overall violence viewing and aggression. While the large majority of correlations between violence viewing (OVV) and other-reportmeas- ures of aggression shown in Table 8 are positive, the size of these cor- relations tends to be somewhat less than the corresponding associations for OVV and the self-report measures of aggression shown in the first report (McLeod et al.. 1971. Table 18). The overall correlation between OVV and the peer report of aggression is + .20. and the teacher report of aggression also correlates +.20 with OVV. The lower correlations for the other-report sum appears to be largely a function of the mother re- port data, which show a .29 correlation with 0 V V for junior boys and a .06 correlation overall. Among the new self-report measures in Table 8, Buss-Durkee verbal aggression has no clear pattern of association with OVV, while aggres- sive attitudes shows low to moderate positive correlations. Television viewing time and aggression. The television viewing time correlatioos with other-reports of aggression in Table 9 are fairly similar to those for OVV by aggressive behavior, in that most correlations are positive ard low. The only exception is among junior girls, where slight negative correlations are shown on most other-report measures. Selfreported verbal aggression is unrelated to viewing time for most sex-grade subgroups. while aggressive attitudes has a positive relation- ship to viewing time among girls only. Family environment and other variables Tables 10, II. and 12 present the correlations of the new family envi- ronment measures with violence viewing and the self- and other-reports of aggression.

260 411111W

ADOLESCENTS. PA RENTS. TELEVISION USE 253 Family environment and violence viewing.Attempts to control the adolescent's television viewing and to interpret violent content are asso- ciated with generallyhigherlevels of violence viewing (Table 10). While this suggests the ineffectiveness of such measures, a more likely explan- asion is that parents whose children are "addicted" to violent fare are likely to take measures to modify that addiction. More direct attempts to affect aggression by emphasizing nonaggression are also positively relat- ed to violence viewing, but only among families with girls. The results for our new measures of physical. verbal, and restrictive punishment that include repoits from the mother as well as from the child are very little changed from the child-only punishment measures of our first report. Low to moderate positive correlations are shown for all comparisons in Table 10. This is consistent with findings of previous research by Bassett. Cowden. and Cohen (1968) regarding physical pun- ishment and with Chaffee and McLeod (1971) for all three punishment measures. Our correlations are considerably larger on all measures than those reported by Chaffee and McLeod. however. The fact that the pun- ishment measures were identical for both studies suggests that the dif- ferences might be due to the fact that the present research had a more reliable measurement of violence viewing (a 65-show index as opposed to two sum mary item s). The tendency toward a positive relationship between the parent's giv- ing of affection and the child's level of violence viewing noted in the first report is somewhat strengthened here. The overall correlation of +.12 is close to the comparable +.07 found by Chaffee and McLeod and is con- trary to the Maccoby (1954) finding of a negative association between affection and "escapist" television viewing. Table 10 shows rather strong positive correlations between socio-ori- entation and violence viewing in all groups except the junior boys. Fami- lies that emphasize harmony and suppress the child's feelings have ado- lescents who watch more violent television than the more egalitarian families. This is consistent with earlier work showing that socio-orienta- tion was related to entertainment media use (Chaffee. McLeod. and Wackman. 1966; McLeod. Chaffee. and Wackman. 1967; Chaffee. McLeod. and Atkin. 1971) and with violence viewing (Chaffee and McLeod. 1971). The very slight negative relationship ( .05 overall) between concept-orientation and violence viewing also bears a close resemblance to the Chaffee and McLeod data ( .04). An analysis of these two dimensions by the four family types they form will be present- ed later in the report. There is also evidence in Table 10 of the expected negative relation- ship whereby children in lower status families watch more violent televi- sion. This association is fairly strong for all but the senior high girls. The overall correlation ( .17) is somewhat stronger than the comparable figure from the Chaffee and McLeod report (.08). 261 154 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Family environment and self-report aggression. Table II indicates an overall positive correlation between parental control and self-report aggression level: however this correlation is large only among junior girls. Direct parental emphasis on nonaggression tends to be slightlyre- lated to lower aggressive behavior among boys. hut it is inconsistent for girls. The conditional interaction of this variable with the viewing-ag- gression relationship will be discussed later. Our first report summarized the literature that would lead us to expect a positive relationship between punishment and aggressive behavior. The addition of data from the mother does littleto change our conclu- sions from that report. A positive relationship between all three forms of punishment and self-report aggression is shown for senior high children only (Table 11). The junior high correlations are low and inconsistent. This contrasts with the findings for punishment and violence viewing discussed earlier, where equivalent levels of associationwere shown for each grade level. The results for parental affection are inconclusive (Table 11). Two moderate correlations are shown, but one is positive and the other is negative. Of the two. the .23 for senior boys resembles thecorre- sponding figure for the Maryland data in our first report. Table 11 shows that there is a tendency toward aggression in socio- oriented homes. The results are strong for boys only. however. The oth- er family com munication dimension. concept-orientation, has a negative correlation overall. hut this relationship is strong onlyamong senior high boys (.39): senior girls show a slight positive relationship. The combi- nations of these two dimensions will be shown later in this report. Our expanded measure of socioeconomic status is not related to self- report aggression in a systematic fashion. Two of the correlations are positive while the other two are negative. Although the two negative correlations are larger. the overall correlation is slightly positive (+ .05). Family environment and self-report aggression. Table 12 corresponds to the previous table in showing the relationship of the family environ- ment variable to aggression. In this table. however, other-report rather than self-report is used as the criterion aggression variable. Once again. there is no clear and consistent pattern for the parental-attempts-to-in- fluence variable. For interpretation of television violence, for example. two of the sex-age groups show moderate positive correlations, while the two others have negative ones. All three punishment variables are positively correlated with other- report aggression in a pattern that closely resembles the results for the self-report comparisons. The overall correlations range from + .18 for verbal punishment to +.41 for restrictive punishment. with somewhat higher associations for senior high boys than for other groups. Contrary to prior theoretical expectations. a small positive relation- ship between affection and aggression is shown for senior high boys and

262 fr,

ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 255 girls in Table 12. We should note that we did not find this pattern for self-report aggression either in Wisconsin or in Maryland. But nowhere did we find the expected negative relationship. Very strong results are shown for both boys and girls in senior high for socio-orientatiln. Families emphasizing harmony and suppression of feelings are more likely than others to have aggressive children. Howev- er, very little association is found in the junior high homes. Concept-ori- entation has no consistent relatic .ship with other-report aggression. Finally, socioeconomic status is negatively related to other-report aggression at the senior high level, but there is no relationship over all adolescents. Family communication patterns and other variables. Table 13 shows the standardized levels of the four family communication types on the major viewing and aggression variables. Protectives. the children whose families emphasize harmonious and hierarchical socio-relations but do not stress exposure to controversy or expression of ideas. are the heaviest television viewers and are also quite high in viewing of violence and Saturday morning fare. They see the possibilities of learning from violent programming, but curiously they are about average in involvement and in linking television to real life. Their parents arc apt to control how much they watch and to place emphasis on nonaggression. They are high on all but the verbal aspects of self-report aggression and peers rate them as high on both assault and verbal aggression. Interestingly, their mothers do not see them as ag- gressive. Consensual adolescents. whose parents emphasize both socio- and concept-orientations, are rather similar to the protectives in being very high on violence viewing and are above average in total television time. Thcy are the most likely to sce a linkage to reality in the television con- tent. and their parents are the highest on all forms of control over their viewing. Despite heavy parental emphasis on nonaggression. peers. teachers, and their mothers are apt to describe these youngsters as ag- gressive. Yet the consensual children themselves are only about average on the self-report aggression measures. Laissez-faire families, where neither socio- nor concept-orientation is stressed. tend to produce relatively low users of television and of violent content. The children are unlikely to see a reality linkage but do report heavier involvement in the programming. In keeping with their commu- nication behavior, laissez-faire parents are unlikely to control viewing. interpret content. or emphasize nonaggression. Regarding aggression. the adolescents from this type of family are slightly above average in self-report aggression hut slightly below average in the reports from pcers and teachers. Plorahstic adolescents. who receive concept-orientation but not a so- cio-emphasis in the home. are very low users of television and violent 263 156 TH.EVISION AND ADOITSCLNT AGGRESSIVFNFSS content and are also below average on cognitive reaction to television. Their parents are not likely to control television or teach nonaggression. but perhaps they do not have to. Their children are clearly the lowest group on both self-report and other-report measures of aggression. Peer integration

Previous research leads to the expectation that the more closdy the adolescent is tied to a group of peers. the less frequent will he his televi- sion viewing generally andhis watching of violence inparticular (Schramm.Lyle. and Parker. 1961; Himmelweit. Oppenheim, and Vince 1958; Riley and Riley, 1951). The relevant data, shown in Table 14. pro- vide little support for theexpectation. The overall relationship to violence viewing is only slightly negative ( .07) and that with viewing time is actually positive 1+.0,1). Then, too. the more popular adolescents'show a slight tendency to report more (rather than less) reactivity to violence on television. Peer integration shows a weak but consistent overall relationship to the aggression measures in Table 14: most of the correlations are posi- tive, and strongly so in the peer reports for the junior high boys. For the most part. peer integration does not seem to he a strong predictor varia- ble Data from the Maryland sample (not shown) yield more consistently positive overall correlations between peer integration and measures of aggressive behavior H-.24 with OAS). Contrary to expectations. there is also a positive overall association between integration and total televi- sion viewing time (+.16) and violence viewing (+ .17); these relation- ships are stronger for boys than for girls. Coorientation of mother and child The general expectation from previous research and theorizing is that all three coorientational measuresagreement. congruency. and accu- racywould be negatively related to violence viewing and aggression. Regarding viewing. Schramm et al. (1961) found children who disagreed with parents over aspirations spent more time with "fantasy" televi- sion. One version of this formulation assumes that lack of agreement with the parent frustrates.the child, which in turn leads the child to "es- cape" television fare and to aggressive outlets for motive states result- ing from frustration. Table 15 shows some suppott for the above expectation. although in general the negative correlations are not strong. Perhaps this is, due to the relatively weak measurement of the coorientation variables in that only one situation was involved, and a rank order of fixed categories was used. 264 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 257

It is interesting to note that for both overall violence viewing (OVV) and for self-report aggression (OAS). the key measure was accuracy (which involves a comparison of the mother's and the child's view). rather than congruency (the comparison using exclusively the child's data of his view and his estimate for his mother). Simple response set would have made the child's congruency the hest predictor. This hap- pens for mother-reported aggression. where mother congruency hits the highest correlation (+ .25) among the five measures. Past violence viewing and aggression We would very much have liked to have used a long-term panel design to check on the crucial question of time-order or causal sequence of our variables Because we lacked this design. we introduced what should be considered a weak substitute, in the form of a question about the fre- quency with which the adolescent watched various programs three or four years ago. From this. we built an index of past violence viewing that is roughly comparable to OVV. the current violence viewing index. If the past viewing predicts to aggression better than current viewing. then it can be argued that the violence viewing-to-aggression direction is made more tenable. On the other hand. better prediction for the current OVV would make aggression-to-violence viewing more likely. Pitting the two viewing measures directly is somewhat unfair to the past view- ing measure. however. The number of shows (13). compared to the 65 shows in the current index. implies that greater reliability of the latter would make better prediction more likely. The greater difficulty of recall of past behavior also operates as a handicap to the past viewing measure. Table 16 shows the predictive power of the current and past violence viewing indices. The two measures. intercorrelated +.35. reveal :very similar aggression correlations with the overall advantage going to the past measure. For reasons discussed above, this is rather surprising and argues for the violence viewing-to-aggression interpretation. It also appears that past viewing tends to be the more strongly related to aggression for boys and for junior high children, while current view- ing has the advantage among girls and the older adolescents. Crime-detective versus westerns: cognitive reactions One of the more perplexing findings presented in our first report (McLeod et al.. 1971) involved differential predictions of aggression for western/ and for other types of violent programs when the viewing of all other ty'pes of programs are partialed out (Table 33). Among thc seven program types. the correlations of crime-detective. adventure-drama. 265 258 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT GGRESSIVENESS and Saturday morning viewing with aggression level (OAS) remained positive when controlling for all other types of watching. The initialpos- itive correlations of westerns and OAS + .20 and 4-.12). however, be- came negative ( .01 and .07) when the controls were introduced. Table 17 explores the possibilik; that westerns are different becausea different set of cognitive reactions are associated with them than with the other types of violent shows. Crime-detective shows were usedas a comparison with westerns on twelve cognitive reaction items. Adoles- cents were asked if these were more likely to be their reactions during crime shows or during westerns, or w hether theywere equally likely (or unlikely) for both program types. It is very clear that westerns are seen as less closely tied to real life than crime shows. For example. 46 percent of the adolescents felt crime shows were more likely to ".. .tell about life the way it really is": only eight percent reported that westerns were more likely to doso. Respond- ents tended to see crime programs as more involving. Only a slight plu- rality perceived greater learning of aggression from crime shows. and most adolescents saw no differences on this attribute. Multivariate analyses

In the remaining tables. we examine the relationship of two ormore variables in predicting various new criterion variables: other-reports of aggressive behavior, violence viewing, overall cognitive reactions.ap- proval of aggression. and the combined index of self- and other-reports of aggressive behavior. For those regression analyses thatare parallel to the models examined in the first report. we use the identical independ- ent variable measures based on the adolescent data only. Violence viewing and other-reports of aggressive behavior. Tables IS- 22 prestnt various partial and multiple correlations for the relationship betwei n violence viewing and the other-ratings of aggression from peers. teachers. and mothers. The control for television viewing time does not alter the raw correia- Lions to any marked degree (Table 18). It tends to reduce the correlations for boys slightly, while reducing some comparisons for girls and increas- ing others to a minor degree. The partials for peer and teacher reports are only slightly lower than those found for self-reports (Table 30. first report). while the relationship to mother reports remains inconsistent and clearly negative for junior boys. Table 19 shows that partialing on socioeconomic status and school performance leaves the overall raw correlations virtually unchanged. Multiple predictors of aggressive behavior. Table 20 presents the mul- tiple correlations for other-report aggression with violence viewing and key third variables taken one at a time. This technique shows the extent to which different variables can improve the prediction of aggression 266 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 259 beyond its raw correlation with the OVV index. The strongestcontribu- tion is made by the child's perceived learning of aggressionfrom violent television programs (from +.17 to +.35 overall),replicating the find- ings with self-reported aggression (Tables 34 and 35. first report).On the other hand, the addition of irritability did not produce thesizable in- crease found in the case ofself-report aggression. Socio-orientetion combined with OVV to produce substantial multiple correlations among seniors (from + .22 to +.53 for boys and from +.09 to + .35for girls) but had no impact on the relationship for juniors.Moderate contributions were made by reality linkage,restrictive punishment. and physical pun- ishment. particularly for senior high students. The combination of violence viewing, parentalpunishment, and pa- rental affection as predictors of the other-report aggression sumis shown in Table 21. Multiple correlations ranging from .20 to .31 areproduced considerably less than the comparable .34 to .53 range forself-report aggression (Table 37. first report). While the other-reportmultiple coef- ficients are relatively similar, the magnitude of thepartials for each of the three predictors varies considerably betweensex-grade categories. Punishment is the only predictor for junior boys. while violenceview- ing and punishment partials are high for senior boys.Punishment also shows strong partials for girls in both grades. whileaffection is negative for junior girls and positive for senior girls.With the exception of the senior boys. the OVV index partials are considerablylower than was the case for self-report aggression. Table 22. using five variables as a regression on other-reported aggres- sion, is comparable to Table 38 of the first report that usedthe same pre- dictor variables for self-report aggression. The .39multiple coefficient is considerably lower than the .51 found previously forself-report aggres- sion, the difference being particularly great amongboys. Perceived learning of aggression has the highest overall partial inboth cases (+.30 for other-reprrt. +.28 for self-report), and punishmentretains the low positive partial correlation it had for self-report data. Onthe other hand. both violence viewing and irritability show muchsmaller partials for other-reported aggression. Each enters as a substantialpredictor for only one sex-grade comparisonviolenceviewing for senior boys and irritability for senior girls. Multiple predictors of violence viewing. The multipleregression anal- yses of violence viewing areshown in Table 23. Predictor variables here are based on responses of boththe mother and child to facilitate com- parison with the Chaffee and McLeod results.Moderate multiple corre- lation coefficients are indicated for thefamily communication variables for all but junior boys. Socio-orientation accountsfor almvst all of the predictive power in this regression analysis. but Table13 ha. previously shown that concept-orientation cannot be ignoredbecause :t clearly in- teracts with socio-orientation in its relation toviolence viewhg. 267 INEMENInk.

ft

260 1 II EVISION AND ADM. ESULNI AGGRESS! V EN ESS

Affection and the three punishment variables produce moderate multi- ple predictions of violence viewing for all four sex-age groups (Table The partials help to make restrictive punishment the strongest over- all predictor of- the affection-punishment set of variables, although its partial is essentially zero for junior girls and senior boys. We have also run the multiple regression amdyses for the aff ection- punishment variables for the Maryland data (Table 24). The family communication measures were not available for this sample. nor were mother reports of affection and punishment. The multiple coefficients are clearly lower than those in Wisconsin. but the general pattern is simi- lar The overall Wisconsin results plus comparable statistics from Chaffee and McLeod (1971) using a different sample of parents and children are shown below:

Overall Correlations with Violence Viewing

Absolute (zeroorder) Relative (partials) Chaffee- Chaf fee- Independent var. Wisconsin McLeod Wisconsin McLeod Coneeptorientation -.05 -.05 -.05 -.01 Socio-orientation +.28 +.14 Multiple correlation .28 .14 Affection +.10 +.08 +.06 +.07 Physical punishment +.07 +.03 +.10 +.03 Verbal punishment +.14 +.03 +.09 +.04 Restrictive punishment +.16 +.10 +.15 .! .07 Multiple correlation .33 .16

The results for the samples appear quite similar in pattern, although most of the partials are small and somewhat stronger predictions are found in the Wisconsin sample. It should be noted that the Maryland results also indicate that restrictive punishment is the strongest predic- tor of violence viewing. Table 25 presents a regression analysis of violence viewing with eight predictor variables for the Wisconsin sample. Multiple correlation coef- ficients over .50 are shown for all sex-age subgroups. Some rather odd partial coefficients are created, however, in that none of the eight predic- tor variables has consistently all-positive or all-negative sets of partial correlations for all four comparisons. This is doubtless the result of us-, ing so many predictor variables with such small subgroup samples. In the partials where all adolescents are combined, no one variable stands out as a dominant predictor. The corresponding regression analysis for the Maryland sample is shown in Table 26. Six rather than eight predictor variables are used, because the family communication measures were not available, and each measureis based on child reports only. Here, the multiple : W68 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. ELEVISION USI: 261 correlations are considerably less andthe partials for the variables change considerably from theWisconsin sample. School performance becomes a good predictor withnegative partials. perhaps comparable to the similar pattern found for I Q in theChaffee and McLeod (1971) data. Affection and physical and verbalpunishment become negligible predic- tors. while restrictivepunishment retains the only conistentpositive partial correlations with violenceviewing. Multiple predictors of cognitivereactions to television violence. Ta- bles 27-30 present multipleregression analyses of cognitive reactions to television. Multiple correlationcoefficients ranging from .27 to .46 are shown for the family communication dimensions in Table 27.Socio-ori- entation has substantial positivepartials for girls, but the boys' data are inconsistent with a negative partialshown at the junior high level. Con- cept-orientation is inconsistent with a .06overall, hut with positive partials for three of the four subgroups. Affection. restrictive punishment, andphysical punishment tend to be fairly good positive predictors of cognitivereactivity. For girls. verbal punishment has a negative partial andis negative overall. In general. the Wisconsin data show better prediction ofcognitive reactivity for both the family communication andparental treatment variables for girls than for boys. The Maryland data in Table 28, based onchild-report predictors. show cognitive reactivity results considerablydifferent from the Wisconsin findings. Restrictive punishmentremains a good predictor of reactivity among girls. butaffection and physical punishmentpartials are negligi- ble overall, and verbal punishment,which has negative partials in Wis- consin. has positive coefficients inMaryland. When the parental treatment andfamily communication measures are combined with school performance andsocioeconomic status in Table 29 for Wisconsin. considerablyhigher multiples ranging from .42 to .65 are produced. Lowschool performance and frequentparental affection are the best and mostconsistent predictors of cognitivereactivity in Wisconsin. Adolescents raised inconcept-oriented homes are less likely to be reactive. Forgirls only. reactivity is related tohigh physical and restrictive punishment and to low verbalpunishment. The six-variable multipleregression on cognitive reactivity inMary- land is shown in Table 30. Schoolperformance is generally negative as in Wisconsin. but the partials iremuch reiduced. Father occupationis negative in contrast to the generallypositive partials for socio-economic status in Wisconsin.Affection is inconsistent and slightlynegative rath- er than clearlypositive, physical punishmentshows no clear pattern. and verbal punishment reversesby revealing positive partials.Only re- strictive punishment is consistent acrosssamples by remaining clearly positive for girls. In general: thereis too much inconsistency infindings across samples andregression analyses to say much aboutthe prediction of cognitive reactivity. 269 161 1E1.1:VISION AND ADOLESCEN1 MailasSIVEN Esti MI/11NC predictorsof combhwd self- and other-reports of a.t.Tression. The aggressive behavior reports hy self and others are summed into a combined index for analyses presented in Tables 31-34. The multiple correlations of ()VV. parental punishment. and affection with the com- bined aggression index shown in Table 31 range w idely from + .29 for junior hoys to + .49 for senior boysslightly lower than the multiples using self-report aggression alone (see Table 37. first report). The + .29 overall zero-order correlation between OV V and the combined aggres- sion index declines slightly to +.26 with the family variables controlled. On this omnibus aggression index, the parental punishment sum shows a solid positive partial correlation of + .20, On the other hand, parental affection yields a mixed pattern across subgroups. Table 32 adds the child's irritahility and perceived iearning ofaggres- sion to the regression analysis. These five predictors producemore simi- lar multiple correlations. from +.19 to + .57 across the four subgroups. Thus, this set of predictors accounts for one-quarter to one-third of the aggression variance. Overall violence viewing, perceived learning of aggression, and irritability have strong positive overall correlations after all other independent variables are controlled. Each predicts well for girls of both grade levels, while violence viewing is weakamong junior boys and learning and irritability are slightly negative among senior boys. The sum of the three types of parental punishment shows consist- ently positive partials. somewhat higher for senior high. Affection is inconsistent with a strong negative partial among the senior high boys. To demonstrate the total contribution of television viewing variables alone, the set of predictors in Table 33 includes only measures of expo- sure and reactions to television violence. The combination of current violence viewing, past violence viewing, and cognitive aggression learn- ing accounts for one-fourth of the overall variance on the combined self- and other-report aggression index, with subgroup multiples ranging from +.32 to + .51.. The partial correlations for 0 VV and past violence view- ing are quite similar, and the perceived learning measure is considerably stronger than either exposure variable. Although the measure of past violence viewing was not available for the Maryland sample, the combination of OVV and perceived aggres- sion learning yields multiple correlations of a comparable magnitude (see Table 34, first report). In the overall Maryland sample, these two viewing factors accounted for almost one-third of the variance on the self-report aggression index. The subgroup multiple correlations vary from +.37 to + .62. These data indicate that a large portion of the aggressive behavior var- iance can be accounted for solely by viewing variables, withoutany supplementary contribution from family factors or internal aggressive- ness states such as irritability. Violence viewing by aggressive behavior within levels of nonaggres- sion emphasis. Table 34 describes the conditional association of OAS 270 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISMN USE 263 with the self-report. other-report. andcombined indices of aggression. Results presented earlier indicate that parentalattempts at emphasizing nonaggression were inconsistently related to aggressivebehavior in Wisconsin (Table 12: Table 24. first report). Respondents were divided into high andlow groups on the continuum of nonaggression teaching. and zero-orderviewing-aggression correla- tions were run at each level. New data onthe association of violence viewing .ind other-report aggressivebehavior show strong differences across high and low levelsof parental nonaggression emphasis.Where parents stress nonaggressivebehavior, there tends to he a slightly nega- tive relationship, particularly for girls.Where there is a lesser emphasis. the correlations are uniformly positive.The magnitude of this distinc- tion is much stronger for the other-reportindex than for the self-report index. When the combined sum of theseaggression indices is examined, the overall correlation with OVVis +.15in the high emphasis condition and +.39 in the low condition. For threeof the four subgroups. there is a zero relationshipwhere parents emphasize nonaggressive responses. The senior boys present a reversal tothis general trend, however, with a +.45 viewing-aggression correlationunder high parental emphasis. It should be noted that there are only 19respondents in this cell. Muhiple predictors of aggressiveattitudes. The Wisconsin seven-item index of aggressive attitudes included twoitems dealing specifically with approval of aggression that werealso used with the Maryland sample. This pair of items was originallyinserted as a test of the Berkowitz (1962) notion that violence portrayals mayteach that aggression is ac- ceptable if it can be justified. However, wefound no consistent differ- ences in the correlationsbetween violence viewing and approvalof hurt- ing an enemy "in order to getrevenge" vs approval of hurting an enemy merely "if you are mad at him." These two items were summed to form aminimal aggression approval index in common I or both samples.Tables 35 and 36 display the regres- sion analyses for Wisconsin andMaryland. The overall and subgroup multiple correlations tend to fall in the+.30 to + .40 range, somewhat lower than the behavioral aggressionmultiples. In analyses not shown, the more extensive seven-itemindex produced substantially larger mul- tiple correlations using these sameindependent variables, indicating that low reliability and limitedvariance may have dampened anyrelation- ships with the two-item index. Among the four predictors ofaggression approval, there is a consist- ent tendency for perceivedlearning of aggression to relate positively and for parental affection torelate negatively when othervariables are controlled. The partial correlationsfor OVV are modest in Maryland and negligible in Wisconsin,while punishment shows no clearrelation- ship. 271 264 1F,LEVISI0 N AND ADOLESIL , AGGRLSSIVENESS

Table 37 presents a six variable regression on the full seven-item ag- gressive attitudes index in Wisconsin. These predictors account for one- fourth of the variance overall, and subgroup multiple correlations vary from + .4 Ito +.62. Again, the strongest partial correlations are found for perceived learning of aggre:;sion (mainly among junior boys) and parental affection (moderate-to-strong negative correlationsforall subgroups). In addition. concept-oriented family communication is neg- atively related to approval of aggression. while socio-orientation relates positively for the junior high students. The partial correlation for OVV is positive among seniors and negative among juniors.

CONCLUSIONS

The purpose of our research is to examine the structure within and the relationships between three sets of variables: adolescent aggression. adolescent television viewing, and the family environment. We will take each of these in turn to discuss the implications of both the new data from this report and the findings presented in our earlier report. Adolescent aggression Our conceptualization divided aggression into four components: ag- gressive behavior, verbal aggression, irritability, and attitudes toward aggression. In addition to the adolescents' self-reports on the four com- ponents. the Wisconsin sample included peer ratings on the first three and teachers' and mothers' ratings on aggressive behavior. Aggressive behavior. Four subindices were combined into the 20-item index of self-reported aggressive behavior (OAS): manifest physical aggression(sixitems); aggressivebehavioral delinquency(three items); Zaks-Walters aggression (11 items); and hypothetical aggressive reactions (four items). These four indices were combined because their manifest content involved overt behavior, and their intercorrelations averaged +.36 in Maryland and + .20 in Wisconsin. The addition of peer ratings of aggressive behavior in Wisconsin also helped justify the over- all index with a correlation of + .42 with OAS. Other empirical findings, however, raise doubts about the wisdom of isolating aggressive behavior from other forms of aggression. The Wis- consin self-report, OAS, showed an average correlation of +.42 with verbal aggression, irritability, and aggressive attitudes. This far exceeds the average internal subindex correlation of +.20. Then too, the peer rating of aggressive behavior has an average correlation of +.65 with peer ratings of verbal aggression and irritability, well above its +.44 cor- relation with overall self-report aggressive behavior. Finally, the teacher report, the mother report, and the self-report of aggressive behavior show only low positive intercorrelations.

Ir. g 265 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE Clearly, the self-report and the other-reports aremeasuring some- thing. because the interitem correlations of thesubindices are high and the test-retest reliabilities are adequate. But theviews of the child, his peers. his teacher, andparticularly his mother are quite ditTerent in what they are measuring. This is in keeping with the Sears (1961) suggestion thatself-report data should be supplemented by reports from all threeof these outside sources, since each provides aunique perspective due to its systemati- cally different stimulus significance to the yougadolescent. The data also indicate that the child and his peers areapparently generalizing or stereotyping across our conceptual dimensions. We have opted for a strategy of retainingdimensions for the possibili- ty that they will have differentrelationships with our television viewing and family environment variables. In addition, wehave summed these individual indices into overall indices for some analyses. Verbal aggression. Verbal aggression may be seen as akind of alterna- tive to more direct physical aggression; therefore weconsidered it a dis- tinctly different type of behavior. Empirically, however,its self-report and peer report versions correlate only +.17, andthe self-report corre- lates more highly with the own-view irritability (+.28) while the peer report is more closely associatedwith peer ratings of aggressive behav- ior (+ .70) and irritability (+ .60). Verbal aggresSionhas some different properties from other forms of aggression: it does notdecline among boys from junior to senior high, and it is unrelated toviewing time. Irritability. A more covert, and perhaps morephysiologically based, measure of aggression isirritability. Perhaps because it is a covert prop- erty, there islittle agreement (+ .02) between self-reported and peer reported irritability. Instead, each type of reportcorrelates more highly with other aggression measures judged by the same source.What clearly distinguishes irritability from other forms of aggressionis the total lack of modeling or similarity between mother and child onthat attribute. Aggressive attitudes. It is clear that there is no necessarydependency between holding aggressive attitudes and otherforms of aggression. An adolescent could approve of aggression as a meansof solving problems without himself behaving aggressively; conversely,without favoring aggression he could behave aggressively (e.g., as aresult of "behavioral contagion" in a peer-gang situation). Empirically,aggressive attitudes are rather closely related toboth self-reported and peer-reported aggres- sive behavior, and they are associated withvarious viewing and family variables in a manner similar to the measures ofaggressive behavior. Adolescent television viewing Four sets of adolescent viewing variables wereincluded in our first report: viewing of specific program types,overall violence viewing, time spent with television, andcognitive reactions to television violence. 273 266 'El:LI:VISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Here we added a final Wisconsin viewing variablepast violence view- ing. the extent of violence in programs watched threeor four years ago. Types of programs watched. Crime-detective. western, and adven- ture-drama programs form a highly intercorrelated cluster of adolescent viewing preferences. It was shown inour first report. however, that westerns differed from the other two types in that they bear no relation- ship to aggressive behavior when viewing of othertypes of programs is controlled. Some clarification was provided here whenwe contrasted the cognitive reactions to westerns and crime shows. The westernswere much less likely to he linked with real life and produced less involve- ment in the plot. Overall violence viewing. On the basis of multiple ratings of the de- gree of violence in 65 programs. a measure of overall violence viewing (OVV) was developed. This index has a positive association with both self-report and other-report aggression. The self-report correlationsare substantial for all but junior boys. There is a +.30 correlation overall. with age-sex subgroup correlations averaging +.24. Other-reportaggres- sion correlations with OVV are considerably lower. averaging +.10 across subgroups and +.17 overall. This decrease is largely a function of one component. the mother report. which has a negative association with OVV. We have subjected these violence viewing-aggressive behav- ior correlations to partial correlation analyses controlling fora variety of third variables. While the moderate zero-order correlationsare reduced. they are not eliminated except for junior boys insome analyses. The re- lationship between violence viewing and aggressive behavior is robust. at least for the variables considered here. Of course. some other set of variables might well eliminate the basic zero-order correlation. Time spent with television. The OVV index is rather highly correlated with the amount of time the adolescent spends with television. It is im- portant, then, to see to what extent the relationship to aggressive behav- ior is a function of the specific violence viewingor the more general tele- vision time. In general. it appears the stronger relationship is with the specific violence viewing for both self-report and other-reportaggres- sion. For both criteria, however, the junior boys violate this pattern with slightly stronger correlations for television time. Perhaps it takes boysa little longer to select out the more violent fare. Past violence viewing. We attempted to get at causal direction ina limited way by asking about the adolescents' viewing threeor four years ago to form a past violence viewing index. This head-to-head competi- tion produces very similar correlations. with the advantage. ifany, going to past v.iewing. Even this slight advantage is surprising because of the obviously greater reliability and, hence. greater predictive potential of the present OVV measure. Although this does not make a violence view- ing-to-aggressive behavior sequence much more likely, it does seriously question the likelihood of the reverse aggressive behavior-to-violence viewing sequence. '474

N+.,. 1111 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS.TELEVISION USE 2167 Cognitive reactions to violent television.We began with five indices of cognitive reactions to violent television: opportunities for learning aggressive behavior, linkage to real life, involvement in programming, identification with violent television characters, and perceived efficacy of violent behavior on television. On the basis of relatively low correla- tions with other reaction items and inconsistent associations with other variables of importance. the last two indices were dropped when we formed an overall cognitive reactivity index. Our results have shown that the cognitive reactions of children to tele- vision are important to consider along with sheer exposure to violent programming. The perceptions of antisocial learning, of a tie between the media and real life experiences, and of involvement in programs are all clearly related to aggressive behavior and, to a less extent, to the amount of violence viewing. Perceived aggressionlearning in particular bears a rather strong relationship to aggressive behavior independent of other aspects of viewing behavior. Family environment We can categorize the host of family environment variables into four rough categories: the adolescents' personal and structural attributes, mothers' aggression and television behavior, parental treatment, and parent-child communication variables. Personal and structural attributes.Perhaps the most consistent and comforting finding in our research is that virtually all indices of aggres- sion, violence viewing, and cognitive reactivity decline from junior to senior high. Sex differences are almost as consistent, with boyshigher on most measures of aggression,violence viewing, and reactivity. The exceptions are that girls tend to be higher on irritability and oninvolve- ment in violent programming. School performance. is another consistent variable: the low perform- ers watch considerably more violenttelevision, have stronger reactions to the content, and are more likely to approveof aggression. They are likewise more likely to behave aggressively, but the findings are not en- tirely consistent. It is difficult to interpret the schoolperformance con- cept; it could reflect either a relatively stablepersonality characteristic like I Q or a learned behavior pattern shaping or reflectingviewing pat- terns and aggressive behavior. Socioeconomic status is a less dominant factor than our reading of the research literature would have led us to suppose. Whilelower-status children do watch more violent television programming, only small and inconsistent correlations are shown for its relationship to aggression, cognitive reactivity and attitudes approving aggression. Finally, peer integration comparisons show a marked departurefrom previous literature, with the well-integrated adolescents tending todis- playslightlyhigher levelsof violence viewing (Maryland only), 273 26 1 ELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT A( GRESSIVENESS aggressive behavior (mostly for boys), aggressive attitudes (seniors only), and cognitive reactions (juniors only). We would have expected the less popular youths to be higher on these indices. Mothers' aggression and tekvision behavior. Our personal interviews with Wisconsin mothers allowed us to test some assumptions about sim- ilarity between mothers' and children's behavior, or "modeling" of the mother's behavior by the child. We found evidence of mother-child sim- ilarity on the assault aggression, verbal aggression, and Zaks-Walters indices, as well as on the approval of aggression items. More important, these correlations were trait-specific with considerably higher mother- child same-index coefficients than for the cross-index comparisons. No evidence of transmission was shown for the covert irritability index. We have interpreted this as indicating that learning of overt aggression pat- terns is more likely than the transmission of internalized and perhaps physiologically based traits such as irritability. It is also clear that ag- gression modeling is greater for mother-daughter than for mother-son pairs. Whether this indicates generally greater "aggression modeling" for girls or the learning of specific and largely nonaggressive sex roles cannot be known without comparable data from the fathers. We have also found mother-child viewing correlations that averaged +.27 for the three violent program types as well as for the three less vio- lent categories. Is this sufficient evidence to infer that the child models his television behavior after that of his mother? Elsewhere we have argued that it is not (Chaffee et al.. 1970. 1971; Chaffee and McLeod, 1971). It is possible for the "parental example" to be either positive or negative. and it seems likely that the mother, who watches violent pro- grams far less than her children, would set a "negative example" by not watching. This alternative explanation is strengthened by the Chaffee and McLeod finding of higher "modeling" for mother than for the father among both boys and girls. The lower same-sex correlations argue against sex role learning, and the greater similarity to mother makes negative modeling more plausible. An even more likely explanation is "reverse modeling," with the child influencing his mother. The child, as the "TV expert" in the home, is often sought for viewing advice here and in our previous research (Chaf- fee et al., 1971). In Wisconsin, 35 percent of the mothers said that they often happen to watch television programs just because their children are watching, while only ten percent felt that their children often watch just because they are viewing television. When the children were asked, 21 percent indicated that they often happen to watch because their par- ents are watching. It seems unlikely to us that direct modeling plays a major role in influencing the child's television behavior. Parental treatment. Included under the parental treatment category are three methods parents may use to influence their child's viewing and aggressive behavior, three types of punishment. and the amount of affection shown the child. 276 ;

ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 269 Parental attempts to influence the child's television behavior by con- trolling watching and by interpreting violence apparently have little ef- fect. They have no relation to aggressive behavior and a slight positive association with violence viewing. The latter association may 'oe the result of the parent attempting to do something about the already heavy violence viewing by his child. Attempt to influence by emphasizing nonaggression has only small zero-order correlations with self-report and other-report aggressive behavior. For all subgroups except senior boys in Wisconsin, however, we have found that nonaggression emphasisbecomes an important con- tingent variable. The average correlation, for all subgroups acrossself- and other-reports in Wisconsin and self-report in Maryland, is + .26 in families where little stress is placed upon nonaggression; in families where such an emphasis is found, the average correlation is only +.07. Wisconsin senior boys show a sharp reversal for self-report and a slight one for other-report aggressivebehavior. Except for this subgroup, the other contingencies are quite remarkable. Despite some inconsistencies between subgroups, samples, and meas- ures, we may conclude that physical, verbal, and restrictivepunishment bear a generally positive relationship to violence viewing and aggressive behavior. When only the child's view is considered, physical and verbal punishment are essentially unrelated to violence viewing, and restrictive punishment has a clear positive relationship. The relationship becomes fairly even across the three punishment measures when the mothers' data are added. Self-report aggression is associated with high levels of all types of punishment, but only for senior high students. Other-report aggression k very strongly and positively correlated with restrictive punishment and moderately associated with the other two punishment measures. In terms of aggressive behavior, then, restrictivepunishment seems to be a somewhat strongerpredictor than physical and verbal punishment. This appears to have implications for explaining the pun- ishment-aggression correlation. If simple imitation is the causal mecha- nism, then we would expect physical punishment to be a better predictor than restrictive punishment. Since this is clearly not the case, we must seek more indirect explanations. While the behavioral forms of aggression are predicted by punish- ment, the attitudinal form of aggression is not. Approval of aggression is not consistently related to any of the threepunishment measures. None of the punishment measures predicts cognitive reactivity for boys, but restrictive punishment has a consistently strong positive correlation for girls. Our literature search led us to an oversimplified view of the function of parental affection. The conceptualization of warmth (affection) as the polar opposite of punitiveness led to the expectation of negative correla- tions with violence viewing and aggression. The empirical reality proved 277 270 iLLEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS to be mueh more complicated than that. Affection is basically unrelated to violence viewing, although among boys the direction is clearly posi- tive. with those receiving more affection also watching more violence. The reversal from the expected negative relationship is in line with the findings of Chaffee and McLeod. No clear connection between affection and aggressive behavior was found. The addition of data from the mother changed the average corre- lation from slight negative to slight positive in the Wisconsin sample. The data for aggressive attitudes are much more clearcut. however, with a fairly strong negative relationship. No c lear pattern is shown for cogni- tive reactivity. These findings suggest that hffection may be a more complex variable than previously anticipated; it is possible that the max- imum end of the affection dimension is not "high," but rather "smother- ing." with the optimum somewhere short of the maximum. Parent-child communication. Socio-orientation appears to be a rather good predictor of both violence viewing and aggression. Children in families where the em phasis is placed on hierarchy and harmony tend to be high on violence viewing and aggressive behavior. Concept-orienta- tionthe emphasis on expression and exposure to new ideashas a negative association with aggressive behavior. The fact that concept- orientation is unrelated to violence viewing does not mean it is irrelevant to media behavior. Previous research has found it to be tied to the use of mass tnedia for public affairs content, while socio-orientation has been associated with entertainment media use (Chaffee et al.., 1966; McLeod et al., 1967; Chaffee et al.. 1971). We need not extend our conclusions to the four parent-child communication patterns formed by dichotcmizing the two dimensions, except to mention that some interaction between the dimensions was evident in our findings and bears examination with larger samples. The final set of variables introduced in the second report were coor- ientational agreement, accuracy, and congruency. In a sense, these vari- ables nre outcomes of communication. We found that interpersonal agreement, rather than intrapersonal congruency, was the key variable in predicting violence viewing and aggression. Children who disagree with their parents over school goals watch more television violence and tend to display more aggressive behavior. This is congruent with pre- vious theorizing and deserving of more concentrated hvestigation in future research. Some final questions Although we would prefer to refrain from overgeneralizing beyond the direct findings of our data, it seems fair to discuss some of the rather difficult and perhaps almost unanswerable questions that require specu- lation about the implications of our research. These questions might be asked of any study of media violence and aggression.

