The Syntax of Case and Agreement: Its Relationship to Morphology and Argument Structure

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Syntax of Case and Agreement: Its Relationship to Morphology and Argument Structure The Syntax of Case and Agreement: its Relationship to Morphology and Argument Structure By Vita G. Markman A Dissertation submitted to the Graduate School – New Brunswick Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Program in Linguistics written under the direction of Professor Mark Baker and approved by Professor Mark Baker Professor Viviane Deprez Professor Ken Safir Professor Carson Schutze New Brunswick, New Jersey October 2005 © 2005 Vita G. Markman ALL RIGHTS RESERVED ABSTRACT OF DISSERTATION The Syntax of Case and Agreement: its Relationship to Morphology and Argument Structure by VITA G. MARKMAN Dissertation Director Professor Mark Baker In this thesis I argue for a non-arbitrary relationship between the syntax of case and agreement and its morphological realization, as reflected in the following linguistic universals: 1. If a language overtly case-marks the subject, it overtly marks the object; 2.If a language has overt object agreement, it has overt subject agreement (Moravcik 1974, Comrie 1988, Lehmann 1982). The goal of this thesis is to explain the nature of the morphology-syntax connection the above universals embody and explore the consequences it has for syntactic theory, grammars of individual languages, and for UG. In this dissertation I depart from the Universal Approach (e.g. Chomsky 1981, Rouveret and Vergnaud 1980, and later in Chomsky 1995, 2000, Harley 1995, Sigurdsson 2003 inter alia) that treats case and agreement as universal properties of language and their overt realization as arbitrary and language specific. Building on a proposal presented in Pesetsky and Torrego 2001 that features are interpretable but may become uninterpetable if placed on a wrong head, I argue that case and agreement features are misplaced interpretable features used by languages to create PF-records of thematic relations. I further argue that misplaced features ii are not universal: in the absence of case and agreement features PF-records of thematic relations are preserved via rigid word order. I further demonstrate that restrictions on feature misplacement together with the inherent properties of misplaced features and the syntactic configurations in which misplaced features are valued account for the above universals, derive a constrained cross-linguistic case and agreement typology, and has consequences for (non)-configurationality. In particular, I argue that languages without case features but with agreement features will be non-configurational, languages that have both case and agreement features may allow but not require NP dislocation, and finally languages that lack case and agreement features will have rigid word order. This is the topic of Chapter 4. In this thesis I also address (quirky) dative subjects (Chapter 2), infinitives (Chapter 3), and ergativity (Chapter 5). iii DEDICATION To All My Parents iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost I would like to thank my advisor, Mark Baker, without whom this work would certainly not be possible. What I can fit into a paragraph in the acknowledgments to my thesis would never be enough to adequately express my gratitude to you. Your book on Lexical Categories instilled in me a desire to write a dissertation in syntax. Thank you for being my inspiration and my role model. Thank you for your intellectual input, for the time you have devoted to me. Thank you for having faith in me and for your understanding, both on a personal and a professional level. To me, you are and always will be an example of a great teacher, a caring mentor and a brilliant researcher. I am honored to be your student. Thank you. I owe many thanks to Ken Safir and Viviane Deprez who took their time to comment on my work throughout the two years that I have worked on this project. Thank you for your patience, your time, your input and your guidance. Thank you to Carson Schutze, for taking the time to be my external committee member, for meeting with me and providing detailed and thought-through comments. You all made a great committee together and I value and appreciate your help! I thank you all for the time and effort you have invested in me. I am also very grateful to other faculty members at the Rutgers Linguistics Department who significantly contributed to my education. Thank you to, Veneeta Dayal, Bruce Tesar, Maria Bittner and Matthew Stone for being my professors, my teachers, my mentors. I would also like to thank Anna Szabolsci and Richard Kayne who were my first professors of linguistics. Because of you I was able to fulfill my dream of getting into Rutgers which was and remains my first choice graduate