Perception of the Missing Fundamental

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Perception of the Missing Fundamental PERCEPTION OF THE MISSING FUNDAMENTAL IN PATIENTS WITH LOW FREQUENCY HEARING LOSS By GARY PAUL HORVATH B.Sc. (Hons.), The University of British Columbia, 1984 A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE in THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES (Department of Physiology) We accept this thesis as conforming to the required standard THE UNIVERSITY OF BRITISH COLUMBIA October 1988 (c)Gary Paul Horvath, 1988 In presenting this thesis in partial fulfilment of the requirements for an advanced degree at the University of British Columbia, I agree that the Library shall make it freely available for reference and study. I further agree that permission for extensive copying of this thesis for scholarly purposes may be granted by the head of my department or by his or her representatives. It is understood that copying or publication of this thesis for financial gain shall not be allowed without my written permission. Department of Physiology The University of British Columbia 1956 Main Mall Vancouver, Canada V6T 1Y3 Date October 17, 1988 DE-6G/81) ABSTRACT The perception of the pitch of a harmonic complex tone missing its fundamental frequency was investigated in patients with unilateral sensorineural low-frequency hearing loss. Using a two-alternative forced choice adaptive method and an adjustment technique, patients matched the pitch of a complex tone consisting of the 3rd-7th or 3rd-10th harmonics, with a pure tone. Component frequencies were chosen such that they would fall within the intact portion of the patient's damaged ear, but whose fundamental frequency would fall within the damaged frequency area. Pitch matches were made monaurally and binaurally, with and without low-pass filtered noise. Results indicated that the patients tended to match the pitch of the lacking fundamentals to the complexes even though the frequency of the residue pitch fell within the range of elevated pure tone thresholds. It is concluded that information regarding the residue pitch is not mediated by cochlear nerve fibers with characteristic frequencies corresponding to the fundamental. Temporal cues carried by fibers with characteristic frequencies corresponding to the partials within the complex stimulus are most likely involved in pitch perception. Ii TARLE OF CONTENTS Abstract i i List of Abbreviations v List of Tables vi List of Figures viii Acknowledgements xi Introduction 1 Methods : 17 A. Subjects 17 B. Stimuli 18 C. Procedure 19 Pure Tone Audiogram 19 Pitch Training 20 Pitch Matching of Complex Tones Using a Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 21 Pitch Matching of Complex Tones Using an Up-Down Adjustment Technique 28 D. Statistical Analysis 30 Results 31 Pure Tone Audiograms 31 Pitch Matching of Complex Tones Using a Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method....38 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS (CONT.1 i) Pitch Matching Control 99 ii) Pitch Matching Control with Noise 99 iii) Pitch Matching Between Ears 100 iv) Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise 101 Pitch Matching of Complex Tones Using an Up-Down Adjustment Technique 102 i) Pitch Matching Control 102 ii) Pitch Matching Control with Noise 113 iii) Pitch Matching Between Ears 113 iv) Pitch Matching Between Ears with No ise 114 Discuss ion 123 Bibliography 140 iv LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS approx. approximation bel. below betw. between dB decibel, freq. frequency fund. fundamental har. harmonic HL hearing level Hz hertz kHz kilohertz mm millimeter msec millisecond MHz megahertz oct. octave P.M. pitch matching sec sec SEM standard error of the mean seq. sequence SL sensation level SPL sound pressure level start. starting w. with v T.TST DF' TABLES Table 1: Stepsizes of the Comparison Pure Tone (£2) 26 Table 2: Decrease in Threshold with Increasing Frequency for Each Patient... 36 Table 3: Selection of Components for the Complex Test Sound Used in the Pitch Matching Experiments 37 Table 4: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient A.D. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 87 Table 5: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient A.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 89 Table 6: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient G.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 91 Table 7: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient L.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 93 Table 8: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient R.L. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 95 Table 9: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient J.W. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Routine 97 vi LIST OF TABLES (CONT.) Table 10: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient A.D. Using the Up-Down Adjustment Technique 103 Table 11: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient A.F. Using the Up-Down Adjustment Technique 105 Table 12: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient G.F. Using the Up-Down Adjustment Technique 107 Table 13: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient L.F. Using the Up-Down Adjustment Technique 109 Table 14: Pitch Values Assigned to the Pitch of the Complex Test Sound by Patient R.L. Using the Up-Down Adjustment Technique Ill Table 15: Summary of the Number of Matches to either the Fundamental and its Octaves, or to the Odd Harmonics and the Octaves below these Partials 116 Table 16: Summary of the Number o£ Matches to either the Missing Fundamental, or to the Presented Partials 120 vii T.TBT OF FIGURES Figure 1: Damaged and Intact Ear Audiograms of Patients A.D., A.F., and G.F 32 Figure 2: Damaged and Intact Ear Audiograms of Patients L.F., R.L., and J.W 34 Figure 3: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient A.D. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 39 Figure 4: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient A.D. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 41 Figure 5: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient A.D. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 43 Figure 6: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient A.D. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 45 Figure 7: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient A.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 47 Figure 8: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient A.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 49 Figure 9: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient A.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 51 Figure 10: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient A.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 53 viii LIST OF FIGURES (CONT.) Figure 11: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient G.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 55 Figure 12: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient G.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 57 Figure 13: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient G.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 59 Figure 14: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient G.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 61 Figure 15: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient L.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 63 Figure 16: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient L.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 65 Figure 17: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient L.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 67 Figure 18: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient L.F. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 69 Figure 19: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient R.L. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 71 lx LIST OF FIGURES (CONT.) Figure 20: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient R.L. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 73 Figure 21: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient R.L. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 75 Figure 22: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient R.L. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 77 Figure 23: Data from the Pitch Matching Control Procedure for Patient J.W. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method ...79 Figure 24: Data from the Pitch Matching Control with Noise Procedure for Patient J.W. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 81 Figure 25: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears Procedure for Patient J.W. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 83 Figure 26: Data from the Pitch Matching Between Ears with Noise Procedure for Patient J.W. Using the Two-Alternative Forced Choice Adaptive Method 85 x ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my sincere gratitude to my supervisor, Dr. Dietrich Schwarz, for his encouragement and guidance throughout the duration of this project. I would also like to thank Ward Tomlinson and Joseph Li for their expert advice and help with instrumentation and computation.
Recommended publications
  • L Atdment OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36