278 ADOLESCENTS. PA RENTS.TELEVISION USE 271 Do the results presented justify theconclusion that there are function- al relationships among media violenceand aggression? This is a conclu- sion that cannot be made from a single setof studies. It would require a concentrated and long-term series of projectsconducted by many re- searchers using a variety of methods andresearch strategies. Such con- tinuing support for research on thisvital public issue has not been pro- vided in the past by either governmentagencies or private foundations. Within the limitations of our research, wehave attempted to control for variables that might uncover thespuriousness of the correlation between violence viewing and aggressivebehavior. These control varia- bles included: socioeconomic status,school performance (perhaps as an index of I Q or sophistication). age, sex,television time, and regional differences. None of these controls erasedthe basic violence viewing- aggressive behavior correlation. Similarly,other parental treatment and communication variables examined for theirassociation with these key variables failed to eliminate this basic correlation.While we cannot as- sume that other variables wedid not use as controls would have no ef- fect, we can conclude that the variables wedid include have not indicat- ed spuriousness. Our analyses of other-reports of aggressionalso serve as checks on potential spuriousness, because of thecollection of self-report data for both violence viewing and aggressiveness.While somewhat lower view- ing-aggression correlations are found forother-reports than for self-re- ports of aggression, the basic patternis the same for the peer and teach- er reports. Only themother report data is different inshowing a slight negative viewing-aggression correlation. What are the causal sequencesimplied by the relationships found cmong violence viewing,aggressive behavior, and other variables?In short, what are the aggressive effectsof watching media violence, if any? Even if we consider "other variables" as asingle combination of fam- ily environment factors rather than as manysingle variables, an almost infinite number of potential causal sequences areformed. To list a few: violence viewing might lead to aggressivebehavior; the more aggressive adolescents might select out violent content ontelevision; the family environment might lead certain children toindependently seek out vio- lent programs and to behave aggressivelywith no causal relationship among the outcomes: orthe family environment might lead toviolence viewing but not to aggression, withviolence viewing then influencing aggression (the reverse is also possible). Thepossibilities become even more numerous if webreak each causal variable into levels andhypoth- esize the effect coming from a particularlevel (e.g., high restrictive pun- ishment resulting in low violence viewing vslack of restrictive punish- ment reducing the amount ofviewing). Our nonexperimental design, lacking insequential measurement over long periods of time, simply does notenable us to disentangle causal 279 AM. 272 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS sequences .n any conclusive way. Our findings for past vs present view- ing do argue for a viewing-to-aggression sequence rather than the re- verse sequence, if in factsthere is a causal connection one way or the other. We did find some family environmental variables that were relat- ed to both violence viewing and aggressive behavior: restrictive punish- ment. perhaps somewhat more than verbal or physical punishment; so- do-orientation: and disagreement over goals between mother and child. In each ease. however, the partialing on these variables merely reduces but does not eliminate the violence viewing-aggressive behavior correla- tions. As we have mentioned, a variety of control variables have also failed to show spuriousness. Whether there are other variables that would reduce the viewing-aggression association to zero cannot be as- certained here: it stands as a challenge to other researchers. We are also left with the task of specifying the precise effects, if any. of violence viewing. At least four different types of possible effects on aggression can be distinguished: immediate aggressive responses trig- gered in some children under some conditions; learned internal respon- ses later operating as personal predispositions; learned behavioral se- quences or techniques used in later aggressive situations; and reduction of inhibitions to aggression in the form of values, attitudes, or cogni- tions. The specification of precise effects is more appropriately answered through experimental manipulation and precise measurement of imme- diate and delayed effects. The work of Berkowitz and his associates (1962, 1963a, 1963b, 1965, 1966, 1967, 1969) presents evidence for the first of these effects. Our data on aggressive behavior could reflect such a process, but our measurement cannot isolate the short-term sequence. The second possible effectthe learning of internal responsesis rele- vant to our data on irritability, where we found it to be the only aggres- sion index having no relationship to violence viewing. At least for this type of internal response, our findings suggest this type of effect is un- likely. The learning of specific techniques or behavior sequences is not in- compatible with our data, but on logical grounds it seems implausible that this could be very common. The specific techniques of violence are seldom shown in detail on television, and such information tends to be more readily available in places other than on television..Finally, the long-term effects of television violence on developing attitudes and cog- nitions favorable to violence is at once more interesting and likely, yet harder to demonstrate. We do have data of potential relevance in index of approval of aggression. It shows the same basic relationship with violence viewing as does aggressive behaVior. Perhaps future research- ers would do well to pay more attention to these more subtle but no less ithportant values and cognitions about the prevalence and efficacy of aggression.

k,`80 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 273 What advice can we offer to parents who are concerned about the effects of television on their children?Our ability to offer useful advice to concerned parents depends upon several assumptions: thatthey are willing to resolve the burden of proof by presuming television violence does have aggressive effects; that they are capable of altering their own behavior: and that the present viewing and/or aggressive behavior of the child is reversible or at least controllable. We are able to say tha.t. beyond the junior high level, both violence viewing and aggressive behavior are apt to decline in frequency.Time and maturation are on the side of the parent. but this is probablyof little comfort to the parent whose child appears in imminent danger ofgetting into trouble. Other more positive strategies are sought. Regarding viewing behavior, Schramm has advised parents that "ex- ample is the best persuader" and that it exercises "a ve..y potent kindof influence" over the child's viewing (Schramm et al., 1961: 182). While our results indicate mother-childsimilarity of viewing particular pro- grams, we do not feel such an inferenceis justified. Other more plausi- ble explanations can be given for the presence of such a correlation. While we are skeptical about the effectiveness of the parent attempting to set a better example, at least this advice has the advantageof being reasonably easy for the concerned parent to change. We believe "parental example" of another typethe mother's own behavior with respect to aggressivenessmay have greater impact than her own media behavior. There is a clear and traitspecific correlation between the mother and child on various aggression measures. The problem here is obviously one of the mother's being able to changeher own aggressive behavior or to practicenonaggressive behavior, even if she realizes that it has consequences for the child's behavior. In addi- tion, the assumption of reversibility of the child's behaviorwould have to be made. Finally, this assumes that the similarmother-child patterns are not biologically based. One potential parental behavior that does not seem to be effectiveis the direct control over the child's television viewing behavior. Such con- trol attempts had no direct connection with the child's aggression, nor did they operate to reduce the correlation between violenceviewing and aggressive behavior. In fact. arbitrary control might serve to increase the child's frustration and make aggressive behavior more likely.Our interpretation of Feshbach (1971) suggests that "forced-feeding" of nonviolent television on institutionalized boys actually increased aggres- sive activity. The control over viewing is really an indirect parental strategy, at- tempting to ultimately limit or reduce aggression, by first limiting the child's intake of media violence. Our data suggest a more direct strategy of emphasizing nonaggression to ,the child. This entails telling thechild not to fight back if other kids pick on him, warninghim not to imitate

281 274 TELEVISION AND ADOLFSC'ENT AGGRESSIVENESS aggressive acts seen on television, and so forth. While this emphasis will not appeal to many parents. our findings indicate that it operates as a strong contingent variable reducing the violence viewing-aggressive behavior correlation markedly. The parent might also consider using restraint in the application of punishment. While most parents seem to realize the negative effects of physical punishment. the potential dangers of verbal and restrictive pun- ishment are less well recognized. In particular. our data indicate that restrictive punishment ("grounding" and taking away privileges) is linked to viewing and aggression. While reverse causation could be op- erating. the relation is consistent and ties in with previous research. We have also found that children in families emphasizing hierarchical and harmonious communication watch more violent television and are more aggressive. This implies that the concerned parent might attempt to avoid being "one-up" in interacting with his child and in allowing the child to bring conflict out into the open. This and the suggested restraint on restrictive punishment should not be mistaken for advocating "per- missiveness." The "laissez-faire" child in our study was about average in aggressive behavior and well above average in his attitudes approving aggression. The parent can still maintain his own values while advocat- ing the child develop his own. and he does not have to avoid communi- cation in providing the child more room for expression. In our discussing of parental strategies. we have extrapolated very widely from our limited source of data. Unfortunately. our evidence is no less sparse than that of researchers who have preceded us. The con- cerned parent at the present time has few alternatives to making his own assumptions about the effects of television and to following advice based on thin evidence and speculation. We can hope that this study will offer some suggestions to help future researchers select variables and design better and more thorough research into the complex causal connection between the family. television violence, and adolescent be- havior.

FOOTNOTES

I. This technical report describes research pursuant to Contract No. HSM 42-70-77 with the National Institute of Mental Health. Health Services and Mental Health Administration. U. S. Department of Health. Education. and Welfare. Jack McLeod and Steven Chaffee were co-principal investigators. Charles Atkin. now at Michigan State University. was the study director. Tanis Turner was responsi- ble for typing and duplicating the report. Others aiding in data analy- sis were William Elliott. William Engels. Kenneth Sheinkopf. and Catherine Willette. ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS.TELEVISION USE 275 REFERENCES

Bassett, T., Cowden, J., and Cohen. M . The audio-visual viewing habits of selected subgroups of delinquents. Journal of Genetic Psycholo- gy,1968. 112, 37-41. Berkowitz, L. Aggression: a social psychological analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. Berkowitz. L. The concept of aggressive drive: some additional consid- erations. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press, 1965. Berkowitz, L. (Ed.). Roots of aggression: a re-examination of the frus- tration-aggression hypothesis. New York: Atherton, 1969. Berkowitz, L.. Corwin. R., and Heironimus, M. Film violence and sub- sequent aggressive tendencies. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1963(a), 27, 217-29. Berkowitz, L., and Geen, R. G. Film violence and the cue properties of available targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1966, 3(5). 525-30. Berkowitz, L., and Geen, R. G. Stimulus qualities of the target of ag- gression: a further study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 1967, 5(3). 364-68. Berkowitz, L., and Rawlings, E. Effects of film violence on inhibitions against subsequent aggression. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963(b), 66(3), 405-12. Buss, A. H., and Durkee, A. An inventory for assessing different ki Js of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 21, 343-49. Chaffee, S. H., and McLeod, J. M. Coorientation and the structure of family communication. Paper presented to meeting of the Interna- tional Communication Association, Minneapolis, Minn., May 1970. Chaffee, S. H., and McLeod, J.M. Adolescent television use in the fami- ly context. In Television and social behavior: a report to the Sur- geon General's Scientific Advisory Committee, Vol.4. Washington. D.C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Chaffee, S. H., McLeod, J.M.. and Atkin, C. K. Parent-adolescent simi- larities in television use. Paper presented at the meeting of the Asso- ciation for Education in Journalism, Washington, D.C.. August. 1970. Chaffee, S. H.. McLeod, JAC, and Atkin, C.K. Parental influences on adolescent media use. American Behavioral Scientist, 1971. 14, 323- 40. Chaffee, S. H., McLeod, J. M.. and Wackman, D. B. Family communi- cation and political socialization. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism, Iowa City, Iowa, 1966. Feshbach, S.. and Singer, R.D. Television and aggression. San Francis- co: Jossey-Bass, 1971. 283 276 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Greenberg, B.S., and Gordon, T.F. Perceptions of violence in television programs: critics and the public. In Television and social behavior, Vol. I. Washington, D.C.: U. S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Himmelweit, H., Oppenheim, A., and Vince, P.Television and the child. London: Oxford University Press, 1958. Maccoby, E.E. Why do children watch television? Public Opinion Quarterly. 1954, 18, 239-44. McLeod, J.M., Atkin, C.K., and Chaffee, S.H. Adolescents, parents, and television use: adolescent self-report measures from Maryland and Wisconsin samples. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 4 (this volume). McLeod, J.M., and Chaffee, S. H. The construction of social reality. In Tedeschi, J. (Ed.) The Social Influence Processes. Chicago: Aldine, in press. McLeod, J.M., Chaffee, S. H., and Eswara, H. S. Family communica- tion patterns and communication research. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism, Iowa City, August, 1966. McLeod, J.M., Chaffee, S. H., and Wackman, D. B. Family communi- cation: an updated report. Paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Education in Journalism, Boulder, Colorado, Au- gust, 1967. McLeod, J.M., Rush, R., and Friederich, K. The mass media and politi- cal information in Quito, Ecuador. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1968- 69, 32, 575-87. Murray, R., Cole, R., and Fedler, F. Teenagers and TV violence: how they rate and view it. Journalism Quarterly, 1970, 47, 247-55. Riley, M.W., and Riley, J. W. A sociological approach to communica- tions research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1951, 15, 445-60. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B. Television in the lives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961. Sears, R. The relation of early socialization experiences to aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961, 63, 466-92. Stone, V., and Chaffee, S. H. Family communication patterns and source-message orientation. Journalism Quarterly, 1970,47, 239-46.

284 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 277 Appendix A: Complete listing of questionnaire items comprising each index

Aggression Indices

Manifest physical aggression:same as in first report, Appendix A.

Aggressive behavioral delinquency: same as in first report, Appendix A.

Zaks-Walters aggression: (mother and child): same as in first report. Appendix A.

Hypothetical aggressive reactions: same as in first report, Appendix A.

Buss-Durkee assault aggression: (mother and child): same as in first re- port, Appendix A.

Approval of aggression: (mother and child): same as in first report, Ap- pendix A.

Buss-Durkee irritability: (mother and child): same as in first report, Appendix A.

Buss-Durkee verbal aggression: (mother and child): "Here are some things others (students, mother ' have said about getting alongwith people. How much is each statement like you?"

I demand that people respect my rights. When people yell at me, I yell back. When people disagree with me, I can't help getting into arguments. I would rather give in than argue about something. (reversed scor- ing) Scoring"a lot like me" (2), "a little like me" (I), "not like me" (0)

Aggressive attitudes: "How much do you agree or disagreewith these statements?"

It's all right to hurt an enemy if you are mad at him. In order to get revenge, it's all right to hurt an enemy. If a student is fed up with his government these days, he issomewhat justified if he sets off a bomb in an empty Army building. During the A merican Revolution, blowing up British buildings was com- pletely justified. 285 278 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCFNT AGGRESSIVENESS The most successful people are the ones who use violence. The rising amount of crime and violence in this country really doesn't bother me very much. It upsets me when I see someone beating up another kid. (reversed scor- ing)

Overall aggression sum:same as in first report. Appendix A.

Buss-DurkJe overall sum: Unweighted sum of 11 items comprising the indices of Buss-Durkee assault aggression. verbal aggression. and irrita- bility aggression. ranging from low (0) to high (22).

Peer ratings of aggression: "Now we would like to know how some of the kids in your homeroom get along with other students. Everybody in the class will give ratings on the other kids in the homeroom. Please mark a number to show whether each person does these three things often (2). sometimes( I). or never (0). Please try to be honest and fair. since it is important that we get an accurate idea of how students feel about each other. (if you don't know a person well enough, put a question mark in the blanks. Do not make marks for your own name, if it is on the list)."

Yells back when yelled at. When loses temper. hits other people. Is patient with others. (reversed scoring)

This was followed by a list of names of students in the rater's home- room.

Scoring"often" (2), "sometimes" (1), "never" (0). slimmed acrois all responses, divided by the number of raters. mid multiplied by 10. For each type of aggression, scores range from 0 to 20.

Teacher ratings of aggression: "As part of the study of sixth graders' television viewing and social behavior, each teacher is being asked to make a brief rating of their students. One question concerns the level of aggressive behavior that each child displays in school. We are particu- larly interested in identifying the most extreme students: those who are either very well behaved, cooperative with others and their teacher, and gentle in their interpersonal relationsand the opposite type who are discipline problems, who pick on other kids, and who get into fights or loud arguments. Please assign a score to each student using this system: 4 = highly aggressive-hostile, troublemaker; 3= more aggressive-hostile than average: 2 = average: 1 = very passive, gets along well. submis- sive." These instructions were followed by a list of names of students in the teacher's class. Scoring as indicated in item. 286 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 279

Mother ratings of aggression: Does (child) seem to get into more fights than other children his age, few fights. or about the same amount of fights? "more" (2), "same" (I), "fewer" (0) When he was younger, how often did he do mean things to other chil- dren while playing? Would you say often, sometimes, or never? "often" (2), "sometimes" (I), "never" (0)

When he was younger, how often did he show aggressive behavior to- ward other children? Would you say often, sometimes, or never? "of- ten" (2), "sometimes" (I), "never" (0)

If your child had an argument wi:h his best friend, how would he nor- mally go about settling it? physical aggression (2), non-aggressive re- sponse (0)

Television Viewing Indices

Program types:(mother and child): "Here is a list of some programs that have been on television this year. About how often have you really watched each of these shows? For each program, make one check show- ing whether you watched it:Almost always (nearlyevery week)or Often (atleast half the time) or Sometimes (at least once or twice) orNever."

Scoring"almost always" (4), "often" (3), "sometimes" (2), "never"(0).

Crime-detective Adventure-drama programs Western programs: programs:

Mod Squad Gunsmoke Medical Center Hawaii Five-0 Lancer Then Came Bronson Ironside Virginian Hogan's Heroes It Takes a Thief Daniel Boone World of Disney Get Smart High Chapparal Land of the Giants Name of the Game Here Come the Brides New People Mission Impossible Bonanza Bracken's World The FBI The Bold Ones

Comedy-variety Situation comedy News programs: programs: programs:

Red Skelton Mayberry RFD Frank Reynolds Laugh-in Julia Walter Cronkite 287

411101,, bate., 280 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Glen Campbell To Rome with Love Huntley-Brinkley Jim Nabors I Dream of Jeannie Local Evening News Dean Martin Flying Nun Lawrence Welk Eddie's Father Ed Sullivan Bill Cosby Leslie Uggams Family Affair Music Hall Bewitched Ghost and Mrs. Muir

Overall violence viewing: same as in first report, Appendix A.

Total viewing time:

On an average weekday. about how many hours do you personally spend watching TV? (1969 and 1970)

hours minutes

Now we would like to find out about your television viewing in the last few days. Think of all the programs you saw yesterday and the day be- fore, and figure out exactly how much time you spent watching TV pro- grams each day.

Yesterday: hours minutes (what day was it)

Day Before Yesterday: hours minutes (what day was it)

On *an average weekday this fall, about how many hours does(child) spend watching TV? (Mother report)

hours minutes

How many hours did your child spend watching TV yesterday? (Mother report)

hours minutes

Scoring(8) 4 hours 40 minutes or more (7) 3 hours 40 minutes-4 hours 39 minutes (6) 2 hours 40 minutes-3 hours 39 minutes (5) 2 hours 10 minutes-2 hours 39 minutes (4)1 hour 40 minutes-2 hours 9 minutes 288 --lEt."..-10.191.Mumtstmmeritemekt... .ww , .1001. ______ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 281

(3)1 hour 10 minutes-1 hour 39 minutes (2)1 hours 40 minutes-1 hour9 minutes (1) less than 40 minutes (0) None

Television violence viewing when younger: "Here is a list of programs that used to be on television several years ago. Try to remember how often you used to watch these shows when you were younger. Tell us whether you watched each show often, sometimes, or never."

Very high violence High violence Low violence Com bat I Spy Gilligan's Island Man from UNCLE Felony Squad Dr. Kildare Rat Patrol Big Valley Gentle Ben Rawhide The Fugitive Donna Reed Patty Duke Scoring"often" (2), "sometimes" (1), "never" (0). Low violence programs were reverse scored to control for overall viewing time during this period. High violence programs were double weighted. Very high violence programs were triple weighted.

Mothers television viewing levels:

About how many hours do you usually spend watching TV during an average evening after 5 p.m.?

hours minutes

How many hours did you spend watching TV yesterday after 5 p.m.?

hours minutes

Scoringsame as above.

Family Environment Indices

Parental control over television: For each item, reports were obtained from both the mother and child, unless otherwise indicated.

Who has the most to say about what (you, he) watch on television? (1969 and 1970) "mother" (2). "father" (2), "either mother or child" (1), "brothers or sisters" (0), "child" (0)

Do (your parents, you) always know what programs (you, he) are watch- ing on TV? (1969 and child only 1970) "yes" (1), "no" (0) 289 IMPIN.17.14,..L.1",12110..Nrrnat.

282 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIV EN ESS "Are there certain programs that (your parents. you) sometimes do not let (you. him) watch?" (1969 and 1970)

No Yes: (mark as many as you have to)

Westerns* Scary shows Cartoons Sexy shows TV movies Crime show s* Violent shows* Adult shows

Scoring"yes" (1), "no" (0). plus 1 point each for starred program types.

As compared with most parents you know, would you say that you are very strict, fairly strict, fairly lenient, or very lenient in controlling what your child watches on television? (Mother only) "very strict" (2), "fair- ly strict" (2), "average" (1), "fairly lenient" (0), "very lenient" (0)

Does (your mother, you) set a limit on how much time (you, he) can spend watching TV on school days? "yes" (1), "no" (0)

"If (your mother, you) sees (you, him) watching a program that (she, you) doesn't think (you, he) should watch, what does (she, you) usually do?" Order (you, him) to stop watching Turn off the set or change channels Suggest (you, he) do some other activity Ask (you, him) nicely to stop watching Scoringone point for answering "often" or "sometimes" to each al- ternative.

Parental emphasis on nonaggression: For each item, reports were ob- tained from both the mother and child, unless otherwise indicated.

Do (your parents, you) punish (you, him) if (you, he) are mean to other kids? (1969 and 1970) "yes" (2), "never mean/don't have to" (1), "no" (0)

Do (your parents, you) want (you, him) to fight back if other kids pick on (you, him)? "no" (1), "yes" (0) r.,-11..^..1,2,-¢,trann,mr.170

ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 283 How important does (your mother, you) think it is for (you, him) to learn to defend (yourself, himself)? "not important" (2),"somewhat impor- tant" (1), "very important" (0)

How often did (your parents, you) say (you, he) shouldn't do the bad things people do on TV? "often" (2), "sometimes" (1), "never" (0)

Do (your parents, you) ever tell (you, him) not to copy the violentthings that some people do on TV? "yes" (2), "don't have to" (1), "no" (0)

If someone called him a dirty name after school one day, which thing would you want him to do? (Mother only) Would you want him to yell at them, hit them, ignore them , or tell on them? "ignore" (2), "yell" (1), "tell" (1), "hit" (0)

Do you feel it is wrong for (him to hit, her to yell at) otherkids who do something to deserve it? (Mother only) "yes" (1), "no" (0)

Suppose someone played a real dirty trick on him. Should he hit them, yell at them, ignore them, or laugh at them? (Mother only) "laugh" (2), "ignore" (2), "yell" (1), "hit" (0)

What if someone cut in front of him in a long line. Would you want him to yell at them, shove them out, or just letit go? (Mother only) "let it go" (2), "yell" (1), "shove" (0)

Do you think it would ever be justified if he hurt another personin order to get revenge? (Mother only) "no" (1), "yes" (0)

Parental interpretation of TV violence:(mother and child): same as in first report, Appendix A.

Parental punishment:(mother and child): same as in first report, Appen- dix A.

Parental affection:(mother and child): "How often do (your parents, you) do these things with (you, him)?"

Show that (they, you) love (you, him). Show (their, your) affection by hugging and kissing (you, him). Tell (you, him) that (they, you) love (you, him).

Scoring"very often" (3), "fairly often" (2), "not too often" (1), "never" (0) 291 284 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Family communication patterns: (mother and child): "Now I would like to read a list of things parents sometimes say totheir children. How often do (your parents, you) say these things to (you, him)? For each item, do (they, you) say it often, sometimes, rarely, or never." Socio-orientation items: Say that (their, your) ideas are correct, and (you, he) shouldn't argue with them. (1969 and 1970) Answer (your, their) arguments by saying, "You'll know better when you grow up." (1969 and 1970) Say that (you, he) should give in on arguments, rather than risk making people angry. (1969 and 1970) Say there are some things that just shouldn't be talked about. (1969 and 1970) Say that (you, he) shouldn't argue with adults.

Concept-orientation items:

Say that (you, he) shoilld always look at both sides on an issue. (1969 and 1970) Say that getting (your, his) ideas across is important, even if others don't like it. (1969 and 1970) Ask for (your, his) opinion when the family is discussing something. (1969 and 1970) Say that every member of your family should have some say in family deckions. Admit that kids know more about some things than adults do.

Scoring"often" (3), "sometimes" (2), "rarely" (1), "never" (0)

Peer integration:

Do you have a bunch of friends that you usually hang around with? "yes" (1), "no" (0) Does he have a group of friends that he usually goes around with? (Mother report) "yes" (1), "no" (0) Compared with the rest of the kids you know, , would you say you have more friends, less friends, or about the same numberof friends? "more" (2), "same" (1), "less" (0) How do you prefer to spend your time: alone, or in the companyof friends? "usually like to be with friends" (2), "sometimes with friends and sometimes by myself" (1), "usually like to be by myself" (0)

roc. *lb AihoiCo ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 285 How many really close friends would you say you had (friends who would gladly help you if you needed help? "more than 10" (3), "6 to 10" (2), "3 to 5" (1), "1 or 2" (0), "none" (0)

Socioeconomic status:raw sum of four items, scored as indicated.

Mother education Subjective social class:

1 = grade school only 1 = lower 2 = some high school 2 = working 3 = high school graduate 3 = lower middle 4 = some college 4 = middle middle 5 = college graduate 5 = upper middle 6 = upper

Father occupation:

2 = unskilled factory worker, equipment operator, house- hold, service, police, fireman, laborer, construction 4 = craftsman, foreman, skilled, semi-skilled, farmer 6 = clerical and sales 8 = professional, managerial, executive, proprietor

Family income:

1 = under $2,000 2 = $2,000 - $3,999 3 = $4,000 - $5,999 4 = $6,000 - $7,999 5 = $8,000 - $9,999 6 = $10,000 - $14,999 7 = $15,000 - $19.999 8 = $20,000 plus

Coorientation:(mother and child): "Of the following four things, rate them in order of their importance for you. Put a "1" by the one you think is most important, a "2" by the one that is second most important, and so on. (Now I would like you to rank four things in order of their importance. Please tell me which you feel is most important for your child to do, which is second, and so on. Answer whatyouthink is impor- tant, not what he might think.") (1969 and 1970) 293 -

286 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Making new friends who share (your. his) interests. Being part of school activities and social life. Learning new ideas and different ways of thinking. Preparing to earn a living.

"Which of these same four things do you think your (mother. child) feels is most important? Rank them in order of importance as you think your (mother, child) sees it." (1969 and 1970)Same items as above.

Scoring agreement: sum of absolute differences betweenmother and child rankings of their own personal views on each item. child accuracy: sum of absolute differences between mother rankings of her personal views and child estimates of mother views on each item. mother accuracy: sum of absolute differences betweenchild rankings of his own personal views and mother estimates of child views on each item.

Scoringchild congruency: sum of absolute differences betweenchild rankings of his own personal views and his esti- mates of mother views on each item. mother congruency: sum of absolute differenoesbetween mother rankings of her own personal views and her estimates of child views on each item.

Cognitive Reactions

Perceived learning of aggression:same as in first report, Appendix A.

Linkage to TV violence in real life: same as in first report, Appendix A.

Involvement in violent TV programming: same as in first report, Appen- dix A.

Cognitive reaction sum: sum of above indices, with perceived learning of aggression items double weighted. Scores range from low (0) to high (40). ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 287 Appendix B: Tables Table 1: Correlations between mother and child aggression levels: Wisconsin data Mother Mother- Aggression measure Grade Son Daughter Overall

BussDurkee assault aggression jr. hi -.03 +.17 (4 item index) sr. hi +.1 7 +.57" BussDurkee verbal aggression jr. hi +.2 2 +.35" (4 item index) sr. hi +.15 +.281 BussDurkee irritability jr. hi +.01 +.03 (3 item index) sr. hi -.1 6 -.04 Buss-Durkee overall sum jr. bi +.14 +.28 (11 item index) sr. hi -.01 +.27* Approval of aggression jr. hi +.31 +.13 (2 item index) sr. hi -.04 +.22 ZaksWalters aggression jr. hi +.19 +.09 (7 item index) sr. hi +.09 +.34* (N) jr. hi (59) (49) sr. hi (60) (59)

Note: Cell entries are correlations between the mother's selfreported aggression and child's self-reported aggression. The bask two-wave sample of N=151 was used for the correlations between mother and child on the Zaks-Walters Aggression Index, which was measured only during the 1970 survey. All other items wereadministered during the 1969 survey with the full sample of N=225 motherchild pairs. *p<.05 *p<.01

Table 2: Correlations bieween mother and child television viewing levels: Wisconsin data

Viewing measure Grade Boys Girls Total television viewing time jr. hi +.25 +.10 sr. hi .00 +.14 Crime.detective programs jr. hi +.29* (average, 8 shows) sr. hi +.24 +.21 Western programs jr. hi +.20 (average, 7 shows) sr. hi +.18 +.42** Adventuredrama programs jr. hi +.30* +.29 (average, 8 shows) sr. hi +.14 +.27* Comedy.variety programs jr. hi +44" +.32 (average, 9 shows) sr. hi +.19 Situation comedy programs jr. hi +20* (average, 10 shows) sr. hi +23 +.21 News programs jr. hi +.15 +.28 (average, 4 shows) sr. hi +.24 Mod Squad jr, hi +49" +.27 sr. hi +.01 +.16 Hawaii Five-0 jr. hi +.24 sr. hi +.29* +26 Mission Impossible jr. hi +.29* +.08 sr. hi +.30" 295 288 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 2 (Cont.)

Gunsmoke jr. hi +.09 +.59" sr. hi +.28" +.61 ' Virginian jr. hi +.24 +.53** sr. hi +.25 +.38 Ironside jr. hi +.53" +.47" sr. hi +.27' +.14 Bonanza jr. hi +.29* +.38" sr. hi +.20 +.51" (N) jr. hi (59) (47) sr. hi (60) (59) Note: Cell entries are correlations between the mother reports and child reports of viewing behavior on each measure.

Table 3: Correlations between mother and child viewing of six program categories: Wisconsin data

Program category Grade Within- Across- category r category r Crime-detective programs jr. hi +.34 +.13 sr. hi +.30 +.15 Western programs jr. hi +.38 +.16 sr. hi +.49 +.17 Adventure-drama programs jr. hi +.31 +.20 sr. hi +.26 +.18 Comedy-variety programs jr. hi +.40 +.17 sr. hi +.29 +.18 Situation comedy programs jr. hi +39 +.18 sr. hi +.18 News programs jr. hi +.29 +.15 sr. hi +.29 +.08 (N) jr. hi (106) sr. hi (119) Note: Cell entries are mean correlations between mother reports and child reports of television program viewing. The within-category r represents the average mother-child correlation of all programs in that category. The across-category r is the average corre- lation of the mother's viewing of one category of program vs. the child's viewing of all other categories, and vice versa; this indicates the amount of within-category correla- tion that is a spuriour artifact of both persons watching any program.

296 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 289

Table 4: Standardized adolescent aggression levels, by age and sex: Wisconsin data

Aggressive measure Grade Boys Girls

Peer report assault aggression jr. hi +40" +20 (10 item index) sr. hi +14 65" Teacher report general aggression jr. hi A13 +04 (1 item) sr. hi _ _ _ Mother report aggression jr. hi 02 06 (4 item index) sr. hi +16 12 Overall other-report aggression sum jr. hi +364. +16 (15 item index) sr. hi +18 65" Overall self-report aggression sum jr. hi +52" +11 (20 item index) sr. hi +24 84" Self-report verbal aggression jr. hi +04 12 (4 item index) sr. hi +22 18 Peer report verbal aggression jr. hi +14 +22 (10 item index) sr. hi +17 51" Self-report irritability aggression jr. hi +05 +44" (3 item index) sr. hi 19 17 Fnar r:.p:.rt irritability aggression jr. hi , +09 +22 (10 item index) sr. hi +24 51'4 Self-report assault aggression jr. hi +19 +13 (4 item index) sr. hi +38" _68.. Approval of aggression jr. ni +39 +02 (2 item index) sr. hi +24 65" Aggrcssive attitudes jr. hi +47" 01 (8 item index) sr. hi +1.9' 76" (N) jr. hi (38), (30) sr. hi . (43) (40) Note: Standard scores represent each cell mean, calculated as a positive or negative devia- . 'ion from the overall mean on the listed index, divided by the overall standard devia- tion. Scores have been multiplied by 100 to eliminate decimals. Asterisks indicate the cell mean is significantly different from the overall mean for the remaining cells. 'p < .05 "p < .01

297 290 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 5: Intercorrelations among self, peer, teacher, and mother reports of aggression: Wisconsin data Aggression measure Correlations, all respondents Peer report - - Teacher report .23 - - Mother report .17 .14 - - Overall-other .95 .50 .45 - - Overall-self .44 .15 .09 .42 - - Verbalself .10 .08 .06 .12 .46 - - Verbal-peer .70 .27 .19 .69 .34 .17 - - Irritabilityself .11 -.07 .07 .11 .30 .28 .14 - - Irritability-peer.60 .14 .13 .57 .27 .1 2 .60 .02 - - Assault-self .40 .02 .03 .32 .7 6 .41 .26 .29 .22 - - Approval .30 -.02 .06 .28 .45 .1 3 .17 -.06 .23 .28 - - Attitudes .38 -.03 .1 3 .37 .50 .1 1 .23 .00 .29 .28 .79

(N=151) &i -a 8 I I. s ti a1 a

Note: Cell entries are zero-order Pearsonian r correlation coefficients. For r.16, p < .05 r = .21, p < .01 Table 6: Standardized adolescent family environment levels, by age and sex: Wisconsin data

Family environment measure Grade Boys Girls Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +46" +54" (28 item index) sr. hi -55" -25 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -06 +13 (18 item index) sr. hi -09 +03 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi +16 +09 (10 item index) sr. hi -21 -22 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +34 -44" (2 item index) jr. hi +18 -22 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi 00 +17 (4 item index) sr. hi +05 -18 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +17 +2 0 (4 item index) sr. hi +13 -44" Parental affection jr. hi +22 +55" (6 item index) sr. hi -43" -16 Parental socioorientation jr. hi +13 +51" (18 item index) sr. hi -27* -21 Parental concept-orientation jr. hi +14 -02 (16 item index) sr. hi -24 +15 Parental socioeconomic status jr. hi +36" -0 6 (4 item index) sr. hi -06 -2 3 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40) 298 mitt!,tit510_WittittVt-11.. t- "...-"-"" ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISIONUSE 291

Table 7: Intercorrelations among familyenvironment variables: Wisconsin data Family environment measure Correlations, all respondents Control over TV Nonaggression emphasis .29 - - TV interpretation .33 .36 - - Physical punishment .01-.07-.03 - - Verbal punishment .15 .02 .03 .30 - - Restrictive punishment .30 .07 .04 .36 .43 - - Affection .21-.01 .12 .01 .03 .08 - - .22 Socioorientation .35 .18 .23 .09 .34 .37 .38-.02 Concept-orientation .16-.02 .27 .01 .01 .04 .16-.25 .41 Socioeconomic status -.02-.08 .05 .00 .02-.08

(N=151)

72

(0) For r.16, p < .05 rZ.21,p<.01

Table 8: Correlations between level of violenceviewing and level of aggressive behavior as reported by others: Wisconsin data Overall Aggression measure Grade Boys G irls +.08 Peer report assault aggression jr. hi +.14 +.20* sr. hi +.20 +.13 Teacher report general aggression jr. hi +.13 +.14 +.204 sr. hi Mott,lx report aggression Jr. hi -.29 +.04 -.06 sr. hi +.09 -.10 OVERALL OTHER-REPORT jr. hi +.01 +.08 +.17* AGGRESSION SUM sr. hi +.22 BussDurIcee verbal aggression jr. hi -.03 +.24 +.03 sr. hi +.10 -.14 +.16 Peer report verbal aggression jr. hi -.01 +.12 sr. hi +.13 +.03 +.12 Peer report irritability aggression jr. hi +.01 +:11 sr. hi +.18 -.07 +.13 Approval of aggression jr. hi +.08 +.09 sr. hi +.03 .00 +.09 Aggressive attitudes jr. hi +.02 +.17* sr. hi +.22 +.11

(N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entriesare zero-order correlationcoefficients between each aggression variable and the Violence Viewing Index. 299 Z VIP.,N, agoe. 292 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 9: Correlations between total viewing time and level of aggressive behavior as reported.by others: Wisconsin data

Aggression measure Grade Boys Girls Overall

Peer report assault aggression jr. hi +.31 -.02 +.19* sr. hi +.03 +.17 . Teacher report general aggression jr. hi +.15 -.23 +.04 sr. hi - - Mother report aggression jr. hi -.22 -.03 .00 sr. hi +.12 -.03 OVERALL OTHER-REPORT jr. hi +.22 -.07 +.17* AGGRESSION SUM sr. hi +.06 +.14 Buss-Durkee verbal aggression jr. hi +.06 -.02 -.01 sr. hi -.02 -.21 Peer report verbal aggression jr. hi +.10 -.22 sr. hi -.03 +.02 Peer report irritability aggression jr. Hi -.07 +.19 sr. hi +.14 -.02 Approval of aggression jr. hi +.11 +.15 sr. hi -.12 Aggressive attitudes jr. hi +.02 +.23

sr. hi . -.24 +.21 (N) jr. hi (M) (30) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries qre zero-order correlation coefficients between each aggression variable and the Total Viewing Time Index.

or d300 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 293

Table 10; Correlations between level of violence viewing and family environment variables: Wisconsin data

Family environment measure Grade Boys Girls Overall jr. hi +.07 Parental control over TV viewing +.18* sr. hi +.13 +.10 jr. hi -.27 +.28 Parental emphasis on nonaggression +.05 sr. hi +.10 +.20 +.07 +.33 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi +.16* sr. hi +.23 .+.09 +.19 +.17 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +.18* sr. hi +.19 +.11 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi +.10 +.20 sr. hi +.30* +.33* Parerital restrictiie punishment jr. hi +.30 +1 1 sr. hi +.14 +.35* 1 +.15 -.01 Parental affection jr. hi +.12 sr. hi +.20 +.03 +.47** Parental socio-orientation jr. hi +.02 +.28** sr. hi +.30* +.31 +.04 Parental concept-orientation h.. hi .00 -.05 sr. hi -.04 -.15 1 Parental socioeconomic status jr. hi -.33* -.23 -.17* sr. hi -.29 -.07 jr. H (38) (30) (N) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zeroorder correlation coefficients between each family variable and the Violence Viewing Index.

