University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository

Arts Arts Research & Publications

2019-05 A Developmental Cascade from Prenatal to Child Internalizing and Externalizing Problems

Hentges, Rochelle F.; Graham, Susan; Plamondon, André R.; Tough, Suzanne C.; Madigan, Sheri L.

Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Pediatric Psychology : Journal of Pediatric Psychology

Hentges, R. F., Graham, S. A., Plamondon, A. R., Tough, S. C., & Madigan, S. L. (2019). A Developmental Cascade from Prenatal Stress to Child Internalizing and Externalizing Problems. "Journal of Pediatric Psychology", 2019, 1-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044 http://hdl.handle.net/1880/111910 journal article https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 Unless otherwise indicated, this material is protected by copyright and has been made available with authorization from the copyright owner. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 2019, 1–11 doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044 Original Research Article Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 A Developmental Cascade from Prenatal Stress to Child Internalizing and Externalizing Problems

1,2 1,2 Rochelle F. Hentges, PHD, Susan A. Graham, PHD, 3,4 2,5,6 Andre Plamondon, PHD, Suzanne Tough, PHD, and 1,2 Sheri Madigan PHD

1Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, 2Owerko Centre, Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, 3Department des fondements et pratiques en education, Universite Laval, 4Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto, 5Department of Community Health Sciences, University of Calgary and, 6Department of Pediatrics, University of Calgary All correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Rochelle Hentges, PhD, Owerko Centre, Third Floor—CDC Building, c/o 2500 University Dr. N.W., Calgary, Alberta T2N 1N4, Canada. E-mail: [email protected] Received October 31, 2018; revisions received May 7, 2019; accepted May 10, 2019

Abstract Objective: This study utilized a developmental cascade approach to test alternative theories about the underlying mechanisms behind the association of maternal prenatal stress and child psychopa- thology. The fetal programming hypothesis suggests that prenatal stress affects fetal structural and physiological systems responsible for individual differences in child temperament, which fur- ther increases risk for internalizing and externalizing problems. Interpersonal models of stress transmission suggest that maternal stress influences child mental health via early parenting behav- iors. We also examined a continuation of stress hypothesis, in which prenatal stress predicts child mental health via the continuation of maternal stress in the postpartum period. Methods: Participants were 1,992 mother–child pairs drawn from a prospective cohort. Mothers reported on their perceived stress, anxiety, and depression during pregnancy and at 4-month post- partum. Birthweight was assessed via medical records of birthweight. At 4-month postpartum, hostile-reactive parenting behaviors were assessed. Child temperamental negative affect was mea- sured at age 3. Child internalizing and externalizing problems were assessed at age 5. Results: Prenatal stress was associated with both internalizing and externalizing problems via postnatal stress and child temperament. Prenatal stress was also associated with externalizing behaviors via increased hostile-reactive parenting. After accounting for postnatal factors, prenatal stress contin- ued to have a direct effect on child internalizing, but not externalizing, symptoms. Conclusion: Results provide support for the fetal programming, interpersonal stress transmission, and continu- ation of stress models. Findings highlight the need for prenatal preventative programs that continue into the early postnatal period, targeting maternal stress and parenting behaviors.

Key words: developmental psychopathology; fetal programming; mental health; prenatal stress.

Introduction role in promoting children’s health and well-being. A robust literature within developmental science sug- However, recent advancements in research have sug- gests that the early social environment plays a critical gested that the developmental origins of health and

VC The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] 1 2 Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough, and Madigan disease (DOHaD) can be traced back to events that oc- development through its spillover onto early parent– cur even before birth (Gillman, 2005). For example, infant interactions and relations (Hammen, Shih, & when rodents are exposed to stressors (e.g., electric Brennan, 2004). For example, Hammen et al. (2004) shocks, forced swim tests) during pregnancy, their off- found that the association between maternal depres-