school. Thank you for the encouragement you have given me, for your help and support. I would like to thank all the students at the Rutgers Linguistics Department for being a part of this great community and making my time here so much fun. I would like to thank Seye Adesola, Slavica Kochovska, Jessica Rett, Seung Hun, Beto Ellias-Ulloa, Natalia Kariaeva and all the members of our Syntax Group *STaR for offering valuable input on my presentations. v Thank you for being my classmates and my friends. Special thanks go to Anubha Kothari for providing judgments on Hindi and to Xiao Li for the judgments on Chinese. Thank you Anubha and Xiao for being my patient and helpful informants. I would like to thank the Rutgers Linguistics Department as a community for helping me get this far. Thank you all for being my colleagues, my mentors, and my friends. You are a great department and I feel privileged to be a part of you. A special thanks goes to Joanna Stoher who has been and continues to be the best office manager in the world. Thank you, Joanna, for all your invaluable help, your infinite patience and understanding. I also owe many thanks to the students and faculty at the Department of Linguistics at UCLA where I spent the Fall 2004 quarter as a visiting student. Special thanks go to Tim Stowell who welcomed me to the department and allowed me to use its numerous resources. Thank you to Hilda Koopman and Anoop Mahajan for teaching an incredibly stimulating seminar on Quirky Subjects. No words would be enough to express the gratitude I feel towards my family, especially to all my parents: Gary, Lyuda, Sasha and Arkadij, my sister Ilona and my brother Denis. I feel especially grateful to my Mom, who unfortunately is not here to share with me this joyful and important occasion, but who is forever in my heart. My dear parents, my debt to all of you is the greatest. Gary, you raised me. You instilled in me the very values that drove me to pursue higher education, and in particular, obtain a doctoral degree. You have taught me to appreciate and seek knowledge, to strive for self-improvement and take joy in creative intellectual pursuits. The impact you have had on my life truly cannot be measured. For this and for much more I am infinitely thankful to you. My dear Sasha and Arkadij, your house I have been calling ‘home’ for over 10 years now. Thank you for your warmth, for your welcome, for your kindness for your moral support. Thank you all for being my family. Always. I owe many thanks to all my friends outside of the department. In particular, to my precious little girl Sophie for her brilliant mind and her kind soul. The help and support you have vi offered and continue to offer me is invaluable. Thank you for the stimulating discussions about linguistics and about life…Thank you for proofreading my papers, handouts, and abstracts countless times. Thank you for listening to my practice talks. Thank you for taking an insane amount of time to help me do the editing and formatting revisions for my thesis. If I were to name one person outside of my committee who had a direct impact on making this thesis possible, it would be you. It is you. Thank you my dear for keeping me from going nuts, for being my best friend and my confidante. My dear friends Felix, Ella M., Ella A. Olja, Yasha, Alik, Robert, Josh, Sam, and Asya, all of you have helped me enormously in your own way. My dear Ellas, when I am around you I rest mentally and emotionally. I thank you both for that. Felix, thank you for being an incredibly kind and sensitive person, a great friend, and for being so much fun to hang out with. Alik, you have taught me to appreciate excellent music. This thesis was written while listening to Chet Baker, Bill Evans, John Coltrane and Jim Hall, to name a few. Thank you for introducing me to these great musicians and for teaching me to love jazz. I would like to extend my thanks to the members of the Argentine Tango Community in New York and Los Angeles for making it possible for me to forget about syntax and my dissertation when I needed to. Last, but certainly not least, I would like to thank my friend and dance partner Alex. You are the soul of tango. Thank you for your kindness, for your incredible patience, for your support and understanding, for giving me the strength to go on. Thank you for your smile, for your dance and for the magic of you. You fill my days with joy and laughter and light. I cannot tell you how grateful I am to you for that. vii Table of Contents Abstract ii Dedication iv Acknowledgements v 1 Introduction: Case, Agreement and the Universal Approach 1 1.1 Introduction 1 1.2 Universals of case and agreement 10 1.3 Theoretical Framework 14 1.4 Restrictions on feature misplacement 30 1.5 Conclusion 32 2 Dative Subjects 37 2 1 Introduction 37 2.2 Dative Subjects 39 2.3 Adverbial vs.