301 294 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 11: Correlations between level of overall self-report aggression and family environment variables: Wisconsin data

Family environment measures Grade Boys Girls Overall

Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +.10 +.33 +.16* sr. hi +.03 -.10 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. hi -.19 +.16 -.07 sr. hi -.09 -.07 jr. hi -.02 +.21 Parental interpretation of TV violence -.03 sr. hi -.18 -.22 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +.06 +.24 +.27" sr. hi +.19 Parental verbal punishment jr. hi +.06 -.13 +.17* sr. hi +.21 +.40" Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +.05 -.08 +.23" sr. hi +.19 +.23 Parental affection jr. hi +.23 +.03 +.07 sr. hi -.23 +.08 Parental socio-orientation jr. hi. +.28 +.04 +.17* sr. hi +.16 +.06 Parental concept-orientation jr. hi -.09 -.10 -.15 sr. hi -.39** +.12 Parental socioeconomic status jr. hi +.05 -.16 +.05 sr. hi -.22 +.10 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between each family variable and the Overall Aggressive Sum (Self-Report). 111," TR.

f ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS. TE1 EVISION USE 295

Table 12: Correlations between level of overall otherreportaggression and family environment variables: Wisconsin data

Family environment measure Grade Boys Girls Overall -.08 -.07 Parental control over TV viewing jr. hi +.04 sr. hi -.02 +.05 -.12 .00 Parental emphasis on nonaggression jr. h i -.08 sr. hi -.14 +.01 +.25 Parental interpretation of TV violence jr. hi -.19 +.02 sr. hi +.22 -.14 Parental physical punishment jr. hi +.13 +.14 +.28 sr. hi +.42" +.19 +.15 +.11 Parental verbal punishment jr. h i +.18* sr. hi +.26 +.05 Parental restrictive punishment jr. hi +.36* +.14 +.41" sr. hi +.46 +.39 -.01 Parental affection jr. hi +.09 +.13 sr. hi +.16 +.16 .00 +.06 Parental socio-orientation jr. hi +.28* sr. hi +.5311 +.23 Parental concept-orientation jr. hi -.09 -.03 sr. hi -.05 +.05 .00 +.10 Parental socioeconomic status jr. hi .00 sr. hi -.23 -.06 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40)

Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between eachfamily variable and the Overall Other-Report Aggression Index.

393 296 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 13: Standardized levels of television viewing, cognitive reactions, family environment, and aggression by family communication pattern: Wisconsin data

Measure Laissez-faire Pluralistic Protective Consensual . Overall violence viewing -14 -41* +25 +32* Saturday morning -31" -31" +21 +42" programs Total TV viewing time -23 -22 +40" +08 Learning of aggression -04 -29* +19 +14 Linkage to real life -27 -06 -02 +33* Program involvement +24 -11 -02 -07 Control over TV -37' -30" +20 +46" viewing Emphasis on -32' -20 +28 +27* nonaggression Interpretation of -42" 00 00 +36* TV violence Physical aggression +15 -29* +14 +04 Delinquency aggression +10 -20 +16 -03 Zaks-Walters aggression 00 -34 +49" -08 Hypothetical aggression +04 -11 +10 -01 SELF-REPORT +12 -35" +30* -01 AGGRESSION SUM Peer report aggression -11 -27" +22 +20 Teacher report -16 -22 +20 +18 aggression Mother report +04 -09 -13 +16 aggression OTHER-REPORT -11 -29" +18 +24 AGGRESSION SUM Verbal aggression -04 +06 +01 -05 Peer report verbal -21 -11 +24 +09 aggression Irritability aggression -07 -01 +28 -17 Peer report irritability +05 -15 +05 +05 Approval of aggression +20 -24 +09 -01 Aggressive attitudes +32* -35" +13 -03 (N) (34) (40) (34) (39) Note: Standard scores represent each cell mean, calculated as a positive or negative deviation from the overall mean on the listed index, divided by the overall standard deviation. Scores have been multiplied by 100 to eliminate decimals. Asterisks indi- cate the cell mean is significantly different from the overall mean for theremaining cells.

1

304 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 297

Table 14: Correlations between peer integration and aggression, violence viewing, and cognitive reactions: Wisconsin data

Measu re Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing Jr. hi -.04 +.17 -.07 sr. hi -.16 -.09

Total viewing time Jr. hi +.18 +.03 +.04 sr. hi -.05 +.20 +.12 +.25 Cognitive reactions to TV violence sum Jr. hi +.06 sr. hi +.03 +.08 Jr. hi +.52s -.04 Overall other-report aggression sum +.11 sr. hi +.07 +.12 Overall self-report aggression sum Jr. hi +.11 +.01 -.03 sr. hi +.07 +.01 Jr. hi +.13 +.08 Self-report verbal aggression +.08 sr. hi -.02 -.09 Jr. hi +.32* +.24 Peer report verbal aggression +.11 sr. hi +.11 -.02 -.17 Self-report irritability aggression Jr. hi +.02 -.10 sr. hi -.01 -.20 +.01 Peer report irritability aggression Jr. hi +.26 +.09 sr. hi +.16 +.11 -.10 Approval of aggression Jr. hi -.19 -.03 sr. hi +.23 +.10 +.07 Aggressive attitudes Jr. I*, -.24 .00 sr. hi +.25 +.05 Jr. hi (38) (30) (N) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between eachviewing and aggression variable and the Peer Integration Index. 298 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 15: Correlations between coorientational agreement, accuracy, and congruency and television viewing, cognitive reactions, and aggression: Wisconsin data Child Mother Child Mother Measure Agreementcongruencycongruencyaccuracy accuracy

Overall violence viewing -.21 -.07 -.07 -.12 -.11 Learning of aggression -.10 -.19 -.12 -.15 -.03 Linkage to real life -.12 -.11 +.03 -.19 +.05 Program involvement +.06 -.04 +.08 +.02 +.15 Physical aggression -.19 -.06 + 04 -.09 -.11 Delinquency aggression -.19 +.01 +.04 -.02 -.11 Zaks-Walters aggression -.03 -.14 -.17 -.01 -.04 Hypothetical aggression -.12 +.01 -.16 -.03 -.04 SELF-REPORT -.21 -.06 -.11 -.06 -.10 AGGRESSION SUM Peer report aggression -.16 -.07 -.14 -.14 -.09 Teacher report aggression +.01 +.02 -.05 +.06 +.03 Mother report aggression -.02 -.01 -.25 -.02 -.02 OTHER-REPORT -.14 -.07 -.20 -.11 -.08 AGGRESSION SUM Verbal aggression -.14 -.07 -.10 -.05 .00 Peer report verbal aggression -.13 -.03 -.12 -.13 +.03 Irritability aggression +.05 +.04 +.07 +.10 +.18 Peer report irritability +.12 +.10 +.13 +.14 +.02 (N 151)

For r .16, p < .05

r .21, p < .01

306 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 299

Table 16: Correlations between current vs pastviolence viewing and aggressive behavior; Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Aggression measure Grade VV Now AA/ Past VV Now VV Past VV NowVV Past

Self: Physical jr. hi +.13 +.18 +.29 +.20 +.17 +.16 sr. hi +.06 .00 -.06 -.08 - - - Self: Delinquency fr. hi - - +.20 +.24 sr. hi +.12 +.27 +.21 +.13 -.02 Self: Zaks.Walters jr. hi +.19 +.23 +.14 +.24 +.22 sr. hi +.11 +.19 +.45 +.34 +.10 Self: Hypothetical jr. hi -.02 +.14 +.24 +.22 +.26 sr. hi +28 +.26 +.06 +.13 +.38 +.19 Self: OVERALL jr. hi +.12 +.26 +.30 +.33 SUM sr. hi +.23 +.27 +.23 +.18 +.08 +.16 Peer: Assault jr. hi +.14 +.24 +.20 +.17 sr. hi +.20 +.01 +.13 +.04 +.03 Teacher: Aggressionjr. hi +.13 +.22 +.14 +.20 +.18 sr. hi - - - - Mother: Aggression jr. hi -.29 -.14 +.04 -.13 -.06 +.02 sr. hi +.09 +.09 -.10 -.01 +.08 +.09 Other: OVERALL jr. hi +.01 +.'d0 +.17 +.17 SUM sr. hi +.22 +.03 +.09 +.04 +.13 +.21 Self: Approval lr. hi +.08 +.21 +.09 +.27 sr. hi +.03 +.42 .00 -.21 +.09 +03 Self: Attitudes lr. hi +.02 +.06 +.17 +.21 sr. hi +.22 +27 +.11 -.07 (N) fr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlationsbetween each aggression variable and the Overall Violence Viewing Index WV Nov4 andthe Violence Viewing When Younger Index (VV Pest). Significance levels not indicated.

307 300 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 17: Percent reporting greater cognitive reactions to television violence on crime-detective vs western programs: Wisconsin data More often More often Cognitive reaction item during crime shows during westerns

The programs show me how to get 18% 11% back at people who make me 1 angry. ILI Sometimes I copy the things I see 11% 16% people doing on the shows. ILI When someone attacks another 15% 11% person and isn't punished, I some- times feel I can get away with it too. ILI When the bad guy gets a beating 18% 15% he deserves, I sometimes feel like getting even with peoplt who have bothered me. 8-1 The shows tell about life the 46% 8% way it really is.(R) The people I see on the shows 32% 8% are just like the people I meet in real life. (R) The stories remind me of 36% 12% frustrating things that have happened to me. (R) Some characters remind me of 44% 19% people who have made me mad.

I am so involved that I get 44% 15% carried away with the story. (I) I get upset when my favorite star 18% 8% is yelled at or threatened. 10 Once in a while I feel like things 16% 11% that happen to my hero are really happening to me. (I) I pay close attention to these 59% 16% shows. (150 Note: Cell entries are percentages of the overall sample indicating that one type of pro- gram affected them comparatively more often than the other. In many cases, the majority of respondents marked the "don't know" category. Items labeled (L) are from the Perceived Learning of Aggression Index; those labeled IR) are from the Linkage of TV Content to Real Life Index; those labeled (I) are from the Involvement in Violent Programming Index.

30/3 ADOLESCENTS. PARENTS. TELEVISION USE 301

Table 18: Partial correlations between violence viewing and other-reports of aggressive behavior, controlling total television viewing time: Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Aggressive measure Grade raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: r TY time r TV time r TV time

Jr. hi +.14 +.01 +.08 +.07 Peer report +.20 +.15 sr. hi +.20+.20 +.13 +.07 +.13+.08 +.14 +.27 Teacher report jr. hi +.20 +.20* sr. hi - - +.06 Mother report jr. hi -.29-.23 +.04 -.06-.06 sr. hi +.09+.06 -.10-.10 +.08 +.12 OTHERREPORT jr. hi +.01 -.08 +.17 +.12 SUM sr. hi +.22 +.21 +.09 +.05 Jr. hi (38) (30) (N) 1151 sr. hi (43) (40)

Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure. Cell entries in partial: TV time columns are partial r correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for the Total TV Viewing Time Index. Signifi- cance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

Table 19: Partial correlations between violence viewing and other-reports of aggressive behavior, controlling socioeconomic status and school performance: Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Aggression Grade raw partial: raw partial: raw partial: measure r SES SP r SES SP r SES SP

Peer report jr. hi+.14 +.13+.08 +.08+.07-.02 +.20+.20* +.18* sr. hi +.20+.15+.27 +.13 +.12+.02 Teacher jr. hi+.13+.17+.09 +.14+.27+.15 +.20+.25* +IV report sr. hi------Mother jr. hi-.29-.26-.30 +.04+.04 .00 -.08 -.05-.07 report sr. hi +.09+.09+.10 -.10-.10-.11 OTHER- jr. hi+.01 .00-.07 +.08+.11 +,02 +.17+.17* +.15 REPORT sr. hi +.22+.17+.26 +.09+.09-.01 SUM (N) jr. hi (38) (30) 11511 sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries in raw r columns are zero-order correlation coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index cnd each aggression measure. Cell entries in partial: SES columns are partial r correlations coefficients between the Violence Viewing Index and each aggression measure, controlling for the Socioeconomic Status Index. Cell entries in partial: SP columns are partial correlation coefficients controlling for the School Performance Index. Each control variable is partialled out separately in this analysit. Significance levels are indicated for partial correlations only.

. 309 302 TELEVISION AND A DOI.ESCENT AGGRESSIVEN F.SS

Table 20: Multiple predictors of other-reports of aggressive behavior: violence viewing and selected third variables: Wisconsin data

Independent variables Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing (OVV) jr. hi +01 +.08 +.17 (raw r) sr. hi +.22 +.09 OVV and Parental emphasis on jr. hi .13 .08 .20 nonaggression (multiple r) sr. hi .27 .09 OVV and Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi .01 .23 .20 sr. hi .22 .24 OVV and Parental physical jr. hi .07 .08 .31 punishment sr. hi .31 .20 OVV and Parental verbal punishment jr. hi .01 .18 .22 sr. hi .24 .13 OVV and Parental restrictive jr. hi .19 .09 .41 punishment sr. hi .28 .24 OVV and Parental affection jr. hi .11 .13 .20 sr. hi .25 .11 OVV and Perceived learning of jr. hi .35 .33 .35 aggression sr. hi .23 .43 OVV and Linkage of TV violence jr. hi .15 .14 .27 to real life sr. hi .36 .17 OVV and Socio-oriented FCP jr. hi .01 .08 .29 sr. hi .53 .35 OVV and Concept-oriented FCP jr. hi .09 .24 .17 sr. hi .22 .12 OVV and Socioeconomic status jr. hi .01 .15 .17 sr. hi .28 .11 OVV and School pe 'ormance jr. hi .26 .54 .29 sr. hi .38 .39 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are multiple correlation coefficients between the OverallOther-Report Aggression Sum and the Overall Violence Viewing Index and each thirdvariable, one at a time. Multiple correlations greater than the raw correlation indicatethe added contribution of the family and TV relction variables to the relationshipbetween aggression and viewing. Significance kvels are not indicated.

310 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 303

Table 21: Multiple regression analysis of other-reports of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental punishment, and affection: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall

Overall violence viewing jr. hi -.04 +.05 +.14 sr. hi +.22 +.04 jr. hi +.17 +.20 Parental punishment sum +17 sr. hi +.18 +.21 jr. hi -.04 -.16 Parental affection -.05 sr. hi -.12 +.15 Multiple correlation jr. hi .20 .24 .25* sr. hi .31 .23 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) 1151 sr. hi (43) (4O) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Other-Report Aggression Sum and each family and viewing variable, controlling for all other independent vari- ables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. All independent variable measures are comparable to Maryland measures.

Table 22: Grand multiple regression of other-reports of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental affection and punishment, child's irritability, and child's perceived learning of aggression from television: Wisconsin data Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing jr. hi -.06 -.04 +.09 sr. hi +.23 +.05 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.36° +.32 sr. hi -.11 +.43° +.30" +.07 +.11 BussDurkee irritability jr. hi +.08 sr. hi -.04 +.29 jr. hi +.12 +.15 Parental punishment sum +.14 sr. hi +.18 +.10 Parental affection jr. hi -.09 -.12 -.06 sr. hi -.14 +.09 Multiple correlation jr. hi .40 .41 .39 sr. hi .33 .51 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) 1151 sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Other-Report Aggres- sion Sum and each family and viewing variable, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. All independent variable measures are comparable to Maryland measures.

311 304 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 23: Multiple regression analysis of violence viewing, by family environment variables: Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Independent variable Grade abs. rel. asns. rel. abs. rel. Family Communication Ps,nerns

Concept-orientation jr. hi .00 -.06 -.01 .00 sr. hi-.02 -.04 -.08 -.11 -.05 -.05 Socio-orientation jr. hi+.02 -.12 +.47" +.37' +.28* sr. hi+.28 +.25 +.28 +.13 +.18* Multiple correlation jr. hi .02 .47" sr. hi .30 .32 .28" Parental Punishment and Affection

Affection jr. hi+.14 +.13 -.04 -.21 sr. hi+.18 +.16 +.17 +.15 +.10 +.06 Physical punishment jr. hi +.11 +.27 +.13 +.02 +.07 sr. hi+.03 +.02 +.01 -.01 +.10 Verbal punishment jr. hi-.06 -.18 +.16 +.12 sr. hi+.25 +.21 +.33' +.31* +.14 +.09 Restrictive jr. hi +.22 +.19 -.07 -.01 punishment sr. hi+.01 +.08 +.37" +.29 +.16 +.15 Multiple correlation jr. hi .34 .24 sr. hi .36 .49* .33"

(N) jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40) (151)

Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between each family environment variable and the Overall Violence Viewing Index, controlling for all other independent variables in the group. The Multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. Figures in the rel. column are predictors of relative OVV, with the chilcrs Total Viewing Time controlled. Figures in the abs. column are predictors of absolute OVV.

212 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 305

Table 24: Multiple regression analysis of violence viewing, by parental punishment and affection: Maryland data

Boys Girls Overall Independent variable Grade abs. rel. abs. rel. abs. rel. Affection jr. hi+.04 +.03 +.05 +.05 +.02 .00 sr. hi +.11 +.10 -.09 -.09 Physical punishment jr. hi-.04 +.02 -.10 -.10 -.05 -.01 sr. hi+.16 +.11 -.14 -.12 Verbal punishment jr. hi-.03 -.03 -.01 -.01 -,.03 -.04 sr. hi-.06 -.07 +.10 +.05

+.10 Restrictive jr. hi +.07 +.03 +.11 +.15* +.12** punsihment sr. hi+.16 +.15 +.08 +.07 jr. hi .08 .15 Multiple correlation .15* sr. hi .19 .21 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136)

Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between each family envirimment variable and the Overall Violence Viewing Index, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. Figures in the rel. column are predictors of relative OVV, withthe child's Total Viewing Time controlled. Figures in the abs. column are predictors of absolute OVV.

. 313

immarid 306 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 25: Grand multiple regression analysis of violence viewing: Wisconsin data

Boys Girls Overall Independent variable Grade abs. rel. abs. rel. abs. rel.

Total viewing time jr. hi +.34 +.42* +.26" sr. hi +.21 +.21 School performance jr. hi-.01 +.06 -.12 -.01 -.06 -.03 sr. hi+.05 +.06 -.12 -.11 Socioeconomic status jr. hi-.39"-.30 +.04 +.27 -.11 -.06 sr. hi-.35"-.32 +.04 +.06 Concept-oriented FCP jr. hi+.02 +.07 +.10 +.11 -.05 -.05 sr. hi -.08 -.09 -.15 -.18 Socio-oriented FCP jr. hi-.25 -.32 +.50* +.51* +.11 +.06 sr. hi-.02 -.05 +.01 -.06 Affection jr. hi +.30 +.26 -.18 -.34 +.11 +.08 sr. hi+.30 +.29 +.21 +.20 Physical punishment jr. hi +.11 +.25 +.06 t.09 +.09 +.10 sr. hi-.08 -.09 +.05 +.04 Verbal punishment jr. hi-.09 -.18 +.27 +.25 +.12 +.08 sr. hi+.34" +.32 +.25 +.25 Restrictive jr. hi +.23 +.25 -.29 -.23 +.11 +.12 punishment sr. hi .00 +.07 +.34* +.30 Multiple correlation jr. hi .54 .58 sr. hi .54 .52 jr. hi (38) (43) (N) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between each variable and the Overall Violence Viewing Index, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. Figures in the rel. column are predictors of relative OVV, with the child's Total Viewing Time controlled. Figures in the abs. column are predictors of absolute OVV.

. 314 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 307

Table 26: Grand multiple regression analysis of violence viewing: Maryland data

Boys Girls Overall Independent variable Grade abs. rel. abs. rel. abs. rel.

Total viewing time jr. hi +.30 +.08 +.31* sr. hi +.37** +.43* School performance jr. hi-.11 -.09 -.11 -.12 -.20** -.17** sr. hi -.28"-.26 -.14 -.06 Father occupation jr. hi -.09 -.07 +.09 +.09 -.10* -.07 sr. hi -.36"-.33 -.11 -.04 Affection jr. hi+.07 +.05 +.05 +.04 +.03 +.02 sr. hi +.17 +.17 -.07 -.08 Physical punishment jr. hi-.04 +.02 -.09 -.09 -.03 .00 sr. hi+.10 +.16 -.14 -.12 Verbal punishment jr. hi-.01 -.01 -.01 -.01 -.02 -.03 sr. hi -.12 -.12 +.09 +.04 Restrictive jr. hi+.09 +.05 +.11 +.10 +.14**+.12" punishment sr. hi+.15 +.15 +.08 +.06 Multiple correlation jr. hi .17 .20 .28** sr. hi .50" .29 (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficietns between each variable and the Overall Violence Viewing Index, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. Figures in the rel. column are predictors of relative OVV, with the child'i Total Viewing Time controlled. Figures in the abs. column are predictorc ibsolute OVV.

,315 308 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 27: Multiple regression analysis of cognitive reactionsto television violence, by family environment variables: Wisconsin data

I ndependent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Family Communication Patterns Concept-orientation jr. hi +.02 +.08 sr. hi -.27 +.14 -.06 Socio-orientation jr. hi -.27 +.29 sr. hi +.09 +46 Multiple correlation. jr. hi .27 .30 sr. hi .28 .46* .20 Parental Punishment and Affection Affection jr. hi +.30 +.14 sr. hi -.10 +.23 +.20* Physical punishment jr. hi +.19 +.21 sr. hi +.05 +.28 +.12 Verbal punishment jr. hi -.05 -.38 sr. hi +.06 -.12 Restrictive punishment jr. hi -.16 +.33 sr. hi +.06 +.42" +.17*

Multiple correlation jr. hi .34 .48 sr. hi .18 .51* .31"

(N) jr. hi (313) (30) sr. hi -1431 (40) (151) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between each familyenvironment variable and the Overall Cognitive Reaction Sum, controlling forall other independent variables in the group. The multiple correlation entries represent thetotal relationship with the entire set of independent variables.

Table 28: Multiple regression analysis of cognitive reactionsto television violence, by parental affection and punishment: Maryland data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Affection jr. hi -.15 +.15 sr. hi -.19 -.06 -.03 Physical punishment jr. hi -.04 -.01 sr. hi +.01 -.14 -.05 Verbal punishment jr. hi +.10 .00 sr. hi +.34 +.15 +.11* Restrictive punishment jr. hi +.12 +.21* sr. hi +.01 +.22* +18" Multiple correlation jr. hi .25 .25 sr. hi .41* .31 .23" (N) jr. hi (122) (108) sr. hi (107) (136) 14731 Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between eachfamily environment variable and the Overall Cognitive Reaction Sum, controlling for allother independent variables. The multiple correlation enti ies represent the total relationshipwith the entire set of independent variables. 316 ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 309

Table 29: Grand multiple regression analysis of cognitive reactions to television violence: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall School performance jr. hi -.40* -.15 -.19' sr. hi -.21 -.20 +.19 Socioeconomic status jr. hi +.07 +.15 sr. hi -.18 +.11 Concept-oriented FCP jr. hi -.10 -.13 -.19* sr. hi -.23 -.03 +.19 Socio-oriented FCP jr. hi -.24 +.13 sr. hi -.09 +38* Affection jr. hi +.37* +.11 +.23" sr. hi +10 +.26 +.21 Physical punishment jr. hi +.08 +.13 sr. hi -.03 +.30 Verbal punishment jr. hi -.03 -.37 -.15 sr. hi +.20 -.32 +.28 Restrictive punishment jr. hi -.12 +.13 sr. hi +.03 +.28 Multiple correlation jr. hi .59 .53 .41" sr. hi .42 .65* (38) (30) (N) jr. hi (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between eachvariable and the Overall Cognitive Reaction Sum, controlling for all other independent variables.The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire setof independent variables.

317 310 TELEVISION AND ADOI.ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 30: Grand multiple regression analysis of cognitive reactions to television violence: Maryland data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall

School performance jr. hi -.02 -.20 -.121 sr. hi +.04 -.04 jr. hi -.10 -.19 Father occupation -.09 sr. hi -.10 -.05 jr. hi -.13 +.14 Affection -.02 sr. hi -.19 -.05 Physical punishment jr. hi -.04 +.03 -.03 sr. hi +.02 +.14 jr. hi +.11 .00 Verbal punishment +.11" sr. hi +.35'1 +.14 Restrictive punishment jr. hi +.14 +.17" sr. hi .00 +.21 jr. hi .27 .38* Multiple correlation .28** sr. hi .42" .32* jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (1 36)

Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between each variable and the Overall Cognitive Reaction Sum, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables.

Table 31: Multiple regression analysis of combined self- and other-reports of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental punishment and affection: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall

Overall violence viewing jr. hi +.01 +.34 +.26" sr. hi +.29 +.13 Parental punishment sum jr. hi +.27 +.17 +.20" sr. hi +.28 +.30 +.18 -.21 Parental affection jr. hi -.07 sr. hi -.33* +.14 Multipla correlation jr. hi .29 .42 .36" sr. hi 49* .36 (N) (38) (30) jr. hi (151) sr. hi (43) (40)

Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Combined Aggression Sum and each family and viewing variable, controlling for all other independent vari- ables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. All independent variable measures are comparable to Maryland measures. is ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 311

Table 32: Grand multiple regression analysis of combined self- and other-reports of aggressive behavior, by violence viewing, parental affection and punishment, child's irritability, and child's perceived learning of aggression from television: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing jr. hi +.05 +.27 +.22" sr. hi +.29 +.16 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.28 +.38* +.36" sr. hi -.02 +.28 Buss-Durkee irritability jr. hi +.18 +.25" sr. hi -.03 jr. hi +.14 +.09 Parental punishment sum +.15 sr. hi +.28 +.24

Parental affection jr. hi -.07 -.16 -.10 sr. hi -.33* +.13 Multiple correlation jr. hi .55* .57 .54" sr. hi .49 .57

jr. hi 1381 (30) (N) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Combined Aggression Sum and each independent variable, controlling all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of in- dependent variables. All independent variable measures are comparable to Maryland measures.

Table 33: Grand multiple regression analysis of combined self- and other-reports of aggressive behavior, by exposure and reactions to television violence: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall

Overall violence viewing (now) jr. hi +.01 +.25 +.16 sr. hi +.23 +.13 Violence viewing when younger jr. hi +.29 -.01 +.20* sr. hi +.14 +.02 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.21 +.38 sr. hi +.04 +.26 +.36" Multiple correlation jr. hi .36 .51 .50** sr. hi .30 .32

(N) jr. hi 138/ 1301 (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Combined Aggression Sum and each independent variable, controlling all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of in- dependent variables.

319 312 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 34: Correlations between violence viewing and aggressive behavior, at high vs low parental emphasis on nonaggressive behavior: Wisconsin data Boys Girls Overall Aggression index Grade Low High LowHigh LowHigh +.42+.31 SELF-REPORT jr. hi +.15+.03 +.34+.23 SUM sr. hi -.12+.56 +.30+.10 +.05 +.33-.27 OTH E R-REPORT jr. hi -.12 +.33-.08 SUM sr. hi +.24+.20 +.24-.20 +.55+.01 COMBINED SUM jr. hi +.16-.06 +.39+.15 sr. hi +.06+.45 +.30+.02 (15) (15) (14) (N) jr. hi (22) (82)(65) sr. hi (23) (19) (22) (17) Note: Cell entries are zero-order correlation coefficients between the ViolenceViewing Index and each aggression index. Respondents were sorted into high vs low groups on the Parental Emphasis on Nonaggression Index by cutting at the midpoint within each grade level. For the junior high group, Low = 0-3 and High = 48; for the senior high group, Low = 0-2 and High = 3-8. Higher scores indicate a greater parental stress on non-violent responses to social conflict situations.

Table 35: Multiple regression analysis of aggression approval, by violence viewing, perceived learning of aggression from television, and parental affection and punishment: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing jr. hi +.11 -.19 sr. hi +.04 -.07 Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.20 +.19 sr. hi +.22 +.15 Parental punishment sum jr. hi -.19 +.09 sr. hi +.04 +.22 Parental affection jr. hi -.22 +.06 sr. hi -.22 -.11 Multiple correlation jr. hi .30 .26 sr. hi .34 .36 jr. hi (38) (30) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Approvalof Aggression Index and each independent variable, controlling for all other independentvariables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire setof independent variables. All independent and dependent variable measures are compar- able to Maryland measures.

320 7 V11.1

ADOLESCENTS, PARENTS, TELEVISION USE 313

Table 36: Multiple regression analysis of aggression approval, by violence viewing, perceived learning of aggression from television, and parental affection and punishment: Maryland data

I ndependent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing jr. hi -.02 +.13 sr. hi +.17 .00 MO* +.39. Perceived learning of aggression jr. hi +.08 +.29" sr. hi +.39.* +.13 Parental punishment sum jr. hi -.12 -.11 +.01 sr. hi +.03 +.15 Parental affection jr. hi -.20* -.03 -.10* sr. hi +.05 -.20* Multiple correlation jr. hi .25 .41" sr. hi .44** .35** (N) jr. hi (122) (108) (473) sr. hi (107) (136) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Approval of Aggression Index and each independent variable, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables.

Table 37: Grand multiple regression analysis of aggressive attitudes: Wisconsin data

Independent variable Grade Boys Girls Overall Overall violence viewing jr. hi -.07 -.22 +.09 sr. hi +.23 +.07 jr. hi +.12 Perceived learning of aggression +.52** +.32** sr. hi +.04 +.01 Parental punishment sum jr. hi -.06 +.21 +.09 sr. hi +.13 +.07 Parental affection jr. hi -.42* -.36 sr. hi -.31 -.26 jr. hi +.24 +.15 Sociooriented FCP +.05 sr. hi +.05 +.02 jr. hi -.04 -.13 Conceptoriented FCP -.15 sr. hi -.29 -.09 Multiple correlation jr. hi .62** .47 .52" sr. hi .55* .41 (N) jr. hi (38) (30) (151) sr. hi (43) (40) Note: Cell entries are partial correlation coefficients between the Aggressive Attitudes Index and each independent variable, controlling for all other independent variables. The multiple correlation entries represent the total relationship with the entire set of independent variables. The Aggressive Attitudes Index subsumes the Approval of Aggression Index. The first four independent variables are comparable to Maryland measures.

321 Attitudes Toward Violence: The Interaction of Television Exposure, Family Attitudes, and Social Class

Joseph R. Dominick and Bradley S. Greenberg

Michigan State University

This project examines the interaction of exposure to television vio- lence with children's attitudes toward violence, within the context of family attitudes toward violence and the child's social environment) Several researchers have specified that television is likely to be most influential when the child is exposed to a set of ideas or behaviors which recur from program to program, when he or she is a heavy user of the medium, and when he or she is likely to have limited contact with or in- formation from other socialization agencies and consequently to have less firm values against which to compare the media themes (Schramm, Lyle, and Parker, 1961; Himmelweit, Oppenheim, and Vince, 1958; Maccoby, 1964). 314 T11,1,

ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 315 Our basic rationale posits that a child who is a heavy viewer of televi- sion violence and whose family has not actively pointed out that vio- lence is noxious will have a more positive view of aggression as a mode of conduct. To test this rationale, we must first specify what ideas about violence television presents to the child, then identify the likely role of family and environment in shaping the ideas he or she brings to this area of socialization. Content analyses indicate that the television world is a violent one. Although definitions of violent content vary greatly, several studies are consistent in this conclusion. A survey by theChristian Science Moni- tor,conducted six weeks after Robert Kennedy's assassination in 1968, found 84 killings in 85 1/2 hours of prime time and Saturday program- ming. The most violent evening hours, according to the newspaper's study, v. ere 7:30-9 p.m.;- when approximately 27 million children aged two to 17 were watching. During that time period, one violenrincid'ehr occurred every 16 minutes, a murder or killing every half-hour. Gerbner (1969) substantiated these findings in a more sophisticated analysis. He found that acts of violence occurred in eight of every ten programs. Dramatic programs averaged seven violent episodes; cartoon shows had three times that number. During one week, 400 people were killed on prime time programs. Gerbner (1969b) also analyzed certain personality attributes of violent characters, who were judged to be more logical and efficient than nonviolent characters in most programs. More germane to the present research are studies which examined the role of violence in problem solving. Stempel (1969) identified the means used to solve problems during one week's network television programs. Of 202 problems presented, nearly 60 percent were solved by violent tactics. One-third were solved nonviolently, and the remainder went unresolved. Larsen, Gray, and Fortis (1968) identified "program goals" and the means by which these goals were achieved. Violent means were the most prevalent. These researchers also found that children's programs were even more likely than adult programs to use violence to achieve goals. These studies support certain generalizations: 1. A child who watches an average amount of television is likely to see a substantial amount of violent content; 2. Violence typically is presented as a highly successful means of goal achievement; 3. During the 1968-69 season, violence was the predominant means of conflict rtsolution found in television drama. What the child brings to this television experience will be the result of his or her prior socialization experiences. Research indicates that the family is the key factor in the development of most children's attitudes toward violence. Most likely a family member becomes the child's first target for violence. As the child grows older, most of his or her conflicts 323 316 'TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS are with siblings. It is primarily the parent who rewards or punishes this aggressive behavior (Sears, Maccoby. and Levin, 1957). The family not only administers positive and negative reinforcements for aggression, but also may provide alternative models for problem solving which are essentially nonviolent: decision-making, arbitration, compromise. Use of these methods varies from family to family. An early study (Sewell, Mussem, and Harris, 1955) isolated one major family pattern along a democracy-autocracy continuum and found parent-child conversations and formalized techniques for solving family conflicts used more fre- quently in the more democratic families. McLeod, Chaffee, and Eswara (1966. 1967) have suggested that com- munication patterns within families can affect a child's socialization. Among four family types they identified, they labeled one "pluralistic." In such a family, they said, a child is likely to be exposed to both sides of an issue, and discussion of controversial matters is encouraged. This family style appears to expose the child more readily to nonviolent methods of problem solving. Parents may also influence their children's ideas about violence in a more direct way. Adult comments about television content can serve as important learning cues for children (Hicks, 1965). If a parent says vio- lence is inappropriate while watching a violent scene with his or her chil- dren, the children may develop more negative attitudes about violence. A child who repeatedly sees his parents watching violence while they calmly eat dinner may come to accept violence as more normative. Par- ents seem to have the opportunity to either counteract or legitimize tele- vision aggression while they watch it with their children (Sakuma, 1968). These studies support certain propositions: 1. The family is the first agency to deal with a child's aggressive be- havior; 2. Families can influence a child's attitudes tdward violence by giving positive or negative feedback when the child is aggressive, by using vio- lent or nonviolent methods of problem solving within the family, and by commenting on scenes of violence. 3. Families vary in their uses of these techniques. Socioeconomic background may also influence the pattern of effects that stem from exposure to television violence. A child from a low-in- come familyison the average afarheavier television viewer thanishis middle-class counterpart (Schramm ,Lyle. and Parker, 1961; Greenberg and Dominick, 1969-70) and is thereby exposed to more violent episodes automatically. Economic background also may effect what the child brings to the television viewing situation. Allinsmith (1960) found that children of low socioeconomic status were likely to respond to potentially frustrating situations with the most direct forms of aggression. Lower-income youngsters habitually expressed more aggression than did their middle-class peers. Further, the environment 324 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 317 of the poor contains more frequent acts of physical violence than does the middle-class environment (U.S. National Commission on Civil Dis- orders, 1968). Fighting with peers, violent incidents among neighbors, and disputes with police characterize many lower-class environments. The lower-class family may also do less to inhibit aggression. Among low-income families, parent-child interactions are often erratic and in- consistent. Parents and children may see one another on a less systemat- ic, more disorganized basis (Minuchin, Braulio, Gurney, Risman, and Schumer, 1967). Because fewer fathers are present in families, mothers are forced to work, further fragmenting interactionwith their children. To this point, we have attempted to pinpoint the interactive roles of exposure to televised violence, the family, and social classin contribut- ing to the child's attitudes about aggression and violence. In essence, the question becomes to what extent the norms of television violence (its frequency, effectiveness, and acceptability), the presence or absence of perceived family sanctions, and the child's social class environment affect the following attitudinal components: 1. Approval of violence: To what extent does the child perceive that violence is an acceptable mode of behavior? 2.Willingness to use violence: When presented with hypothetical "real-life" problems, to what extent will the child choose violent solu- tions? 3. Effectiveness of violence: How effective does the child perceive violence to be as a means of problem solving? 4. Solutions to conflict situations: Given an opportunity to propose a solution to a problem, does the child suggest a violent one? The child's sex is another important factor in the way he or she ex- presses hostile and aggressive behavior. Not surprisingly, boyshave been found to be more overtly aggressive in many studies (Walters, Pearce, and Dahms, 1957). Boys have been shown to be significantly more aggressive in play (Levin and Sears, 1956) and theirplay to be more violent and physically damaging than that of girls(Sears, 1951). Attitudes toward the use of aggression show similar differences. Sears (1961) found that girls displayed higher levels of anxiety about aggres- sion than did boys and were significantly less tolerant of what Sears termed "antisocial" aggression. In one study of the effects of mediated violence, girls exhibited less imitative behavior than boys after watching adults perform violent acts (Bandura, 1965). When offered an incentive, however, girls remembered as many aggressive acts as did the boys. Hypotheses Three main antecendent variables (exposure, family attitudes, and social class) have been discussed; each should exert a'separate influence

. 325 318 1ELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS on a child's attitudes toward violence. In addition,the child's sex may affect violence attitudes and behavior. The rationale of this studyyields these hypotheses: 1.Youngsters with more exposure to television violence will indi- cate greater approval of violent acts. he morewilling to use vio- lence. perceive violence to be a more effective way of solving problems. and more readily suggest a violent means of resolving a problem. 2.Youngsters who perceive that their families are strongly opposed to the use of aggression will indicate less approvalof violence. perceive violence to be a less effective means of solving prob- lems. and less readily suggest a violent means of resolving a prob- lem. A discussion of the effects of social class differences yieldsparallel hypotheses. but the separate impact of this variable is of secondary in- terest here. Youngsters from more disadvantaged homes areexpected to indicate greater approval of violence, to believe it to be more effective. and so on. Of more interest is the predicted interaction of socialclass with the other antecedant variables. The impact of exposure to television violence should interact both with a child's social class and with his or her family's attitudes. Low- income children who watrh more television are more likely tohave preexisting favorable attitudes about violence than are middle-class youngsters. Therefore, in terms of first-orderinteractions, a third hy- pothesis is: 3.More exposure to television violence in conjunction with low so- cioeconomic status will result in greater approval of violence, more willingness to use violence,higher perceived effectiveness of violence, and greater readiness to suggest violent solutions to problems. In addition, as Schramm. Lyle. and Parker (1961) emphasized. televi- sion's potential effects should be the reciprocal of the influenceof more personal sources. Given families which provide the child little orambig- uous information about the appropriatenessof violence, and in which .. he is heavily exposed to television violence, a fourth hypothesisdirectly parallels those made for the interaction of exposwe and socialclass: &More exposure to televised violence among children whosefami- lies have not stipulated antiviolence attitudes willbe related to greater approval of violence. more willingness to useviolence, higher perceived effectiveness of violence4 and greater readiness to suggest it in problem solving. Finally. the intersect of all these antecedent conditions is expected to maximize tolerance for aggression. The lower-class youngsterwho is a heavy viewer of violence and who rzceives little contraryinformation

326 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 319 from his family should be most accepting of the norms of the world of television violence. Thus: 5.The interaction of more exposure to televised violence with low exposure to counterinformation and low socioeconomic status will manifest itself in more approval of violence, higher perceived effectiveness of violence, and higher salience for violent solutions to problems. Data obtained from boys and girls were analyzed separately. The gen- eral expectation was that the same antecedent variables would be relat- ed to the shaping of aggression attitudes among both boys and girls. If anything, television exposure was expected to be an even more impor- tant factor in the shaping of such attitudes for girls than for boys.since girls generally have fewer experiences with physical violence and ag- gression in their daily lives.