    *;JiQYL~dW~llbk~ieira - - ~-- -, - ., · LAtDMENT OFFICF QO9ENT ROOM 36 ?ESEARC L0ORATORY OF RL C.f:'__ . /j16baV"BLI:1!S INSTITUTE 0i'7Cn' / PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF THE PITCH OF COMPLEX TONES ADRIANUS J. M. HOUTSMA JULIUS L. GOLDSTEIN LO N OPY TECHNICAL REPORT 484 OCTOBER I, 1971 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS CAMBRIDGE, MASSACHUSETTS 02139 The Research Laboratory of Electronics is an interdepartmental laboratory in which faculty members and graduate students from numerous academic departments conduct research. The research reported in this document was made possible in part by support extended the Massachusetts Institute of Tech- nology, Research Laboratory of Electronics, by the JOINT SER- VICES ELECTRONICS PROGRAMS (U. S. Army, U. S. Navy, and U.S. Air Force) under Contract No. DAAB07-71-C-0300, and by the National Institutes of Health (Grant 5 PO1 GM14940-05). Requestors having DOD contracts or grants should apply for copies of technical reports to the Defense Documentation Center, Cameron Station, Alexandria, Virginia 22314; all others should apply to the Clearinghouse for Federal Scientific and Technical Information, Sills Building, 5285 Port Royal Road, Springfield, Virginia 22151. THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN APPROVED FOR PUBLIC RELEASE AND SALE; ITS DISTRIBUTION IS UNLIMITED. MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY RESEARCH LABORATORY OF ELECTRONICS Technical Report 484 October 1, 1971 PERCEPTION OF MUSICAL INTERVALS: EVIDENCE FOR THE CENTRAL ORIGIN OF COMPLEX TONES Adrianus J. M. Houtsma and Julius L. Goldstein This report is based on a thesis by A. J. M. Houtsma submitted to the Department of Electrical Engineering at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, June 1971, in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • Contribution of the Conditioning Stage to the Total Harmonic Distortion in the Parametric Array Loudspeaker
    Universidad EAFIT Contribution of the conditioning stage to the Total Harmonic Distortion in the Parametric Array Loudspeaker Andrés Yarce Botero Thesis to apply for the title of Master of Science in Applied Physics Advisor Olga Lucia Quintero. Ph.D. Master of Science in Applied Physics Science school Universidad EAFIT Medellín - Colombia 2017 1 Contents 1 Problem Statement 7 1.1 On sound artistic installations . 8 1.2 Objectives . 12 1.2.1 General Objective . 12 1.2.2 Specific Objectives . 12 1.3 Theoretical background . 13 1.3.1 Physics behind the Parametric Array Loudspeaker . 13 1.3.2 Maths behind of Parametric Array Loudspeakers . 19 1.3.3 About piezoelectric ultrasound transducers . 21 1.3.4 About the health and safety uses of the Parametric Array Loudspeaker Technology . 24 2 Acquisition of Sound from self-demodulation of Ultrasound 26 2.1 Acoustics . 26 2.1.1 Directionality of Sound . 28 2.2 On the non linearity of sound . 30 2.3 On the linearity of sound from ultrasound . 33 3 Signal distortion and modulation schemes 38 3.1 Introduction . 38 3.2 On Total Harmonic Distortion . 40 3.3 Effects on total harmonic distortion: Modulation techniques . 42 3.4 On Pulse Wave Modulation . 46 4 Loudspeaker Modelling by statistical design of experiments. 49 4.1 Characterization Parametric Array Loudspeaker . 51 4.2 Experimental setup . 52 4.2.1 Results of PAL radiation pattern . 53 4.3 Design of experiments . 56 4.3.1 Placket Burmann method . 59 4.3.2 Box Behnken methodology . 62 5 Digital filtering techniques and signal distortion analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Large Scale Sound Installation Design: Psychoacoustic Stimulation
    LARGE SCALE SOUND INSTALLATION DESIGN: PSYCHOACOUSTIC STIMULATION An Interactive Qualifying Project Report submitted to the Faculty of the WORCESTER POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Science by Taylor H. Andrews, CS 2012 Mark E. Hayden, ECE 2012 Date: 16 December 2010 Professor Frederick W. Bianchi, Advisor Abstract The brain performs a vast amount of processing to translate the raw frequency content of incoming acoustic stimuli into the perceptual equivalent. Psychoacoustic processing can result in pitches and beats being “heard” that do not physically exist in the medium. These psychoac- oustic effects were researched and then applied in a large scale sound design. The constructed installations and acoustic stimuli were designed specifically to combat sensory atrophy by exer- cising and reinforcing the listeners’ perceptual skills. i Table of Contents Abstract ............................................................................................................................................ i Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ ii Table of Figures ............................................................................................................................. iii Table of Tables .............................................................................................................................. iv Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz Christopher D
    Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research Volume 2 Article 11 2013 An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz Christopher D. Lacomba University of North Georgia Steven A. Lloyd University of North Georgia Ryan A. Shanks University of North Georgia Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/papersandpubs Part of the Behavior and Behavior Mechanisms Commons, Biological and Chemical Physics Commons, Biological Psychology Commons, Cognition and Perception Commons, Cognitive Neuroscience Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Computational Neuroscience Commons, Dynamic Systems Commons, Investigative Techniques Commons, Mental Disorders Commons, Non-linear Dynamics Commons, Numerical Analysis and Computation