Recommended publications
  • A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions
    A Crosslinguistic Approach to Double Nominative and Biabsolutive Constructions: Evidence from Korean and Daghestanian∗ Andrei Antonenko1 and Jisung Sun2 Stony Brook University1,2 1. Introduction Distribution of case among distinct grammatical relations is one of the most frequently studied topics in the syntactic theory. Canonical cases are, in accusative languages, subjects of both intransitive and transitive verbs being nominative, while direct objects of transitive verbs are usually marked accusative. In ergative languages, subjects of intransitive verbs share properties with direct objects of transitive verbs, and are marked absolutive. Subjects of transitive verbs are usually ergative. When you look into world languages, however, there are ‘non-canonical’ case patterns too. Probably the most extreme kind of non-canonical case system would be so-called Quirky Subject constructions in Icelandic (see Sigurðsson 2002). This paper concerns constructions, in which two nominals are identically case-marked in a clause, as observed in Korean and Daghestanian languages. Daghestanian languages belong to Nakh-Daghestanian branch of North Caucasian family. Nakh-Daghestanian languages are informally divided into Nakh languages, such as Chechen and Ingush, spoken in Chechnya and the Republic of Ingushetia, respectively; and Daghestanian languages, spoken in the Republic of Daghestan. Those regions are located in the Caucasian part of Russian Federation. Some Daghestanian languages are also spoken in Azerbaijan and Georgia. This study focuses on Daghestanian languages, such as Archi, Avar, Dargwa, Hinuq, Khwarshi, Lak and Tsez, due to similar behaviors of them with respect to the described phenomenon. 2. Ergativity in Daghestanian Aldridge (2004) proposes that there are two types of syntactically ergative languages, based on which argument is performing functions typical for subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • Non-Nominative Subjects in Comparison
    NON-NOMINATIVE SUBJECTS IN COMPARISON Josef Bayer University of Konstanz Fachbereich Sprachwissenschaft Universität Konstanz Universitätsstr. 10 D-78457 Konstanz Germany [email protected] 1 NON-NOMINATIVE SUBJECTS IN COMPARISON Josef Bayer University of Konstanz 1. Introduction Among languages with a sufficiently rich system of morphological Case we observe unmarked constituent orderings which deviate from the “nomina- tive preceding non-nominative” pattern. This deviation, if one wants to call it that way, is to a large extent lexically and semantically predictable. Languages of this kind are classified as languages that permit non- nominative subjects. As is well known, however, they differ quite radically as to certain syntactic consequences which the non-nominative-first pat- tern may have. German and Icelandic are closely related Germanic lan- guages which – not surprisingly – show strong similarities in their argu- ment structures and syntax of Case. Nevertheless, they differ by the fact that non-nominative prominent DPs in Icelandic behave like genuine sub- jects while they do not (or do to a lesser extent) in German. The goal of the present article is to explore the possibility of deriving differences in “sub- jecthood” from the basic order of constituents. Icelandic has a head-initial 0 VP which is separated from an external argument by a functional head F 0 (or a number of functional heads), i.e., the order is SpecFP F [VP V …]. Ger- man has a head-final VP instead. There are strong indications that the or- 0 der [VP … V] F does not give rise to a functionally defined position SpecFP.