METHODS

Questionnaires were completed by 434 boys and 404 girls in the fourth, fifth. and sixth grades in six Michigan schook during class ses- sions in May 1970. The schools were chosen on the basis of social and economic variation. About nine percent of the sample were black. Data from boys and girls were obtained at the same time, althoughtiley were analyzed separatdy. Antecedent variables For each sex. three antecedent variables were examined: the chil- dren's exposure to televised violence, their perceptions of their families' attitudes toward violence, and their families' socioeconomic status. Exposure to television violence.Each youngster received a list of 28 locally available television programs. Twenty of these programs had been judged by a sample of newspaper and magazine critics tocontain violence (Greenberg and Gordon. 1971). The number of programs of these 20 which the respondents reported watching each week were summed. Totals ranged from none to 20 among the boys and from none to 19 among the girls and were normally distributedwith a standard de- viation of 3.7 for boys and 4.4 for girls. Family attitudes toward violence.The children were asked seven questions about how they thought their parents felt about various forms of violencefor example. "Suppose you and your parents were watch- ing a TV show together and one of the people on TV shot another per- son. What do you think your parents wouldsayT or "Suppose one of your friends hit you. What do you think your parentswould want you to do?" Each item had two, three. or four response categories.The an- swers to all seven items correlatedsignificantly with one another. Corre- lations ranged from .38 to .70. The seven item scores were summed into 327 320 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS an index rangingfrom 7 (low approval of violence) to 17 (high approval of violence). Social class. Each child wrote down the job(s)of his parents. The principal job was coded on a 13-position scale ofoccupational prestige (Troldahl. 1967). The three antecedent variables were found to beintercorrelated from .09 to .10 for the boys and from .17 to .12 for the girls. Dependent variables

Four dependent variables were used. Approval of violence. Eight modified itemsfrom the Sears (1961) An- tisocial Aggression Scale were used. These weredeclarative sentences (for example. "I see nothing wrong in afight between two teenage boys" or "It's all right if a man slaps his wife")with three response cat- egories (agree; not sure; disagree). Scores were summedfor the eight items into an index ranging from 8 (low approval) to24 (high approval). Willingness to use violence. This index measuredthe child's willing- ness to use violence in "reallife." Five scale items were adopted from the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (Buss.1957). with "agree" to "disagree" as the available responses. Declarative sentencesdealt with whether or not the individual would use some sort ofphysical violence in certain situations ("Anybody who saysbad things about me is look- ing for a punch in the nose"). Item scores weresummed into an index with scores of 5 indicating low willingness to resort toviolence and 10 indicating high .villingness. Perceived effectiveness of violence. Five constructeditems measured the children's opinions of how effective violence was as a meansof solv- ing problems ("Sometimes a fight is the easiest way toget what you want" or "A fight is the best way to settle an argument onceand for all"). Three agree-disagree response categories wereused. Item scores were summed; 5 representedlow perceived effectiveness. and 15 repre- sented high perceived effectiveness. Suggested solutions to conflict situations.In four open-ended ques- tions, a potentially frustrating situation wasdescribed. The child wrote down the one thing he would most likely do onthat situation ("Pretend somebody you know takes something from youand breaks it on pur- pose. What would yeu do?" or"Pretend somebody you know tells lies about you. What would you do?") Responsesjudged to be nonviolent were scored 1. those judgedviolent scored 2. Violence was defined as behavior which would produce physical painin another. An index score of 4 indicated all nonviolent responses; a scoreof 8 represented all yio- lent responses. All items for each dependent variable weresummed into the con- structed indices. Interitem correlations forthe modified Sears Antisocial Aggression items were low and inconsistent.Interpretation of reiults for 328 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 321 this index should be restrained. Although some items in otherindices had low interitem correlations, they were retained for these analyses because of the overall interitem reliability for those indices. The four dependent variables intercorrelated from .13 to .37 among girls and from .22 to .43 among boyl. Therefore, they are to be interpret- ed not as completely independent attitudinal assessments,but as partial replicates of general attitudes toward the use of aggression. Each was analyzed as a dependent variable. Analytic procedures

The respondents were divided into eight subgroups of boys andeight subgroups of girls. A median split was made according to the occupa- tional prestige of the child's family. Those in the three lowestcategories of the ,13-step prestige scale (218 boys. 153 girls) were classifiedin the low-income category; those in the other prestige categories (216boys, 251 girls) were classified in the middle-income group. A second median split for each subgroup was madeaccording to the number of violent programs each child watched each week. Themedian was eight programs per week for theboys and seven program s for the girls. Finally, each subgroup was divided according to the indexof the child's family's attitudes toward violence. The distribution wasskewed toward the low-approval end of the scale. Scores of 7-10(216 boys. 210 girls) were placed in the low-approval group. More than 90 percentof the remaining children's scores indicated that they were unsure ordidn't know how their parents felt about violence. Scores of 11 orhigher (218 boys, 194 girls) were categorized as "undefined." Fewer than ten per- cent of the boys reported that their families gave strongapproval to vio- lence. Data for the girls' and boys samples were analyzedseparately. The results will be presented separately for Analysis 1 (boys'sample) and Analysis 2 (girls' sample).

RESULTSANALYSIS 1

Results are presented for four dependent behaviors: theboys' ap- proval of aggression; their willingness to use violence;the extent to which they perceive violence to be effective; and theirreadiness to sug- gest violent solutions to problems. For each, hypotheses were made step-wise throughmain effects and interactions. The results will be discussed in that fashion,although the interactions. where found, qualify interpretations of themain effects. 329 311 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Because of the lack of correlation among the antecedent variables, a three-way analysis of variance with unequal cells (Snedecor. 1956) was performed on each of the dependent measures. Approval of aggression

Table 1 presents the results of the three-way analysis for this measure as well as the individual cell means.

Table 1: Approval of aggression (boys) Cell means (The higher the score, the more approval of aggression) Exposum Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: r toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined Low 14.13 15.03 15.29 16.65 (rv,47) (n=60) (n=62) (n=40) High 14.14 16.52 14.68 16.17 (n=57) (n=52) (n=50) (n=66) Analysis of variance table Source of variation MS df

Exposure to violence 4.0 1 0.59 n.s. Family attitudes 223.0 1 33.14 .0005 Social class 54.0 1 7.86 .025 TV Violence X Family attitudes 28.0 1 4.15 .05 TV Violence X Social class 24.0 1 3.56 .10 Social class X Family attitudes 1.0 1 0.01 n.s. Violence X Family X Class 30.0 1 4.46 .05 Error 6.74 426 Total 433

Significant differences were obtained in terms of perceived family atti- tudes toward aggression and the social class of the youngster, but there was no main effect difference between those more and less exposed to television violence. Cell comparisons indicate that in the four possible comparisons be- tween youngsters whose families gave low approval to violence and those whose attitudes were ill-defined, the mean differences were con- sistent and large. Three of four social class cell comparisons yielded similar results. The two first-order interactionsof exposure to television violence with either family attitudes or social classwere also as predicted. High exposure to television violence coupled with less certainty about family attitudes maximind the approval of aggression. Low exposure to televi- sion violence in conjunction with a middle-class background minimized 330 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 323 the approval of aggression. Thus, althoughtelevision exposure by itself was insufficient to yielddifferences in aggression approval, its interac- tion with each of the other antecedent variables was nottrivial. Table I also indicates a significant three-wayinteraction which is diffi- cult to interpret, particularly because the patternof means within the lower class is inconsistent with the predictions. To clarify this anomaly. one additional analysis wasdone. This was a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA)within each of the social class groupings. It was repeated for all dependent measures.The pattern found here was consistent. Among the middle-class youngsters. expo- sure to television violence made somedifference (p<.10), as did family attitudes (p<.01) and the interaction of the two (p<.05). Among the lower-class boys, only family attitudes were an important discriminant (p<.01). Willingness to use violence Table 2 contains the results of the three-wayanalysis of variance for this attitudinal variable. Table 2: Willingness to use violence (boys) Cell means

(The higher the score, the more willingness to useviolence)

Exposure Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined 7.70 7.77 8.42 Low 7.27 (n.40) In.47) (n=60) 111..62) 8.53 High 7.28 8.60 7.64 (n.57) (n.52) (n=50) Ina661 An analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS df .05 Exposure to TV violence 6.9 1 4.06 40.34 .0005 Family attitudes 68.5 1 9.41 .005 Social class 16.0 1 12.2 1 7.18 .025 TV violence X Family attitudes Mt TV violence X 3ocial ciass 1.2 1 0.70 Social class X Family attitudes 0.1 1 0.10 n.s. .10 Violence X Family X Class 5.2 1 3.06 Error 1.71 426 Total 433 Main effects predictions were supported for allthree antecedent vari- ables. Maximum willingness to resort toviolence in conflict situations came from more exposure toviolent television content. from families with less defined attitudes toward aggression4 andfrom the lower in- 331 324 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

come groupings. Here. as for all attitude segments. the family variable was the most discriminating. Exposure to violence and family attitudes interacted in thesame man- ner as in the Approval of Aggression index. High exposure and unde- fined attitudes in the home maximized the willingness touse violence. The predicted interaction between exposure and social classwas not supported. The three-way interaction was weaker in this analysis butmore con- sistent with predictions. Willingness to use violencewas increasingly present in the lower-class conditions. except in the cell comprised of boys with high exposure to television violence and undefined family atti- tudes, where there was no mean difference. Again the two-way analysis aided interpretation. Only among the middle-class youngsters was a difference attributable f.o extent' of expo- sure to television violence (p <.05). It washed out among the lower-class boys. For both groups. family attitUdes were critical (p <.01). But only for the middle-class youngsters did family attitudes interact significantly (p < .05) with television exposure. These latter results exactly parallel those found for the appro'val of aggression index. Use of violence in conflict situations

This measure was a second approach to the one just described. The principal difference was that the youngsters were freely suggesting vio- lent or nonviolent solutions rather than evaluating proposed ones. Re- sults of the analysis are in Table 3. Main effects were found for family attitudes and for social class. Tele- vision exposure made no difference in the freeresponses. Neither pre- dicted first-order interaction was significant. The second-order interac- tion was significant, but the same inconsistencies are present in the data for the lower-class youngsters. In the analyses done for each of the social class groupings. family atti- tudes toward violence were again crucial. For the middle-class youngs- ters, the predicted interaction between ttelevision violence and family attitudes was again significant (p <.01), but not for the lower-class boys. For neither group was television exposure alone critical.

Perceived effectiveness of violence

Table 4 contains the results of the three-way analysis of variance for this dependent variable. Each of the main effects was significant and large. Violence was considered to be more effective in all four high tele- vision exposure conditions, the four undefined family attitude condi- tions. and the four lower-class cells. 332 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 325

Table 3: Use of violence in conflict situations (boys) Cell means (The higher the score, the more of ien the child usesviolence to solve conflicts)

Exposure Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined Low 4.57 4.65 4.85 5.25 (n=47) (n=60) (n=62) (n=40) 5.10 High 4.49 5.26 4.78 (n=57) (n=51) (n=50) (n=66) An analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS clf n.s Exposure to TV violence 2.0 1 2.08 14.58 .005 Family attitudes 14.0 1 .025 Social class 1 7.29 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes ;..°8 1 2.96 n.s. TV violence X Social class 2.2 1 2.29 n.s. Social class X Family attitudes 0.5 1 0.19 .05 Violence X Family X Class 4.5 1 4.68 Error 0.96 425 Total 432

Table 4: Perceived effectiveness of violence (boys) Cell means (The higher the score, the more violence is seen as beingeffective)

Exposure Middle class Lower class Family attitudes to TV Family attitudes 1 violence: toward aggression toward aggression ) Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined 10.90 Low 7.83 9.22 8.68 I (n=47) (n=60) (n=62) (n=40) 1

11.50 1 High 8.67 11.08 9.54 (n=66) '(n=57) (n=52) (n.--50) An analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS df .005 Exposure to TV violence 135.0 1 15.79 .0005 Family attitudes 416.0 1 49.81 , .005 Social class 93.0 1 10.93 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes 12.0 1 1.44 n.s TV violence X Social class 0.5 1 0.01 n.s. Social class X Family attitudes 5.5 1 0.64 n.s. Violence X Family X Class 12.0 1 1.44 Error 8.55 426 Total 433

333 326 TEI.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

None of the predicted two-or three-way interactions approached sig- nificance. Parallel two-way analyses of variance were made for each of the so- cial class groups. For both the middle-class and lower-class boys, televi- sion exposure and family attitudes were significant antecedent condi- tions. No interaction existed. Violence was judged to be maximally effective when television exposure was high or family attitudes were unclear. Summary of analyses

Given four dependent measures with moderate intercorrelations for three antecedent conditions, the degree of consistency across measures can be examined. Table 5 provides an overall summary of the analyses.

Table 5: Summary across dependent variables (boys) Dependent variables

Antecedent Approval WillingnessUse of violencePei ceived variables of to use in conflict effectiveness aggression violence situations of violence Exposure to TV violence n.s. .05 n.s. .01 Family attiti:des .01 .01 .01 .01 Social class .05 .01 .05 .01 TV Violence X Family .05 .05 n.s. n.s. TV Violence X Class .10 ns. n.s. n.s. Family X Class n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Violence X Family X Class .05 .10 .05 n.s. Middle-class

Exposure .10 .05 n.s. .01 Family attitudes .01 .01 .01 .01 Exposure by Family attitudes .05 .05 .01 n.s. Lowerclass Exposure n.s. n.s. n.s. .05 Family attitudes .01 .01 .05 .01 Exposure by Family attitudes n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s.

For two of four measures-the individual's willingnessto use vio- lence and its perceived effectiveness when used-television exposure makes a direct contribution. With higher exposure comes more approval of violence. For all four measures, both family attitudes toward aggression (as known to the child) and the social environment of the family havea per- sistent impact. Family attitudes account for the largest portion of vari- ance. followed by social class differences. Where television exposure does interact with either family attitudes or social class, the two variables serve to intensify the acceptance of violent norms. but exposure interacts irregularly. in three of eight possi- ble instances. 334 .4=1.`

ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 327

The irregularities or inconsistencies arelargely clarified in the analy- ses which partial outthe social class differences. Amongmiddle-class boys, the television exposure variable is morepredictive, alone and in interaction with the attitudes of the youngsters'family. Among the low- er-class boys. only family attitudes are auseful predictor of attitudes toward aggressive behavior.

RESULTS-ANALYSIS 2

Results are presented for the four dependentvariables. Willingness to use violence

Table 6 presents the three-way analysisof variance for this attitudinal variable. Two of the main effects weresignificant. The strongest predic- tor of willingness to use violence wasthe perceived attitude of the fami- ly. The main effect for exposure totelevision violence was also signifi- cant. Preteen girls who were moreregularly exposed to television vio- lence expressed more willingness to useviolence than did those less exposed. Neither the main effect forsocial class nor any of the interac- tions was significant. Use of violence in conflictsituations

The principal difference betweenthis measure and the respondent's expressed willingness to use violence wasthat here a free response was given and coded. Results are in Table 7. Again, main effects were found for the familyattitude variable and for exposure to television violence.Social class did not further differen- tiate. Girls from families whose attitudes towardviolence were ambigu- ous offered more violentsolutions. Similarly. more violent suggestions were made by those youngsterswho were heavier viewers of televised violence. No interactions existed. Perceived effectiveness of violence

As is evident in Table 8. family attitudesshowed a strong relationship with this attitudinal measure. An equally strongrelationship was found on the basis of exposure totelevision violence. In general, those girls who watched a great deal of television violence were morelikely to per- ceive violence as effective. Social classdifferences did not emerge. One interaction was significant. Maximumperceived effectiveness of violence existed among lower-class familieswhose attitudes toward violence were ambiguous: violence wasminimally effective for the middle-class youngsters with clear antiviolence norms. 335 318 TELEVISION AND ADOI.ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 6: Willingness to use violence (girls)

Cell means

1 (The higher the score, the more willingness to use violence) Exposure Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined Low 6.60 7.42 6.71 7.28 (n=73) (n-64) (n=38) (n=29) High 7.02 7.67 6.90 7.76 (n=59) (n=55) (n=40) (n=46) Analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS df

Exposure to TV violence 11.94 1 5.76 .025 Family attitudes 57.16 1 27.49 .0005 Social class 0.01 1 n.s. TV violence X Social class 0.12 1 0.06 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes 0.72 1 0.34 n.s. Family X Class 0.12 1 0.06 n.s.

Violence X Family X Class 0.01 1 Error 2.07 396 Less than 0.01

Table 7: Suggested use of violence in conflict situations (girls)

Cell means (The higher the score, the more frequently the child suggests violence to solve conflict)

Exposure Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined Low 4.30 4,72 4.32 4.41 (n=73) (nr64) (n=38) (n=29) High 4.37 4.89 4.48 4.93 (n-59) (n=55) (n=40) (n=46) Analysis of variance table Source of variation MS df F P

Exposure to TV violence 4.00 1 5.88 .025 Family attitudes 15.84 1 23.30 .0005 Social class 0.01 1 0.02 n.s. TV violence X Social class 1.28 1 1.88 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes 0.57 1 0.84 n.s. Family X Class 1.16 1 1.78 n.s. Violence X Family X Class 1.14 1 1.77 n.s. Error 0.68 396 ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 329 Table 8: Perceived effectiveness of violence(girls) Cell means

(The higher the score, the more violence is seen asbeing ef fective) Lower class Exposure Middle class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined 9.03 Low 7.12 8.80 8.24 (n=73) (n=64) (n=38) (n=29) 8.73 9.91 8.22 10.77 High (n=46) (n=59) (n=55) (n.40) Analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS df .0005 Exposure to TV violence 173.71 1 24.39 .0005 Family attitudes 200.43 1 28.15 n.s. Social class 15.16 1 2.13 TV violence X Social class 0.01 1 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes 9.09 1 1.27 Family X Class 65.61 1 9.21 .025 n.s. Violence X Family X Class 11.99 1 1.68 Error 7.12 396

Approval of aggression

Only the main effect of the familyattitudes variable was significant for this scale. the least reliable of the measuresused. In Table 9, girls from families negatively inclined towardviolence had lower scores than girls from families whose attitudes were moreundefined. No other main ef- fect nor any interaction wassignificant. Summary

The results show strongconsistency for the four dependent measures. The measures themselves weremoderately intercorrelated (.13 to.37). For all four measures. familyattitudes toward aggression (asreported by the child) showed the mostpersistent relationship to the child's ag- gressive attitudes. Exposure to television violencealso made a consistent. independent contribution to the child's notionsabout violence. The greater thelevel of exposure to televisionviolence, the more the child waswilling to use violence, to suggest it as a solution toconflict, and to perceive it as effective. Contrary to expectations.there were no social class differencesin at- girls titudes toward aggression.Perhaps both lower- and middle-class litera- receive similar instructions aboutits undesirability, although the ture suggests otherwise. 337 330 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT GGRESSIVENESS

Table 9: Approval of aggression (girls) Cell Means (The higher the score, tho more expressed approval of aggression) Exposure Middle class Lower class to TV Family attitudes Family attitudes violence: toward aggression toward aggression Low approval Undefined Low approval Undefined Low 13.63 1 5.09 13.61 14.34 (n=73) (n=64) (n.38) (n.29) High 14.44 1 4.9 3 13.50 15.61 (n=59) (n=5 5) (n=40) (n.46) Analysis of variance table

Source of variation MS df F P

Exposure to TV violence 16.00 1 2.8 9 n.s. Family attitudes 164.77 1 29.66 .0005 Social class 3.84 1 0.69 n.s. TV violence X Social class 2.56 1 0.4 8 n.s. TV violence X Family attitudes 0.08 1 0.01 n.s.

Family X Class 10.94 1 1.9 8 n.s. Violence X Family X Class 13.82 1 2.4 9 n.s. Error 5.53 396

DISCUSSION

The hypotheses and the rationale behind them were generally support- ed by the data. Among the middle-class boys. ill-defined family attitudes and above-average viewing of violence interacted to yield the highest level of approval of aggression, willingness to use violence, and suggest- ed use of violence to solve problems. Among the lower-class boys. the interaction was not evident; perceived family attitudes were the key predictors. Among the less advantaged, only perceived effectiveness of violence was directly related to television exposure. Among the girls, the strongest indicator was undefined family atti- tudes toward aggression; this factor was associated with more approval of aggression, more willingness to use violence, more perceived effec- tiveness of violence, and more suggestions of violence to solve prob- lems. High exposure to television violence was related to more aggres- sive attitudes on all measures except the Approval of Aggression scale. Social class was not a significant predictor, nor did television violence interact with either family attitudes or social class to intensify any dif- ferences. In this study, certain factors which were theorized to be critical in the kind of impact that large-scale exposure to televised violence would have on the impressionable minds of children were tested empirically. In par- ticular, we examined the notion that television would play a prominent role among youngsters who are less socialized by families and social environments and would influence their beliefs about the appropriate- ness and effects of using violence. Bmur Nproach to this problem, we d3u ATTITUDES TOWARD VIOLENCE 331 found substantial support in the data. At the sametime. it is incumbent on us to identify certainlimits to this approach and to discuss certain of their implications in concert with the findings. The model used implies causation, butthe data-gathering process only permitted us to make associative statements.It cannot be stipulated from these data alone that among youngsterswith minimum family in- fluence. exposure to violence precedes andleads to the development of attitudes which are more accepting ofviolence. (That, however, seems to be as plausible a sequence as onewhich would argue that some social- izing agent other than the family or thetelevision set precedes. At the least. there is ample evidence that exposure totelevision violence ac- companies the development of proviolenceattitudes. There is no evi- dence that it countermands such development.) Much variance in attitudes towardviolence remains unexplained. Tel- evision exposure is a weak, but significant,predictor. Family attitudes and social class are stronger determinants.All together, however, only ten to 15 percent of thevariance among the boys and eight to 15 percent among the girls has beenexplained by these factors. Studies exploring the contributions of peers. school, and otherfactors appear to be need- ed. Television exposure is most extensively related,for these youngsters, to the perceived effectiveness ofviolence. Television violence works, for both the good and the bad guys; it getsthings done. This may be a quite realistic assessment of the efficacy of thatmode of conflict resolu- tion. If the use of violence is also condoned orif alternate, effective means are not known or available tochildren for whom television is a principal socializing agency, the implications warrantconsideration. The study focuses solely on attitudes towardviolence, not on actual uses of violent behavior. Towhat degree more favorable attitudes to- ward violence are manifested in greater use ofviolence remains equivo- cal. The focus of future studies mightbe directed toward ascertaining those conditions, if any, under which the morefavorable attitudes are accompanied by reduced inhibitions or reducedanxiety about the uses of aggression. One could argue that the acceptanceof violence as appro- priate, effective, and useful is a behavior deservingof study in its own right. Does the greater acceptance of suchbeliefs, for example, inter- fere with or deter the development of other, moreproductive or socially accepted, behavior? Some caution is necessary when we examinethe present measure of exposure to televisionviolence. A program is a gross unit of measure. Although a televised dramatic series maybe consistently more violent than other programs, there is substantialvariation among its episodes. What in the violent programs is having theobserved effect is unknown. It may be the atmosphere of the entire program orseries; it may be spe- cific incidents. The segments calledviolent by researchers may not be 339 NE!111=1

332 .11.1.1N Imo!. AN I) ADOLESCFNT AGORESSIvENSS

the same items viewers would label violent. This Lick of specificity isa crucial issue for subsequent research. The children's perceptions of their parenti' attitudes towardaggres- sion was the predominant correlate of their own beliefs. Thegap that remains here is that the data on parents' attitudes originated with the youngsters. More direct data from parents should be obtained. Does the youngster know. reflect, or guess at the parents' attitude? Is the youngster rationalizing his or her own beliefs by making those of key reference groups consistent with them? What of other viable reference groups like peers? And what of possible contradictory information from parents and peers about responding to frustrating or mutually aggressive situations? Current research by Chaffee and McLeod (1971) at the Uni- versity of Wisconsin has obtained data from both parents and their chil- dren: this study may bear on the unanswered questions. The central point of this discussion might well be the combination of findings which indicate thc relatively greater impact of televisionexpo- sure on girls and boys from middle-class homes. The literature abounds with arguments that if television has some kind of impact. it will beprev- alent among the disturbed or "nonnormal." Although these arguments typically refer to the instigation of violent acts. rather than to the atti- tudes favorable to violent acts. the suggested locus of effect is thesame. Yet the present findings, which allowed us to examine youngsters from more and less advantaged homes (the latter a common operationali- zation of "nonnormal") indicates more television impact on attitudes among the former. Among the middle-class boys and among most girls, persistent exposure to televised violence showed a clear relationship to attitudes about violence. For the middle-class boys, exposure interacted with family attitudes; for all the girls, exposure had its own independent impact on their attitudes. The girls' socialization experience in both low- and middle-income families seemed to have similar influences on their personal beliefs. The girls from different environments reported learning equally well the undesirability of being physically aggressive. The fact that we do not observe this clear relationship between view- ing and attitudes among the lower-class youngster may stem from other factors. Their consistently higher scores on all the dependent measures may have created a ceiling effect on the opportunity for exposure to in- teiact with family attitudes. These youngsters' more likely direct experi- ences with violence could have superceded television influence or made it only reinforcing. Certainly the expectation that family attitudes would be less influential among the less advantaged was not borne out with respect to the one aspect of socialization studied here. But others have suggested that this lack of influence may be the case with respect to ag- gression (Maccoby and Gibbs. 1954; Sears, Maccoby, and Levin, 1957). For relatively average children from average home environments, however, continued exposure to violence is positively related to accept- 340 Arrau ims TOWARD VIOLENCE 333 ance of aggression as a mode ofbehavior. When the home environment also tends to ignore the child's development of aggressionattitudes, this relationship is even more substantial and perhaps more critical. Among the most disadvantaged boys. the pressures of their experi- ence with real violence coupled withequivocal family attitudes may have effectively eliminated an added impact from televisionviewing. It is among the more advantaged boys, and among most of the girls, that programming seems to modify attitudes about aggression. If made avail- able. alternatives to violence as problem solvers could alter acceptance of aggression as a mode of conduct among these children. Females in our culture receive strong family training to inhibitdisplay of physical aggression (Sears. Maccoby, and Levin, 1957; Sears,1961). There is much variance in that training. Nevertheless, it would seem fruitful for future research to focus, not on whether televisionviolence stimulates girls to initative acts of violence, but rather on whether the content of televised violence induces moreproaggression attitudes greater tolerance of violence and morereliance on its effectiveness. These attitudes could be in turn passed on ,o their own children by those women who become mothers.

FOOTNOTES

I. The research upon which this report is based wasperformed pur- suant to Contract No. HSM 42-70-32with the National Institute of Mental Health, Health Services and Mental Health Administration, U.S. Department of Health. Education and Welfare. ProfessorDom- inick is now at Queens College, New York City.

REFERENCES

Allinsmith. B.. cited in D. Miller and G. Swanson.Inner conflict and defense. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1960. Bandura, A. Influence of models' reinforcementcontingent on the ac- quisition of imitative responses. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 1965, 5. 589-95. Buss. A.B., and Durkee. A. Assessingdifferent kinds of hostility. Jour- nal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 21, 343-48. Chaffee. S.H., and McLeod. J.M. Adolescent television usein the fami- ly context. In Television and social behavior: report tothe Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee (Vol. 3).Washington. D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Gerbner. G. The television world. In Baker, R.K.. and Ball,S.J. (Eds.) Mass media and violence: staff report to theNational Commission on the Causes andPrevention of Violence. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1969. 341 334 II VISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Gerhner. G. The case for cultural indicators. Paper presentedat the American Political Science A ssociation September 1969 (h). Greenberg. B.. and Dominick. J. Race and social class differences in teenagers use of television. Journal of Broadcasting. 1969. 13. 331- 44. Greenberg. B.. and Dominick. J. Television behavioramong disadvan- taged children. In Greenberg. B.. and Dervin. B. Use of themass media by the urban poor. New York: Praeger.. 1967. Greenberg.B..and Gordon. T.Perceptions ofviolencein TV programs: criticsand thepublic.InTelevisionand social behavior: a report to the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee (Vol. D. Washington. D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. 1971. Hicks, D. Imitation and retention of film-mediatedpeer and adult mod- els. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1965. 2. 97-100. Him melweit. H.. Oppenheini.. and Vince, P. Television and the child. New York: Oxford University Press, 1958. Larsen. O.N.. Gray. L.. and Fortis. J. Goals and goal-achievement methods in television content: models for anomie? Social Inquiry, 1963. 33, 180-96. Levin. H.. and Sears. R.R. Identification with parentsas a determinant of doll play aggression. Child Development. 1956. 27. 135-53. Maccoby. E. Effects of the mass media. In Hoffman. M..and Hoffman. L. Review of child development research. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 1964. Maccoby, E.. and Gibbs. P. Methods of child-rearing in two social class- es. In Martin, W.. and Stendler. C. Readings in child development. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World. 1954. McLeod. J.. Chaffee, S., and Eswara, H. S. Family com munication pat- terns and communication research. Paper presented to the Associa- tion for Education in Journalism. 1966. McLeod. J.. Chaffee. S.. and Wackman. D. Family communication: an updated report. Paper presented to the Association for Education in Journalism. 1967. Minuchin, S.. Braulio. J., Gurney, J., Risman. B., and Schumer. F. Families of the slums. New York: Basic Books, 1967. Sakuma, A. Values and the mass media. Unpublished paper, Syracuse University, 1968. Schramm. W., Lyle. J.. and Parker, E. Television in the lives of our children. Stanford: Stanford University Press. 1961. Sears. R.R. A theoretical framework for personality and social behav- ior. American Psychologist, 1951, 6, 476-83. Sears, R.R., Maccoby. E., and Levin, H. Patterns of child rearing. New York: Row. Peterson, 1957.

. 342 ATTITUDES TOW A RD VIOLENCE 335

Sears, R.R. Relation of early socialization experiences to aggression in middle childhood. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 1961, 63. 466-92. Sewell, W.H., Mussen, P.H.. and Harris, C.W. Relationships among child training practices. American Sociological Review. 1955, 20 136-48. Snedecor, G.W. Statistical methods. Ames, Iowa: Iowa State Universi- ty Press, 1956. Stempel. G. Unpublished study cited in Baker and Ball (Eds.) Mass media and violence. Troldahl, V. Occupational prestige scale. Department of Communica- tion, Michigan State University, 1967 (mimeo). U.S. National Commission on Civil Disorders. Report of the National Commission on Civil Disorders. New York: Bantam Books, 1968. Walters, J.D., Pearce, D., and Dahms. L. Affectional and aggressive behavior of preschool children. Child Development, 1957, 28, 15-26. Mass Media Use and Aggression: A Pilot Study

Herbert L. Friedman and Raymond L. Johnson American Institutes for Research

The literature of research attempting to relate television programming to subsequent aggression on the part of its viewers presents a very con- fusing picture. The most clearcut evidence of the existence of such a relationship comes from laboratory studies whose subjects were not drawn from groups particularly noted for their aggression (Weiss, 1970). On the other hand, evidence from survey samples among adolescents and young adultsthe groups responsible for most violent behavior in societyis contradictory (Eron, 1963; Schramm, Lyle, and Parker, 1961). No in-depth studies have been conducted which have attempted to identify specific ways in which television viewing might adversely affect youths who are predisposed to violent behavior.

336 344 SIASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 337 The purpose of this pilot study was to advance understanding of the possible relationship between television viewing and social aggressive- ness among adolescent boys. Attention wasfocused on developing ade- quate instruments for collecting and assessing data on the twotopic areas.

PROCEDURES

A junior high school in Baltimore was selected as the source of inter- viewees because of its racial (50 percent black and 50 percent white) and socioeconomic (predominantly blue and white collar families) mix of students. The director of research and development of the Baltimore public school system and the principal of the chosen school gave full cooperation to the researchers. The vice principal in charge of discipline and the school counselors selected 19 white and 20 black eighth and ninth grade boys who had re- cords of interpersonal aggressiveness in school. This group of 39 "ag- gressive" male students was matched with 41 other boys (equally drawn from both raas) from the same classes who did not have notable re- cords of aggressiveness. (Valuable consultation was provided through- out the project by a psychiatrist, Dr. Herbert S. Gross, of theUniversity of Maryland School of Medicine.) Robert L. Derbyshire, Ph.D.. director of the Division of Human Rela- tions and Urban Studies of the Psychiatric Institute of the University of Maryland School of Medicine, was retained as the supervisor of inter- viewing. Dr. Derbyshire provided and supervised the staff of interview- ers, who were selected from participants in the MentalHygiene Techni- cians Career program of the University of Maryland. Since the investigators were interested in obtaining responses to nu- merous items in a number of different topic areas, they decided thatthe respondents would be asked to complete two separate questionnaires. This procedure would minimize the possibility of overtaxing the atten- tion span of the subjects, would permit different kinds of answers to similar questions (i.e.. enable the respondents to give both open- and closed-ended responses to some questions). and would compare the effectiveness of each of two ways of asking questions. In addition, the investigatois felt some items lent themselves particularly well to one or the other of the two question methods used. The first questionnaire was a paper and pencil form which was admin- istered to subjects in a group setting. It required about 35 minutes to complete and, with a few exceptions, was made up of closed-ended, objective questions with appropriate response boxes to be checked off by the subjects. The ten-page questionnaire explored the subjects' tele- vision viewing parameters (time spent watching television: types and 345 338 TEI.EvIsioN .AND ADOLESCENT AG(RESSIVENESS frequency of particular programs viewed; reasons for viewing; and complaints about television content) and their attitudes toward aggres- sion (definitions of situations calling for aggressive actions; reactions to aggressive actions; personal propensities toward overt aggression, indi- rect aggression, and verbal aggression as taken from the Buss-Durkee scale and the Sears aggression scale). The second questionnaire was an open-ended interview administered to each boy individually by a professional interviewer. The interview required about one hour and allowed each respondent to express in his own words the way he felt about many of the same aspects of televi- sion and social behavior covered in the first questionnaire. The questions asked covered: )Favorite television programs: aspects of programs enjoyed, things learned from the programs, reality of the programs, characters enjoyed, least favorite programs, favorite advertisements; 2) Factors in a particular day's viewing. For each program watched on the preceding day, the subject was asked how much attention he paid to the program; his reason for choosing that program; how much he recalled of the program's plot; how he evaluated the program; who watched it with him and whether they influenced the program's selec- tion; what parts of the program's content were upsetting; what parts were worth seeing again; what parts were missed because of other activity; 3) Television news programs: frequency of viewing; complaints about news; news material that was upsetting; reactions to coverage of the Vietnam war, civil disorders, and local news; 4) Use of other media: frequency of use and appealing content in newspapers, magazines, movies, and radio; 5) Personal sports activity and viewing of sports on television; 6) Background information: age, grades in school, parental occu- pations and education, place of birth, ages and sex of siblings, and number of close friends; and 7) Self-reported participation in antisocial activities. Each of the 80 adolescent subjects completed one questionnaire ad- ministered in a group setting and one conducted by a professional inter- viewer during an individual interview session. To encourage cooperation, the students were paid one dollar to com- plete each of the two questionnaires. Because of the many variances in students' individual schedules, school holidays, and the differences in the time required to get individual parents to consent to their child's par- ticipation, the interviews were scattered over several weeks. The major- ity of the group questionnaires were completed at school, though a few were administered in the interviewees' homes. .The group questionnaire was the least successful of the two in terms of completeness of responses and ease of administration. kThe group -346 MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 339

questionnaire probably should be administered to groups nolarger than ten or 15. In this study, a group of 30 behavedwith greater cooperative- ness and responsiveness than did the groupof 57 students who were first treated.) The setting of the individual interview is animportant factor. When the interview was conducted in a quiet room atschool, the subjects demonstrated considerable "openness" and seemedwilling to respond with sincerity. When the interviews wereconducted at home, particular- ly when siblings or parents were prelent, responses were moreguarded and less expansive. While individual interviews with the "aggressive" groupof students were more difficult to arrange,members of this group tended to answer interview questions more honestly and thoughtfullythan members of the "nonaggressive" group. The use of two questionnaires with some questionoverlap between them gave rise to the suspicion among theinterviewees that the repeti- tion was an attempt to check on the honesty oftheir responses. The in- terviewers commented that when a question previouslyanswered on the group questionnaire was repeated atthe individual interview session, the subjects seemed to be less accurate and sincere in their responses.

RESULTS

Three general research questions were explored in this study: I. What types of mass media did teenage boys pay attention toin the early part of 1970? 2. Were there significant differences among boys labeledaggressive and nonaggressive in attention to and selection of this media content? 3. Could aggressive and nonaggressive boys be accuratelydistin- guished by various self-administered questionnaire items? Media usage. That these boys watch a good deal of televisionbe- tween two and six hours on an average daywasobvious before the study began, but the exact amount of reported viewingdepended on the question being asked. The respondents gave higher figures whenasked how much TV they viewed "on an average day" than when askedabout "yesterday," and both estimates were higher than when they were asked to check from a list all the specific programsthey viewed on the preceding day. It is obvious that better measurement toolsneed to be employed to provide accurate estimates of actual viewing time. In obtaining information about specific television contentviewed by the subjects, the investigators found considerable agreementof respon- ses to the interview andself-administered questionnaire approaches. Both methods showed that the boys' favorite programs wereMod

. 347 - -.,-4...... -...... 4. 340 I E LEvIsION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Squad. It Takes a Thief. Hawaii Fiye-O. Mission Impossible. TheCourt- ship of Eddie's Father. Marcus Welby. M.D.. The FBI, Laugh-Inand The Bill Cosby Show. Three programs The New People. Room 222 and Music Scenewere rated higheron the open-ended question form than on the checklist, and two programsLand of the Giantsand Adam- 12were rated higher on the checklist than in the open-ended question- ing. More than half of these favoritesare shows which emphasize a con- siderable amount of violent and aggressive behavior. Suchprograms were seldom mentioned spontaneously when the boys were asked to name three programs they would like to have taken off the air. Television characters that these boys said they "wouldmost like to he" tended to be drawn from these favoriteprograms. Alexander Mun- dy ( It Takes a Thief). Line Hayes ( Mod Squad). Pete Dixon( Room 222), and Bill Cosby were among those most frequently named.Howev- er. three of the most popular charactersTom Jones, Bronson, and Mannixwere drawn from less heavily viewed shows. The boys were equally divided on the question of whether they had learned more about life in school or from television. However, when queried about what they had learned specifically from their favorite dramatic programs, less than half could givea concrete example. Those things they did list ranged from new words to the necessity of regular doctor visits; from (perhaps inaccurately) theway police and govern- ment agencies work to how life was in the Old West; from what it is like to take dope to how to get along without a parent: from methods of committing crime to the lesson that crime doesn'tpay. The investigators were reminded of the Schram m, Lyle, and Parker (1961) conclusion that what children do to television is a better statement of the problem than what television does to children. A more complete listing of open-ended responses to questions about the ten most popular programs is presented in Appendix A. The boys mentioned "enjoyment" and "time-killing" most oftenas general reasons for viewing. Nine in ten said they "usually" watched television to view a specific program they enjoyedvery much; yet when asked about the programs they viewedon the preceeding day, the boys described 25 percent of them as "a waste of time." Twenty-ninepercent of the total programs viewed on the day preceeding the interview "just came on" the particular channel the boys were watching. These reasons for watching are quite similar to those given by adults in Steiner's (1962) nationwide study, although the teenagerswere more willing to admit they watch because "there is nothing better to do"or because they "get stuck for an evening" and less likely tosay they view to "learn some- thing." (Less than one in four said he usually watched television for this reason.) The boys in this study were considerably more likely than Stei- ner's adults to say they watched because they "might 'be missingsome- thing good."