spring exhibit maladjusted behaviors, including anxi- sion and child depression at age 15 years was entirely Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 ety and depressive behaviors, heightened emotional mediated by family and interpersonal stress effects and physiological stress reactivity, and aggression (e.g., parenting). However, much of this research has (Abe et al., 2007; Eaton, Edmonds, Henry, Snellgrove, been conducted with mothers experiencing postnatal & Sloman, 2015). These findings have been replicated depression or anxiety, thus it’s unclear whether the in observational research with humans. For example, same mechanisms underlie the link between prenatal maternal perceived stress, anxiety, and depression dur- stress and child mental health. To our knowledge, ing the prenatal period have been associated with in- only two studies have examined how prenatal stress creased risk of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and maternal mental health impact postnatal parent- (ADHD), depression, and behavior problems ing, with mixed results. Belsky, Ruttle, Boyce, (Madigan et al., 2018; Rodriguez & Bohlin, 2005), Armstrong, & Essex (2015) found that prenatal ma- with some indications that prenatal stress might even ternal stress, defined as a composite of depression, increase the risk for more severe psychotic disorders, marital conflict, and financial stress, did not predict such as (Van Os & Selten, 1998). negative parenting during infancy, whereas Lereya and Wolke (2013) found that prenatal anxiety and de- A Search for Intermediary Factors: Two Proposed pression predicted higher maternal hostile and puni- Models tive parenting during preschool. Parents who are A critical next step is to move beyond cataloguing unable to control their own negative emotional reac- these associations and toward identifying intermediary tions are further proposed to engender child problems mechanisms that explain why and how prenatal stress in regulating their own behaviors (i.e., externalizing influences later child mental health. In examining the problems) and emotions (i.e., internalizing problems; underlying mechanisms, researchers often cite the fetal Grusec & Davidov, 2010). programming hypothesis, which postulates that expo- A notable limitation of the current body of research sure to events or stressors during gestation (e.g., is that most studies tend to examine the association heightened maternal anxiety) “programmes persisting between prenatal stress on either neonatal physiology changes in a range of metabolic, physiological, and and health outcomes (e.g., birth weight), or child tem- structural parameters,” which impact fetal neural de- perament, or child mental health outcomes. velopment and regulatory capabilities (Barker, 1995, Accordingly, there is a significant lack of longitudinal p. 171; O’Donnell, O’Connor, & Glover, 2009). One research that examines how these complementary pro- such “structural parameter” is fetal growth and size. cesses unfold over time. Through a developmental cas- Accordingly, prenatal stress has been associated with cade approach, the primary aim of this article is to a higher risk of prematurity and low birthweight examine the potential downstream consequences of (Bussie`res et al., 2015; Grote et al., 2010). In addition, prenatal stress on child mental health through a longi- research has suggested that prenatal stress alters the tudinal series of proposed underlying mechanisms. An physiological structure of the growing , thereby advantage of a developmental cascade model is that it increases the likelihood of infants and toddlers show- allows for the examination of how early predictors cu- ing greater temperamental negative emotional reactiv- mulatively influence a developmental outcome (e.g., ity (Davis et al., 2004; Madigan et al., 2018), defined child mental health) over time through multiple pro- as early emerging, biologically based individual differ- cesses and systems (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Not ences in negative affect, such as anger, fear, and sad- only is this a more holistic approach to examining the ness (Rothbart, 2011). Notably, low birthweight has developmental evolution of a child exposed to prena- been tied to increased risk of both temperamental neg- tal stress, but it also allows for the identification of ative affect (Baibazarova et al., 2013) and the two multiple developmentally salient targets for interven- broadband dimensions of psychopathology, namely tion. Here, we simultaneously modeled proposed fetal internalizing and externalizing behaviors programming (i.e., birthweight, child temperament) (Aarnoudse-Moens, Weisglas-Kuperus, van and interpersonal transmission effects (i.e., hostile par- Goudoever, & Oosterlaan, 2009). enting). To our knowledge, these alternative hypothe- While much of the recent research on prenatal ses have not been examined in conjunction. stress has operated from the perspective of the fetal programming hypothesis, interpersonal models of Cascades of Stress stress transmission offer the alternative hypothesis In addition, while recent studies often control for post- that maternal stress and depression impact child partum stress (e.g., Neuenschwander et al., 2018), few Prenatal Stress Developmental Cascade 3

< 25 weeks Gestation Birth 4-months 3 years 5 years

Hostile-Reactive b Parenting b Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019

a a Temperamental a Prenatal Stress Birthweight Child Mental Health Negative Affect

c c

Postnatal Stress

Figure 1. Conceptual model of developmental cascade from prenatal stress to child mental health. Pathway (a) denotes pro- posed fetal programming processes; Pathway (b) represents the hypothesized processes from the perspective of interper- sonal models of stress transmission; and Pathway (c) denotes the effect of the continuation of stress into the postpartum period. studies assess the impact of the continuation of mater- that prenatal stress would predict poorer child mental nal stress or mental health concerns into the postpar- health through a set of biologically informed pro- tum period on child socioemotional development and cesses, including lower birth weight and toddler tem- mental health (Madigan et al., 2018). When studies in- peramental negative affect. Next, we hypothesized clude postpartum stress, they generally do so as a sta- that prenatal stress would predict poorer mental tistical covariate or by examining the relative health through its association with higher hostile- predictive roles of prenatal and postnatal stress, inde- reactive parenting during infancy. Finally, we hypoth- pendent of each other (e.g., Neuenschwander et al., esized that prenatal stress would be associated with 2018; O’Connor, Heron, Golding, Glover, & later child mental health problems through the contin- ALSPAC Study Team, 2003). Further, researchers fo- uation of maternal stress during the postpartum pe- cusing on postnatal stress are rarely able to control for riod. Given recent research suggesting that prenatal prenatal stress. Thus, it is unclear whether the effects stress might operate differently across sexes of postnatal stress can be accounted for by the contin- (Braithwaite et al., 2017; Plamondon et al., 2015), we uation of stress from the prenatal period. As a notable also examined whether the developmental cascade exception, Barker, Jaffee, Uher, & Maughan(2011) model differed as a function of child sex. examined the direct and indirect effects of prenatal de- pression and anxiety on child internalizing and exter- nalizing through its association with postpartum Methods maternal anxiety and depression, finding that both de- velopmental periods were important for subsequent Sample child mental health. However, this study did not in- Participants were drawn from an epidemiological pro- clude other potential postnatal mediating mechanisms, spective pregnancy cohort in a large city in western such as parenting and child negative affect. Canada. Pregnant women were recruited through Simultaneously modeling prenatal stress and postnatal community advertising and at local primary health- stress in a developmental cascade model will allow for care offices and the local laboratory services. Inclusion greater understanding of the timing of effects between criteria included being at least 18 years of age, ability maternal stress and child mental health, which is criti- to communicate in English, a current gestational age cal when developing targeted preventive interventions. of less than 25 weeks, and receiving prenatal care. Women with known multiple were ex- cluded. Between August 2008 and July 2011, 3,387 The Current Study women were enrolled in the study. The original study The current study examines the intermediary processes was meant to end after 4-month postpartum, but sub- proposed by different theoretical perspectives in one sequent funding allowed for additional follow-up developmental cascade model (Figure 1), using an epi- assessments. Due to delays in funding acquisition, demiological pregnancy cohort that spans from the some participants had aged out of eligibility at these prenatal period to age five. Specifically, we hypothe- follow-up timepoints, resulting in smaller sample sizes sized that prenatal maternal stress would predict child (see Figure 2 for a flow chart of participant retention). mental health (i.e., internalizing and externalizing) Only participants with data available at age five through three unique pathways. First, we predicted (N ¼ 1992) were included in the current study. 4 Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough, and Madigan