    [Show full text]
  • Person Versus Default Number in Agreement Peter Ackema University of Edinburgh Ad Neeleman UCL
    Chapter 2 Default person versus default number in agreement Peter Ackema University of Edinburgh Ad Neeleman UCL In this paper, we compare the behaviour of the default in the person system (third person) with the default in the number system (singular). We argue, following Nevins (2007; 2011), that third person pronouns have person features, while singu- lar DPs lack number features. The evidence for these claims comes from situations in which a single head agrees with multiple DPs that have contrasting person and number specifications. In cases where the number of morphological slots inwhich agreement can be realized is lower than the number of agreement relations estab- lished in syntax, such contrasting specification may prove problematic. As it turns out, conflicts between singular and plural do not result in ungrammaticality, but conflicts between third person and first or second person do. Such person clashes can be avoided if the morphological realization of the relevant person features is syncretic. Alternatively, languages may make use of a person hierarchy that reg- ulates the morphological realization of conflicting specifications for person. The argument we present is rooted in, and supports, the theory of person developed in Ackema & Neeleman (2013; 2018). 1 Introduction The problem addressed in this paper is an apparent paradox involving singular number and third person. On the one hand, there is evidence that in the per- son system the default is third person, while in the number system the default is singular. For example, dummy pronouns and verbs that fail to agree (as in impersonal passives) show up in the third person singular: Peter Ackema & Ad Neeleman.
    [Show full text]
  • CASE ALTERNATIONS in ANCIENT GREEK PASSIVES and the TYPOLOGY of CASE Elena Anagnostopoulou Christina Sevdali
    CASE ALTERNATIONS IN ANCIENT GREEK PASSIVES AND THE TYPOLOGY OF CASE Elena Anagnostopoulou Christina Sevdali University of Crete Ulster University This article presents and discusses evidence that genitive and dative objects regularly become nominative in Ancient Greek passives of monotransitives and ditransitives. This is a typologically and theoretically significant state of affairs for two reasons . (i) As is well known, nonaccusative objects are, in many languages, not allowed to enter into Case alternations, a fact that has been ac - counted for in the government-binding /principles-and-parameters literature on the basis of the as - sumption that nonaccusative objects —prototypically datives —bear inherent , lexical , or quirky Case. By this reasoning, Ancient Greek genitives and datives must be concluded to have structural Case. (ii) Even in languages where dative -nominative (DAT-NOM) alternations do obtain, they are often limited to ditransitives, a fact that can been taken to suggest that dative qual - ifies as structural Case only in ditransitives. A language like Ancient Greek , which allows genitive and dative objects to become nominative in all passives (monotransitives and ditransitives) , shows that it is, in principle, possible to have a linguistic system where genitive and dative qualify as structural Cases in both monotransitives and ditransitives. Case theories must be designed in such a way as to allow for this option. We argue for an analysis of Case alternations that combines the view that alternating datives and
    [Show full text]
  • Raising to Quirky Subject in Tatar∗ 1 Introduction
    A-2 Raising to Quirky Subject in Tatar∗ Chihiro Taguchi Nara Institute of Science and Technology [email protected] Keywords— Tatar, Turkic, syntax, morphology, raising, quirky subject Abstract Tatar (< Kipchak < Turkic) has subject-to-subject raising predicates where the raised argument is marked by nominative, dative and genitive case, of which the latter two are “quirky (i.e., non-nominative) subjects” (Sigurðsson 1992). Poole (2016) proposed the Quirky Subject Hierarchy, a three-level hierarchy of the subjecthood of quirky subjects, and classified languages with quirky subject into three categories: Hindi type, Icelandic type, and Laz type. However, the Tatar data in this paper demonstrate that a language may fit more than one categories; a raised subject with dative case-marking in Tatar displays the Icelandic-type characteristics, and a raised subject with genitive displays the Laz-type. I propose that these properties are not language-specific but lexicon-specific. 1 Introduction The Tatar language is a Kipchak language of the Turkic language family. It is an agglutina- tive language with several nominal case suffixes. The canonical word order is SOV, and the modification order is AN. This paper deals with two kinds of subject-to-subject raising (SSR) predicates whose subject is marked by genitive and dative. (1)-(3) are examples of raising constructions in Tatar, (1) with a nominative subject which is a typical way of marking a subject, (2) with a dative, and (3) with a genitive, all having by and large similar deontological meaning. Section 2 outlines what raising is and how it is analyzed in general theoretical linguistics.