348 MASS NInIA USE AND AGGRESSION 341 Few of these teenagers felt there was too much sex (15 percent of the total group) or too much violence (20 percent on the television screen. Only 24 voiced any complaints at all, and only two boys spontaneously mentioned violence when asked for their complaints about television. More objectionable to these respondents were too many advertisements (80 percent) and too many news programs and news specials (65 percent). Blacks thought there should be more black actors in programs and ad- vertisements; whites agreed with them about ads but not about regular programs. The boys were critical of commercials and skeptical about their trust- worthiness. social utility, entertainment value, or realism. Blacks were less negative about advertising than whiies, however. In response to questions about their specific television viewing on the day preceding the individual interviews, the boys indicated three-quart- ers of their viewing was done when they were alone inthe viewing situation: even if others were present, the boys said they gave the pro- gram their complete attention. There seemed to belittle conflict with others over choice of programs, and few respondents were unable to recall the main highlights of each program viewed. More than one boy in five reported being bothered or upset by something he had seen on tele- vision the previous day. While sports were the major source of dismay for these boys (if their favorite team iost the game), a variety of other concerns were also reported. These included: "Agirl (in a movie) who was running around with other men even though she wasmarried," "A man (in Room 222) tried to talk a studentinto going to a college who did not have good economic standing," "When the good guy alwayswins that makes me mad," "A woman (in The Survivors ) was about to stab the girl," and "The way the boys (in The New People ) treated the col- ored boy." More than half of the boys indicated their involvement with the pro- gram s by saying that there was something ontelevision that daj that they would like to see again. In addition, over one-quarter said that due to other obligations they missed something ontelevision that they had wanted to watch. On the other hand, almost half said that there was some time during the day they wanted towatch television but didn't because there wasn't anything worthwhile on the air at that time. One-third of these teenagers said they no er watched either network or local news programs, but halfsaid they watched at least two such programs each week. About 20 percentof the boys said they had seen things on television news that bothered or upset them; and in contrast with the entertainment programs, most of the disturbing news content dealt with violence (especially related to the fighting in Vietnam). Three respondents complained about local news coverage, one noting that this news should be broadcast throughout the day,another citing the lack of detailed local news, and the third wanting more pictures on the news. 349 342 iEI.IVIS1ON AND ADOLESCENT ACCREtitily EN ESS One youngster was disturbed by "the way cameramen hug people who don't want to he bothered. There was little consensus about whether the media had given too much coverage to the fighting in Vietnam or to civil disorders. Three-quarters of respondents said they read a newspaper at least a few times a week. and 43 percent said they read it every day. The com- ics appeared to he the most popular item in the newspaperi: sports ran a close second. the front page a distant third. Stories about "shootings and robberies" aroused more interest than those about Vietnam or civil disorders. Political stories were "skipped over" by 77 percent of these boys. About 60 percent of the boys reported reading a magazine regularly. Life. Look. Sports Illustrated. and Boy's Life were read most often. Sixty-two percent of the boys said they read hot rod magazines, 35 per- cent read gun magazines, and 20 percent read crime and detective m aga- ziner. Roughly three-quarters of the subjects had seen a movie during the previous six months, and 33 percent averaged at least one movie a month. Particularly favorite movies cited were rather high in violent content: On Her Majesty's Secret Service (the latest James Bond mov- ie), Butch Cassidy and the Sundance Kid, Goldfinger, Bullitt, and Bon- nie and Clyde. Less violent movies mentioned more than once included The Reivers and Gone with the Wind. Well over half the favorite movies listed seemed to have a high emphasis on violence, and practically all of the reasons that the teenagers gave for liking these movies had to do with the violence they contained. (Cougan's Bluff: "I like how the de- tectives crack down on people"; Bonnie and Clyde showed "cool rob- beries"; Boston Strangler: "I liked the fact he was killing women.") Music is almost the sole reason that these boys listened to the radio two and one-half hours per day. Half of them listened to only one sta- tion. When asked to make a choice, 31 percent said they would most enjoy listening to their favorite music; 56 percent would rather watch their favorite television program, and 13 percent would rather read their favorite magazine. Interest in playing and watching football was practically unanimous. Only about 10 percent had not played any football in the fall of 1969, and not one respondent had not seen at least one televised National Football League (NFL) game. The average respondent had seen three college games, ten NFL games, and four American Football League (AFL) games during the previous season. Twelve percent of the boys reported having watched more than ten college games. Sixteen percent reported having watched more than ten AFL games, and 49 percent reported hav- ing watched more than ten NFL games. There seemed little doubt that professional football had more appeal than the college game; many re-

250

,_r c 4 ..192,4.1.... MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 343 spondents mentioned that they preferred the pro game because of its more intense physical contact. Presented with a list developed by the Michigan Survey Research Center's study of juvenile delinquency, respondents were asked to check off specific acts of antisocial behavior in which they had engaged. The results were much like those obtained from a nationwidestudy of tenth grade boys (except for school problems like expulsion).Sixty-two percent reported that they got into a serious fightwith another student in school; 50 percent had taken part in a gang fight; 42 percent had gotten something by telling a person that something bad would happen to himif he did not cooperate; 25 percent had hurt someone badly enough toneed bandages or a doctor; ten percent had hit a teacher, eight percenthad used a knife or gun to get something from a person; five percenthad hit their fathers; and five percent had hit their mothers. Differences between "aggressive" and "nonaggressive" boys.The "aggressive" boys were considerably more likely than the nonaggres- sive boys to report getting into fights, participating in gangfights, hitting a teacher, or using a gun orknife to "get something" fromanother per- son. Overall, aggressivewhites admitted to an average of seven aggres- sive acts; nonaggressive whites reported three. Thedistinction among blacks was consistent with this finding, but far lesssignificant: 4.1 ag- gressive acts were admitted by aggressives and 3.5 bynon-aggressives. Thus there was independent evidence to supportthe judgment of school counselors in assigning students to the aggressiveand nonaggressive groups.- Unfortunately for our research purposes,boys of both races who were tagged "aggressive" weresignificantly more likely to comefrom working class homes in which parents had lessformal education.3 This circumstance leads to some confusionabout whether the differences were due to aggressiveness or tosocial status. On all three questions about time spentviewing (average day esti- mate, previous day estimate, logof previous day), the "aggressive" boys reported roughly 20 percent moreviewing than the "nonaggrec- sives." However, when presented with alist of evening programs and asked to check those which they had seen atleast five times during the previous fifteen weeks, the aggressive boysindicated far less viewing than the nonaggressive boys. Table Ishows the breakdowns by race and by aggressiveness among the groups. There were distinct differences amongthe groups in naming the top ten favorite dramaticseries offered on the commercialnetworks (movies are not consideredin Table I). Aggressive whiteslisted eight violent programc among their top ten,while nonaggressive whites listed five. Among blacks the aggressiveslisted six, the nonaggressivesthree.4 In absolute viewing rates (asopposed to the Table I relative ratesof viewing), however, no differencesbetween aggressives and nonaggres- sives emerge. with one exception:the aggressive boys of both races

351 344 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS viewed Then Catne.Bronson (a program not on the top ten listing of any of the four groups in Table I) more than did nonaggressives.5 The main reasbn for the overall lack of differences may be that nonaggressives listed more programs viewed than did aggressives. Table 1: Television programs viewed most frequently by four types of boys (percent viewing program at least once between September and January in parentheses)

Aggressive whites Nonaggressive whites

1. The FBI 193) 1. Mod Squad (85) 2. Mod Squad (87) 2. Bill Cosby (80) 3. Land of the Giants (73) 3. Land of the Giants (75) 4. Adam-12 (73) 4. Courtship of Eddie's Father (75) 5. It Takes a Thief (73) 5. Mission Impossible (74) 6. Music Scene (71) 6. Hawaii Five-0 (74) 7. Gunsmoke (69) 7. Laugh-In (70) 8. Hawaii Five-0 (69) 8. Newlywed Game (70) 9. Mission Impossible (76) 9. Room 222 (68) 10. Room 222 (67) 10. Adam-12 (63)

Aggressive blacks Nonaggressive blacks

1. Mod Squad (92) 1. Bewitched (94) 2. It Takes a Thief (85) 2. Mod Squad (94) 3. Hawaii Five-0 (85) 3. Room 222 (93) 4. Room 222 (85) 4. /t Takes a Thief (88) 5. Mannix (85) 5. Mayberry RFD (88) 6. Mission Impossible (79) 6. Ed Sullivan (88) 7. Laugh-In (79) 7. Green Acres (88) 8. Julia (77) 8. Bill Cosby (88) 9. Walt Disney (77) 9. Petticoat Junction (87) 10. Land of the Giants (77) 10. Land of the Giants (84)

The conclusion that does emerge from both the relative and absolute viewing rates (derived from the check list) is that the aggressive boys view family comedy shows like The Courtship of Eddie's Father, My Three Sons, Green Acres, and Family Affair with considerable distaste. The same conclusion emerges from an analysis of the questionnaire item which asked respondents to list their four favorite programs. The num- ber of times family comedies were listed was twice as high among non- aggressives as among aggressives. The vgressive boys listed an average of 1.8 violent programs among the four programs they said were their favorites, while the non-aggres- sives listed an average of 1.6. On other questions, aggressives were only half as likely as nonaggressives to name a violent program as one they would like to take off the air. Not one aggressive boy spontaneously mentioned excess violence to the interviewer as a complaint about tele- vision. Aggressive boys were also less likely to agree with the statement that there was "too much violence" on television. (Thirteen percent agreed versus 30 percent of the nonaggressives.) In line withtheir rejection of 252 " - MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 345 family comedy shows, the aggressives were more likely to endorse the statement (74 percent versus 52 percent of thenonaggressives) that there were "not enough comedy shows" on television. No further sys- tematic differences of this magnitude between the two groups could be found among other complaints about television or television commer- cials. The two groups differed little about other viewing parameters such as things learned, the amount of realism subjects felt the shows depicted. and preference for the stories over the characters in the stories. There was some evidence of more selectiveand intensive viewing among ag- gressives: aggressives were more likely to use television guides: they had greater ability to recall program themes: they were more likely to report being upset or bothered by some of what they saw ontelevision: they appeared to regret missing their favorite programs more: and they reported a higher incidence of turning off the set because of poor pro- gram choice. However, many of these factors wouldhave to be adjusted by more extensive analyses because the aggressives viewed more pro- grams. There were no systematic differences between the two groups in the amount of television news watched. However,while the aggressive boys voiced more complaints about news programs, they reported being less upset by specific news content than the nonaggressive boys. Differences in the amount and type of newspaper and magazine read- ing between the two groups were not significant, although aggressives reported somewhat more interest in newspaper stories about shootings and robberies than about riots and civil disorders. Aggressives were more likely to name Popeye. Dick Tracy, and Peanuts astheir favorite comic strips. More noteworthy differences emerged in responses about movie at- tendance and radio listening. Aggressives attended movies less often than nonaggressives. but when they did attend, their favorite movies tended to be considerably more violent than the favorites of nonaggres- sive boys. Aggressive boys listened to the radio more, and they were more than twice as likely as nonaggressives to saythey would prefer lis- tening to their favorite music to watching television or reading a maga- zine.6 Aggressives were no more likely than nonaggressives to report active engagement in sports, nor did they watch more football ontelevision. The aggressives felt that they had significantly more close friends than nonaggressives thought they had, and they were less likely to have been born in the Baltimore area. Their grades in school (60 percent hnd a C average or below) were considerably lowerthan those of nonaggressive boys. Discriminating power of aggression items. The various psychological inventories used inthis research effortfailed to distinguishwith 346 TELEVISION AND ADOLESaNT AG612ESSIVENESS

satisfactory validity between the aggressiveand thc nonaggressive boys. The experimenters hoped that thispretest would provide a base for de- veloping a short scale of optimal reliabilityand validity which could he used to relate personal aggressivenessto the viewing of violent televisi- sion content. Of the 85 items drawnfrom various scales. less than ten elicited substantial response differencesbetween aggressives and nonag- gressives.7 Items from thescales with widest use. those of Buss-Durkee (1957) and Sears (described in Schramm. Lyle.and Parker. 1961) fared less well than those from othersources. As can be seen in Table 2. only two Buss-Durkee items and one Sear.; item showedsatisfactory validity. The lack of satisfactory discriminationheld for entire scale scores for these instruments as wellas the individual items. Listed at the bottom of Table 2are a few items. adaptt.d from a hostili- ty scale developed by Grace (1949), which didgenerate satisfactory dis- crimination between aggressives and nonaggressives.The items are in forced choice format and may beseen as verbal equivalents of the Ro- senzweig Picture Frustration Test. Even here.however, some items which elicited a good deal ofresponses in a hostile direction (what would you do if your younger brother is beatenup by an older child. or if a child throws a glass on the floor) failed to discriminate between thetwo groups.

Table 2: Items showing clear distinctions between aggressives andnonaggressives Percent agreement

Aggressives Nonaggressives Whoever insults me or my family is asking fo. a fight (BussDurkee) 65 44 Lately I have been kind of grouchy (BussDurkee) 52 27 Even if you don't like a person, you should still try to help him 50 78 It makes me uncomfortable to see two of my friends fighting (Sears) 28 53 You're waiting in a long line and someone tries to cut in ahead of you. You feel like: a) pushing him out of line 63 52 A friend of yours tells other people a secret about you that embarrasses you. You feel like: b) threatening to beat him up 45 22 A car splashes mud on your clothes. You feel like: c) throwing something at the car 35 23

Two sets of itemsone asking the respondent if he feltpeople could be trusted and one probing intohis feelings ot alienationwere included mainly as "dummy items to detect possible differentialan- 354

3+1611,1. MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 347 swers due to response set. Aggressives gave slightly more alientated re- sponses than nonaggressives. but much larger differences were obtained with the "trust-in-people" items. As would be expected, aggressive whites evidenced much lower trust in people' than nonaggressive whites. These items did not discriminate within the black population. however. Moreover, in this study aggressive blacks were not likely to be much differentinself-reported aggressive behaviors than nonaggressi've blacks. Replicability of these results. In the previous section we noted som e uncomfortable implications of our designation of boys as either aggres- sive or nonaggressive. Boys noted as aggressive were more likely to come from lower status families. In addition, the aggressive boys were identified as such by school counselors; identification was made on the basis of the boys' misbehavior at school, not on the basis of their inter- personal aggressiveness or self-reported deviancy. In order to extend the generalizability of our findings, the above anal- yses were replicated using the self-report aggressive behavior items as the criterion of aggressiveness rather than the designations of school counselors. This procedure did not offer direct control on social class differences, but it was employed as an attem pt to provide independent verification of the aggressiveness dimension. A number of the findings did not survive the replication check. The major casualty of the replication was the set of findings regarding the subject groups' viewing of certain types of television shows. Where- as Table 1 showed aggressives placing almost twice as many violent pro- grams on their top ten list as nonaggressives (in terms of frequency of viewing), exactly the opposite occurred with comparable data from the new groupings based on self-reports. White boys who said that they had -1gaged in a good deal of aggressive behavior listed six violent programs among their top ten; those who reported themselves less aggressive list- ed seven. Among blacks, aggressives noted only three violent shows; less aggressives noted six. The earlier findings of aggressive boy's aver- sion to fam ily comedy shows also failed to hold when this self-report cri- terion was used for aggre ssion. On the other hand, the item which asked the boys to list their favorite programs (rather than how often they viewed each program) showed the rate of choice of violent programs among aggressive boys to be even higher than it had been in the previous analyses. Among their four favor- ites, aggressive boys (according to the self-i eport criterion) mentioned 2.2 violent programs; less aggressives mentioned 1.5. Furthermore, the aggressives were still only half as likely (12 percent versus 26 percent of nonaggressives) to say that there was too much violence on television. In the other question areas in which differences between aggressives and nonaggressives had been found in the previous analyses. the record is also mixed. As before, aggressives reported more overall television

. 355 348 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

viewing. but the difference between the groups was less than tenper- cent: previously it had been 20 percent. Aggressives again appeared slightly more selective in their television viewing habits. In answer to questions about their use of the other mass media. aggressives again reported more attention to newspaper stories about robberies and civil disorders. and again preferred listening to their favorite music to watch- ing their favorite television programs. In a number of other question areas, the results obtained in the initial analyses failed to hold, but the new criterion resulted in new avenues of exploration. The small sample sizes and definitional problemsare al- ready apparent. But it might be constructive to note the personality items which consistently differentiated between aggressives andnonag- gressives. Of the seven items in Table 2, the first three8 and the final items survived replication; these four items deserve special considera- tion in future questionnaires where measures of aggressionare desired.

CONCLUSIONS

In this brief review of a pilot study of the television habits of 80teen- age boys from a lower middle class urban junior high school, we have found a number of interesting ways in which teenagers interact with the mass media. When attempting to establish a link between television viewing and aggressive behavior, it is necessary to take into account the fact that teenagers are exposed to violence in other mediaas well. We have seen that while almost half of the subjects' favorite televisionpro- grams emphasized violence, these adolescents also pay a good deal of attention to newspaper stories emphasizing violence andare quite likely to attend movies in which violence (usually more blatant and prevalent than that shown on television) is a central element. Inquiry into the ef- fects of exposure to popular music might bc fruitful, since this study confirms earlier evidence that aggressive boys enjoy popular music more than nonaggressive boys (Halloran, 1969). Inquiring into the ways in which their peers influence boys' perceptions of when aggression and violence are justified would also be useful. This research has relied on multiple indicators in various question areas:closed-ended and open-ended questions, personal interviews and questionnaires, check-lists and direct questions. As in most re- search using this multiple indicator approach, the varying research methods do not consistently point in the same direction. On balance, however, more evidence suggests that aggressive boys are more attract- ed to violent television content than nonaggressive boys than suggests the reverse. This tendency shows up most consistently inresponses to questions about favorite televisionprograms and to questions about whether there is "too much violence"on television. 356 MASS M EDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 349

Statistically, however, the results offer no direct evidence of televi- sion's "deleterious effects." Much larger and more representative sam- ples and appropriate statistical controls on allthe other variables would be needed before any claims of that nature could be advanced. Even with such evidence, researchers will eventually require data on how viewers interact with what they see on the screen on a continuing day-to-day basis before we can begin to understand the processes by which television affects its audience. The data on a single day's viewing habits in this present study reinforce the views of previous researchers (Schramm, Lyle, and Parker, 1961) that there is tremendous variation in how viewers react to television and that we should begin to dev.flop measurement techniques that will reflect the subtle ways in which televi- sion "affects" its viewers.

FOOTNOTES

1. Blondie, Peanuts, Dick Tracy, and Beatle Bailey were the most popu- lar comic strips. 2. However, the antisocial item that shows greatest distinction was "been suspended or expelled from school," which indicates that the counselors used this as their major criterion of aggression. 3. The following percentage data were obtained for various parental characteristics: White Black Aggr. Nonaggr. Aggr. Nonaggr. Father attended college 10% 40% 25% 38% Father in white collar job 20 55 15 60 Mother attended college 0 17 5 25 Mother is housewife 45 65 25 25 Mother in white collar job 75 80 57 93 (if employed)

4. The designation of a program as violent conforms to ratings obtained from the critics and the public by Greenberg and Gordon in a sepa- rate report to the NIMH Television and Social Behavior program. 5. Bronson was named by aggressive whites as a television personality they would most like to be; and Linc Hayes performed this role for black teenagers. Aggressives and nonaggressives of both races iden- tified with Alexander Mundy, the thief on It Takes a Thief. 6. This finding of interest in music jibes with results from a British study of delinquents by Halloran et al. (1969). 7. Almost as many items worked in the opposite direction of that pre- dicted. 357

^a"^".",",1YA* *OM 350 TELEVISION AND ADOI.ESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS 8. The Sears item. "It makes me uncomfortableto see two of my friends fighting," is especially interesting theoreticaHy sinceit sug- gests that aggressive people value aggression above cognitive bal- ance, the latter phenomenon considered an extremely strong drive by social psychologists.

REFERENCES

Buss, A., and Durkee, A. An invcrwry for different kinds of hostility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 21, 343-49. Eron, L. Relationship of TV viewing habits and aggressive behaviorin children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963,67, 193- 96. Grace, H. A study of the expression of hostility in everyday, profession- al, and international verbal situations. Unpublished doctoraldisser- tation, Columbia University, 1949. Halloran, J., Brown, R., and Chaney, D. Mass media delinquencypro- ject. Appendix A in Television Research Committees secondprog- ress report and recommendations. Leicester, England: Leicester University Press, 1969. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B. Television in thelives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961. Steiner, G. The people look at television. New York: AlfrriA. Knopf, 1963. Weiss, W. Effects of mass media communication.In Lindzey, G., and ronsen. E. (Eds.) Handbook of social psychology, Vol. 5. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1970.

258 MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 351 Appendix A: Responses to open-end questions regarding favorite television programs

ModSquadj

Why watch?

I.like detective stories 2. about young people who become cops 3. lots of fighting and pretty girls 4. police show with kids in it 5. to see who they are going to help 6. exciting climaxes 7. about teenagers and things they do 8. for story and plot 9. like to see how the stars changed from hippies to police work 10. like policemen who fight crime

1 1. lots of action 12.it is exciting 13. likes detective stories 14. teaches a lesson 15. excitement. adventure 16. different problems of college people who take dope 17. the fights the squad gets into 18. something always active going on 19. exciting 20. likes stories and people 21. learn how today's police work

What is learned from watching this show?

I.learn about different countries 2. that "crime doesn't pay" 3. learn about self-defense 4. about how it is to be on dope 5. learn how to keep out of trouble 6. how police rule 7. learn how guys catch thieves 8. learn about crime 9. people should not steal 10. not to do anything bad around people you don't know II. how real life is for police officers 12. learn about being addicted to drugs 1359 .- --...... A.,araw... 352 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

How is this show realistic?

1.real life things happening to young people 2. shows how easy to get in trouble if not careful 3. stories seem real 4. how they catch robbers 5. students getting killed, while participating in rallies 6. situations are not way out 7. how rich people do bad things 8. all the crime that is real 9. shows real crimes 10. shows everyday teens with adult problems 11. shows things like rioting 11. shows how young people get kids their own age out of trouble 13. shows how people are today

How is this show unrealistic?

1. TV hardly ever tells the real truth 2. just not like everyday life 3.I don't believe they have any young.cOps. 4. because all that happens is exaggerated 5. things on show do not happen in real life

Why are people in show liked?

1. like Pete, Linc, and Julie because they are not like other police 2. the kids-they act kinda cool like kids today do

3. the kids-good actors . 4. the kids-way they help people out 5. the kids-the experience they have 6. the kids-action they get into 7. shows that black people are just as important as whites 8. way they try to help others

It Takes a Thief

Why watch?

1. exciting 2. suspense in the story 3. likes Alexander Mundy 4. exciting-great deal of action 5. different from other shows 6. lots of action, suspense 360 MASS M EDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 353

7.likes fights Mundy is in and tricks he does 8. the way he operates looks real 9.likes private agent shows 10.likes the storiesthe way Munday has different assignments 11. unusualman steals for the U.S.

What is learned from watching this show?

1. educational because he travels to foreign lands 2.learn about government 3. learn what foreign countries are like

How is this show realistic?

1. way he steals looks professional 2. in real life there is a secret service and thieves 3. way they get their information

How is this show unrealistic?

1.not natural life 2. because no thief steals and gets away with it all the time 3. gets out of jams unrealistically 4. fictitious stories 5. too phony

Why are people in show liked?

1. Mundythe way he's never afraid 2. Mundyseems so smooth a person 3. Mundybecause he 'acts like he really knows what he's doing 4. Mundyplays especially cool like nothing is hippening 5. the way they do weir thing

Xi-vaii Five-01

Why watch?

I.filmed in Hawaii 2.likes detectives 3.likes the 'chasing' action 4. good plot, interested in law and police action 5. mysterious happenings 6. people getting shot 7.likes the way the police arrest people 361

,-....., ....II, 1. ...r ....--rsasg±ageab,ragtgwkwArrems*. 354 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

What is learned from watching this show?

I. that crime doesn't pay 2. how he captures criminals 3. how to be a deteCtive 4. how criminals escape 5. how to find criminals

How is this show realistic?

1. criminals get chased 2. programs seem as if they could really happen 3. lots of realistic devious methods of criminals 4. the crimes are punished 5. because they base shows on real crime

[Mission Impossible!

Why watch?

1. to see how they make all kinds of escapes 2. lots of action and suspense 3. likes private agent shows 4. shows different problems government has 5. stories with weird gailgets 6. suspenseful, can't tell what's coming next

What is learned from watching fijis AoW?

I. learn that some people can be detectives; learn the problems involved in crime 2. how to handle people in an organization 3. way some people try to overrun governments 4. how to operate gadgets

How is this show both realistic and unrealistic?

1. stories could be real, but not ways missions are carried out

How is this show unrealistic?

1. people aren't as mean as some of the characters 2. might be done but isn't real 362 MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 355 rt'ourtship of Eddie's FatherI

Why watch?

1. like the little boy 2. shows how small boy and father get along 3. like the sense in boy 4. like Eddie 5. for the trouble boys get into 6. cute, educational, it's funny 7. likes how boy and father get along withoutmother

What is learned from watching this show?

1. how some people have to get alongwithout one parent 2. how a small boy lives without a mother 3. when you're in trouble you should tell your parents sothey can help you 4. learn about how little kids ask questions

How is this,show realistic?

1. how kids without mothers do the best they can 2. how the father helps Eddie learn right from wrong 3. how Eddie confronts his dad with why notfight with girls 4. how some people can tell parents abouttheir troubles without being punished 5. how some people live with only one parent

Why are people in show liked?

1. Eddie's father understands his son 2. Eddie and father play their parts well 3. Eddie is advanced for his age 4. Eddie and his dad get along so well

'Marcus Welby, M.D.I

Why watch?

1. adventures 2. the doctor is real and is like a father image 3. shows real people having medical problems 4. find out different diseases 5. shows different problems of a doctor

. 363 '- 356 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

What is learned from watching this show?

1. more about medicine and human dram a 2. how doctor gets involved with patients 3. how they go about operations 4. how to treat medical problems

How is this show realistic?

I. there are doctors like Marcus Welby 2. Dr. Welby's character is real 3. problems similar to those of my friends 4. good account of some things in real life 5. operations seem real Why are people in show liked?

1. way they try to help people 2. Welby never panics 3. Welby, understanding father image

The NewPeople

Why watch?

1. entertaining 2. how young people can act mature and liveon an island 3. comedylike it 4. young people and adults are appealing 5. teaches a lesson 6. likes idea of starting new civilization 7. likes the teenagers in it 8. likes different ways they live on the island and how they tryto get off 9. learn how people live on deserted islands 10. interesting stories 1. fun the way they started a new life

What is learned from watching this show?

1. truth of segregation 2. shows how to survive stranded on an island 3. teenagers could start their own civilization and have own laws 4. shouldn't join bad groups 5. why teens fight and have arguments 364 MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 357 6. that not all young people take pot 7. about taking dope

How is this show realistic?

I. people take dope as in real life and alsolots of realistic fights 2. plots are realistic 3. the people seem sorta real 4. trying to promote unity like the world today 5. people gamble in real life 6. yes, because it could happen

Why are people in show liked?

I. Georgehe's strong and smart 2. act like teenagers of today 3. leaderway he keeps contiol over everyone

Room 222

Why watch? 1. related to show life 2. likes stories with kids in them 3. to get a laughfunny show 4. learn about classroom life 5. not too funny nor too serious 6. because it is funny 7. likes the comedy 8. shows what real life is like 9. interesting facts about daily life 10. all about schoolpeOple are up todate 11. it's funnynot everyday thing What is learned from watching this show? 1. shows clothes and things that really happen im school 2. how high school studentsibehave, theirmanners, how they deal with teachers 3. how to choose a college 4. how problems can be worked out 5. you can get into trouble in school easily

How is this show realistic? 1. shows how people live and behave 2. shows all races work and live together

. 365 358 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

3. shows true high school life 4. believe people really act this way 5. call see some of the characters in my friends 6. most of the things really happen 7. same courses 8. same things happen in my school 9. things on show really happen in school W. faculty and kids help each other in realistic way

How is this show unrealistic?

I. teachers don't act as in real life 2. teachers pick on individual students instead of letting students volunteer

Why are people in show liked?

I. Pete is a good teacher, doesn't just teach 2. Dixon. the way he teaches history 3. Jason, way he smiles 4. Karen because she's silly 5. Pete is smart and understanding 6. they act realistically 7. objectivity of people and liberalness 8. Dixon's understanding toward kids

Why watch?

I. entertainment, comical action 2. likes to see show-downs 3. likes cowboy stories 4. likes cowboy shows 5. shooting guns 6. likes westerns

7. likes westerns, hard life and guns and horses ,

What is learned from watching this show?

I. ways different people act 2. how life was in the past 3. learn not to steal because you'll get caught anyway 4. learns about old west

266 MASS MEDIA USE AND AGGRESSION 359 How is this show realistic?

I. shows real life in long ago west 2. real life when Marshall Dillon grew up 3. Marshall had to clean out the fastshooters 4. duel and gun draws How is this show unrealistic?

I. westerns aren't real for today I. western days are over 3. aren't that many fights in real life

Why are people in show liked?

I. the characters' fighting is comical 2. characters are good actors 3. especially Festus and Marshall Dillon

[Then Came Bronson 1

Why watch?

I. interesting way Bronson is so free 2. way he drives motorcycle 3. lots of action, travels all over 4. because of his understanding of problemsand way of helping others 5. always wanted a motorcycle 6. Bronson's cool and free and travels all over 7. Bronson's always free despite howpeople get involved with one another 8. because he sings and does differentthings

What is learned from watching this show?

I. how people react to freeness 2. likes where he travels.would like to doit 3. about dishonest situations andhow young people must be care- ful 4. need an education to move upcan'tlive on motorcycle alone

How is this show realistic?

I. different patterns of living aroundthe U.S.A. 2. many real life situations 360 TEI.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIV ENESS

3.in life you see free guys riding around like Bronson 4. men do roam country like Bronson 5.lots of teens travel and like to be on their own 6. shows how a person can work to help others

How is this show both realistic and unrealistic?

1.situations sometimes real, sometimes not

Why are people in show liked?

1. especially Bronson 2. especially Bronson because he rides motorcycle and has long hair 3. especially Bronson's sensibility, what he thinks 4. Bronsonhis understanding toward people

368

411110-11111.a/ft..... 1 ...Po .1

Four Masculine Styles in Television Programming: A Study of the Viewing Preferences of Adolescent Males

Raymond L. Johnson, Herbert L. Friedman American Institutes for Research and Herbert S. Gross University of Maryland School of Medicine

The influence of television in changing a person's attitudes or behav- ior may be most evident when the person is already undergoing change. While experiencing the uncertainties of transition, a person becomes active and purposeful in seeking relevant information (Feather, 1967), and television can be a ready and profuse source. In this study, we at- tempted to interpret observed patterns of selective exposure to televi- sion programs as an instance of information seeking stimulated by one of the major transition phases in human development, the onset of ado- lescence.1 361 369 362 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Erikson (1968) describes adolescence as one of the eight critical peri- ods of change in the life cycle of an individual. The male. at about age 14. begins to consolidate a sense of identity around the biological nucle- us of his maturing sexuality. He searches for a way of thinking about himself that provides a coherent account of his childhood successes and failures, enabling him to view his own life in continuous perspective. At the same time, he is concerned with the question of what kind of man he might become:

Young people. beset with the physiological revolution of their genital matunt- tion and the uncertainty of the adult roles ahead...are...preoccupied with what they appear to be in the eyes of others as compared with what they feel they are. and with the question of how to connect the roles and skills cultivated earlier with the ideal prototypes of the day (Erikson. 1968. p. 128).

To achieve a viable concept of the self, the boy chooses and conforms to a masculine style from among the options available in the society. The definition of manliness which the boy adopts as his own will enter into the formation of his sense of identity. Television programs portray a wide variety of masculine styles. and this aspect may become televi- sion's most salient feature for adolescent boys seeking information about ideal prototypes. The purpose of this study was to identify and describe some of the styles of masculine behavior depicted on programs especially popular among teenage viewers. The end result was a provisional classification schema for television programs. based upon variants of the masculine "image." METHOD Subjects. The eighty boys who participated in this study attended the eighth grade of a racially mixed junior high school in a lower-to-m iddle- income neighborhood of Baltimore. Maryland. School counselors se- lected 39 boys with histories of aberrant social aggressiveness and 41 who had not exhibited marked aggressive tendencies.

White (Ai) Black (A2)

Aggreuive(81) n=20 n=19

A181A281

Non-aggressive 1921 n=20 n=21

A182A282

Table 1: Types of adolescent male viewers. The two variables used to categorize the subjects (race and interpersonal behavior) are designated by the letters A and B, respectively. 370 FOUR MASCULINE STYLES 363 Procedure.Subjects filled out a questionnaire which listed all prime time (7:30-11 p.m.) network television programs seen in the Baltimore area during the 1969-70 season. Theinstructions were to indicate which programs a subject had watched at least fivetimes in the preceding four months (the period from September to December). Data Analysis.The plan of this study was to empirically construct a classification schema for a set of selected television programs.Relation- ships among the programs were inferred from thedegree of overlapping (i.e., substantially duplicated) audience. Ourassumption was that pro- grams attracting the sameviewers portrayed similar versions of the masculine role. By ranking programs on the basis ofshared audience, it was possible to identify programssimilar in appeal and to form hy- potheses about the nature of the shared attributes.Once abstracted, the attributes provided the conceptual frameworkneeded to construct a classification schema. As the initial step in constructing the schema,12 programs were se- lected for detailed examination. Discarded were programspopular among all four groups of subjects:Mod Squad, Room 222, It Takes a Thief, Bill Cosby, Hawaii Five-0, Land of theGiants.Retained were programs more likely to havebeen frequently watched by the viewers belonging to one group than by those belonging to theother three.The FBI,for example, was watched (at least five times) by 80 percentof white aggressive viewers, while 63 percent of theothers indicated they had seen the program that often. White aggressives werealso more like- ly to have often watchedThen Came Bronson(55 percent, compared with 30 percent for others) and were slightly moreattracted toAdam 12 (65 percent, others 60 percent). White nonaggressivesshowed a differ- entiating preference forThe Courtship of Eddie's Father(75 percent, others 63 percent),The Newlywed Game(70 percent, others 50 per- cent), andMy Three Sons(55 percent, others 30 percent). Popular pro- grams among black aggressive boys wereMannix (84 percent, others 60 percent),Mission Impossible(84 percent, others 65 percent), andJulia (74 percent, others 45 percent). Black nonaggressives were morelikely than others to have watchedBewitched(76 percent, others 60 percent), Green Acres(76 percent, others 55 percent), andMayberry R.F.D.(67 percent, others 40 percent). The programpreferences of each group of subjects were thus represented by three different programs. To measure the audience overlap among these twelve programs,the identities of the viewers watching any two programs were determined, and tallies were made of the number who watched both programs,the number who watched one but not the other, and the number who report- ed watching neither program. These frequency tallies were castin the form of two-by-two tables, and a phi-coefficient was computed as an index of correlation between each of the 66 pairs of programs. 364 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS The phi-coefficients were then used to rank-order the programs on the basis of audience overlap. But this step raised a procedural question: where should the list begin? There was no necessary starting point. Hence, the ranking had to be a circular one which would allow each pro- gram , in turn, to be ranked first. Our approach was to attem pt to arrange the phi-coefficients to form a circumplex (Guttman, 1966). In a correlation matrix exhibiting "circumplicial" structure, the corre- lations are largest next to the principal diagonal which runs from the upper left corner to the lower right corner. Moving away from the diago- nal, the correlations first decrease and then begin to increase in a con- sistent way. This systematic descending-ascending pattern is observed in both the rows and the columns of the matrix. Guttman has shown that a correlation matrix that can be arranged to form a circumplex repre- sents a circular rank ordering of variables. The arrangement is circular because it represents a sequence with neither a beginning nor an end. A circumplex can be graphically displayed as a by spacing the variables around the circumference. Starting with any one variable and tracing around the circle (in either direction), correlations decrease in magnitude and then increase again as the departure point is neared. Var- iables which occupy adjacent positions are most highly correlated, and thus more similar, while variables on the opposite side have the smallest correlations and are least similar.

RESULTS

It was possible to arrange intercorrelations among five of the pro- grams to form a circumplex. The matrix of phi-coefficients is found in Table 2,and a graphic version is displayed in Figure 1.

Eddie's Mayberry Bronson Father Bewitcrwo Mannix

Bronson 1.00 .11 .02 .05 .11

Eddie's Father .11 1.00 .23 .13 .02

Bewitched .02 .23 1.00 .27 .05

Mayberry R.F.D. .05 .13 .27 1.00 .19

Mannix .11 .02 .05 .19 1.00 ,

Table 2: Circumplex matrix of intercorrelations among five television programs.

372 FOUR MASCULINE STYLES 365 The differences in audience composition between any two programs ...;ere usually slight. The number of viewerswatching one program but not another was statistically significant in only four of the 66pair-wise comparisons. Nevertheless, the shifts in the makeup of audiences as one moves around the circle are systematic. The action-adventure series popular among aggressive viewers (Bron- son and Mannix) occupy adjacent positions aroundthe circle, as do the situation comedies preferred by nonaggressive viewers (Eddie's Fa- ther, Bewitched, and Mayberry. R.F.D.). Likewise, programs especial- ly popular among black viewers (Mannix, Mayberry, R.F.D., and Be- witched) are contiguous, as are those programs which appealed especial- ly to white viewers (Bronson and Eddie's Father). The turnover pattern from program to program around the circle is encoded as the changing sequence of letters and subscripts in Figure I. The circumplex includes at least one program especiallypopular among each type of viewer. Other programscould be substituted, but the result would be a less well-formed circumplex. However,the de- scending-ascending pattern of correlations is less seriously violatedif

BRONSON

Figure 1: A graphic representation of the circumplex Matrix. Letters with subscripts correspond to the viewer categories in Table 1, and indicate progrem(s) pre- ferred by each type of viewer.

373 366 VOUR MASCULINE STYLES the replacement is another program popular with the same type of view- er. For example. The FBI and Adam-I2 are partially interchangeable with Bronson. All three series were favorites of white aggressive view- ers. But neither is a permissible substitute for other programs in the cir- cum plex .These constraints on interchangeability indicated that the three programs preferred by each of the four types of viewers formed discrete, cohesive sets (an interpretation confirmed by a cluster analysis of the complete 12 x 12 matrix of correlations). The construction of a circumplex was useful in helping us recognize the com mon features among seemingly dissimilar programs. Each pro- gram most resembled (in the make-up of its audience) the two on either side, and this relationship implied that adjacent programs possessed some attribute in common. Such a circumplex. linking a diversity of tel- evision programs. enabled us. in this study. to structure our thinking about the determinants of program preferences.

DISCUSSION

A male's understanding of his sexual role develops during two peri- ods. each lasting about six years (Kagan, 1969). Before puberty. a boy learns about the expectations and demands of thc masculine role through his associations with other boys. During adolescence. his under- standing is significantly altered as he learns to relate to girls. The four- teen-year-olds who participated in this study were at the juncture of these two periods. The information-seeking hypothesis proposes that under these conditions of transitional uncertainty, the adolescent male is highly motivated to search for prototypes of adult masculine behavior. Television is a readily accessible source of high-definition portraits of the masculine styles common in our mass culture. Masculine style refers to those characteristic aspects of a man's rela- tionships (with either women or other men) which are relatively invar- iant from person to person or across social contexts. The results of our data analysis of television program preferences led us to devise aclassi- fication schema of styles using two sets of paired, contrastingattributes. Within this format it was possible to elaborate a definition of masculine style which resembled semantic differential definitions of concepts. In- deed. the specific meanings we chose to assign to the abstract attribute system were derived from semantic differentialstudies intended to de- termine the meanings of diverse social roles (Friedman and Gladden. 1964) and of nonverbal communications (Mehrabian, 1970). These at- tributes reflect two different aspects of interpersonal behavior: status and reactivity. The status relationship between two people is determined when one person assames the dominant role (i.e., high status) and the other assumes the subordinate role. Reactivity refers to a person's tend- ency either to actively initiate interpersonal contacts or topassively re- act to the social moves of others.

. 374 MM

I FOUR MASCULINE STYLES 367 1 I Permutations of these contrasting attributes yield four styles of mas- I culine portrayal, presented in Table 3 together with our inferred trait characterization for each style. These are briefly discussed, in turn.

White (A1) Black (A2)

high status/actIve low status/actIve

Aggressive (131) 'THE FORCEFUL MALE' 'THE TACTICAL MALE'

AIB1 A281

high status/passive low status/passive

Non.aggressive (132) 'THE PROTECTING MALE' 'THE VULNERABLE MALE'

A1B2 A2B2

Table 3: Four Styles of Masculine Behavior Portrayed in Television Programs Popular among Adolescent Boys.