All Our Families (0.5%), or mixed/other (4.1%). Participants were gen- parcipants enrolled erally from middle- to high-socioeconomic back- n=3387 grounds, with 79.6% of participants having a university, college, or trade school degree and 80.8%

with an annual family income level of $70,000 Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 Completed T1: Canadian dollars or more per year (Tough et al., <25 weeks pregnant Withdrawn 2017). All procedures were approved by the n=3362 (99%) parcipants a University of Calgary Research Ethics Board. n=807 (24%)

Pregnancy Completed T2: Measures loss/child death 4 months postpartum n=3057 (90%) Maternal Stress n=34 (1%) During pregnancy and at 4-month postpartum, moth- Acve ers completed the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen, withdrawal Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983), Edinburgh n=339 (10%) Agreed to be contacted for Postnatal Depression Scale (EDPS; Cox, Holden & Passive follow-up research Sagovsky, 1987), and the Spielberger State Anxiety withdrawal n=3223 (95%) Inventory (SAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, n=434 (13%) & Jacobs, 1970). Internal reliabilities were all satisfac- Completed T3: b tory (as range from .80 to .93). Consistent with previ- n=2909 eligible 3 years post birth n=1994 (69%) ous literature conceptualizing perceived stress, anxiety, and depression as indices of prenatal stress (e.g., Belsky et al., 2015; Madigan et al., 2018; Completed T4: n=2818 eligible 5 years post birth Maxwell et al., 2018), these three scales were used as n=1992 (71%) indicators of latent variables of maternal stress during the prenatal and early postnatal periods. Figure 2. Participant flowchart from recruitment through a age five of follow-up. Participants who completed at least Birthweight one prenatal questionnaire but did not complete any fol- low-up surveys, up to 5 years. bSample size at follow-up Maternal pregnancy medical records were available time points decreased due to timing of questionnaire de- for 1,760 of the 1,992 participants and provided infor- velopment and implementation, ethics approval, funding mation regarding infant birthweight (M ¼ 3353.33, constraints, and attrition. Only participants who were still SD ¼ 516.49, range: 595–5071 g) at the time of eligible, based on their child’s age, were mailed follow-up delivery. questionnaires. Adapted and reprinted with permission from Tough et al. (2017). Hostile-Reactive Parenting About 24% (n ¼ 807) of women were considered At the 4-month postpartum assessment period, moth- withdrawn from the study, defined as not completing ers responded to two items (“I have been angry with any assessments after the pregnancy period. my baby when he/she was particularly fussy” and “I Approximately 13% of participants had passively have raised my voice with or shouted at my baby withdrawn from the study, due to geographical moves, when he/she was particularly fussy”) from the lost to follow-up, or unknown reasons. Common rea- Parental Cognitions and Conduct Toward the Infant sons for active withdrawal (10%) included loss of in- Scale (PACOTIS; Boivin et al., 2005) hostile-reactive terest, lack of time, and lack of partner support. A subscale. Item responses were on a scale from 0 (not at small proportion of women (1%) withdrew due to all what I did)to10(exactly what I did). The two pregnancy loss or child death. Participants who were items (r ¼ .77) were averaged together to create a vari- retained at age five were more likely to have lower pa- able of hostile-reactive parenting. rental stress symptoms, have higher educational at- tainment and family incomes, be older, have fewer Child Negative Affect antepartum medical risks, and have infants with lower When children were three years old, mothers com- birthweights. However, differences tended to be small pleted the 12-item Negative Affect subscale of the in magnitude (see Supplementary Table S1 for mean Child Behavior Questionnaire-Very Short Form comparisons). (CBQ-VSF; Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). This scale At the first wave of data collection, the average age assesses the degree to which the child shows high tem- of mothers was 30.87 (SD ¼ 4.36) years. Mothers peramental negative affect, including anger, fear, sad- were predominantly white (81.9%), with smaller per- ness, and general upset. Item responses were on a scale centages of women identifying as Asian (10.7%), from 0 (extremely untrue)to7(extremely true). Latin American (1.6%), black (1.2%), indigenous Internal consistency was adequate (a ¼ .69). Prenatal Stress Developmental Cascade 5