    [Show full text]
  • Modality and Causation in Serbian Dative Anticausatives
    MODALITY AND CAUSATION IN SERBIAN DATIVE ANTICAUSATIVES: A CROSSLINGUISTIC PERSPECTIVE A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE DIVISION OF THE UNIVERSITY OF HAWAI‘I AT MĀNOA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY IN LINGUISTICS DECEMBER 2013 By Tatjana Ilić Dissertation Committee: Kamil Ud Deen, Chairperson William O’Grady Yuko Otsuka Bonnie D. Schwartz Shuqiang Zhang ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to take the opportunity to thank to all my committee members for providing patient guidance in my work over the years. First and foremost, a warm thank you goes to my committee chair and the advisor of many years, Dr. Kamil Ud Deen, for being such a friend and support on this journey, and for providing the sound of reason when my ideas were pulling me astray. I deeply appreciate the liberty I was given in handling this topic, and admire his willingness and the ability to survive the countless versions, changes of perspective, and even of the theoretical approach that this dissertation has gone through. I have grown as a linguist, as a writer and as a person, and I can only hope to benefit much more from his professional and personal friendship in the years to come. A further warm thank you goes jointly to Dr. Kamil Ud Deen and Dr. Bonnie Schwartz for many a discussion on the first language acquisition, child second language acquisition, second language processing and more. I am particularly indebted to Dr. Bonnie Schwartz for all the hearty laughs I had with her over the years. A further deep appreciation goes to Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Impersonal Constructions in Old French and the Agreement Puzzle∗
    IMPERSONAL CONSTRUCTIONS IN OLD FRENCH AND THE AGREEMENT PUZZLE∗ Eric Mathieu University of Ottawa 1. Introduction Quirky subjects are subjects surfacing with non-nominative case. Although they have clear subject properties (they can appear in first position in main and embedded clauses; they can undergo subject-verb inversion - in V1 and V2 environments; they can bind an anaphor; they can participate in raising constructions; they can be a controllee; and they can participate in conjunction reduction), they have the striking property of not entering into an Agree relation with the predicate. This puzzle remains to be explained, especially since subject- verb agreement is often taken to be a prototypical property of subjecthood. The present paper focuses on Old French impersonal constructions where quirky subjects can be found. The paper relies on the facts introduced in Mathieu (2006a) which show that pre-verbal non-nominative elements in Old French are true subjects. They have all the properties listed above: they can bind an anaphor, be a controllee, etc. In the present study, I concentrate on agreement issues: first, I give a solution to the puzzle as to why there is no Agree relation between the oblique subject and the predicate in Old French impersonal constructions, and second, why in the absence of agreement nominative objects can nevertheless be licensed in these constructions. Section 2 constitutes the body of the paper while Section 3 concludes with a summary. 2. Agreement In Mathieu (2006a), I argue that Old French exhibits structures which call upon a quirky-subject analysis. Dubbed ‘impersonal constructions’ in the traditional literature, such structures typically involve an empty subject position that a dative, accusative or genitive XP comes to fill1.
    [Show full text]
  • Agreement, Case and Locality in the Nominal and Verbal Domains
    Edinburgh Research Explorer Default person versus default number in agreement Citation for published version: Ackema, P & Neeleman, A 2019, Default person versus default number in agreement. in L Franco, MMM & M Reeve (eds), Agreement, Case and Locality in the Nominal and Verbal Domains. Open Generative Syntax, Language Science Press, Berlin, pp. 21-54. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3458062 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.5281/zenodo.3458062 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Published In: Agreement, Case and Locality in the Nominal and Verbal Domains General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Chapter 2 Default person versus default number in agreement Peter Ackema University of Edinburgh Ad Neeleman UCL In this paper, we compare the behaviour of the default in the person system (third person) with the default in the number system (singular). We argue, following Nevins (2007; 2011), that third person pronouns have person features, while singu- lar DPs lack number features.