The vulnerable male (low status/passive) is portrayed in the three se- ries preferred by nonaggressive black viewers:Bewitched, Green Acres, and M ayberry, R. F.D. In these programs, a common plot device is to entran a man in a humiliating situation which exposes his impo- tence. Tou weak and inept to rescue himself, he is dependent upon others for help. The help often comes from a masterful woman. Darrin, in the Bewitched series, depicts a fantasy version of this style. He is the lone mortal in a family of witches and warlocks, and his total subjugation through the malevolent magic of Endora, the hostile mother-in-law, is staved off only through the repeated interventions of his wife, Saman- tha. Green Acres portrays the harried life of a gentleman farmer outma- neuvered by his witless wife and the crafty folk of Hooterville. The comic incompetence of the male is the recurrent story idea of another program in this category, Mayberry, R.F.D. In one episode, fixit man Emmett Clark broke his arm and had to hire a high school boy to help in the shop. The townspeople soon discovered that the boy was much bet- ter at repair work than Clark. In another episode, one of the main char- acters, Sam Jones, unsuccessfully competed with a hired farm hand for the respect and admiration of his own son. The tactical male (low status/active), a favorite of aggressive black viewers, is represented by Mannix, Mission Impossible, and by certain aspects of Julia. The lead characters are all adept problemsolvers. A client is in trouble or in danger and must be rescued. One writer for the Mannix series considers Joe Mannix a "Christ figure," noting that "he 375 368 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS really is everybody's ombudsman: he'll make it right" (quoted in TV Guide, October 31, 1970). As a consequence of his low status, however, the tactical male must work under severe handicaps. He is always in an exposed, vulnerable position: a private detective who must act aggres- sively without the legal authority of the police, an undercover team sent on dangerous missions behind enemy lines, a black nurse (the widowed mother of a small boy) who works for a domineering white doctor. In each case, their underdog positions require that they resort to cunning strategy and surprise rather than force and coercion. The tactical male maintains close ties with others, and his band of associates often includes a woman. The continuing characters on Mis- sion ImpOssible form a closely-knit group of collaborators with one woman in a supporting role. Mannix depends upon his black secretary. At least one black actor appears regularly in all three series. The protective male (high status/passive) describes the key adult roles in The Courtship of Eddie's Father, My Three Sons, and The Newlywed Game, three programs especially popular among nonaggressive white viewers. The emphasis of these series is the man's capacity to fill a woman's place in the family. Eddie's Father and My Three Sons both are concerned with fathers' determined attempts to rear their motherless sons. The affectionate rela- tionship between father and son is clearly conveyed in The Courtship of Eddie's Father. As originally conceived, the "courtship" was to denote the boy trying to fix up dad with dates every week. But as the show evolved in concept, the courtship became that between father and son. In the 26 episodes of the 1969-70 season, only twice did eight-year-old Eddie try to find a wife for his father. James Komack, the producer, considers the single parent format necessary for delving into the parent- child relationship with any depth. He said, "A woman would interfere would take away half the time, half the affection, half the moment" (TV Guide, July 4, 1970). The vividness of the protective male style was demonstrated by Fos- ter (1964), who found that even the most acceptable real-life fathers seemed less attractive to their sons than did the fathers portrayed on popular television series. In our study, the appeal of this style was rela- tively limited to white boys, however, since blacks avoided white fami- ly-type situation comedies. A similar racial pattern in program prefer- ences was reported several years ago by Carey (1966). The role of the protective male is seen in a somewhat different version in The Newlywed Game. Success in playing the game depends on the collaboration, not competition, of husband-and-wife teams. To score, a husband or wife must be able to correctly guess how the other partner answered a question. The winning team is the couple which can best take the place of each other. 376 FOUR MASCULINE STYLES 369 The forceful male (high status/active) was a style attractive to white aggressive viewers. High status is accorded the male lead either because he is empowered to compel compliant behavior from others (Inspector Erskine of The FBI, officers Reed and Malloy of Adam-I2), or because he is so unassailable in his independence that he alone determines his own actions ( Bronson). The forceful male is dominating, self-sufficient, ag- gressively on the offensive. His response to threat is immediate and di- rect. There is seldom need for subterfuge or surprise. Typically, these shows have few continuing characters and shift lo- cales from one episode to another. The male leads are constantly on the move, and hence plot development does not depend upon complex inter- actions among characters. Moreover, the fictional world the forceful male inhabits is a man's world. No women appcar regularly in any of the three series, and the roles which are assigned to women are usually inci- dental. The low involvement of wouen characters is reminiscent of Hemingway's short story collection, Men Without Women, in which the thrills of fishing. boxing, and bullfighting are preferred to the pleasures of women.2 The style of the forceful male further resembles a conventional Hem- ingway character in its associations with high adventure and violence and its hard-sell presentation as a life style worthy of imitation. The Soames (1969) have argued that "much of the effect of observational learning depends upon the success and prestige of the model who is imi- tated." Heavy propaganda infuses all three series which portray the forceful male. The FBI is an officially sanctioned account of Bureau heroics, while Adam-12 often introduces law-and-order editorializing into its dialogue. Bronson promotes a quick getaway life style through- out each program, from the opening title vignette (which shows a man beset with the cares of middle age betraying his envy of Bronson's easy- riding freedom) to the lyrics of the closing theme: "Goin' down that long, lonesome highway... gonnalive like my way.. .1 won't be hangin' 'round." Of the four masculine styles described, the forceful male is the most escapist since, as Lucy Komisar (1970) has noted, "this definition of manhood can no longer exist for most men, except through the shal- low medium of television." The programs we have considered both reflect and perpetuate mass culture stereotypes of masculine roles. The popularity of these particu- lar programs among adolescent boys is probably due to the clarity with which the male leads embody stock styles of masculine behavior. When television influences a boy to adopt one of these styles as his own, his choice serves to maintain and reinforce its viability as a model for man- hood.

377 370 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS FOOTNOTES

I. This study was based upon data gathered for the Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior. under contract with the National Institute of Mental Health. The au- thors acknowledge the assistance of Dr. Robert L. Derbyshire. Uni- versity of Maryland School of Medicine, who supervised the survey phase of the research, and Mary G. Kalis, for the statistical analyses.

2. One consequence of a program winning popularity among white ag- gressive viewers may be a loss of women viewers. According to the Home Testing Institute. Adam-I2 was the only program in our selec- tion to appear among its TvQ "top ten" list of nighttime network shows toward the end of the 1969-70 season. A TvQ score is the pro- portion of all viewers familiar with a program who single it out as one of their favorites. For each nighttime network show. the Home Test- ing Institute releases separate TvQ scores for men and women (18 years and older). children (6-11), and teenagers (12-17). The audience strength of Adam-I2 derived from the fact that it was the most popu- lar show among teenage viewers, the second most popular among children (close behind The Wonderful World of Disney), and was one of two top-rated nightime shows more popular with men than women (Bonanza was the other). No other program on the "top ten" list, however, had a lower TvQ score among women viewers. (Source: Advertising Age, July 13, 1970.)

REFERENCES

Carey, J. Variations in negro/white television preferences. Journal of Broadcasting, 1966, 10,199-212. Erikson, E.H. Identity: youth and crisis. New York: W.W. Norton and Co., 1968. Feather, N. T. An expectancy-value model of information-seeking be- havior. Psychological Review, 1967, 74, 342-60. Foster, J. E. Father images: television and ideal. Journal of Marriage and the family. 1964, 26,353-55. Friedman, C. J., and Gladden, J. W. Objective measurement of social role concepts via the semantic differential. Psychological Reports, 1964, 14, 239-47. Guttman, L. Order analysis of correlation matrices. In Cattell, R. B. (Ed.)Handbookofmultivariateexperimentalpsychology. Chicago: Rand McNally and Co., 1966. 378 FOUR MASCULINE STYLES 371

Kagan. J. Sex-role identity.Psychology Today,1969. 3, 39-42. Komisar, L. Violence and the masculine mystique. TheWashington Monthly, 1970,2,39-48. Mehrabian, A.Asemantic space for nonverbal behavior.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,1970, 35, 248-57. Soames, L. M., and Soames, A. T. Social learning and social violence. Proceedings,77th A nnual Convention. American Psychological Association, 1969.

379 Television Viewing Habits and Aggression

John P. Robinson and Jerald G. Bachman

Survey Research Center, University of Michigan

The Survey Research Center's Youth-in-Transition project has been studying a nat onal probability sample of about 2.200 young men to de- termine their attitudes, plans. and behaviors, particularly those relating to educational and occupational aspirations. Data collections, using per- sonal interviews and written questionnaires. have spanned a period of nearly four years. The first data (autumn 1966) were collected when the boys were in tenth grade at 87 public high schools scattered throughout the United States. The second collection (spring 1968) was made when the majority of boys were finishing eleventh grade. The third (spring 1969) took place before those boys still in high school had graduated. 372 .ao VIEWING HABITS AND AGGRESSION 373

The final data were collected during June and July of 1970 from 70 percent (or more than 1.500) of the boys in the original sample. Most of the boys had been out of high school for about a year; a large proportion had just finished their first year of college, although many others were in military service or in the work force. The Youth-in-Transition project added questions on television use to this final data collection, with the objective of examining the ways in which experiences with television might relate to the massive body of attitude and behavioral variables al- ready available from the project. Of particular interest was the relation between television viewing and$the self-reports of delinquent behavior (much of which was highly aggressive in nature) which had been regular- ly included in all four data collections. Boys in the age range encompassed by this project are probably in a stage of life characterized by considerably more active, aggressive, and destructive behavior than any other stage of the life cycle; hence they comprise a most crucial sample in which to examine possible relation- ships between television violence and aggressive social behavior. From the level of delinquent behavior reported in each data collection, it would appear that these boys' aggressive behavior at age 19 was about the same as that when they were in eleventh or twelfth grade. The boys have reported dramatically higher levels of participation in aggressive delinquent behavior than one would surmise from the already shocking- ly high official public records of such behavior. Further details on study design and sampling procedures of the Youth-in-Transition project can be found in Bachman (1970). Media questions Three television use questions were asked in the final (June-July 1970) interview, Each boy was asked how many hours of television he watched on an average day. what his four favorite television program s were, and whether he felt he had learned more about life fromtelevision or in school. The last questionin answer to which some 15 percentof these boys chose television. while 80 percent named schoolis exam- ined in a separate report in this series (Robinson. 1971). Respondents reported using television 1.7 hours on an average daya low figure when compared with the three-hour figure given by national samples of adults (Roper. 1969; LoSciuto, 1971). Some 18 percent of the boys estimated no hours per day; 37 percent estimated an average of one hour. 22 percent two hours. and 23 percent three hours or more. (The last viewing figure is at or above the national average for adults.) Differ- ences between college and noncollege youth weresurprisingly trivial. The 19-year-olds' aversion to television is further evidenced by the high proportion (44 percent) who would not or could not list at least three favorite television programs when asked to do so. For the 56 per- cent who did list at least three or four favorites, an index wasdeveloped 381 382 374 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

to assess the prepondcrance of violent content in these programs. All television programs rated as notably violent by both the public and criti- cal samples surveyed by Greenberg and Gordon (1971. elsewhere in this series) were given a score of 2. Those ratedas moderately or occasional- ly violent were given a score of I: those with littleor no violence, a score of 0. Programs scored as 2 induded Mod Squad. Mannix. and most of the other programs falling into the "adventure" category. Suchpro- grams as Walt Disney. Get Sman. Bracken's World. Bronson. and Ho- gan's Heroes were scored asI. Programs like Marcus We lby. M.D.. Family Affair. Laugh-ln. and Dean Martin were scoredas 0. Two types of programs not evaluated by Greenberg and Gordon's raters but still listed by several respondents were football (arbitrarily rated 2) andmov- ies (rated 1). 1 hus a respondent who listed Mod Squad. NFL Football. Get Smart. and Dean Martin as his four favorites would receive a score of 5on this index (2 + 2 + 1 + 0). The favorite programs of thoseyoung men who listed three or four favorites skewed somewhat toward the nonviolent end of the index (with an average score of 3.0 on a scale running from 0 to 8), but more than 40 percent scored 4 (the theoretical midpoint) or more on the index. Only about 14 percent listed favorites that contained no rated violence.

Relation between violent programs and aggressive behavior

For purposes of analysis, scores on this violence viewing indexwere divided into four categories: "almost none" (scores 0-1), "some" (scores 2-3), "much" (scores 4-5), and "a great deal" (scores 6-8). The group which failed to list at least three favorites was also considered; it served as a useful "control" group against which to compare the differ- ential behavior of young men who mentioned favoriteprograms with varying amounts of violence. The eight items dealing with interpersonal aggression in the delinquent behavior scale are listed in Table I. along with the proportions whore- ported having engaged in such behaviors in the previousyear for each of these violence viewing categories.1 Thus, in the first row of Table I,it can be seen that 37 percent of young men whose favorite programs con- tained "a great deal" of violence reported getting intoa serious fight, compared with 33 percent of those listing programs containing "much" violence, 30 percent of those who listed programs containing "some" violence, and 25 percent of those listing programs containing "almost no" violence. Thus the percentage of those who got into serious fights is almost half again as high among those with heavy concentrations of vio- lence in their favorite programs as among those who mentionedno vio- lent favorites. A steady monotonic increase in aggressive behavior is reported by those with intermediate television violence preferences. The 37 percent rate of participation in aggressive behavior reported by 11111, 4

Table 1: Percent engaging in various aggressive behaviors by viewing of TV violence Amount of violence in four favorite programs (June July 1970) Total sample (n=1559) Almost none (n=244) (n=283)Some (n=239)Much Great deal(n=120) leastCould three not mentionfavorites at (n=673) Item: 5. 1.Gotten Gotten something into a serious by tslunq fight a atperson school something or work bad would happen to him it he didn't Percent 2515 Percent 3019 Percent 2033 Percent 3718 Percent 1326 12. Hit an instructor9. Hurt someone or supervisor bed enough to need bandages ora doctor 17 6 23 7 21 7 1128 18 6 1.4.18.17. Hit TakenHit your your partfather mother in a fight where a bunch of your friends are against another bunch 19 35 23 83 24 48 28 48 21 2 21. Used a knife or gun or some other thing (like aInterpersonal club) aggressioneightto get index items) something (based fromon the a personabove 119 2 125 4 126 3 132 8 119 3 taa 376 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS boys who showed extreme preferences for violent programs was also considerably higher than the 26 percent participation rate reported by young men (presumably less serious television fans) who failed tolist at least three favorite programs. The participation rate for these nonwatch- ers was comparable to that reported by preferrers of nonviolent pro- grams. Very much the same pattern emerges for each of the seven remaining items indicating interpersonally aggressive delinquent behavior. In sev- eral instances no differences or reversals can be observed, but, by and large, tbe higher the concentration of violence in a young man's favorite programs, the more likely he is to report having participatedin aggres- sive behavior. Particularly dramatic differences appear in rerorts of hit- ting an instructor/supervisor and using a weapon to get something from another person. At the same time, these progressive increases in aggressive behavior among more avid fans of violent television are not of sufficientmagni- tude to gain statistical significance for many items. In order to obtain a clear overall picture for such statistical testing purposes, these eight items have been merged into a single interpersonal aggression index of from 100 (no reported aggression in the previous year) to 400 (participa- tion five or more times during the previous year for all eight items).2 From the low participation rates in Table 1, we can infer that scores would skew highly toward the low end of this scale; this is borne out by the overall average score of 122 for the entire sample. Deviations around this average score for individuals with varying numbers of violent televi- sion favorites are presented at the bottom of Table I. It can be seen that there is a clearly monotonic, although not strong, relation between the two variables. As favorite television programs con- tain more violence, reported aggressive behavior inches upward. More- over, the amount of aggressive behavior by those listing some,much, or a great deal of violent favorites does turn out to besignificantly higher than that reported either by young men listing less than three favorites (t = 4.7, 1305 df.) or by those listingfavorites with no violence (t = 3.9, 878 df.). The question naturally arises whether such a result could be attributed to other factors like social class or personal characteristics.Controls on four such competing explanatory factors are examined in Table 2. These factors include mother's education (one key indicator of social class), 'race, amount of television viewed, and interpersonalaggression report- ed in the previous interview (spring 1969). Of these four variables, only hours of television viewing fails to predict higher reported aggressive behavior. Sons of mothers with less education, blacks, and those report- ing more aggressive behavior as high school seniors all reported more aggressive behavior at age 19 than the sons of the better educated, whites, and those reporting less high school aggressive behavior.

. 384

111111VIII 1111.

Table 2: Scores on interpersonal aggression by viewing of television violence for certain demographic groups AlmostAmount none of violence in favorite TV shows Some Much Great deal programsLess than mentionedthree favorite Mother'sInterpersonal education aggression index (n=1559) (n=244) 119 (n=283) 125 (n=239) 126 (n=120)132 (n=673) 119 AtHighDid least not School finishsome graduate highcollege school (n=292)(n=722) (n=458) 131110115 130127123 117125127 121125139 120113123 ifkrCA) AmountRace of viewing BlacksWhites (n=167)(n=1351) 139115 149121 133125 160125 137118 Cit Interpersonal aggression reported4 +20 inhours/day 1 previous hours/day (n=167)year (n=845) (ix.. Spring 1969) 3 hours/day (n=541) 125119117 133119128 126117127 134131132 140118119 WellSomewhatAverage-below above aboveaverage average average (n=156) (n=1243) (n=149) 159131113 205146115 167139115 221137115 112172135 378 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIV EN ESS The smooth monotonicity found overall fails to hold up when controls for these subdivisions of the sample are applied. Monotonicity holds for sons of mothers who were high school graduates. but not among sons of mothers who did not finish high school or who attended college. It holds for whites, but not for blacks who fall into the moderate violent viewing category. The same nonmonotonic pattern holds true for young men who watch the most hours of television. Perhaps the most dynamic test. however, is whether the television violence-interpersonal aggression relation continues to hold for respond- ents reporting varying amounts of identical aggression in the interview one year earlier. If it can be shown that viewers with a strong preference for violent programs were previously more prone to aggressive behavior and that this relation serves to essentially nullify any differentials noted in Table 1, a strong argument can be made that the Table 1 results are more attributable to previous levels of aggression than to any contribut- ing factor from television violence. The relation, examined at the bottom of Table 2, indeed indicates that no consistent difference'in aggressive behavior accompanies increased preference for television violence among young men who reported aver- age or somewhat above-average aggressive delinquent behavior in the previous year. The picture for the most aggressive individuals (self-re- portedly) in the previous interview is more complex, however. Within this group, those mentioning favorites with almost no violence report the least aggressive behavior (159), while those listing favorites contain- ing a good deal of violence report the most aggression (221). However, we are again faced with contradictory results for the middle groups. Respondents listing few violent favorites reported more aggressive be- havior (205) than respondents listing a moderately high number of vio- lent favorites (167). Overall, however, the contrast between those listing programs with "some," "much," or "a great deal" of violent favorites (mean = 196) and those listing "almost no" favorites (mean = 159) is statistically significant ( t = 2.8, 93 df.), as is the contrast between the some-much-great deal groups and respondents listing less than three favorite programs (mean = 175; t = 2.3, 142 df.). In essence, then, once previous aggression is controlled, we are able to corroborate a link between aggressive behavior and preference for violent television programs only among those subjects who reported themselves most aggressive to begin with. Even here the link is neither monotonic nor dram atic enough to implicate preference for violent tele- vision as a primary determinant of aggressive interpersonal behavior. Before concluding this examination of the relation between television preferences and behavior, it might be worthwhile to consider one fur- ther set of da'ta that could offer a further competing explanation for the Table 1 results. That explanation centers around the greater activity lev- els, tendencies toward any delinquent behavior (interpersonally aggres- 386 lorampour*

VIEWING HABITS AND AGGRESSION 379 sive or not), or adventure-seeking of those who expressed greater pref- erence for violent television. Such relations are examined in Table 3 for 13 delinquent (but not interpersonally aggressive) types of behavior. It can be seen in Table 3 that the more avid fans of television violence are not more likely to report engaging in seven of these 13 actions: petty theft, trespassing, arguing with parents, running away from home, school vandalism,shoplifting, and drinking. These more avid viewers of television violence are slightly more likely to report arson, getting into' trouble with the police, and minor car theft; they are considerably more likely to report major theft, especially of cars and 'car parts. Overall, these results tend to suggest that boys who are more likely to prefer vio- lence on television are distinguished from those whose tastes run to less violent fare (or who have few favorite television programs) by their greater participation in more serious delinquent behavior. Only insignifi- cant differences by varying f,:tevision taste patterns exist for less serious delinquency. Summary and conclusions

We have found a significant and monotonic relation between partici- pation in aggressive delinquent behaviors (fighting, armed robbery) and preference for violent television programs among a national probability sample of over 1,500 boys who are one year out of high school. The rela- tion tends to become nullified or qualified as controls for mother's edu- cation, race, amount of viewing, and previous aggressive behavior are imposed. There is limited support for the view that preference fot vio- lent television content is associated with higher aggressive behavior among boys previously most active in such behavior. The latter finding is our most ambitious attempt to add any causal flavor to the interpretation of these results. Nevertheless, even this lim- ited evidence must be advanced with utmost caution. The predictor vari- able used herethe mention of violent programs among one's favorites is a most subjective measure, which obviously says more about the tastes of the respondent than about the effects of what he has seen on television. In essence, then, preference for violent television content is primarily a personality variable, much in the way that Rorschach or thematic apperception test cards are intended to elicit those features of one's environment that are most salient to the individual.3 While it prob- ably does correlate significantly with pure exposure to such violent fare, it remains a contaminated measure of this variable. At best, then, the strongest possible conclusion boils down to the well-worn maxim that media content only serves to reinforce the pre- existing tendencies of its viewers. While this is a relatively common- place finding for effects of the media in other areas (e.g., in changing at- titudes, increasing passivity or knowledge), seldom has appropriate rec-

3E37 Table 3: Percent participating in various delinquent behaviors (not interpersonally aggressive) by viewing of TV violence (June Amount of violence in favorite television program Less than three favorite July 1970) Item 2. Taken something not belonging to you worth under $50 Almost none Percent 46 Percent Some 49 Percent Much 47 Great dealPercent 44 programs mentioned Percent 43 4.3. SetWent fire onto to someone someone's else's land property or into some on purpose house or bebuilding there when you weren't supposed to 56 4 52 3 54 6 52 5 53 4 8.6. 7.GottenArgued Run away intoor had trouble from a fight home with with police either because of your of parents something you did 316413 301275 427011 376815 3070 9 Gel05C5 11.10.13. DamagedTaken Drunk something beer school or propertyliquor from awithout store on purpose withoutparents paying permission for it 744016 442075 472274 436516 693915 16.15. Taken ana car expensive that didn't part belong of a car to someonewithout owner's in your permissionfamily without permission of the owner 46 78 69 10 12 67 20.19. TakenTook something an inexpensive not belonging part of a tocar you without worth owner'sover $50 permission - 10 8 12 13 1316 1210 VIEWING HABITS AND AGGRESSION 381 ognition been given to the dysfunational consequences of such a rein- forcement effect in the case of violent media content. Put another way that violent content only serves to support or activate the violent tend- encies of people who are already violentthis research finding takes on entirely different implications. The present analysis, however, is not sufficient to argue decisively for such a reinforcement effect, since numerous other variables must be considered at length before the Table 1 linkages can be advanced with- out undue skepticism. Many of these variables are available in the enor- mous file of background variables that the Youth-in-Transition project has assembled: sources of frustrations and disappointments at school or at home, relations with parents, self-esteem, life satisfaction, irritabili- ty, impulse to aggression, and so on. The present analysis represents only a superficial mining of this rich body of data. It does indicate, how- ever, that preference for violent television content is a variable that de- serves further exploration as a facilitating factor in the expression of aggressive interpersonal behavior.

FOOTNOTES

1 These questions on delinquent behavior, unlike most of the informa- tion obtained from these boys, were answered individually in a spe- cial booklet marked "Confidential Information." This procedure was followed because we felt that anyone would find it extremely embar- rassing to truthfully reveal this type of information verbally to an in- terviewer. Scrupulous care was taken to ensure the confidentiality of this information. The boys were asked their frequency of participa- tion in each activity, although Tables1 and 3 differentiate only be- tween those who took part in the activities and those who did not. 2. There is a compelling rationale for examining results with the index rather than as individual behaviors. Because of the highly confiden- tial and self-incriminating nature of these questions, responses to all individual items were destroyed soon after they were coded. Fre- quency counts and the runs on Tables 1 and 3 were the only analyses performed with these variables before they were destroyed. 3. In this sense it is interesting to find a strong correlation between pref- erence for violent television content and support for the Vietnam war in these data. Thirty-one percent of the young men who became more supportive of the war between 1969 and 1970 listed "much" or "a great deal" of violent content; only 21 percent of those who became more opposed to the war during that period listed violent content. Among those who were opposed to the war in both years, only 13 percent list favorite programs with much or a great deal of violence. 389 382 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS REFERENCES

Bachman, J.G.Youth in transition. Vol. 2.Ann Arbor. Mich.: Survey Research Center. Institute for Social Research. 1970. Greenberg. B.S.. and Gordon.. T. Perceptions of violence in television programs: critics and the public. InTelevision and social behavior.

Vol. 1 (this series). Washington. D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office. 1971. LoSciuto. L.A. A national inventory of television viewing behavior. In Television and social behavior.Vol. 4 (this series). Washington. D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971. Robinson. J.P. On defining the functions of television. InTelevision and social behavior.Vol. 4 (this series). Washington, D.C.: U.S. Gov- ernment Printing Office, 1971: Roper, B. Aten-year view of public attitudes toward television and oth- er mass media.1959-1968.New York: Television Information Office, 1969.

390 Television Violence and Deviant Behavior

Jennie J. McIntyre and James J. Teeyan, Jr., with the assistance of Timothy Hartnagel

University of Maryland

The role of the church in moral education has withered to a pallidweekly ses- sion at Sunday school. As we have seen, the family, primarily because of changes in the larger social order beyond its control. is no longer in a position to exercise its responsibilities. As for the schoolin which the child spends most of his timeit is debarred by tradition, lack of experience, and preoccupation with subject matter from concerning itself in any major way with the child's development as a person....The vacuum, moral and emotional, created by this state of affairs is then filledby defaulton the onehand by the television screen with its daily message of commercialism and violence, and on the other by the socially isolated. age-graded peer group. with its impulsive search for thrills and its limited capacity as a humanizing agent (Bronfenbrenner. 1970. pp. 115-16).

383

391 384 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR Although more emphatic than many, the above statementby a social scientist concerned with the socialization of youngsters in theUnited States is illustrative of the widespread concern withtelevision's influ- ence. particularly on the characterdevelopment of children. In part this question of the influence of television stemsfrom its ubi- quity. Although the most recent, itis the most popular of the mass media. The 1969 report of the staff of the NationalCommission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence notes that by 1968, 94 percentof American homes had sets, that average daily operating hours, per set varied from 4.8 in midsummer to 6.8 hours in January, andthat of the 66 percent of adults who do watch sometelevision on a given day. , the aver- age daily viewing time was just overthree and one-half hours (Baker and Ball, 1969). Another reason for the widespread concern about thepossible influ- ence of television is thebelief, exemplified by the above quotation, that it may be a major factor in the socialization of the young.At an age when their character development may be most susceptible toinfluence, children not only spend much time in front of thetelevision set, but do not limit their viewing to those programsintended for them. The Com- mission data show that more children are watching in the earlyevening than during the late afternoon "children's hours" and that theirviewing continues into the late evening. On one Monday during the period cov- ered, over five million children under the age of 12 and nearly 6.4million 12- to 17-year-olds were still watching between 10:30 and11 p.m. (Baker and Ball, 1969, p. 207). Given the amount of public (and especially ofchildren's) exposure to television, it is not surprising that recent years have seen muchattention focused on the content of television programming. The purposeof this scrutiny has been to examine the possible relationship between thein- creasing use of television and the concomitant increase in manysocial problems--for example, crime. It seemed to many people that the simul- taneous occurrence of these two developments was notcoincidental (see references 46-49 cited in Baker and Ball, 1969, p. 452). Violence in television programming thus became the focus of much public attention. Many observers felt that there was a possibilitythat the viewer of television violence would benore likely to engage in criminal or violent behavior. Others suggested thatthe violence on television might influence the viewer's perception of the amount of crime andvio- lence in the country, thereby affecting his level of fear andanxiety (President's Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of

Justice, 1967. p. 52). . While there may not be consensus regarding the effects of television violence, there is-more agreement on the presence of violence in pro., gram content. Baker and Ball, for example,reported the following con- clusions: 392 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 385

I. ... There is a great deal of violent content available, at all times of the day. for all manner of intended audience. 2. The presentation of violence is typically as a means of achieving virtuaLy any type of goal. 3. Violence is the predominant means of conflict resolution suggested in televi- sion drama, as of 1968. 4. The use of violence, whether sanctioned or not. is likely to be a successful means Of obtaining such goals. 5. Character depictions are stereotYped, emphasize the unusual behavior, and promote (through both emphasis and absence)* certain behavioral values, a majority of which are socially disapproved or undesirable. (Baker and Ball. 1969. pp. 441-42). Furthermore, on the basis of a content analysis of commercial televi- sion entertainment program ming done for the Commission's Media Task Force, it was concluded that violence is pervasive, occurring in 81 per- cent of all 1967 programs analyzed and 82 piercent in 1968. The content analysis also revealed that though the extent of violence varies by type of program, a majority of all types of programs containstviolence and that no network had less than 77 percent of all its programming (prime time, Oct. 1-7,1968) containing Violence (Baker and Ball, 1969, p. 333). The question which follows from a consideration of the content of tel- evision program ming, the extensive exposure to such programming, and its possible effects on behavior, attitudes, and beliefs has'been stated as follows: If models for violent behavior are repeatedly presented with few competing no- tions. and people. particularly children, repeatedly expose themselves to such materials, what could be a more favorable arrangement for learning about vio- lence. if not learning to do violence? (Baker and Ball. 1969. p. 237). Professor Otto Larsen, who poses this question, also points out, how- ever, that the abundance of violent media content and the frequency of exposure to it do not suffice to prove that the mass media can modify at- titudes or induce, violent behavior. He goes on to argue that the question cannot be simply whether the media have an effect. It must be discov- ered under what conditions, for whom, how much. and what kind of effect the media are likely to have. Furthermore, whatever may be the effects of the mass media upon their audience, these must be assessed in relation to the way other aspects of the larger social system affect these same persons (Baker and Ball. 1969, pp. 238-40). In the pages that follow the present investigators take seriously the recommendations of Professor Larsen and the other authors of the staff report.1 They first present a brief review of the relevant literature con- cerning the effects of television. They then proceed to the specification of a conceptual model for analyzing the effects of television violence and the development of hypotheses from this model. These hypotheses are followed by a discussion of the methodology of the present study and the presentation of the findings. Throughout the report the central con- cern will be questions raised but not answered in the National Commis- sion's Media Task Force report:1) does exposure to television violence 393 386 TEI.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

increase the probability of violent and/or deviant behavior; 2) do norms and values projected by the television world of violence affect the view- ers' norms for violence; and 3) does the world view presented by televi- sion foster belief in a society characterized by a high level of violence? Previous findings

A comprehensive review of the literature .is unnecessary since this task has been done quite recently (Weiss. 1968; Baker and Ball, 1969. Rather, this section looks selectively at three different aspects of the lit- erature pertinent to this research: 1) the effects of television on deviant behavior; 2) the social psychology of mass communications; and 3) ado- lescent deviant behavior. There has been much discussion of the possible effects of television on deviant behavior. Much of the research has been confined ,to labora- tory studies of aggression. The results of these experiments, however. are not uniform. Some researchers have concluded that television has little if any effect in causing aggression or deviant behavior in adoles- cents. According to this position. when presented with the same televi- sion stimuli, aggressive children will be aggressive, adjusted children will not. Banay argues before a congressional subcomminee on juvenile delinquency: "... The young people who are influenced by television toward crime seem to be different from others who are not so influ- enced, even before they are' influenced by television" (Schramm, 1961, p. 164). Schramm concurs: "Television then interacts with the needs and emotions the Child bfings toit. ...The most that teleitision can del iS to feed the malignant impulses that already exist" (II: 166): According to these views, television plays bUt a niitior part in CaUSing deViant behati- ior. It elicits that behavior whieh is aiready In the,child's repertoite,: Schranim (1961, p. 165) conclUdes, "therefOre, Our Lieliet is that the kind of child we send to television, rather than television itself, is the chief element in delinquency." Agreement comes from Bailyn (1959) in one study and Haines (1955) in another, who concur that it is "misfits" who select violent shows and that television has an effect only on "susceptible teenagers." Riley and Riley (1951) discovered that children who do not wish to be isolates, yet who cannot gain peer group acceptance, use television for compensato- ry fantasy purposes. Those children who are well integrated with their peers have less preference for action and violence on television. Gerson (1963) reports that a family/school context in which an adolescent is not integrated is more likely to generate the use of the mass media as a so- cializing agency than is a social context in which the adolescent is well integrated. In their review of the findings regarding the effects of observer char- acteristics, Bandura and Walters (1963, p. 85) report that a person's pre- vious experiences are among the major determinants of the influence of

. 394 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 387 a model. Persons with low self-esteem, who areincompentent. who have been previously rewarded for .the same behavior, who are highly dependent persons, or who believe themselves to be similar to a model, are especially likely to imitate or to match the responsesof a model and hence, by inference, to be influenced by the behavior of television char- acters. The demographic comparisons made in Baker and Ball (1969) are con- sistent with these views that previous experiences affect the way in which an individual uses television. Baker and Ball report that adults and teenagers who approve of violence or who have experienced vio- lence (as victim, observer, or assailant) have the same age, sex, race, and residence characteristics as those respondents who: 1) most fre- quently choose television for relaxation; 2) approve of television's por- trayals of violence; 3) prefer programs including violence. Olher researchers have found, however, that even the normal child can learn violence from television. Eron(1963) found a positive relation- ship between violence ratings of favorite programs and aggressive be- havior among third-grade boys (but not girls). Zajonc (1954) and Brod- beck (1955) found that children will imitate heroes who are successful, whatever their means. Schramm (1961) summariies the implications of Brodbeck's studies: "... Children may remember (and presumably be able to use) violence, even though it is in conflict with their ethics and values" (p. 163). Thus violence on television may lead to actual violence and deviant behavior. Bandura, for example, finds that imitative respon- ses may be acquired fromobservation, although performance of that response may be dependent on expectations ofreward for that behavior. The response pattern has been learned and is available if there later is an incentive for the behavior. Goranson (1969), in his recent review of the psychological effects of media portrayals of violence, summarizes the literature as follows:

Novel aggressive behavior sequences are learned by children through exposure to realistic portrayals of aggression on television or in films. A large proportion of these behaviors are retained over long periods of time if they are praoiced at least once.....The actual performance of aggressive behaviors learned from the media is largely contingent on the child's belief in the effectiveness of aggres- sion in attaining his goals while avoiding punishment. The mass media typically present aggression as a highly effective form of behavior (Baker and Ball. 1969.

pp. 409-10). , Learning from television includes the learning of attitudes as well as of behavior patterns. Walters's investigation of responses to a violent film sequence, for example, found that the outcome for observers was not using a weapon as such but taking the expressed motivations of the model as their own (Walters and Llewellyn-Thomas, 1963). In a similar vein, Himmelweit, Oppenheim, and Vince (1958) found that although television does not make kids more aggressive, children pick up values and information from television if they are needed and 295 388 "t ELEVISION AND ADOLESCEN1 AGGRESSIVENESS not available elsewhere. Thus adolescents watching television may pick up the information and attitudes needed for the commission ofdeviant acts. Some additional issues cut across these two major positions in the lit- erature. First. does television lead to aggression or does it lead to the draining off of aggression? Bandura's studies (1963) as well as those of Berkowitz (1964) cast doubt on the:catharsis or drive reduction theo- ries. The catharsis controversy has been summarized in papers by Gor- anson and Feshbach in Baker and Ball (1969). If television violence does not lead to aggression and deviance.direct- ly. a second issue is whether it might not le3d to an acceptance of vio- lence in others and a belief that violence is both common and socially acceptable. The Lovibond (1967) and the Thompson (1959) studies raise this issue. Wertham makes the similar suggestion that telev,ision makes viewers callous to the ugly aspects of violence: "The trouble is not that they get frightened (by television). but that they do not get frightened" (quoted in Larsen. 1968. p.38: emphasis added). Other writers have suggested directions in which future research on the effects of mass media violence should move. Maccoby (1968). for example. argues that the central question should be reformulated to read: how much effect on what kind of children, and under what cir- cumstances will the effects be exhibited? She points out further that the occurrence of television-stimulated violence depends to some degree on the probability of thc occurrence of appropropriately corresponding real-life situations. Halloran (1968) suggests the hypothesis that where a television program can be associated with an individual's personal con- flicts, the individual is more likely to carry the stimulation over into real life and to increase the amount of directly expressed aggression. In his review of the effects of the mass media. Weiss (1968) states the view that whether symbolically acquired information is ever used depends on a number of factors, such as the motivation to exhibit the learning in actual behavior, the ability to dó so. the proper opportunity to do so. the strength of internal and external restraints against doing so, and the sim- ilarity between the individual's actual environment and the media setting (p.126). Bandura (1968) has argued for a distinction I Itween learning and per- formance. He found that subsequent rewards to a child may effcit the performance of earlier learned forms of aggression. In another context. Maccoby (1954) notes that tendencies toward performing aggressive behavior will enter as one element in the set of behavior tendencies aroused later in semme relevant situation. Whether it actually occurs is a function of the strength of competing responses and the restraints acting upon the media-acquired behavior. Thus the media can beseen as a source of norms, but. as Larsen points out. it is necessary toinvestigate its relative rank vis-a.vis other soutces of norms. Thus he states that we

. 396 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 389 must establish how children perceive,identify with, 3nd use mass meoia content and sort this impact from the continuingimpact of other agen- cies of socialization which influence audience members before.during. and after exposure to mass. communication. In another context.Larsen not only poses the question of thepromotion of violence as a norm by the media but also asks whether there may be thefurther effect of op- portunity lost. The media, by focusing so strongly on violence, does not present to the audience the use of alternative meansof goal attainment (Baker and Ball. 1969. pp. 237-46).De Fleur (1966) has noted that persua- sive messages presented via the mass media mayprovide the appear- ance of consensus with respect to agiven object or goal. shortcutting the process of consensual validation.This shortcutting occurs particularly with respect to objects or practices about which groupsdo not yet have institutionalized cultural interpretations.DeFkur further notes that such messages can imply that adoption of thecommunicator's goal is normative in the group and hence the individual'sadoption of it will re- sult in social approval (p. 136). Suchfunctioning of the media may be particularly applicable to children and adolescentswho are in the pro- cess of being socialized and may not asyet have acquired the existing normative standards about appropriate means forachieving goals. In a review of the literature on masscommunications. Larsen (1964) has pointed out that while early work conceivedof the media as having direct effects upon the individual, this view hasgiven way to the position that social linkages between individuals play animportant part in me- diating the influence of mass communications.Selectivity in exposure and reaction arise out of organized social processes.Wright (1959) makes the similar point that although the individualis anonymous to the communicator, he is rarely anonymous in his socialenvironment. Rath- er, he is ordinarily amember of a network of primary and secondary gtoups which influence his opinions andattitudes. Inevitably these groups affect the way inwhich he is exposed to mass communication. how he interprets or reacts to any specificcommunication, and the ex- tent to which he will or canmodify his behavior in compliance with the message: Since the mass media of communication areonly one of a number of socializing agents. one important question to beraised concerns the in- terrelationships among these several agents. It would appear necessary to incorporate into the model a setof variables which at least potentially can be conceivedof as either increasing or decreasing theeffects of media stimuli. First, with respect to individual recipientsof communications. Hov- land et al. (1953) demonstrated that persons'with low self-esteem are more easily persuaded by acommunication than are persons with higher self-esteem. Voting behavior studies(Lazarsfeld et al.. 1948) indicate that audience self-selection occurs suchthat individuals listen to that 397 390 1 F.I.F.VISION ND ADOILFSCEVI AGGRESSIVFNEtis which they want to hear and which supports what theywant to believe. However, although perception is selective,itis also related to the groups to which the individual belongs. Kelley and Volkart (1952) dem- onstrated that boys who were most strongly motivatedto retain their membership in the Boy Scouts were the most resistantto a com m u nica- tion that ran counter to their group standards. Butgroups weakened by external or internal st resses may accept new attitudes which under other conditions would have been resisted (Riley et al.. 1951). Kelley and Woodruff (1956) have shown that perceivedsupport for a contrary opinion leads to a change in attitudes and norms. Experiments by Schramm and Danielson (1958) and by Zimmerman andBauer (1956) have shown that when positive referencegroups are lied in with the re- ceipt of a communication. these groups influence the individual to inter- pret the message as being consonant with the attitudes of thegroup. However. others (Festinger. 1950: Kelley. 1955: Charters and New- comb. 1958) have shown that a referencegroup must be salient before it is able to affect the attitudes of individuals. Variables suchas these must be utilized in the model. Since one of the dependent variables of interest is deviant behavioron the part of adolescents. the model being developedmust, of necessity. also incorporate variables that have been shown to affect such behavior. A major school of thought is the anomie tradition. It has been oriented primarily to explaining lower-class delinquency and has focusedmuch of its attention on occupational aspirations and class-based differences in the opportunity structure (Merton. 1938: Cohen. 1955: Clowardand Ohlin. 1960). On the other hand. Miller(1958)has argued that delinquen- cy is largely the natural result of conformity to lower-class cultural ex- pectations which conflict with the legal norms established by the middle class. Cohen(1966)has recently pointed out that much of deviant be- havior. rather than being a means to some end. may perhapsrepresent an expression of an individual's self-concept formed in his social rela- tionships with others. This notion is in turn related to labeling theory. which emphasizes that the way in which others define and behaveto- ward the individual may have the effect of confirming him ina deviant career (Lemert.1967:Becker. 1963: Scheff.1966). Whether children and adolescents who are motivated to deviateac- tually do so depends in part on the degree to which they experiencecon- straints against such behavior (Briar and Piliavin. 1965). Thesecon- straints or commitments to conformity include not only fear of material loss and punishment resulting from discovery, but alsoconcern about the consequences of such a discovery on one's attempts to maintaina consistent self-image. to sustain valued relationships. and topreserve current and future statuses and activities. The point to be emphasized in the present context, however, is that if one wishes to examine the impact of television stimuli on deviant behav- 398 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 391

ior among adolescents, it is necessary to include in the model those vari- ables which have been prominent in the literature on delinquency.The broader implication of this view is that there is no simple direct relation- ship between television stimuli and adolescent deviance. On the contra- ry. the present researchers expect toobserve some rather complex inter- actions among the sets of variables drawn from both the massmedia and delinquency literaturevariables which they conceive of as mediating the effect of television stimuli.