Child Mental Health (Little, 1988). Results of Little’s MCAR test suggested When children were five years old, mothers completed that data were not MCAR, v2 (967) ¼ 1,181.39, p < the Behavior Assessment System for Children-Second .001. Unlike MCAR, there is no way to rigorously test Edition (BASC-2), a widely used assessment of child whether the data are missing at random (MAR) or not psychological problems with established reliability missing at random (NMAR). However, including Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 and validity (Kamphaus & Reynolds, 2007). The cur- covariates thought to be related to missingness or val- rent study focused on the Externalizing (a ¼ .86) and ues on the outcomes (e.g., maternal education, family Internalizing (a ¼ .88) scales of the BASC. All analyses income, maternal stress/depression) can make the were conducted using the combined-sex norm-refer- MAR assumption more likely, thus limiting the poten- enced T scores. tial bias occurring if the data were NMAR (Collins, Schafer, & Kam, 2001; Graham, 2009). Therefore, Covariates missing data were estimated using full information We controlled for child sex (male¼ 1; female ¼ 2), maximum likelihood (FIML), which is robust to con- maternal education, and family income in all analyses. ditions of MAR (Enders & Bandalos, 2001). FIML Maternal education was reported on a scale of 1 (did procedures do not impute scores for missing data but not complete high school) to 6 (completed graduate rather utilize the raw data to establish parameter esti- school), while income was reported in increments of mates (Enders & Bandalos, 2001). The model was es- $10,000 (1 ¼$10,000; 11 ¼$100,000). We also timated in MPlus using the maximum likelihood with controlled for antepartum health risk with the inclu- robust standard errors estimator (MLR). sion of an Antepartum Risk Score (APRS; Alberta Perinatal Health Program, 2009) from the pregnancy medical records. In Alberta, the APRS is completed at Results every birth by the physician or midwife caring for the Descriptive statistics for all study variables are in- mother at the time of delivery. The APRS assigns risk cluded in Table I. Results of the developmental cas- scores to mothers based on a number of pre-existing cade model are shown in Figure 3. Only significant risk factors (e.g., obesity, maternal age <18 or 35 pathways are shown. Note that previously described years), past pregnancy history (e.g., stillbirths), current covariates were included as predictors of all longitudi- pregnancy complications (e.g., gestational diabetes or nal pathways but are not shown for clarity of presen- hypertension), and other risk factors (e.g., substance tation. Consistent with best practices, covariances use). A weighted value is assigned to each condition were also estimated between identical measurements based on the severity of the condition, and all values across time but are not shown in the figure (Marsh, are then summed to create a total risk score. Based on Morin, Parker, & Kaur, 2014). Fit indices suggested prior validation (Parboosingh, 1986), scores of 0 to 2 that the model provided a good representation of the are considered low risk, scores of 3–6 indicate moder- data (v2 [47, N ¼ 1,933] ¼ 117.23, root mean square ate risk, and scores of 7 or more reflect high risk. of error of approximation (RMSEA) ¼ .03, compara- tive fit index (CFI) ¼ 0.99, Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) Analytic Plan ¼ 0.98). To examine the direct and indirect associations between prenatal stress and child internalizing and externalizing Direct Effects problems at age five through the set of hypothesized As shown in Figure 3, prenatal maternal stress had a mediating mechanisms, we conducted a structural equa- direct effect on child internalizing at age five, b ¼ .16, tion model (SEM) in MPlus 8.0 (Muthen & Muthen, p < .001, but not externalizing symptoms, b ¼ .05, 1998–2017). Specifically, we examined the longitudinal p ¼ .15. In addition, prenatal stress negatively pre- relations between: (a) prenatal maternal stress at dicted birthweight, b ¼.07, p ¼ .008, and positively <25 weeks gestation; (b) birthweight; (c) maternal post- predicted both postpartum stress, b ¼ .60, p < .001, natal stress and hostile-reactive parenting at 4 months; and hostile-reactive parenting at four months, b ¼ .26, (d) child negative affect at 3 years; and (e) child inter- p < .001, and child negative affect at three years, b ¼ nalizing and externalizing symptoms at age five.All .11, p ¼ .002. Hostile-reactive parenting, in turn, pre- possible predictive pathways between exogenous and dicted higher child externalizing problems at age five, endogenous variables were estimated, as were covarian- b ¼ .12, p ¼ .001. Postnatal stress positively predicted ces between predictors assessed at the same time point child negative affect at three years, b ¼ .14, p ¼ .001, (e.g., internalizing and externalizing at 5 years). and both child internalizing b ¼ .09, p ¼ .01, and ex- ternalizing, b ¼ .11, p ¼ .001, symptoms at age five. Missing Data Birthweight did not predict subsequent parent (i.e., To evaluate whether data were missing completely at parenting, stress) or child functioning (i.e., tempera- random (MCAR), we conducted Little’s MCAR test ment, internalizing, externalizing). Finally, higher 6 Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough, and Madigan

¼ temperamental negative affect at age three predicted both higher internalizing, b ¼ .32, p < .001, and ex- 3 years; b < ¼ ternalizing, ¼ .24, p .001, symptoms at age five.

Indirect Effects Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 Next, we examined the indirect effects of prenatal stress on child internalizing and externalizing symp- toms through its association with birthweight, parent- ing, postpartum stress, and child negative affect. Indirect effects were estimated using the bootstrapping 4 months postpartum; T3

¼ procedure in MPlus 8.0. Birthweight did not predict subsequent child or parental functioning and were therefore not included in the indirect effect estimates. Results for all standardized estimated indirect .04 .03 .77* .05* .74* .77* .05 .34* .32* .27* effects are presented in Table II. There were four sig- Perceived Stress Scale (possible range: 0–40); EDPS nificant indirect effects from prenatal maternal stress ¼ to child externalizing symptoms. First, there were indi- 25 weeks gestation; T2 .10*

< rect effects from prenatal stress to child externalizing

¼ through hostile-reactive parenting, b ¼ .03, p ¼ .002, postnatal maternal stress, b ¼ .07, p ¼ .001, and child .08* temperamental negative affect, b ¼ .03, p ¼ .003. In addition, the indirect pathway between prenatal stress, postnatal stress, child negative affect, and child exter- .06*

nalizing symptoms was also significant, b ¼ .02, p ¼ Antepartum Risk Score; PSS .001. There were three significant indirect effects be- ¼ tween prenatal stress and child internalizing symptoms .18* .18* .74* .14* .69* .71* .16* .53* .48* .47* .12* .44* .48* .43* .13* .45* .43* .52* .08* .23* .18* .23* .12* .22* .21* .18* .02 .22* .20* .18* .10* .06* .15* .16* .15* .04 .21* .20* .19* .19* .28* .05* .23* .24* .20* .01 .20* .24* .11* .11* .39* .51* at age five. Specifically, there were indirect effects through postnatal maternal stress, b ¼ .05, p ¼ .01, 34567891011121314 .08* .06* .06* .03 .02 .01 .05* .07* .02 and child negative affect at age three, b ¼ .03, p ¼ female. APRS

¼ .002. The indirect pathway between prenatal stress, postnatal stress, child negative affect, and child inter- .03 .03 .30* .17* .02 .04 .01 .04 .01 nalizing symptoms was also significant, b ¼ .03, p < male; 2

¼ .001. .02 .09* .02 .09* .05 .19* .01 Sex Differences We conducted multigroup comparison analyses to ex- Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory (possible range: 20–80); T1

78 .00 .11* plore whether the effects of prenatal stress differed ¼ according to child sex. First, we ran the SEM simulta- Range 1 2 neously for males and females, specifying a fully con- strained model in which the coefficient of every structural path was set to be equal across males and females. We then compared the relative fit of this model to a nested model in which all the structural paths were free to vary between males and females. MSD The free-to-vary model did not evidence significantly better fit than the constrained model (Dv2 ¼ 25.98, Ddf ¼ 19, p ¼ .13, DRMSEA ¼ .004), showing that the results of the model were comparable across male and female children in the sample.