    [Show full text]
  • LB-Sonderheft 14: Endangered Languages
    Peter Austin / Andrew Simpson (Hg.) Buske Endangered Languages Endangered Languages Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft 14 Edited by Peter K. Austin and Andrew Simpson HELMUT BUSKE VERLAG HAMBURG Bibliografische Information der Deutschen Nationalbibliothek Die Deutsche Nationalbibliothek verzeichnet diese Publikation in der Deutschen Nationalbibliografie; detaillierte bibliografische Daten sind im Internet über <http://dnb.d-nb.de> abrufbar. ISBN 978-3-87548-465-6 LB Sonderheft · ISSN 0935-9249 www.buske.de © Helmut Buske Verlag 2007. Alle Rechte, auch die des auszugsweisen Nach- drucks, der fotomechanischen Wiedergabe und der Übersetzung, vorbehalten. Dies betrifft auch die Vervielfältigung und Übertragung einzelner Textabschnitte durch alle Verfahren wie Speicherung und Übertragung auf Papier, Filme, Bän- der, Platten und andere Medien, soweit es nicht §§ 53 und 54 URG ausdrücklich gestatten. Druck und Bindung: Druckhaus „Thomas Müntzer“, Bad Langensalza. Werkdruckpapier: alterungsbeständig nach ANSI-Norm resp. DIN-ISO 9706, hergestellt aus 100% chlorfrei gebleichtem Zellstoff. Printed in Germany. Contents Peter K. Austin and Andrew Simpson Introduction .................................................................................. 5 Nicholas Evans Warramurrungunji undone: Australian languages in the 51st Millennium 19 Knut J. Olawsky ObViouS OVS in Urarina syntax ....................................................... 45 Larry M. Hyman and Imelda Udoh Length harmony in Leggbó: a counter-universal? ...............................
    [Show full text]
  • Case, Agreement and Movement in Arabic a Minimalist Approach
    Case, Agreement and Movement in Arabic A Minimalist Approach Mamdouh Musabhien B.A. AHU / Jordan (2001) M.A. University of Essex (2004) A thesis submitted to the School of English Literature Language and Linguistics for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at Newcastle University November,2008 © 2008 Mamdouh Ayed Ghayad Musabhien All Rights Reserved 2 Abstract This thesis proposes a minimalist analysis that accounts for a number of word-order- related issues in Modern Standard Arabic (MSA) and Jordanian Arabic (JA). Assuming Chomsky's (2005) feature inheritance model, the thesis investigates the issues of Case, the interaction between subject positions and verbal agreement in addition to object movement. In verb-subject-object word orders, subjects are invariably nominative; the Case value on the postverbal subject is an outcome of an Agree relation between these subjects and T, the head of Tense Phrase (TP), which inherits its feature from the complementiser. Chapter four argues that the Case variability on the preverbal subject in subject-verb-object structures is dependent on the type of the complementiser. The complementiser which introduces subject-verb-object clauses has a lexical Case feature that is not interpretable on T, hence T does not inherit this feature. Consequently, the lexical Case feature of the complementiser in subject- verb-object structures is discharged under a local Agree relation between the complementiser and the preverbal noun phrase which is raised from a lower position. It is also claimed in chapter four that the structure of zero copula sentences contains a light Noun Phrase (nP) functional projection that compares to the light Verb Phrase (vP) functional projection in verbal sentences.