MODEL AND HYPOTHESES The preceding review of the literature suggests. then, that certain dis- tinctions should be made with respect both to the independent and the dependent variables and to :he relationship between them. With respect to the independent vaiiabletelevision violenceat least two elements are crucial: the objective character of television programming andthe respondent's subjective perception of the character of these programs. Thus, given a set of television shows, it is necessary first to inquire into the objective amount of violence (defined in some standardized manner) present in these shows. The subject's viewing habits must also be ascer- tained: how much time he spends watching television, which programs he watches, and which are his favorites. The favorite programs take on a special significance because these are likely to be the programs he watches attentively. Given some objective measure of the violent content of programs ac- tually watched, it must be determined, for the same set of programs. how the subjects perceive that violence. For example. do they think that it is a true reflection of the way people behave in the real world or that it is make-believe? Perhaps equally impottant, do the programs seem to the adolescent to present a picture of the way people ought to behave? Is violence seen as the means used to achieve goals: isit effective in achieving these goals, or is the user of violence punished for his behav- ior? The literature on mass com munications indicates that additionhl sets of variables have an impact upon the way in which an individual uses the media. For present purposes these variables can be broken down into two classes: (I) demographicbroad social categories such as race, age. and social class, which indicate anindividual's position in the social structure: (2) primary group relationshipssocial relationshipswith parents and peers in particular. Turning to the dependent variable, violence and/ot deviance, an es- sential distinction must be made between attitudes and beliefs about vio- lence and deviance, and deviant and violent behavior. In makingthis distinction, the investigators are explicitly raising and testing the notion, expressed by several authors, that the effect of television violence may 399 392 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS vary from the attitudinal to the behavioral realms. More specifically. they will inquire into the subject's commission of delinquentacts, using self-report techniques. In addition. data about the subjects' personal acceptance of various levels of violent behavior faced with varying lev- els of provocation will be examined, as wellas data about their beliefs about the level of violence in their schools and neighborhoods. Since the data do not permit a clear identification of the temporalse- quence. it is difficult to answer the question of whether it is televisionor the kind of child sent to television which causesany observed relation- ship between television violence and the child's behaviorand attitudes. One question which can be answered, ofcourse, is whether watching violent television programs is associated with increaseddeviance or with attitudes such as approval of violence. Additionally,however, by controlling for gender. race, socioeconomicstatus, age. and various tics to the social structureloosely labeled insulating factors, it becomes possible to see whether television violence is relatedto certain behavior and attitudes, whether it has an effect forsome but not all categories of children, or whether its effect varies according to these characteristics of the child. The major hypotheses of the present studyare: I. There is a relationship between television violence watchedand both deviant behavior and attitudes and beliefs about aggression. These attitudes and beliefs include personal and perceived social acceptability of violence and perception of the level of violencepresent in American societ y. 2. This relationship will be strengthened for those respondentswho subjectively perceive the violence in their favorite showsas an effective means to an end, who find it realistic, and in general who perceive vio- lence to be an accepted mode of behavior in such shows. Those who do not perceive these factors should be less affected by objective violence. 3. The relationship between television violence and deviantbehavior will be stronger for these categories of respondents whoare more vul- nerable to deviance, specifically: younger respondents, males, blacks, lower-class respondents, and those individuals who havefewer ties to the social structure.

THE SAMPLE The data for this and several related studies were collected during April 1970 in public junior and senior high schools in Prince Georges County. Maryland. bordering the District of Columbia.2 This county was selected because it includes areas which are quite rural. middle- class and blue collar suburbs, and some areas which approximate condi- tions in an inner city.

aof ViOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 393

The schools, five high and eight junior high schools, were selected in such a way as to provide a probability sample of students, with two re- strictions: roughly equal numbers were selected from each grade. sev- enth through twelfth, although lower grades were somewhat larger: and a predominantly black school was oversampled to provide sufficient black respondents to facilitate comparisons. The diversity of the sample schools can be noted from the proportion of respondents who were black and from varying socioeconomic levels (Table 1). Although the median percentage of black respondents was 4.4. in three of the schools more than 45 percent of the respondents were black. Three schools could be considered relatively affluent, with more than 40 percent of the respondents for whom this information was avail- able reporting father's occupation classified as high.3 In another three schools, 30 percent of the respondents were classified as low socioeco- nomic status. using father's occupation. Two of these schools are located near the District of Columbia; manyof their students had recently moved the short distance from the city. The third. in a rural area. has a student population with farm backgrounds as well as parents who work in the District of Columbia or at service jobs in the area.

Table 1: The sample

Senior high school students 1242 Junior high school students 1057 Total 2299 White male 964 Black male 146 White female 1011 Black female 168

ADOLESCENTS' VIEWING HABITS

One can estimate the potential influence of television byexamining the amount of television viewing. Respondents were asked: "On an average day. about how many hours do youpersonally spend watching television?" In spite of the attempt to ask the question in a manner cal- culated to focus attention on actual viewing time, there mayhave been a tendency to overstate. The impression gained, nonetheless,is one of many youngsters spending hoursin front of a television set (Table 2). Nearly 50 percent say they watch more than three hours a day. Someof these hours may represent time spent in the same room with a setwhich is turned on. rather than time spent paying close attention towhat is on the screen. If the tendency to overstate the case is constantfrom one race, age. or gender category to another,it is possible at least to esti- mate the relative amount of viewing forthese subsamples. The younger 401 Hours TV Male White Table 2: Hours television viewing by age, race, and gender Female Male Black Female Total 20 31 (11 43% 7% 14) (15 49%16% 19) (11 37% 5% 14) (15 48%14% 19) (11 13% 4% 14) (15 21% 2% 19) (11 14% 14) (15 3% 19) 10% (221) 3More than (320)51% 35%(578) 58%(367) (596)39% 83%(46) 77%(87) 87%(74) 24%(81)74% 48%41% (1038)(890) (2149) VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 395

adolescents spend more time viewing than the older adolescents, fe- males more than males, and black youngsters more than white. Adoles- cents from lower socioeconomic status homes watch television more hours per day than do those from higher status homes (Table 3). News programs. while not the focus of the present study. are an im- portant aspect of an individual's television habits. At least onewriter has suggested that newscasts may have greater influence on violence poten- tial than comparable dramatic programs (Greenwald. 1971). The young viewer may. in fact. be acquiring beliefs and attitudes about a variety of subjects in this fashion. Adolescents in the present study generally watch news programs quite frequently (Table 4). More than 50 percent say they watch news at least two or threetimes a week: 20 percent say every night. There is no clearcut patternof subsample differences, al- though males and blacks tend to watch news more frequently than fe- males and whites. Within the white subsample, older adolescents watched somewhat more frequently. but the converse was true for the black respondents. There was little difference between news watching of poor and of the more affluent youngsters (Table 5). Thus. the pattern of news watching is quite different from the pattern for overall viewing. This finding suggests the importance of measuring what is watched on television, not how much.

Table 3: Hours of television viewed daily by social class Social class Hours Lower Middle Upper 0 - 1 7% 10% 17% 2 3 3396 42% 47% More than 3 60% 48% 37% Total 100%(270) 100%(812) 100% (559)

A major question in the present study is the level of violence in the programs being watched. Respondents were asked to list theirfour fa- vorite programs. "the ones you try to watch every time they are on the air." They were then asked to select from that list their one most favor- ite show. Attention is focussed on the favorite programs because it seems liekly that they may be the most influential. Assuggested above, it is possible that some viewing time is simply time spent in the same room with a television set. A person might watch the screen rather cas- ually or might be more likely to watch a favorite show attentively from the beginning until it is finished. A violence rating was assigned to each favorite program. and a sum- mary average was computed for the four favorite programs. The ratings are taken from a survey of television critics and a sampleof adults in a large city (Greenberg and Gordon. 1970). The definition of violence used 403 trJsC Watchnews Male White Table 4: Frequency of watching news by Female age, race, and genderMale Black Female Total 2Every night 3 times !It 22% 14) (15 25% 19) (11 14% 14) (15 18% 19) (11 31% 14) (15 18% 19) (11 26% 14) (15 19% 19) 20% en: weeklyLessOnce than a week 21%35% 35%15% 20%30% 21%33% 25%15% 37%17% 25%16% 22%27% 33%19% 111A Neveronce a week (326)14% 7% (606)16% 9% 22%(375)14% (629)20% 9% (48)10%19% (93)20% 8% 25%(80) 8% 27%(83) 6% (2240)19% 9% VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 397 was as follows: "Byviolence I mean how much fighting, shooting, yell- ing, or killing there usually is in the show."The correlation between rat- ings by critics and by adult public was .86. A problem arises in the case ofshows which cannot be codedfor example,Saturday Night at the Movies.The violence level may vary from one episode to another, and there was norationale for including these shows in either the low, moderate, orhigh violence categories.

Table 5: Frequency of watching television news bysocial class Social class Upper Frequency Lower Middle 20% Every n igh t 18% 18% 34% 2 3 times weekly 33% 35% 21% Once weekly 21% 18% 18% Less than once weekly 20% 20% 7% Never 8% 10% 100% 1582) Total 100% (277) 100%(839)

About one-third of the respondents listed a programrated as "low vi- olence" as their favorite: 12 percent namedmoderate violence pro- grams: a quarter selectedhigh violence shows, and another quarter picked shows which could not be rated (Table 6).There were no great differences in the selections of subsamples,although females, younger adolescents, and upper-class respondents namedlow violence shows somewhat more frequently. Distributionof average violence ratings (of four favorite shows) is very similar. It might seem reasonable to assume that the moretime a child spends watching television, the greater is the likelihood thathe will be exposed to violent program content. The presentstudy does not include a mea- sure of a child's total exposure toviolent programs for any period of

Table 6: Violence ratings, favorite program, by race, gender, age, and social class Violence rating

Low Moderate High Not codeable Number

White male 27% 12% 28% 3396 1953) White female 42% 14% 22% 22% (997) Black male 30% 7% 31% 32% 1142) Black female 40% 3% 36% 21% 1164) 11 -- 14 years 39% 13% 22% 26% 1824) (1415) 15 19 years 33% 11% 28% 28% Lower-class 73% 11% 22% 34% 1273) Middle-class 36% 12% 27% 25% (823) Upper-class 39% 12% 25% 23% (566) Total 35% 12% 28% 27% 12286)

405 398 TEI.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS time. It can be noted, however, that the respondents who spend much of their time viewing television are neither more nor less likely to choose violent programs as their favorites (Table 7).

Table 7: Hours of television viewed daily and violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite Hours program 0 - 1 2 3 4 or more Low 49% (69) 46% (301) 50% (398) Moderate 19% (27) 18% (120) 14% (110) High 33% (46) 36% (233) 37% (295) Total 100% (142) 100% (654) 100% (803)

TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR Deviance measures In order to measure deviance, a self-report checklist of deviant behav- ior was compiled from the usual measures of delinquency found in the literature. In order to comply with the requirements of the school sys- tems, items concerning sexual behavior. drugs. and the more serious types of delinquency, such as stealing, were not included: For this study. five measures representing different types of deviance were used. The first type is aggressive or violent acts. It was measured by responses to the questions about how often the respondents: (1) got into a serious fight with a student at school; (2) got something by telling a person something bad would happen to him if they did not get what they wanted: (3) hurt someone badly enough for him to need bandages: (4) had taken part in a fight where a bunch of their friends were against an- other bunch. The second type of deviance is composed of petty delin- quent acts. Respondents were asked how often they: (1) went onto someone's land or into a house or building when they weren't supposed to: (2) damaged school property on purpose. The third type of deviance is defiance of parents and was measured by how often the respondents said they: (1) stayed out later than parents said: (2) ran away from home: (3) argued or had a fight with either of their parents: (4) drank beer or liquor without parent's permission. Political action by the young can be and often is defined as a form of deviance. Merton calls this deviant behavior nonconformity, as opposed to aberrant behavior: the individual knows but does not accept the norms and violates them openly in an effort to bring about change (Mer- ton. 1966). It was measured through responses to questions on how of- ten the students: (1) participated in a sit-in or demonstration at school: (2) participated in a sit-in or demonstration at places other than school: 406 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 399

(3) asked a school official to change anyregulations or courses. The last measure of deviance-involvementwith legal officials-is presumably the most serious and may be used as asubstitute for some of the serious delinquencies not listed. The students wereasked about their contacts with the police and howoften they had: (I) been stopped by the police; (2) been picked up and taken down to thepolice station; (3) been arrested; (4) been brought to the juvenile court;and (5) spent time in a juvenile detention facility. Such involvements maybe used as an indi- rect measure of serious delinquency.

Table 8: Aggressive deviance by race, gender, and social class

Aggressiveness White males White females Black males Black females Lower class

Low 32.4 52.0 25.0 58.8 Moderate 33.3 31.6 46.9 23.5 High 34.3 16.4 28.1 17.7 Total 100%(108) 100%(95) 100%(32) 100%(34) Middle class

Low 39.3 59.2 21.6 47.4 Moderate 32.2 31.1 43.2 34.2 High 28.5 9.7 35.2 18.4 Total 100%(351) 100%(402) 100%(37) 100%(38) Upper class

Low 45.9 67.2 a 45.0 Moderate 33.7 25.5 a 30.0 High 20.4 7.3 a 25.0 Total 100%(270) 100%(271) a 100%(20) aNumber too small for stable percentages (less than 20)

Items were included in the various indices on the basisof an a priori relationship between the behavior reported and the type ofdeviance being measured. Summary scores, based simply oncumulative inci- dence. were derived for the five types of deviance(scored one for "did it once" and two for "twice or more"). Based on the marginals.each type is divided into three logical subdivisions of low. moderate.and high de- viance for tabular presentation; original scores are used to compute cor- relations. In this section each type of deviance is examinedin turn in order to discover the relationship between devianceand television vio- lence. Distribution of deviant behavior In order to determin if the present data are comparablewith those of previous studies, the distribution of the five deviantbehavior measures 407 1E LEV IsioN ANDA was(' NI AGGR EssivFN Ess 400 are presented. controlling for race. gender. and social class as measured by father's occupation. Since race and social class are often related, the effects of race are presented controlling for social class. The results are presented in Tables 8 through 12. Inspection of Table 8 reveals that among lower-class respondents. gender and not race is the important .variable in explaining aggression. Boys are more aggressive than girls, and this holds true for both races. Among middle-class respondents, however, race and gender are both important. Again, boys are more aggressive than girls, but in this social class blacks are also more aggressive than whites. This same pattern holds again for the upper-class respondents. Being middle- or upper- class insulates whites from aggression but does not insulate blacks. The general conclusion is that as social class increases, aggression decreas- es. but only for whites. The opposite is true for blacks. Upper- andmid- dle-class blacks tend to be more aggressive than do lower-class blacks. In every social class, boys are much more aggressive than girls. A different pattern emerges with respect to petty delinquency (Table 9). There is a positive relationship between social class and this type of deviance. Upper-class respondents admit to more of these petty delin- quencies than do lower-class respondents. This type of deviance is the kind that erases social class differences in self-reported delinquency studies and shows the middle and upper classes to be more deviant than they appear to be in official statistics (Gibbons. 1970. pp. 20-31). As be- fore, there is more male than female deviance in all social classes. Race. however, presents a different and inconsistent picture. White males are more deviant than blacks except in the upper class, where theN is too small to make a comparison. Among females, however, it is the upper- and middle-class white girls who commit more petty delinquent acts than do their racial counterparts. In the lower social class, whites and blacks do not differ. A possible explanation for this mixed pattern is that petty delinquency is the domain of the privileged, their normal deviance, and thus is found among the advantagedthatjc, whites and upper-class respondents. Inaddition, compared with aggressive deviance, girls commit much more of this more minor type of deviance. The differences in rates between boys and girls and among the social classes narrow considerably for fighting with parents (Table 10). This deviance is the most prevalent of the five and appears to be neither gen- der- nor class-specific. There are racial variations, however, in all social classes and for both genders. Black respondents appear to fight much less with their parents than do whites. Table 11 presents the results for the distribution of political deviance. The gender differences .reported for aggression again disappear; boys and girls are almost equally involved in political action. One might pre- dict that the more advantaged social classes would be more politically sophisticated and thus engage in more political activity. This appears to 408 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 401

Table 9: Petty delinquency by race, gender, and social class

Amount of petty crimeWhite males White females Black males Black females

Lower class

Low 43.4 62.6 37.5 57.1 Moderate 15.1 22.2 40.6 25.7 High 41.5 15.1 21.8 17.1

Total 100% (106) 100% (99) 100%(32) 100% (35)

Middle class

Low 39.0 60.8 50.0 65.8 Moderate 14.7 17.2 10.5 21.1 High 46.3 22.0 39.5 13.1

Total 100% (354) 100% (401) 100%(38) 100% (38)

Upper class

Low 37.6 50.7 a 50.0 Moderate 18.1 20.8 a 40.0 High 44.2 28.4 a 10.0

Total 100% (271) 100% (274) a 100% (20)

a Number too small for stable percentages

Table 10: Fighting with parents by race, gender, and social class

Amount of fighting White males White females Black males Black females

Low 25.0 27.5 48.4 71.4 Moderate 31.5 30.6 25.8 14.3 High 43.5 41.9 25.8 14.3

Total 100% (100) 100% (98) 100% (31) 100% (35)

Middle class

Low 21.4 24.5 35.1 44.8 Moderate 26.9 30.1 37.8 39.5 27.0 15.8 o High 51.7 45.3

I Total 100% (350) 100% (399) 100% (37) 100% (38)

Low 24.7 27.0 a 21.1 Moderate 28.8 34.3 a 63.2 High 46.4 38.7 a 15.8

Total 100% (271) 100% (274) a' 100% (19)

a Number too small for stable percentages 409 402 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 11: Political deviance by race, gender, and social class

Amount of political de% iance White males White females Black males Black females

Lower class

Low 52.3 49.5 40.6 42.9 Moderate 15.9 24.2 9.4 17.1 High 31.8 26.3 49.8 40.1

Total 100% (107) 100% (99) 100%(32) 100% (35)

Middle class

Low 50.4 47.9 36.1 37.8 Moderate 21.4 24.8 25.0 27.0 High 28.3 27.4 38.9 35.1

Total 1.00% (351) 100% (399) 100%(36) 100% (37)

Upper class

Low 42.8 44.9 a 50.0 Moderate 23.6 23.5 a 10.0 High 33.6 31.6 a 40.0

Total 100% (271) 100% (272) a 100% (20)

a Numb Ar too small for stable percentages

be the case; the amount of "low" political activity decreases as social class increases. Blacks, perhaps because of the momentum of the Black Movement, have attempted political action more often than whites. This is not unexpected. and again it is the upper-status blacks who are the most active; they were also the most aggressive. For males there is an inverse relationship between social class and involvement with legal officials or serious deviance (Table 12). Thus, these data agree with most other studies of involvement with legal au- thorities. Boys greatly outnumber girls for this type of deviance, again in line with other research. What is not expected is that there are no racial variations. Social class is more important than race, and when social class is held constant, the differences between the races disappear. Most other studies, including those which use self-report data, however, show an overrepresentation of blacks among those youngsters involved with the police. A possible explanation for this study's discrepant finding is the sample. All of the respondents were students. It is possible that among school dropouts there is a higher rate of official involvement and that blacks may be overrepresented in the dropout population. Had a truly representative sample of all adolescents been taken, the racial dif- ferences would perhaps appear. 410

44. VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 403

Table 12: Serious deviance by race, gender, and social class

Amount of

serious 1 deviance White males White females Black males Black females

Lower class

Low 28.3 64.6 30.3 65.7 Moderate 54.7 30.3 51.5 34.3 High 17.0 5.0 18.2 o

Total 100% (106) 100% (99) 100% (33) 100% (35)

Middle class

Low 34.6 64.7 30.6 73.7 Moderate 47.6 30.0 52.8 23.7 High 17.8 5.1 16.7 2.6

Total 100% (353) 100% (400) 100% (36) 100% (38) Upper class

Low 44.9 68.2 a 70.7 Moderate 41.9 26.6 a 25.0 High 13.1 5.2 a 5.0

Total 100% (267) 100% (274) a 100% (20)

a Number too small for stable percentages To summarize the .distribution of the five dependent variables, one notes that in general boys ate Mote deviant than gitis, especially On the niore diitteftie type§ Of &Mande Stich as aggretsion Atid Serb:Ws deVi- ance. SiMilarly, blades and loWet-dass responderitS ate ovetrepteSented fot these same two types Of deviance. Thtis; genetally speaking; With the one exception of official involvement (which is apparently due to the lack of inclusion of school dropouts in the present sample), the deviance scores are distributed by gender, race, and class in a manner consistent with the literature on deviance. These findings give some measure of confidence in the reliability of the self-report measures of deviance used in this research. The effects of television The general hypothesis is that there will be a weak but oositive rela- tionship between the amount of violence viewed on television and de- viant behavior. However, this hypothesis is qualified in three respects. First, itis predicted that the relationship will be stronger for certain types of deviance. For example, it is predicted that the relationship be- tween television violence and aggressive deviance will be stronger than the relationship between television violence and nonaggressive or less violent types of deviance.

['IL". -.7. '..0...... ±-,-,-,Vo., --...-,..,-,A00/0MMpl.1 404 r:I.EVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS The second qualification concerns the effects of the respondent's per- ception of the content of television programming. It is argued that such perception is an important variable to be considered in examining the effects of television violence on behavior. The literature suggests that thc perception of the content of the media presentation interposes itself between the objective content and any behavioral response. For present purposes. then, itis relevant to examine not only the relationship be- tween the objective amount of violence viewed and deviance, but also the relationship between thc subject's perception of the media content and that same deviance. It is hypothesized that the relationship between television violence and deviant behavior will be increased for those re- spondents who perceive that violence is an effective means to an end and that the characters portray acceptable behavior. The third qualification refers to the effects of other variables known to have a relationship with deviant behavior. For example, there are gen- der differences in both the rate and the type of deviant behavior; males have higher rates in general and also are more likely to engage in the more aggressive types of deviance. It is predicted that the relationship between television violence and deviant behavior will be stronger for those respondents who are more vulnerable to deviance: males, blacks, lower-class or young respondents. and those respondents with fewer ties to the social structure. Thus the main interest is the examination of the possible joint effects of television violence and other variables on deviant behavior. Tables 13 through 17 present data on the objective violence rating of the respondent's favorite television program and his score on each type of deviant behavior. These tables indicate that there is a very small positive relationship between the objective violence rating of the re- spondent's favorite program and deviant behavior; correlation coeffi- cients are not significant except for serious deviance. They do, however, consistently show more deviance in the high violence condition than in the low violence condition. Thus the general hypothesis receives slight support. The first qualification to the general hypothesis is in part reject- ed since the relationship between television violence and deviance is not stronger for aggressive deviance but is stronger for serious deviance. When the average violence rating of the adolescent's four favorite shows is considered, however, the relationships with deviance are somewhat stronger (Tables 18 and 25). The general hypothesis is con- firmed then. using this latter measure of television violence viewed. The first qualification is also confirmed, as the relationships are stronger for average violence and for aggressive and serious deviance than for the other deviance measures. It will be remembered that respondents were asked to list as their fa- vorite shows those which they "tried to watch every time they are on the air," and then to select their most favorite from that list. It is, of 412 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 405

Table 13:Aggressive deviance by vjolence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate H igh

Low 49.9 48.6 48.2 Moderate 31.6 34.7 30.9 High 18.5 16.7 21.0

Total 100% (776) 100% (259) 100% (573)

r = .038 (n.s.)

Table 14: Petty delinquency by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Amount of petty delinquency Low Moderate High

Low 49.1 51.3 46.3 Moderate 19.7 18.0 17.6 High 31.2 30.7 36.1

Total 100% (782) 100% (261) 100% (579)

r = .043 (n.s.)

Table 15:Fighting with parents by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Amount of fighting Low Moderate High

Low 28.4 27.7 28.4 Moderate 30.6 25.8 30.2 High 41.1 46.5 41.4

Total 100% (772) 100% (256) 100% (570)

r = .017 (s)

Table 16: Political deviance by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Amount of political deviance Low Moderate High

Low 48.4 46.9 46.1 Moderate 22.9 23.3 22.8 High 28.7 29.8 31.1

Total 100% (783) 100% (258) 100% (571)

r (n.s.) 413 406 TELEVISION .AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 17: Serious deviance by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High

Low 56.9 50.8 46.5 Moderate 34.5 38.0 43.9 High 8.5 11.3 9.6

Total 100% (780) 100% (258) 100% (574)

r = .058 p < .01 course, not feasible to obtain a violence rating of all programs watched by the subjects of this study. Their four favorite programs, however, will represent a substantial proportion of their total viewing in addition to being an indicator of preference per se. In comparing results using the two measures, it should also be remembered that no violence rating was available for a quarter of the respondents' most favorite shows. The av- erage violence rating, on the other hand, could be computed for nearly all respondents. (If one of the four could not be coded, average was computed for the remaining three.)

Table 18: Aggressive and serious deviance by average violence rating of four favorite shows

Amount of Violence rating aggressive deviance Low Moderate High

Low 53.8 48.0 40.4 Moderate 31.7 31.5 34.2 High 14.4 20.4 25.5

Total 100% (637) 100% (1104) 100% (339)

r = .109

Amount of serious deviance

Low 63.1 49.1 42.4 Moderate 30.9 40.9 41.5 High 5.9 10.0 16.1

Total 100% (640) 100% (1109) 100% (335)

r = .158 4141 " p < .001

41.4 VIOI.ENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 407

Since the objective violence rating of the respondent's mostfavorite television show does not bear any strong relationship to the amount of deviant behavior, it is important to examine the possibility that a strong- er relationship may exist betweenthe subject's perception of the vio- lence content of his favorite show and his deviant behavior. Thefollow- ing were included in a list of statements and the respondent asked to se- lect those which describe his favorite show: (I) the main characterpush- es the others around: (2) the guywho gets rough gets his way. Selection of the first statement is taken as evidence that the respondent perceives violence to be used: selection of the second, that it is rewarded. Table 19 presents the data for these relationships betweensubjective perception of television violence and deviant behavior. Only the moreviolent and serious deviances are presented. (It should be noted that these tables use the entire sample of respondents who rated their favorite shows and are not limited to those respondents for whose favoriteshows there was an

Table 19: Perception of television violence

Aggressive deviance by perception of television violence

Violence used Violence not Violence Violence Aggressiveness as means used as means rewarded not rewarded

Low 31.5 49.6 28.3 50.3 Moderate 27.0 32.1 27.0 32.1 High 41.4 18.2 44.7 17.6

Total 100% (111) 100% (2081) 100% (159) 100% (2034)

Petty delinquency by perception of television Wolence

Amount of Violence used Violence not Violence Violence petty crime as means used as means rewarded not rewarded

Low 36.8 49.9 35.0 50.4 Moderate 17.5 17.9 20.0 17.7 High 45.6 32.2 45.0 32.0

Total 100% (114) 100% (2096) 100% (160) 100% (2051)

Serious deviance by perception of television violence

Amount of serious Violence used Violence not Violence Violence deviance as means used as means rewarded not rewarded

Low 43.2 52.1 37.2 52.9 Moderate 36.0 38.1 44.9 37.4 High 20.7 9.8 17.9 9.7

Total 100% (111) 100% (2085) 100% (156) 100% (2041) 415

.11-. 408 1 ELINISIoN AND:mm..0(1:N] A( RESSIVENESS objective rating.) Those subjects who perceive violent means as being used on their favorite show are somewhat more likely to engage in de- viant behavior; those who perceive the use of violence as being reward- ed are similarly somewhat more likely to engage in deviance. In addition to the violence used and rewarded. other subjective per- ceptions expected to increase the likelihood of deviance included per- ceived realism and normative content. Perceived realism was indicated by the item "it shows life as it really is" and normative content by "it shows the way people ought to act." A combined index was construct- ed, scoring one for each of these items indicating perceived violence, realism, and normative content. The relationship between this index (la- beled "perceived violence and realism") and deviant behavior is gener- ally greater than that between the objective violence rating of the favor- ite program and deviance (Table 27). It should be remembered that only a very small minority perceive their favorite show asviolent. however. It is not known how often violence is subjectively perceived on the four favorite shows, so the comparison between objective and subjective violence can be made for this one show only. Summarizing the findings to this point, then, the data indicate a small but generally consistent relationship between the objective violence rat- ing of television shows and deviant behavior. As expected, a stronger relationship does exist between the subject's perception of the violence content of his favorite television show and the amount of deviant behav- ior, and this relationship is strongest with respect to aggressive devi- ance. It appears, then, that the subject's perception of violence is more closely related to deviant behavior than is the objective rating of the violence content of television shows. This comparison is made for vio- lence rating and perception of the respondent's one favorite show only, as it is for this program that both kinds of information areavailable. The next point to be considered concerns the possible joint effect of objective television violence and the subject's perception of that vio- lence on deviant behavior. It would be expected that the amount of devi- ance, particularly the more aggressive type, would be increased with the joint occurrence of a high objective violence rating and the subject's perception of the presence of such violence. Examination of Table 20 reveals that this is not the case. There is no consistent pattern of relationship between the objective violence rating of favorite program and deviant behavior, even among those respon- dents who perceive violence used and rewarded on their favorite shows. With respect to petty delinquency, it does appear that if the respondent perceives violence as not rewarded, and if his show is objectively vio- lent, then there is a tendency toward higher deviance. It can be noted in Table 25, however, that controlling for perceived violence and realism does not change the relationships between favorite program violence and deviance. 416

t., VIOI.ENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 409 Table 20: Objective and subjective measures of television violence

Aggressive deviance by violence, favorite program, and perception of favorite program

Violence used as means Violence not used as means Aggressive deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 38.2 a 24.2 50.4 49.2 49.4 Moderate 17.6 a 45.5 32.3 35.3 30.0 High 44.2 a 30.3 17.3 15.5 20.5

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 134/ a (33) 17421 12521 1536/

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded

Low 36.8 17.6 27.8 50.6 51.0 50.1 Moderate 18.4 29.4 29.6 32.2 34.9 31.1 High 44.8 52.9 42.6 17.1 14.2 18.8

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (30) 1171 (54) 17351 12411 15151

Petty delinquency by violence, favorite program, and perception of favorite program

Violence used as means Violence not used as means Petty delinquency Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 40.0 a 29.4 49.5 52.0 47.1 Moderate 11.4 a 17.6 20.1 17.7 17.6 High 48.5 a 52.9 30.3 30.3 31.2

Total 100% a 100% 100% 100% 100% 1351 134/ (745) (254) 15411

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded

Low 37.8 41.2 33.9 49.7 51.9 47.4 Moderate 21.6 11.8 17.9 19.5 18.5 17.5 High 40.5 47.0 48.3 30.8 29.6 35.1

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (37) (17) 1561 (742) 12431 (519)

Serious deviance by violence, favorite program, and pereption of favorite program

Amount of Violence used as means Violence not used as means serious deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 35.3 a 44.1 57.9 50.8 46.8 Moderate 47.1 a 32.4 33.9 38.1 44.4 High 17.6 a 23.4 8.2 11.1 8.7

Total 100% a 100% 100% 100% 100% 134/ 134/ 17441 12521 1536/

41.7 410 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 20 (Cont.)

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded

Low 27.8 a 29;6 58.4 51.9 48.4 Moderate 50.0 a 55.6 33.6 37.8 42.4 High 22.2 a 14.9 7.9 10.3 9.2

Total 100% a 100% 100%. 100% 100% (36) (54) (741) (241) (516)

The issue was previously raised that the joint occurrence of violent television and other variables related to deviant behavior could increase deviance rates. An examination of the relationship between the objec- tive violence rating and aggressive deviance, while controlling forthese other variables, reveals that this is not correct (Tables 21 to 23). Neither gender. race, social class, nor age strengthens the originalrelationship between favorite program violence and deviance. The differences be- tween the deviance rates of those who watch the low andthose who watch the high violence shows are still quite small; in some tablesthere are reversals and in general noconsistent pattern emerges. One tentative generalization is that the relationship is strongest for lower-class respon- dents.

Table 21: Aggressive deviance by violence, favorite program, gender, race, social class, and age

Mite males Black males Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 43.3 34.2 38.0 23.8 a 30.0 Moderate 29.9 36.0 31,8 35.7 a 32.5 High 26.8 29.7 30.2 40.5 a 37.5

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (254) (111) (255) (42) a (40)

White females Black females Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 56.9 62.9 63.0 46.0 a 50.8 Moderate 31.3 32.6 29.6 38.1 a 28.8 High 11.8 4.5 7.5 15.9 a 20.4

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (415) (132) (216) (63) (59) VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 411

Table 21 (Cont.)

Young (11-14) Older 115 +I Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 44.9 44.6 40.6 53.4 51.3 52.3 Moderate 34.3 45.5 34.9 29.6 27.9 28.8 High 20.0 9.9 24.5 1 7.0 20.7 18.9

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (312) (101) (175) (09) (154) (392)

Lower class Middle class Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 40.4 43.7 47.5 50.3 44.6 48.0 Moderate 34.8 40.6 25.4 30.8 36.6 31.2 High 24.7 15.5 27.2 18 8 18.9 20.8

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (89) (32) (59) (292) (101) (221)

Upper class Violence, favorite program

Aggressiveness Low Moderate High

1 Low 56.6 61.4 55.6

1 Moderate 29.4 31.4 28.5 High 14.0 7.2 16.0

Total 100% 100% 100% (221) (70) (144)

a Number too small for stable percentages

It rimy be. however, that race, gender, and age are not the crucial char- acteristics of the child for understanding the effects of television. There is some reason to expect that the strength of the child's ties to the social structure may influence the manner in which he uses television. Family troubles, insecurity, and unsatisfactory social relationships, for exam- ple. have been found characteristic of "heavy viewers" (Himmelweit

et at.. 1958; Schram m et al.. 196 n.Empey has suggested that the absence of strong personal relationships would make the child more dependent on the images portrayed by television and movies (1967, p. 40). M acco- by's investigation supports that hypothesis (1954). Another argument. concerning deviant behavior rather than televi- sion, is that strong ties to the social structure will inhibit deviant behav- ior (Briar and Piliavin, 1965). This argument includes personal relation- ships but also other kinds of ties to the social structure. High occupa-

416 412 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS Table 22: Petty delinquency by violence, favorite program, gender, race, social class, and age

White males Black males Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amt. of petty delinquency Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 40.8 45.9 31.4 35.7 a 53.7 Moderate 18.0 9.0 18.6 31.0 a 14.6 High 41.2 45.0 50.1 33.3 a 31.7

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (255) (111) (258) (42) (41)

White females Black females Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amt. of petty delinquency Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 53.7 54.5 59.2 58.7 a 57.6 Moderate 19.3 24.6 16.5 22.2 a 20.3 High 27.0 20.8 24.3 19.1 a 22.0

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (419) (134) (218) (63) a (59)

Young (11-14) Older (15 +) Violence, favorite program Violence favorite program

Amt. of petty delinquency Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 47.6 50.5 46.3 49.8 50.6 46.2 Moderate 20,2 24.3 20.3 19.6 14.3 16.7 High 32.2 25.3 33.3 30.7 35.0 37.1

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (317) (103) (177) (460) (154) (396)

Lower class Middle class Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amt. of petty delinquency Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 52.2 51.6 48.3 50.9 52.5 49.8 Moderate 25.6 19.4 19.0 18.8 12.9 16.1 High 22.0 29.1 32.8 30.4 34.6 34.1

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (90) (31) (58) (293) (101) (223)

420 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 413

Table 22 (Cont.)

Upper class Violence, favorite program

Amount of petty delinquency Low Moderate High

Low 46.6 40.3 39.3 Moderate 18.1 27.8 22.8 High 35.3 32.0 37.9

Total 100% 100% 100% (221) (72) (145)

a Number too small for stable percentages p (.01 tional aspirations and expectation of apprehension and punishment for wrongdoing are two such inhibiting factors. The individual with high occupational aspirations would have more to lose by getting into trouble than would the child who has no such aspirations. The child who be- Table 23: Serious deviance by violence, favorite program, race, gender, social class, and age

White males Black males Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite provam

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 45.8 25.7 30.7 35.7 a 36.6 Moderate 41.9 53.2 52.9 50.0 a 56.1 High 12.3 21.1 16.4 14.3 a 7.3

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (253) (109) (257) (42) (41)

r = .078 (n.s.) r .055 (n.s.)

White females Black females Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 65.6 72.9 62.3 59.7' a 64.4 Moderate 28.2 24.8 34.4 33.9 a 32.2 High 6.2 2.3 3.2 6.5 a 3.4

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% 1 (419) (133) (215) (62) (39)

r = .032 (n.s.) r = .022 (n.s.) 421 414 TELEVISION AND ADOLESaNT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 23 (Cont.)

Young (11-14) Older (15 -1-) Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 66.6 70.6 54.6 50.8 38.2 43.4 Moderate 29.0 22.5 37.4 38.1 47.4 46.7 High 4.4 6.9 8.0 11.2 14.5 9.9

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (317) (102) (174) (457) (152) (394)

r=.110" r = .013 (n.s.)