Follow-Up Sensitivity Analyses To ensure that results were not biased by only in- cluding participants who had data available at age . Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Among the Variables in the Primary Analyses of the Study .05. . Range values reflect observed range, not all possible values. Child sex: 1 five, we also reran the model using the full sample < 5 years. p

¼ of 3,387 participants, using FIML to estimate miss- Note * Table I Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (possible range:T4 0–30); SAI 1. Child sex2. APRS3. Maternal education4. Family income5. T1 PSS 4.00 1.04 9.39 1.94 1–6 2.36 2.04 13.12 1–11 .02 0–14 6.13 1–2 .00 0–36 .01 .08* 6. T1 EPDS 4.90 4.22 0–24 .02 .09* 7. T1 SAI 30.35 8.36 20–73 .01 .12* 8. Birthweight (g) 3353 516 595–5071 9. T2 PSS 11.49 6.44 0–40 10. T2 EPDS 4.03 4.09 0–30 .00 .05* 11. T2 SAI 29.70 8.81 20 12. T2 hostile-reactive parenting 1.39 1.97 0–10 13. T3 negative affect 3.62 .79 1–7 .06* .01 14. T4 externalizing 49.21 8.24 33–93 15. T4 internalizing 50.46ing data. 9.55 28–98 The .03 pattern of results was the same, with Prenatal Stress Developmental Cascade 7

Prenatal Birth 4 Months 3 Years 5 Years

.11**

Hostile-Reactive .11*** Externalizing Parenting .26*** Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019

.24*** -.07** Prenatal Stress *** Birthweight .26 .11*** Negative .45*** Affect .85*** .83*** .86*** .32*** *** *** PSS EPDS SAI .60 .14

.09** Postnatal Stress Internalizing

.86*** .85*** .90***

PSS EPDS SAI .16***

Figure 3. Standardized path coefficients in developmental cascade from maternal prenatal stress to child internalizing and externalizing symptoms at age five. Only significant pathways are shown. EDPS ¼ Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale; PSS ¼ Perceived Stress Scale; SAI ¼ Spielberger State Anxiety Inventory. **p .01; ***p .001.

Table II . Indirect Effects of Prenatal Stress to Child prenatal stress predicted both externalizing and inter- Externalizing and Internalizing Problems nalizing problems through child negative affect, sup- b 95% CI porting the fetal programming hypothesis. Prenatal stress also predicted externalizing problems at age five To externalizing through its effect on hostile-reactive parenting during Direct effect .05 [.02, .11] Specific indirect effects infancy, supporting interpersonal transmission mod- Hostile-reactive parenting .03 [.01, .05] els. We did not find evidence of sex differences. While Postnatal stress .07 [.03, .10] some evidence suggests that prenatal stress might have Negative affect .03 [.01, .04] different effects on males and females (Braithwaite Hostile-reactive parenting ! .00 [.004, .004] et al., 2017), our finding is consistent with a recent negative affect Postnatal stress ! negative affect .02 [.01, .03] meta-analysis on prenatal stress and child socioemo- Total indirect effect .14 [.10, .18] tional outcomes, which failed to find gender differen- Total effect .18 [.13, .23] ces (Madigan et al., 2018). To internalizing According to fetal programming hypotheses, expo- Direct effect .16 [.10, .22] sure to stressors in utero may alter the growing physi- Specific indirect effects Hostile-reactive parenting .00 [.01, .02] ological systems responsible for aspects of Postnatal stress .05 [.01, .09] temperament, such as the hypothalamus–adrenal–pi- Negative affect .03 [.01, .06] tuitary (HPA) axis (Gartstein & Skinner, 2018). Hostile-reactive parenting ! .00 [.01, .01] However, few studies have simultaneously modeled negative affect the effects of prenatal stress and of postnatal stress on Postnatal stress ! negative affect .03 [.01, .04] Total indirect effect .11 [.08, .15] child temperament. Thus, it is notable that the current Total effect .27 [.22, .33] study found a significant effect of both prenatal stress and postnatal stress on child negative affect. As tem- Note. Bolded values represent significant effects. perament is thought to be at least partially genetically determined (Rothbart, 2011), these results might un- some minor differences in standardized effect sizes, derscore an underlying genetic component that gives ranging from .00 to .03 (see Supplementary Figure S1). rise to heightened maternal stress, anxiety, and depres- sion in both the pre- and postnatal periods as well as child temperamental negative affect. Higher toddler Discussion negative affect, in turn, increased the risk for both in- Using a developmental cascade model capturing stage- ternalizing and externalizing problems at age five. salient processes at five timepoints, this study provided Providing partial support for the interpersonal support for both the fetal programming and interper- transmission of stress models, we found that prenatal sonal models of stress transmission. Specifically, stress was associated with increased hostile-reactive 8 Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough, and Madigan parenting at four months. Hostile-reactive parenting suggested that low birthweight infants are at risk for during infancy, meanwhile, predicted greater external- greater internalizing and externalizing problems at izing, but not internalizing, problems at age five. This school age, much of this research has focused on very is also in line with coercive process models, which preterm infants born prior to 32 weeks old and with