    [Show full text]
  • Agreement and Its Failures
    Agreement and its failures Omer Preminger £§u-£¶fhZ± o§Z± August ä, óþÕì – o ± o««uZ±u – to Saa Yaa Contents Acknowledgements ix Abbreviations xi Õ Introduction Õ ó Modeling the obligatoriness of φ-agreement ¢ ó.Õ A working denition of “agreement” .......................... ä ó.ó ree models for the obligatoriness of agreement .................. ß ó.ó.Õ e derivational time-bombs model ..................... ß ó.ó.ó e violable constraints model ......................... Õþ ó.ó.ì e obligatory operations model ........................ ÕÕ ó.ì Failed agreement, and why we should be interested in it ............... Õ¦ ì Agreement in the Kichean Agent-Focus construction: e facts Õß ì.Õ Some basic facts about Kichean and Agent-Focus .................. Õ ì.ó Agreement in Kichean Agent-Focus .......................... óó ì.ì eAFpersonrestriction ................................ ó ì.ì.Õ e phenomenon ................................. ó ì.ì.ó Against a purely morphological analysis of agreement in AF ....... óÉ ì.¦ e morpho-phonology of Kichean agreement markers ............... ìþ ¦ A derivational account of absolutive agreement in Kichean ì¢ ¦.Õ Background: e PCC, and Béjar & Rezac’s (óþþì) account of it .......... ì ¦.ó Relativized probing .................................... ¦É ¦.ó.Õ What’s good for [wh] is good for [plural] and [participant], too . ¢þ ¦.ó.ó Feature relativization in a feature-geometric approach ........... ¢¦ ¦.ó.ì Valuation in a feature-geometric approach .................. ¢É ¦.ó.¦ Summary ..................................... äó vi ¦.ì On the featural coarseness of clitic doubling ...................... äì ¦.¦ Applying Béjar & Rezac’s (óþþì) account to Kichean ................ äÉ ¦.¦.Õ e basic clause structure and derivation ................... äÉ ¦.¦.ó Licensing asymmetries in Kichean AF, and the AF person restriction .. ßÉ ¦.¢ Some alternative analyses, and their drawbacks ...................
    [Show full text]
  • Quirky Subjects in Icelandic, Faroese, and German: a Relational Grammar Account
    Quirky subjects in Icelandic, Faroese, and German: A Relational Grammar account Andreas Pankau Freie Universitat¨ Berlin Proceedings of the Joint 2016 Conference on Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar and Lexical Functional Grammar Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland Doug Arnold, Miriam Butt, Berthold Crysmann, Tracy Holloway King, Stefan Muller¨ (Editors) 2016 CSLI Publications pages 499–519 http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HeadLex/2016 Keywords: quirky subjects, Germanic languages, multistratalism, subject tests, Arc Pair Grammar Pankau, Andreas. 2016. Quirky subjects in Icelandic, Faroese, and German: A Re- lational Grammar account. In Arnold, Doug, Butt, Miriam, Crysmann, Berthold, King, Tracy Holloway, & Muller,¨ Stefan (Eds.), Proceedings of the Joint 2016 Conference on Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar and Lexical Functional Grammar, Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland, 499–519. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Abstract This paper presents a new analysis of quirky subjects according to which quirky subjects bear multiple grammatical relations and hence differ syntactically from regular subjects. This contrasts with the standard analysis of quirky subjects according to which quirky subjects are regular subjects bearing lexical case and therefore differ only morphologically from regular subjects. Based on the behavior of quirky subjects in Faroese and German, I argue that the syntactic account is superior. Faroese shows that the case borne by a quirky subject is not lexical, whereas German shows that quirky subjects are not regular subjects to begin with. The behavior of quirky subjects in Icelandic, on which the standard analysis is based, is argued to be the result of a morphosyntactic peculiarity of Icelandic 1 The Standard Analysis of Quirky Subjects Quirky subjects is the term used to refer to constructions where a subject bears unexpected (“quirky”) case, namely some object case instead of the expected nominative case.1 Two examples from Icelandic are given in (1).
    [Show full text]