Lower class Middle class Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 53.3 41.9 40.0 53.4 49.5 47.1 Moderate 40.0 38.7 46.7 37.3 35.6 43.9 High 6.6 19.3 13.4 9.3 14.8 9.1

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (90) (31) (60) (292) (101) (221)

r=.112 (n.s.) r.022 (n.s.)

Upper class Violence, favorite program

Amount of serious deviance Low Moderate High

Low 62.6 62.9 49.7 Moderate 29.3 32.9 41.3 High 8.1 4.3 9.1

Total 100% 100% 100% (222) (70) (143)

r = .063 (n.s.)

a Number too small for stable percentages p ( .001

lieves that wrongdoers are likely to be punished has more reason to ab- stain from misbehavior than the child who thinks such punishment is unlikely. 422

.10. -16

VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 415 The general question being askcdhere is whether such ties to the so- cial structure mediate betweentelevision violence and aggressive devi- ance. It is predicted thatweaker and fewer insulating ties to thesocial structure will strengthen therelationship between television violence and deviance. The insulating factors are asfollows:1. Educational expectations (Do you expect to finish high school, go tocollege, finish college?) 2. Occupational aspirations (What kind of jobwould youlike to havewhen you completely finishschool?) 3. Occupational expectations(What kind of job do you think youactually will getwhen you completely finish school?) 4. Participation in schoolactivities. 5. Relationship with peers (How often in the past have youfound yourself with someplace to go but no friends to go with?) 6. Fearof punishment (Suppose that a person your age beats upanother kind at school and hurts him badlyenough so a doctor is called.Do you think he would a) be arrested;b) be picked up by police andtaken home; c) be lectured by the police;d) get a talk- ing to by a principal or teacher; e) getbeaten up by the other kid's friends; f) get in trouble with his parents:g) get suspended from school; h) nothing would happen. If a person your agegets into trouble with the police, what effect would thishave on his life when he is older? i) no effect at all; j) no differencein the long run: k) might affect the kind of job he could get; 1)might make his whole life harder. A summary score, expectationof punishment, was assigned based onthe number of positive responses to a - g. k, and 1.The combined index, in- sulating factors. is described in Table 1, AppendixA. As can be noted in Table 24. respondentswith few or weak insulating ties to the social structure do hv ehigher aggressive deviance scores (see percentages in "total" column).There is not a consistent patternof stronger relationships betweenfavorite program violence and aggres- sion, however. As can be noted in Table25, the relationships between either measure of television violence and eachof the deviance measures is not changed appreciablywheninsulating factors are controlled. Itis not possible to conclude,based on the present evidence, thatweakness of these ties to the social structureleaves the child more vulnerable to the effects of television violence. To summarize this section, it can bepointed out that the objective violence rating of the most favorite program is notconsistently related to deviance. The percentagedifferences tend to go in the predicted direction. but they are very small. Thisrelationship is not strengthened when characteristics of the child, insulatingties to the social structure, or perception ofviolence on favorite program are controlled.There is, however, a small but consistent relationshipbetween the average vio- lence rating of four favorite programs andthe deviance scores. The rela- tionship remains significant when age, socialclass, and insulating ties to the social structure are contolled; therelationship is strongest for ag- gressive and serious deviance. .423

..*Ustioftsre-Vistetirga/Mmiwk .h.are .00.1. ..yoren. nz 4 1 6 VIOI.ENCE.AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR Table 24: Aggressive deviance by violence, favorite program, and insulating factors

(Percent with high aggressive scores)

Vic fence Low Moderate High Total

Educational expectations Low 25% 21% 23% 25% (623) Moderate 27% 29% 30% 26% (196) High 14% 12% 19% 16% (1369)

Occupational expectations Low 29% a 36% 38% (94) Moderate 18% 12% 20% 19% (736) High 15% 8% 14% 14% (560)

Occupational aspirations Low 32% a 36% 37% (96) Moderate 19% 13% 22% 20% (650) High 17% 11% 19% 16% (766)

School activities Low 20% 20% 23% 21% (820) Moderate 18% 16% 20% 20% (1120) High 8% 14% 12% 10% (164)

Fear of punishment Low 22% 27% 20% 21% (391) Moderate 18% 16% 21% 20% (1172) High 17% 14% 21% 18% (617)

I ntegrated with peers Poor relationshio 20% 12% 20% 20% (1403) Good relationship 17% 24% 22% 19% (804)

Combined index 1 (low) 37% a 14% 28% (87) 2 23% 21% 24% 24% (1060) 3 14% 13% 19% 15% (954) 4 (high) 51% a 23% 8% (88)

a Number too small for stable percentage

TELEVISION VIOLENCE AND APPROVAL OF VIOLENCE

The finding that those adolescents who watch violent programs exhib- it only slightly more aggressive behavior does not mean that they are not learning about aggression and violence. They may learn about patterns 424 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 417 of behavior which then.are available for use when an appropriate occa- sion arises. What could have more important long-term consequences. both for the individual and for the society. then. may be the attitudes formed about violence. If the adolescent views violent program content. begins to believe that violence is a usual rather than an extraordinary means of achieving goals and that many peopleapparently approve of such means, then he too may be more likely to approve of such behav- ior. If he has not yet acted out this behavior, he may more readily do so at some later date. Moreover, he may be more tolerant ofviolence on

Table 25: Summary of relationships between television violence and deviance

x = Violence, x = Average x = Perceived favorite violence, four violence and program favorites realisrn rxy rx y.a rxyrxy.a rxy y = aggressive deviance .038 (n.s.) .109 " .152** a = age n.s. .106" a = ses n.s. .108" a = insulating factors n.s. .096" a = perceived violence and realism n.s.

upetty delinquency .043 (n.s.) .083 *" .053** a = age n.s. .045* a = SES n.s. .063* a = insulating factors n.s. .079" a = perceived violence and realism n.s. y = fighting with parents .017 (n.s.) .089 -.041 (n.s.) a = age n.s. .060 a = SES n.s. .089" a = insulating factors n.s. .079" a = perceived violence and realism n.s. y = political deviance .029 (n.s.) .063 I* .064* * a = age n.s. .045* a = SES n.s. .063 a = insulating factors n.s. .060 a = perceived violence and realism n.s. y = serious deviance .058 .158 " .04S* a = age n.s. .144" a = SES n.s. .157" a = insulating factors n.s. .142" a = perceived violence and realism n.s.

p (.01 " 4001 425

s. 418 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS the part of others and less ready to interfere. come to the rescue of a vic- tim. or be concerned about the fate of others. On a societal level, an entire population may become indifferent to large-scale violence or may approve the use of indiscriminate mass violence. Whatever might be the consequences of ever larger numbers of persons approving of violent behavior, if such an increase were to occur. or if it is occurring, the for- mation of such attitudes as a possible consequence of media program content is in itself worthy of consideration. In this section the hypothe- sis that viewing violent programs leads to the approval of violent behav- ior is tested. The measures of television violence again arc the objective violence rating of the adolescent's favorite program and the ave,.age violence rat- ing of his four favorite programs. To reiterate. although there are other potential indicators of the independent variable, the violence score of the youngster's favorite program may be of special significance. The youngster is more likely to watch his favorite shows attentively from beginning to end than to let his attention shift to other things from time to time. If this is so, he will be more aware of the characters' actions and their consequences as well as the general context in which violence oc- curs. Assuming that approval of violence depends at least in part on (1) the characteristics of the assailant and the victim. (2) the level of provoca- tion. or the context in which the behavior occurs. and (3) the level of violence under consideration. these dimensions are included in the mea- sures of approval. The assailant-victim pairs include man and adult male stranger. teenage boy and teenage boy. and policeman and adult male citizen. For each pair the respondent was asked first whether he would approve a lesser degree of violence under various circumstances and then a more serious violent act under similar sets of circumstances.

Approval of adult violence

Approval of adult male-aduli male violence was measured by the fol- lowing items (scored I for each "yes" response): Would you approve of a man punching an adult male stranger if the stranger: (a) was in a protest march showing opposition to the other man's views; (b) was drunk and bumped into the man and his wife on the street; (c) was beat- ing up a woman and the man saw it:(d) had broken into the man's house; (e) had knocked the man down and was trying to rob him? Would you approve of a man shooting a stranger if the stranger: (a) was in a protest march showing opposition to the other man's views: (b) was drunk and bumped into the man and his wife on the street; (c) was beat- ing up a woman and the other man saw it; (d) had broken into the man's house; (e) had knocked the man down and was trying to rob him? 426 J VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 419 There is a small but positive relationshipbetween the violence rating of the respondent's favorite program andapproval of violence (Table 26). The respondent's subjective perceptionsof his favorite program are expected to influence the likelihood ofviolence approval, just as it has influenced the likelihood of aggressive behavior.It will Ile recalled that a number of phrases were listed and the respondentasked to check those which described his favorite program. Theresults can be summarized as follows: When violence is viewed as havingbeen a means to a goal (main character "pushes the others arownd"),and when it is rewarded (the "guy who gets rough gets his way"),approval is more frequent than if these features are not checked asdescriptive of the program (Table 27). Whether the characters were viewed asbehaving the way people ought to act has no effect on the frequency ofapproval.

Table 26: Approval, adult violence by violence, favorite program

Approval, Violence, favorite program adult violence Low Moderate High

Low 13.2 12.1 10.4 Moderate 38.9 44.5 37.6 High 47.9 43.5 52.1

Total 100%17861 100%12651 100%15881 r = .071 *4' 13(.001 The relationship between the perceived violenceand realism index and approval of violence is statisticallysignificant for two of the three indices of approval (Table 36). The relationship betweenfavorite pro- gram violence and approval ofviolence is not appreciably changed when perceived violence is controlled.

Approval of teen violence The second measure of approval of violence includeditems concern- ing teenage male victim and assailant. The items are asfollows: "Would you approve of ksidy, a teenageboy, punching Bill, another teenage boy, if: Andy didn't like Bill; Bill had made fun of Andy andpicked on him; Bill had challenged Andy to a fist fight, Bill had hit Andy;Bill had at- tacked Andy with a knife?" "Would you approveof Andy, a teenage boy, knifing Bill, another boy, if: Andy didn't likeBill; Bill had made fun of Andy and picked on him; Bill had challengedAndy to a fist fight; Bill had hit Andy, Bill had attacked Andy with a knife?" There is a relationship between the objective level offavorite program violence and approval of teen-teen violence (Table 28), butagain the re- 9 lationship is stronger when the average violencerating is used (Table 427 420 TELEVISION AND ADOLF.SCENT AGGRESSIVEN.ESS

Table 27: Approval, adult violence, by violence, favorite program, and perception of favorite program

Violence, . Violence used as means Violence not used as means favorite program (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low . 54% (35)11 47% (749) Moderate a a 44% (259) High 72% (34) 52% (550)

Total c 54% (116) 48% (2123)

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low 53% (38) 47% (745) Moderate 65% (17) 42% (247) High 67% (57) 50% (527)

Total c 63% (165) 47% (2075)

The way people ought Not the way people ought to behave to behave (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low , 51% (217) 47% (569) Moderate 39% (66) 45% (198)

High . 51% (80) 52% (503)

Total c 48% (475) 48% (1767)

a Number too small for stable percentage b Number in parentheses is number on which percentage is based c Total sample, including respondents for whom there is no violence rating for favorite program

36). This relationship between average violence and appreval of adult violence remains when age, socioeconomic status, and ties to the social structure are controlled. Those adolescents whose favorite programs are more violent more frequently approve of a teenage boy punching or knifing another teenage boy. If the favorite progi am is described as de- picting violence as a means to an end or violence rewarded, teen vio-

Table 28:Approval, teen violence by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Approval, teen violence Low Moderate H igh

Low 18.0 23.0 17.5 Moderate 47.5 42.6 44.3 High 34.5 I 34.4 38.2

Total 100%(787) 100% (265) 100% (589)

r = .021 (n.s.)

. 428

4".ft .?t,.."..t:Otigilie.P.i~inblittkAft.a_ MP*, IL.. ta_ae, ,,..wir=tarArar -fu VIOLENCF. AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 42 I lence is approved more often than if the programis not so described (Table 29). Whether or not the program "showsth6 way people ought to act" does not influence frequency ofapproval. When perceived violence and realism are used as controls, theeffect of objective vio- lence is not appreciably changed (Table 36).

Table 29: Approval, teen violence, by violence, favorite program, and perception of favorite program

Violence, Violence used as means Violence not used as means favorite program I% with high epprovall (% with high approval)

Low 44% (34)b 34% (751) Moderate a a 34% (259) High 56% (34) 37% (551)

Total 49% (114) 35% (2122)

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low 38% (37) 34% (747) Moderate a a 33% (247) High 53% (57) 37% (528)

Total c 51% (162) 35% (2075)

The way people ought Not the way people ought to behave to behave (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low 38% (219) 33% (568) 1 Moderate 35% (66) 34% (198) High 35% (81) 39% (503)

Total c 35% (476) 36% (1763)

a Number too small for stable percentages b Number in parentheses is number on which percentage is based c Total sample, including respondents for whom there is no violence rating for favorite program Approval of pOlice violence The approval of violent behavior, or the use of force,by a policeman has a somewhat different significance than approvalof the behavior of a private citizen. The policeman is a represehtative of a lawenforcement agency and therefore of societyitself, and there is a general understand- ing that he may on occasion be required to use force either todefend himself or to carry out his responsibilities. It is to be expectedthat ado- lescents, as well as adult citizens, will approve the use of force on the part of a policeman at least as often as theywould approve similar be- havior on the part of a. private citizen. It is not possible tomake clear comparisons between the approval of violence on the part ofpolicemen, 429 422 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

adult males, or teenage males, because the number and content of the items which make up the indices are not identical. There does not, how- ever, appear to be a great disparity between the number of approval re- sponses for roughly comparable items in the three indices. (Tables of responses to individual items omitted.)

Table 30: Approval, police violence, by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Approval police violence Low Moderate High

Low 56.5 58.8 58.0 Moderate 35.7 34.1 31.1 High 7.8 7.1 10.9

Total 100% (791) 100% (267) 100% (588)

r = .013 (n.s.)

The items included in the approval of police violence index are as fol- lows: "Would you approve of a policeman striking an adult male citizen if the citizen: had said vulgar and dirty things to the policeman; was demonstrating against the war in Vietnam; was being questioned as a suspect in a murder case; was attempting to escape from custody; was attacking the policeman with his fists; was threatening the policeman with a weapon?" "Would you approve of a policeman shooting an adult male citizen if the citizen: had said vulgar and dirty things.to the police- man; was demonstrating against the war in Vietnam; was being ques- tioned as a suspect in a murder case; was attempting to escape from cus- tody; was attacking the policeman with his fists; was threatening the po- liceman with a gun?" There is not a significant relationship between violence rating of fa- vorite program and approval of police violence (Table 30). Again, how- ever, the relationship between the average violence rating of four favor- ite programs and approval is positive although small (Table 36). The re- spondents who have indicated that in their favorite programs violence has been used as a means to an end are more likely to be high approvers of police violence (18 percent compared to nine percent high approvers) (Table 31). The perception of violence having been rewarded is also re- lated to approval, but normative content is not. The index of perceived violence and realism of favorite,program was not significantly related.to approval, however, nor did it strengthen the relationship between the objective rating of the favorite program and approval (Table 36). Considering race and gender, the positive relationship appears con- sistently only for White males (Tables 32, 33, and 34). This finding.does not indicate that race and gender, per se, are the important determinants 430 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 423

Table 31: Approval, police violence, by violence, favorite program, and perception of favorite program

Violence, Violence used as means Violence not used as means favorite program (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low 14.3 135113 74 (754) Moderate a a 7.3 (260) High 23.5 (34) 10.2 (550)

Total c 17.7% (119) 8.8% (2131)

Violence rewarded Violence not rewarded (% with high approval) (% with high approval)

Low 10.5 (38) 7,6 (750) Moderate 11.1 (18) 6.9 (248) H igh 19.6 (56) 10.1 (528)

Total c .16.3% (166) 8.8%(2085)

The way people ought Not the way people to behave ought to behave (% with high approval) 1% with high jpprovall

Low 9.6 (220) 7.2 (571) Moderate 12.1 (66) 5.5 (200) High 9.8 (82) 11.2 (501) Total 10.1% (483) 9.2% 117701

a Number too small for stable percentages b Number in parenthesis is nunther on which percentage is based c Total sample, including respondents for whom there is no violence rating for favorite programs of the level of approval of violence, however. Thereis no consistent re- lationship between approval and race/gender across thethree measures of approval. White males consistently approve moreviolence than white females, but no other pattern emerges. White males approve moreadult violence but less teenage violence than do black males,'for example (Table 2, Appendix A). It seems more reasonable to conclude atthis point that rar:.e and gender are not major determinantsof approval of violence nor of the manner in which television influences approval. Age, however, is negatively related to approval of teenviolence (r = .094, p < .001). The relationship between average violence and approv- al remains but is not greatly strengthened when age iscontrolled (Table 36). Social class is negatively associated with approval of teen violence (r = .114, p <.001), and with approval of adult violence (r =.082, p<.001), with lower-class respondents approving more violence than upper-class respondents; when the assailant-victim pair is policeman- adult male, there is no such relationship. The relationships between violence ratings of favorite programs and approval do not change appre- ciably when social class is controlled (Table 36). 431 424 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 32: Approval, adult violence, by violence, favorite program, race, and gender

Approval, White males White females adult violence Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 10.8 8.0 4.9 14.9 15.2 15.1 Moderate 34.0 38.1 27.5 43.4 48.6 50.5 High 55.2 54.0 67.5 41.7 36.2 34.4

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (259) (113) (265) (417) (138) (218)

Black males Black females

Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 16.3 a 13.6 11.1 a 13.8 Moderate 30.2 a 40.9 33.3 a 32.8 High 53.5 a 45.5 55.5 a 53.4

Total 100% a 100% 100% a 100% (43) a (44) (63) (58)

a Number too small for stable percentages

Table 33: Approval, teen violence, by violence, favorite program, race, and gender

Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program Approval, White males White females teen violence Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 12.7 17.7 10.9 22.1 27.5 26.6 Moderate 42.9 34.5 41.1 52.4 50.0 50.0 High 44.4 47.8 47.9 25.5 22.4 23.4

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% 100% (259) (113) (265) (416) (138) (218)

Violence favorite program Violence, favorite program black males black females

Low Moderate High Low . Moderate. High

Low 7.0 a 9.1 20.0 a 20.3 Moderate 34.9 a 36.4 43.1 a 45.8 High 58.1 a 54.5 36.9 a 33.9

Total 100% a 100% 100% a 100% (43) (44) (65) (59)

a Number too small for stable percentage's

Some of the variables which have been termed insulating ties to the social structure may be of greater consequence than the demographic variables just considered. Occupational aspirations and expectations of .the adolescent are both such ties to the social structure. The child who 432 VIOLENCii AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 425

aspires to, or expects to achieve, a high rather than a low status occupa- tion has stronger ties to the society. It was expected that television vio- lence would have more influence on the respondents with lower aspira- tions and expectations. For similar reasons, educational expectations, grades in school, integration into school activities, and relationships with parents and peers are expected to serve as insulating factors.

Table 34: Approval, police violence, by violence, favorite program, race andgender

White male Black male Approval, Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program police violence Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 41.1 49.1 45.4 62.8 a 68.2 Moderate 44.2 38.6 36.0 34.9 a 25.0 High 14.7 12.3 18.6 2.3 a 6.8

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (260) (114) (264) (43)

White female Black female Violence, favorite program Violence, favorite program

Low Moderate High Low Moderate High

Low 62.3 65.2 66.5 73.9 a 74.6 Moderate 32.7 31.2 29.8 23.1 a 18.6 High 5.0 3.6 3.7 3.1 a 6.8

Total 100% 100% 100% 100% a 100% (419) (138) (218) (65) (59)

a Numbar too small for stable percentages

Another measure of the strength of the child's ties to the society is his expectation that an offender will be apprehended and punished. These ties to the social structure were combined to form the index of "insulat- ing factors" described earlier. The relationship between these insulating factors and approval of violence varies according to the assailant-victim category. Strength of ties to the social structure have no appreciable affect on approval of violence by the police (r = .021, n.s.). When adult males or teenage males comprise the assailant-victim pairs, however, the relationship between approval of violence and insulating factors is nega- tive (r = .052, p <.01; r= 153, p <.001). It might be suggested that strong ties to the social structure do not lessen approval of violent be- havior by policemen because these youngsters expect that policemen are using force only when necessary and legitimate. The samelegitima- cy would not be accorded other assailants. Thesedistinctions notwith- standing, the strength or weakness of these insulating factors does not alter relationships between either measure of television violence and approval of violence. 433 426 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 35: Insulating factors

Approval, teen violence, by violence, favorite program, and insulating factors

Violence, favorite Insulating factors program (% with high approval)

1 (low) 2 3 4 (high)

Low 46% (35)13 42% (198) 35% (345) 24% (208) Moderate 50% (22) 43% (84) 31% (109) 20% (49) High 58% (40) 46% (166) 36% (255) 26% (128)

Total 52% (97) 44% . (448) 35% (709) 24% (385) Approval, adult violence, by violence, favorite program, and insulating factors

Violence, favorite Insulating factors program (% with high approval)

1 (low) 2 3 11 (high)

Low 46% (33) 54% (197) 48% (347) 42% (208) Moderate 59% (22) 51% (84) 38% (109) 37% (49) High 58% (40) 56% (165) 51% (255) 48% (128)

Total 54% (95) 54% (446) 47% (711) 44% (385)

Approval, police violence, by violence, favorite program, and insulating factors

Violence, favorite Insulating factors program (% with high approval)

1 (low) 2 3 4 (high)

Low 6% (35) 6% (199) 10% (348) 6% (208) Moderate 9% (23) 7% (85) 9% (109) 2% (49) High 23% (40) 12% (166) 10% (254) 9% (128)

Total 13% (98) 9% (450) 10% (711) , 7% (385) b Number in parentheses is number on which percentage is based In summary. it can be noted that there is a small relationship between the objective rating of the child's favorite television program and his approval of violent behavior on the part of adult males but not other cat- egories of assailants. It should be noted that this relationship is found for the average rating of the four favorite programs and all three mea- sures of approval, however. Furthermore, it is the objective rating rath- er than the subjective perception of violence which is more strongly re- lated to approval. Demographic characteristics of the respondentsrace, gender, age, and social classare not useful in explaining the relationship between television violence and approval of violence, although the lower-class 434 ...... 1.....t 4,...,6/4 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 427

Table 36: Summary of relationships between television violenceand approval of violence

$ = Violence, x2Average x = Perceived favorite violence, four violence and program favorites realism

x y r x y.a rxyrxy.a rxy y = approval, adult violence .071 " .096" .041 ** 8=age .074 .093** a = social class .071** .095" a = insulating factors .0680* .092" a = perceived violence and realism .068** y = approval, teen violence .021 (n.s.) .084" .110 " 8 2age n.s. .094" a = social class Ms. .086" a insulating factors n.s. .072" a = perceived violence and realism n.s.

y = approval, police violence.013 (n.s.) .076** .026 (n.s.) a = age n.s. .078' a=social class n.s. .075 a = insulating factors n.s. -.069 a = perceived violence and realism n.s. * p fe .001 respondents and the younger adolescents are morelikely to approve. Nor was another set of variables, representingthe strength or weakness of the child's ties to the social structure, morehelpful in explaining the influence of television violence. Generallyspeaking, the children with more and strongerinsulating ties are more likely to approvepolice violence, less likely to approve teenage violence, butequally likely to be influenced by television violence. Although it is necessary to remember that thedifferences between children who watched low and high violence programs werein most cas- es small, the relationship wasconsistent for the four favorite programs and in no case was greatly reduced with theintroduction of other varia- bles. BELIEFS ABOUT LEVEL OF VIOLENCE INSOCIETY It has been demonstrated thus far that thechild's viewing of television violence is associated with aggressive and deviantbehavior and also with approval of violence on the part of others.A related question is whether the child believes that he is learningabout the real world as he watches dramatic programs. If so, and if heoften selects the high violent shows, he might well decide that he livesin a society with a high level of violence. 435 428 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Table 37: Realism and violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Realism Low Moderate High Not codeable Total

7% (57) 10% (27) 9% 1541 13% (78) 10% 12161 1 28%(223) 28% 1751 28%11641 34%12061 30% 16681 2 28%12181 26% 1691 28%11651 28%11681 28% 16201 3 37%12901 36% 1951 34%11991 26%11581 33% 17421

Total 100%17881 100%12661 100%15821 100%16101 100%122461

Using a measure of how realistic the child believes his favorite program to be (shows life as it really is, people just like people in real life, and not much like the real world [reverse scoring]), fewer than half the respond- ents, about 40 percent, think that their favorite shows are not very true- to-life, while one-third think that both the situations and the characters are realistic (Table 37). It is conceivable that the children who think these shows are realistic are referring to educational programs, docu- mentaries, or news programs. This is not the case, however, for more of the high violence shows than those in the "non-codeable" category were termed realistic, although the latter included news, documentaries, and similar shows. Furthermore, the high violence shows are just as likely to be considered realistic as the low or moderate shows (Table 37).

Table 38: Belief in frequency of crime by violence, favorite program

Violence, favorite program

Frequency of crime Low Moderate High

1 (low) c c c 2 4% 3% 4% 3 44% 45% 43% 4 47% 47% 47% 5 (high) 5% 5% 6%

Total 100%17751 100%(258) 100%(584)

c Less than one percent Many of the programs which had been rated as violent include por- trayals of crime and criminals. It might be expected, then, that many youngsters who selected these high violent shows would have a pkture of their society as one in which serious crimes are frequent. To test this hypothesis, respondents were asked: How often do serious crimes like these (robbery, assault, car theft, burglary, etc.) occur "in your neigh- borhood," "in downtown Washington," and "in this part of the coun- try?" Respondents were assigned summary scores based on their re- sponses of "never," "rarely," "fairly often," or "very often" for crime

. 436 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 429 in each location. As is apparent in Table 38, the children whose favorite is rated high in violence are neither more nor less likely to believe in a high level of crime in the society. As was the case with deviance and approv- al of violence, however, the average violence rating of respondents' four favorite programs is clearly related to belief in level of crime in the society. The relationship is not great. however, and is no longer signifi- cant when age is controlled (Table 39). Socioeconomic statusand insu- lating ties to the social structure do not change the relationship greatly.

Table 39: Summary of relationships between telavision violence and belief in crime level

r x y r x y.a

x = violence, favorite program y = belief in crime lave! .012 (n.s.) a = age n.s. a = socioeconomic status n.s. a = insulating factors n.s. a = perceived violence and realism n.s.

x = average violence y = belief in crime level .048* a = age .039 (n.s.) a = socioeconomic status .048* a = insulating factots .045* * p (.01

To summarize, two-thirds of the respondents, including those whose favorites are high in violence, believe that their favorite shows are true to life. The small but significant relationship between average violence of four favorite programs and belief in crime level in society gives addition- al support to the hypothesis that youngsters use these programs in form- ing their views of society.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

It had been anticipated that the violence ratings 'of the adolescents' favorite television programs would be associated with deOiant behavior, especially with aggressive and serious deviance. This has been found to be true when the violence ratings of four favorite programs are con- sidered, but (except for serious deviance) not for the violence rating of the one most favorite show. The four favorite programs, the ones "you try to watch every time they are on the air," constitute a greater propor- tion of all television programs viewed, and it seems likely that this may account for their greater relationship with behavior. Ithad been expect- ed also that the adolescents' subjective perceptions of violencewould be more strongly related to deviant behavior thanwould the objective .437 430 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS violence ratings. This comparison is possible for the one favorite show only, and here it proves to be true. Nonetheless, four favorite shows are for the most part more strongly related to deviant behavior than the subjective perception of violence on the one most favorite show. If subjective perception of violence alone were associated with deviant behavior and approval of violence, it might be concluded that the rela- tionship between television violence and deviant behavior existed only for certain youngstersthose already prone to deviance and aggression. It is not, however, only those youngsters who subjectively perceive violence for whom there is a relationship between the violent content of their four favorite shows and deviance. Nor is the latter relationship generally diminished by including in the analysis characteristics of the youngster which would be expected to increase the likelihood of his being deviant or aggressive. When the adolescent's attitudes about violence (that is, approval of violence on the part of others) and his beliefs about violence in the so- ciety are considered, the ratings of the four favorite programs are again consistently associated with greater approval of violence and belief in higher level of violence in the society. The high proportion of young- sters who take their favorite programs to be a fairly accurate picture of the real world, including those youngsters whose favorite is a program with high violence content, may be related to this finding. Many children choose to watch those show s which are more violent and take them to be a reflection of the real world; the selection of these shows is related to the belief that violence and crime are frequent oc- currences in the society and to the approval of violent behavior. This finding is as important as the finding of an association between television violence and aggressive behavior itself. These youngsters may become inured to violence and later as citizens be indifferent to its occurrence. Second, it is also possible that the approval of violence may increase the likelihood of future violent behavior. Even if the consequences are lim- ited to the firstthat people become more willing to tolerate violence in othersthe implications are not trivial. There is one last issue: the discussion of causality. In the present in- vestigation, which used survey research techniques, information is lim- ited to the associations between television programs preferred (and viewed) and behavior and attitudes. The magnitude of correlatiOns is not great, and certainly television can be no more than one among many fac- tors influencing behavior and attitudes. However, there is consistently a significant relationship between the violence rating of four favorite pro- grams and the five measures of deviance, three of approval of violence and one of beliefs about crime in the society. Furthermore, these rela- tionships remain when variables expected to decrease the likelihood of deviance are introduced. The regularity with which these relationships appear suggests that they should not be overlooked. 428 VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 431 FOOTNOTES

I. The research upon which this reportis based was performed pur- suant to Contract No. HSM42-70-52 with the National Institute of Mental Health, Health Services andMental Health Administration, U.S. Department of Health, Education,and Welfare. The research staff for the projectincluded Mary T. Batt, Douglas Schocke, and Stephen Wolfe. We aregrateful to the public schools of Prince Georges County, the principals,the teachers, and the students whose cooperation made the projectpossible. Dr. Teevan is now at the Universityof Western Ontario. 2. Data were collected in one schoolin September 1970. The additional questionnnaires were administered to include alarger proportion of black students. 3. Father's occupation was codedusing the Duncan Socio-Economic Index (Reiss, Duncan, Hatt, andNorth, 1961). Those occupations coded 24 or below (at fifth decile,included "bus driver") were arbi- trarily considered "low" SES. Thosefrom 25 to 64 were considered "middle" SES, and those 65 and over(included ninth and part of eighth decilesprofessionals, technicaland kindred workers, etc.) were considered"upper" SES.

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY Bailyn, L. Mass media and children; A study of exposurehabits and cognitive effects. Psychological Monographs, 1959, 73,1-48. Baker, R.K., and Ball, S. J. (Eds.) Mass media andviolence: a staff report to the National Commission on theCauses and Prevention of Violence. Washington, D.C.: Government PrintingOffice, 1969. Banay, R. S. Testimony before the Subcommittee toInvestigate Juven- ile Delinquency. Committee on the Judiciary,S. Res., Apr. 1955, 62, Washington, D.C.: Government PrintingOffice. Bandura, A. What television violence can do to yourchild. In Larsen, 0. N. (Ed.) Violence and the mass media. NewYork: Harper and Row, 1968. Bandura, A., and Walters, R. H. Social learning andpersonality devel- opment. New York: Holt, Rinehart andWinston, 1963. Becker, H.S. The outsiders, New York: Free Press,1963. Berkowitz, L., Corwin, R., and Heiromimous, M.Film violence and subsequent aggressive tendencies. Public OpinionQuarterly, 1963, 27, 217-29. Block, H. A., and Niederhoffer, A. The gang.New York: Philosophical Library, 1958 Briar, S., and Piliavin, I. Delinquency, situationalinducements, and commitment to conformity, Social Problems,1965, 13, 35-45. 429 432 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS

Brod,beck, A. J. The mass media as a socializing agency. Paper present- ed at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, 1955. Bronfenbrenner, U. Two worlds of childhood: U.S. and U.S.S.R. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1970. Charters, W. W., and Newcomb, T. M. Some attitudinal effects of ex- perimentally increased salience of a membership group. In Macco- by, E. E. et al. (Eds.) Readings in social psychol9gy (3rd ed). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1958. Cloward,R., and Oh lin,L.Delinquency and opportunity. New York: Free Press, 1960. Cohen, A. K. Delinquent boys. Glencoe: Free Press, 1955. Cohen, A. K. Deviance and control. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall,

1966. , Cohen, A. K. Middle class delinquency and the social structure. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Sociological Association, Chicago, 1957. Cohen, A. K., and Short, J. F. Research in delinquent subcultures. Jour- nal of Social.Issues, 1958, 14, 20-37. Commission on Law Enforcement and Administration of Justice, The challenge of crime in a free society. Washington, D. C.: Govern- ment Printing Office, 1967. DeFleur, M. L. Theories of mass communication. New York: David McKay, 1966. England, R. W. A theory of.middle class juvenile delinquency. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology, and Police Science, 1960, 50, 535-40. Eron, L. D. Relation of TV viewing, habits and aggressive behavior in children. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 193- 96. Feshbach, S. The catharsis effect: research and another view. In Baker Ball (Eds.) Mass media and Violence. Festinger, L. Laboratory experiments: the role of group belonging- ness. In Miller; J. G. (Ed.) Experim,ents in,social process:a sym- posium on social psychology. New York: ,McGraw Hill, 1950. Gerson, W. M. Social structure and mass media socialization. Unpub- lished doctoral dissertation, University of Washington, 1963. Gibbons, D. C. Delinquent behavior. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1970. Goranson, R. E. A review of recent literature on psychological effects of media portrayals of violence. Baker and Ball (Eds.) Mass media and.violence. Greenberg, B. S., and Gordon, T. Perceptions of televisionvio- lence: critics and the public. In Television and social behavior, Vol. 1. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1971.

440

00.1110.1.4911.1111~11111M...M J(..111011.01e; -*ha. : . VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 433

Greenwald, A. G. Do crime and violence in the mass news media modify behavior? Columbus: Ohio State University, 1971 (mimeo). Haines, W. H. Juvenile delinquency andtelevision. Journal of Social Therapy, 1955, 1, 192-98. Halloran, J. D. Television and violence. In Larsen, 0. N. (Ed.)Violence and the mass media. NewYork: Harper & Row, 1968. Himmelweit, H. T. Oppenheim, A. N., and Vince, P.Television and thechi/d. London: OxfOrd University Press, 1958. Hovland, C. 1., Janis, I. L., and Kelley, H. H.Communication and per- suasion. NewHaven: Yale University Press, 1953. Kelley, H. H. Salience of membership and resistance tochange of group-anchoredattitudes. Human Relations,1955, 8, 275-90. Kelley, H. H., and Volkart, E. H. The resistance to change of group- anchored attitudes.American Sociological Review, 1952,17, 453- 65. Kelley, H. H., and Woodruff, C. L. Members reactions 'to apparent group approval of a counter-normcommunication. Journal of Ab- normalSocial Psychology,1956, 52, 67-74. Larsen, O.N. Social effects of mass communication. In Faris, R.E. (Ed.) Handbookof modern sociology.Chicago: Rand-McNally, 1964. Larsen, 0. N.Violence and the massmedia. New York: Harper & Row, 1968. Larsen, 0. N. Posing the problem of effects. In Baker and Ball(Eds.) Mass media and violence. Lazarsfeld, P. F., Berelson, B., and Gaudet, H.The people's choice. New York: Columbia University Press, 1948. Lemert, E. M. Humandeviance, social problems, and socialcontrol. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall, 1967. Lovibond, S. H. The effect of media stressing crime and violence.upon children's attitudes.Social Problems,1967, 15, 91-99. Maccoby, E. E. Effects of the mass media. In Hoffman, M. L., and Hoffman, L. W. (Eds.) Review of child development researeh, Vol. I. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1964. Maccoby, E. E. Effects Of the mass media. Larsen (Ed.)Violence and the mass media. Marwell, G. Adolescent powerlessness and delinquency.Social Prob- lems,1966, 14, 35-47. Matza, D., and Sykes, G. M.Juvenile delinquency and subterranean values. American Sociological Review,1961, 26, 712-19. Merton, R.K. Social structure and anomie.American Sociological Re- view, 1938,3, 672-82. Miller, W.B. Lower class culture as a generating milieu of gang delin- quency. Journal ofSocial Issues,1958, 14, 2-19. 441 434 TELEVISION AND ADOLESCENT AGGRESSIVENESS National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence. Com- mission statement on violence in television entertainment programs. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office, 1969. Nye, F.I., and Short, J.F. Scaling delinquent behavior. American So- ciological Review, 1956, 22, 326-31. Reiss, A., Duncan, 0., Hatt, P., and North, C. Occupations and social status. New York: The Free Press, 1961. Riley, M.W., and Riley, J.W. A sociological approach to communica- tion research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1951, 15, 445-60. Rilay, J.W., Schramm, W., and Williams, F.W. Flight from commu- nism: a report on Korean refugees. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1951, 15, 274-86. Scheff, T.J. Being mentally ill. Chicago: Aldine, 1966. Schramm, W., Lyle, J., and Parker, E.B. Television in the lives of our children. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1961. Sellin, T. and Wolfgang, M. The measurement of delinquency. New York: John Wiley, 1964. Thompson, R.J. Television crime-drama: its impact on children and adolescents. Melbourne: F.W. Cheshire, 1959. Turk, A. T. Toward construction of a theory of delinquency. Journal of Criminal Law, Criminology, and Police Science, 1964, 55, 215-29. Walters, R. H., and Llewellyn-Thomas, E. Enhancement of punitive- ness by visual and audio-visual displays. Canadian Journal of Psy- chology, 1963, 17, 244-55. Weiss, W. Effects of the mass media of communication. In Lindzey, G. and Aronson, E. (Eds.) The handbook of social psychology. Read- ing, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1968. Wertham, F. We are teaching our children that violence is fun. In Lar- sen (Ed.) Violence and the mass media. Wilensky, H. L., and Lebeaux, C. N. Industrial society and social wel- fare. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 1958. Wright, C. R. Mass communication. New York: Random House, 1959. Zajonc, R. Some effects of the space serials. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1954, 18, 367-74. Zimmerman, C., and Bauer, R. A. The influence of an audience on what is remembered. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1956, 20, 238-48.

442 ,...... ,

VIOLENCE AND DEVIANT BEHAVIOR 435

Appendix A Table 1: Index of insulating tries to the social structure

One point was added to the summary score for eachof the following factors for which the respondent scored high: 1. Peer relationships: How often have youfound yourself with someplace to go but no friends to go with? (Hardly ever, Never) 2. Parental relationships: When you want toask your parents for or about something are they around or willing to talk to you? (Always,Usually) 3. Grades in school: What are your averagegrades in school? (A's. A's and B's, or B's) 4. Fear of punishment. Range = 0 9; high = 6 or above. 5. School activities: What school activitiesdo you participate in? (2 or more activities) 6. Educational expectations: Expects to go tocollege or finish college. 7. Occupational aspirations: What kind of jobwould you like to have when you com- pletely finished school? (High = Duncan Socio=EconomicIndex score of 25 or above) 8. Occupational expectations: What kind ofjob do you think you actually will get when you completely finish school? (High =Duncan Socio-Economic Index score of 25 or above)

Table 2: Distribution of approval of violence scoresby race, gender, age, and social class (% with high approval)

Approval Approval Approval adult violence teen violence police violence 06% White male 59% 4% White female 37% 24% 0516 10% Black male 47% 50% 09% Black female 52% 39% 11% Lower class 56% 44% 11% Middle class 50% 36% 07% Upper class 44% 29% 11 14 years 51% 40% 11% 09% 15 19 years 46% 34% .11rrittrwArtmorestrorsams,..

DHEW Publication No. HSM 724058 Printed 1972

U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION, AND WELFARE Health Services and Mental Health Administration

National Institute of Mental Health

444