posit that early hostile and over-reactive parenting birthweights less than 1,500 g (see Bhutta, Cleves, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 engenders increased noncompliance and difficultness Casey, Cradock, & Anand, 2002 for a meta-analysis). from the child, which further increases hostile parent- Samples characterized by extreme values (e.g., ing (Patterson, 2002). Over time, this cyclical and es- <1,500 g) can result in higher correlations between calating process results in the development of variables, increasing the potential for inflated or bi- externalizing problems characterized by greater ag- ased effect sizes (MacCallum, Zhang, Preacher, & gression, impulsivity, and/or inattention. To our Rucker, 2002). Thus, while extremely low birthweight knowledge, this is the first study to show that prenatal infants might be at risk for psychological problems, stress is associated with externalizing problems five our results suggest that birthweight measured along a years later via hostile parenting in early infancy. continuous scale in an epidemiological sample does Taken together, these results highlight concepts not impart greater risk of mental health problems at within the developmental psychopathology perspec- age five. However, it is possible that there is an indi- tive, which posits that there are a multitude of path- rect effect of low birthweight on child psychopathol- ways and processes that contribute to the ogy through other intermediary variables not assessed development of adaptive or maladaptive behaviors in this study (e.g., neurocognitive development). (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). For example, the princi- Future research should explore this possibility. ple of multifinality suggests that a single starting point (i.e., prenatal stress) can diverge into multiple path- ways and child outcomes (e.g., externalizing and inter- Implications nalizing), while the principle of equifinality suggests Increasing evidence and awareness about the effects of that a single outcome can be reached through a multi- prenatal stress and depression on the developing child tude of pathways or processes (e.g., externalizing has led to calls to develop effective intervention and problems predicted by pathways involving prenatal prevention programs for pregnant women specifically maternal stress, postpartum stress, hostile parenting, aimed at curbing prenatal stress (Nolvi et al., 2016; and child negative affect). By using a developmental Zhang, Cui, Zhou, & Li, 2019). However, interven- cascade model, the current study showcases both con- tions are generally constrained to the pregnancy pe- cepts, underscoring the complex psychobiological pro- riod, and few intervention studies have included cesses that occur between prenatal maternal stress and follow-up assessments into the postpartum period, later child mental health problems. particularly in regard to child socioemotional out- In line with previous research (Bussie`res et al., 2015; comes. Meanwhile, interventions aimed at reducing Grote et al., 2010), prenatal stress was also associated postpartum depression in high-risk samples generally with poorer fetal growth in the form of lower birth- start after birth or in late pregnancy (Sangsawang, weight. Contrary to our expectations, birthweight did Wacharasin, & Sangsawang, 2019). As the current not predict later negative affect at age three or mental study underscores, however, maternal stress, anxiety, health problems at age five. Although research has sug- and depression are relatively stable and are thus likely gested that low birthweight infants have more difficult to occur during both pregnancy and the postnatal pe- temperament characteristics (e.g., Baibazarova et al., riod as well. While prenatal stress had a direct effect 2013), these studies have almost exclusively measured on child internalizing problems at age five in the cur- temperament during infancy, when temperament is rent study, prenatal stress was only indirectly associ- more labile and subject to change compared with tod- ated with child externalizing problems via postnatal dlerhood (Durbin, Hayden, Klein, & Olino, 2007). processes (e.g., postnatal stress, early parenting). Thus, it is possible that low birthweight predisposes Results highlight the complex, dynamic processes that infants to higher negative emotional reactivity, but that occur between prenatal stress and increased risk for this effect is not long-lasting and may not represent sta- child mental health problems. While the fetal pro- ble temperamental characteristics. Indeed, one study gramming and interpersonal stress transmission mod- found that irritability (a form of temperamental nega- els are often discussed and examined separately, tive affect) was moderately stable from three to seven results from the current study suggest that they occur months among full-term infants but evidenced no sta- simultaneously in a mutually informative manner. bility among preterm, low birthweight infants (Garcia Therefore, intervention and prevention programs may Coll, Halpern, Vohr, Seifer, & Oh, 1992). be more effective if they continue into the postpartum It is also noteworthy that our study did not find period for high-risk women and also include psycho- long-lasting implications of birthweight on child men- educational components and parenting supports that tal health problems. While previous research has foster positive parent–child relationships. Future Prenatal Stress Developmental Cascade 9 research should also attempt to capture other theoreti- stress and child mental health problems. Prenatal cal models of stress transmission, including niche se- stress continued to exert a direct effect on internalizing lection, which proposes that biological (e.g., fetal problems at age five, even after controlling for postna- programming), environmental (e.g., parenting), and tal stress, birthweight, hostile-reactive parenting, and

individual (e.g., temperament) characteristics mutually child negative affect. However, prenatal stress was Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 influence an individual to self-select into specific only indirectly related to child behavior problems at niches or situations (e.g., problematic peer groups) age five, through multiple pathways, including postna- that operate to maintain continuity of stress over time tal stress, hostile parenting, and child negative affect. (Ellman, Murphy, & Maxwell, 2018; Wachs, 1996). Study results highlight the potential benefit of preven- tative mental health programs that start during the prenatal period and continue into the early infancy Limitations period. Several limitations of the current study also merit dis- cussion. First, the current sample was a relatively low- risk community sample that evidenced high incomes Supplementary Data and maternal educational status, so our results may Supplementary data can be found at: https://academic.oup. not generalize to more at-risk or specialized samples com/jpepsy. (e.g., adolescent mothers). Second, although we in- cluded medical reports of child birthweight and mater- nal medical risk, the majority of our constructs were Funding measured by maternal report. Therefore, we cannot All Our Families are funded through Alberta rule out the possibility of reporter bias. For example, Innovates Interdisciplinary Team Grant 200700595, perceptions of child temperamental negative affect the Alberta Children’s Hospital Foundation, and the could be influenced by the mother’s own psychopa- Max Bell Foundation. Dr. Hentges is supported by a thology and negative affect (i.e., depression, anxiety). fellowship from the Talisman Energy Fund in Support Thus, future research should attempt to utilize multi- of Healthy Living and Injury Prevention and the informant and multimethod constructs of maternal Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute stress, parenting, temperament, and child mental through the Owerko Centre. health symptoms. Third, while our assessment of pre- natal stress is consistent with prior literature, distinct Conflicts of interest: None declared. sources of maternal stress (e.g., depression, anxiety, negative life events) may operate differently on child References outcomes and should be explored further in future re- Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S. H., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van search. Fourth, only hostile-reactive parenting was Goudoever, J. B., & Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis measured in the sample at 4 months. Our construct is of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very consistent with other prior studies, which have low birth weight children. Pediatrics, 124, 717–728. assessed the association between prenatal stress and Abe, H., Hidaka, N., Kawagoe, C., Odagiri, K., Watanabe, harsh or hostile parenting behaviors (e.g., Belsky Y., Ikeda, T., ...Ishida, Y. (2007). Prenatal psychological et al., 2015; Lereya & Wolke, 2013). However, it is stress causes higher emotionality, depression-like behav- possible that other domains of parenting (e.g., sensitiv- ior, and elevated activity in the hypothalamo-pituitary- ity) could also be involved in the developmental cas- adrenal axis. Neuroscience Research, 59, 145–151. cade between prenatal stress and child mental health Alberta Perinatal Health Program. (2009). Delivery problems. In addition, temperament was only mea- Record—Part One: Antenatal Risk Assessment. Retrieved sured once at 36 months, which precludes our ability from http://aphp.dapasoft.com/PublicHTML/doc/AB %20Del%20Rec%20HS0001-126-1.pdf to examine the effect of infant temperament on Baibazarova, E., van de Beek, C., Cohen-Kettenis, P. T., hostile-reactive parenting behaviors. Finally, although Buitelaar, J., Shelton, K. H., & van Goozen, S. H. (2013). previous studies have argued that links between prena- Influence of prenatal maternal stress, maternal plasma cor- tal stress and both birthweight and child temperament tisol and in the amniotic fluid on birth outcomes support the fetal programming hypothesis, physiologi- and child temperament at 3 months. cal indices of neonatal functioning may be more ro- Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 907–915. bust indicators of programming effects and should be Barker, D. J. (1995). Fetal origins of coronary heart disease. explored in future developmental cascade approaches. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 311, 171–174. Barker, E. D., Jaffee, S. R., Uher, R., & Maughan, B. (2011). The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxi- Conclusions ety and depression to child maladjustment. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 696–702. The current study provides support for multiple mech- Belsky, J., Ruttle, P. L., Boyce, W. T., Armstrong, J. M., & anisms of risk in explaining the links between prenatal Essex, M. J. (2015). Early adversity, elevated stress 10 Hentges, Graham, Plamondon, Tough, and Madigan

physiology, accelerated sexual maturation, and poor Gartstein, M. A., & Skinner, M. K. (2018). Prenatal influen- health in females. Developmental Psychology, 51, ces on temperament development: The role of environmen- 816–822. tal epigenetics. Development and Psychopathology, 30, Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., 1269–1303. & Anand, K. J. S. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral out- Gillman, M. W. (2005). Developmental origins of health and comes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A disease. The New England Journal of Medicine, 353, Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 meta-analysis. JAMA, 288, 728–737. 1848–1850. Boivin, M., Perusse, D., Dionne, G., Saysset, V., Zoccolillo, Graham, J. W. (2009). Missing data analysis: Making it M., Tarabulsy, G. M., ...Tremblay, R. E. (2005). The ge- work in the real world. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, netic-environmental etiology of parents’ perceptions and 549–576. self-assessed behaviours toward their 5-month-old infants Grote, N. K., Bridge, J. A., Gavin, A. R., Melville, J. L., in a large twin and singleton sample. Journal of Child Iyengar, S., & Katon, W. J. (2010). A meta-analysis of de- Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 612–630. pression during pregnancy and the risk of preterm birth, Braithwaite, E. C., Pickles, A., Sharp, H., Glover, V., low birth weight, and intrauterine growth restriction. O’Donnell, K. J., Tibu, F., & Hill, J. (2017). Maternal pre- Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 1012–1024. natal cortisol predicts infant negative emotionality in a Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different sex-dependent manner. Physiology & Behavior, 175, perspectives on socialization theory and research: A do- 31–36. main-specific approach. Child Development, 81, 687 Bussie`res, E. L., Tarabulsy, G. M., Pearson, J., Tessier, R., Hammen, C., Shih, J. H., & Brennan, P. A. (2004). Forest, J. C., & Gigue`re, Y. (2015). Maternal prenatal Intergenerational transmission of depression: Test of an in- stress and infant birth weight and gestational age: A meta- terpersonal stress model in a community sample. Journal analysis of prospective studies. Developmental Review, of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 511–522. 36, 179–199. Kamphaus, R. W., & Reynolds, C. R. (2007). BASC-2 be- Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and mul- havioral and emotional screening system manual. Circle tifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development Pines, MN: Pearson. and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600. Lereya, S. T., & Wolke, D. (2013). Prenatal family adversity Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global and maternal mental health and vulnerability to peer vic- measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social timisation at school. Journal of Child Psychology and Behavior, 24, 385–396. Psychiatry, 54, 644–652. Garcia Coll, C. T., Halpern, L. F., Vohr, B. R., Seifer, R., & Little, R. J. (1988). A test of missing completely at random Oh, W. (1992). Stability and correlates of change of early for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the temperament in preterm and full-term infants. Infant American Statistical Association, 83, 1198–1202. Behavior and Development, 15, 137–153. MacCallum, R. C., Zhang, S., Preacher, K. J., & Rucker, D. Collins, L. M., Schafer, J. L., & Kam, C. M. (2001). A com- D. (2002). On the practice of dichotomization of quantita- parison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in modern tive variables. Psychological Methods, 7, 19–40. missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, Madigan, S., Oatley, H., Racine, N., Fearon, R. P., 330–351. Schumacher, L., Akbari, E., ... Tarabulsy, G. M. (2018). Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection A meta-analysis of maternal prenatal depression and anxi- of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item ety on child socio-emotional development. Journal of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57, of Psychiatry, 150, 782–786. 645–657. Davis, E. P., Snidman, N., Wadhwa, P. D., Glynn, L. M., Marsh, H. W., Morin, A. J., Parker, P. D., & Kaur, G. Schetter, C. D., & Sandman, C. A. (2004). Prenatal mater- (2014). Exploratory structural equation modeling: An in- nal anxiety and depression predict negative behavioral re- tegration of the best features of exploratory and confirma- activity in infancy. Infancy, 6, 319–331. tory factor analysis. Annual Review of Clinical Durbin, C. E., Hayden, E. P., Klein, D. N., & Olino, T. M. Psychology, 10, 85–110. (2007). Stability of laboratory-assessed temperamental Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cas- emotionality traits from ages 3 to 7. Emotion, 7, 388–399. cades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491–495. Eaton, L., Edmonds, E. J., Henry, T. B., Snellgrove, D. L., & Maxwell, S. D., Fineberg, A. M., Drabick, D. A., Murphy, S. Sloman, K. A. (2015). Mild maternal stress disrupts asso- K., & Ellman, L. M. (2018). Maternal prenatal stress and ciative learning and increases aggression in offspring. other developmental risk factors for adolescent depression: Hormones and Behavior, 71, 10–15. Spotlight on sex differences. Journal of Abnormal Child Ellman, L. M., Murphy, S. K., & Maxwell, S. D. (2018). Pre- Psychology, 46, 381–397. and perinatal risk factors for serious mental disorders: Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus User’s Ethical considerations in prevention and prediction efforts. Guide. Eighth Edition. Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Journal of Ethics in Mental Health, 10, 1–14. Muthen. Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative perfor- Neuenschwander, R., Hookenson, K., Brain, U., Grunau, R. mance of full information maximum likelihood estimation E., Devlin, A. M., Weinberg, J., ... Oberlander, T. F. for missing data in structural equation models. Structural (2018). Children’s stress regulation mediates the associa- Equation Modeling, 8, 430–457. tion between prenatal maternal mood and child executive Prenatal Stress Developmental Cascade 11

functions for boys, but not girls. Development and Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, Psychopathology, 30, 953–969. 102–112. Nolvi, S., Karlsson, L., Bridgett, D. J., Korja, R., Huizink, A. Rodriguez, A., & Bohlin, G. (2005). Are maternal smoking C., Kataja, E. L., & Karlsson, H. (2016). Maternal prena- and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms tal stress and infant emotional reactivity six months post- in children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, partum. Journal of Affective Disorders, 199, 163–170. 46, 246–254. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jpepsy/advance-article-abstract/doi/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz044/5511639 by University of Calgary user on 13 June 2019 O’Connor, T. G., Heron, J., Golding, J., & Glover, V; Rothbart, M. K. (2011). Becoming who we are: ALSPAC Study Team. (2003). Maternal antenatal anxiety Temperament and personality in development. New York: and behavioural/emotional problems in children: A test of Guilford Press. a programming hypothesis. Journal of Child Psychology Sangsawang, B., Wacharasin, C., & Sangsawang, N. and Psychiatry, 44, 1025–1036. (2019). Interventions for the prevention of postpartum O’Donnell, K., O’Connor, T., & Glover, V. (2009). Prenatal depression in adolescent mothers: a systematic review. stress and neurodevelopment of the child: Focus on the Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 22, 215–228. HPA axis and role of the placenta. Developmental Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E., Vagg, P. Neuroscience, 31, 285–292. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1970). State-trait anxiety inventory. Parboosingh, I. J. (1986). The role of standardized risk as- Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. sessment in the provision of prenatal care. Canadian Tough, S. C., McDonald, S. W., Collisson, B. A., Graham, S. Family Physician, 32, 2115–2120. A., Kehler, H., Kingston, D., & Benzies, K. (2017). Cohort Patterson, G. R. (2002). The early development of coercive profile: The all our babies pregnancy cohort (AOB). family process. In J. B. Reid, G. R. Patterson, & J. Snyder International Journal of Epidemiology, 46, 1389–1390k. (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A Van Os, J., & Selten, J. P. (1998). Prenatal exposure to ma- developmental analysis and model for intervention (pp. ternal stress and subsequent schizophrenia: The May 1940 25–44). Washington, DC: American Psychological invasion of the Netherlands. The British Journal of Association. Psychiatry, 172, 324–326. Plamondon, A., Akbari, E., Atkinson, L., Steiner, M., Wachs, T. D. (1996). Known and potential processes under- Meaney, M. J., Fleming, A. S., & Mavan, rt. (2015). lying developmental trajectories in childhood and adoles- Spatial working memory and attention skills are predicted cence. Developmental Psychology, 32, 796–801. by maternal stress during pregnancy. Early Human Zhang, J. Y., Cui, Y. X., Zhou, Y. Q., & Li, Y. L. (2019). Development, 91, 23–29. Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction on prenatal Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development of stress, anxiety and depression. Psychology, Health & short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Medicine, 24, 51–58.