Commons, Psychological Phenomena and Processes Commons, Speech and Hearing Science Commons, and the Systems Neuroscience Commons Recommended Citation Lacomba, Christopher D.; Lloyd, Steven A.; and Shanks, Ryan A. (2013) "An Infrasonic Missing Fundamental Rises at 18.5Hz," Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research: Vol. 2 , Article 11. Available at: http://digitalcommons.northgeorgia.edu/papersandpubs/vol2/iss1/11 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Center for Undergraduate Research and Creative Activities (CURCA) at Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Papers & Publications: Interdisciplinary Journal of Undergraduate Research by an authorized editor of Nighthawks Open Institutional Repository. The Missing Fundamental (MF) phenomenon creates perceptible tones, which do not actually exist in the audible range, from harmonic series. Sounds that follow a harmonic series are perceptually significant since they stand out from random background noise and frequently represent sounds of biological origin and importance. For example, both animal vocalizations and human speech produce harmonic series of sounds.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 – Music Temperament and Pitch
    11 – Music temperament and pitch Music sounds Every music instrument, including the voice, uses a more or less well defined sequence of music sounds, the notes, to produce music. The notes have particular frequencies that are called „pitch“ by musicians. The frequencies or pitches of the notes stand in a particular relation to each other. There are and were different ways to build a system of music sounds in different geographical regions during different historical periods. We will consider only the currently dominating system (used in classical and pop music) that was originated in ancient Greece and further developed in Europe. Pitch vs interval Only a small part of the people with normal hearing are able to percieve the absolute frequency of a sound. These people, who are said to have the „absolute pitch“, can tell what key has been hit on a piano without looking at it. The human ear and brain is much more sensitive to the relations between the frequencies of two or more sounds, the music intervals. The intervals carry emotional content that makes music an art. For instance, the major third sounds joyful and affirmative whereas the minor third sounds sad. The system of frequency relations between the sounds used in music production is called music temperament . 1 Music intervals and overtone series Perfect music intervals (that cannot be fully achieved practically, see below) are based on the overtone series studied before in this course. A typical music sound (except of a pure sinusiodal one) consists of a fundamental frequency f1 and its overtones: = = fn nf 1, n 3,2,1 ,..
    [Show full text]
  • Impairment of the Missing Fundamental Phenomenon in Individuals with Alzheimer’S Disease: a Neuropsychological and Voxel-Based Morphometric Study
    EXTRA Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra 2018;8:23–32 DOI: 10.1159/000486331 © 2018 The Author(s) Received: November 17, 2017 Published by S. Karger AG, Basel Accepted: December 7, 2017 www.karger.com/dee Published online: February 1, 2018 This article is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Interna- tional License (CC BY-NC-ND) (http://www.karger.com/Services/OpenAccessLicense). Usage and distribu- tion for commercial purposes as well as any distribution of modified material requires written permission. Original Research Article Impairment of the Missing Fundamental Phenomenon in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Neuropsychological and Voxel-Based Morphometric Study a a, b a a Makiko Abe Ken-ichi Tabei Masayuki Satoh Mari Fukuda c d a Hironobu Daikuhara Mariko Shiga Hirotaka Kida a, b Hidekazu Tomimoto a Department of Dementia Prevention and Therapeutics, Graduate School of Medicine, b Mie University, Mie, Japan; Department of Neurology, Graduate School of Medicine, c Mie University, Mie, Japan; Graduate School of Medicine, Mie University, Mie, Japan; d Mie Prefectural Dementia-Related Disease Medical Center, Mie, Japan Keywords Alzheimer’s disease · Dementia · Pitch perception · Missing fundamental phenomenon · Magnetic resonance imaging · Voxel-based morphometry Abstract Background/Aims: The missing fundamental phenomenon (MFP) is a universal pitch percep- tion illusion that occurs in animals and humans. In this study, we aimed to determine wheth- er the MFP is impaired in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) using an auditory pitch per- ception experiment. We further examined anatomical correlates of the MFP in patients with AD by measuring gray matter volume (GMV) on magnetic resonance images via voxel-based morphometric analysis.
    [Show full text]
  • Locomotion-Induced Sounds and Sonations: Mechanisms, Communication Function, and Relationship with Behavior
    Chapter 4 Locomotion-Induced Sounds and Sonations: Mechanisms, Communication Function, and Relationship with Behavior Christopher James Clark Abstract Motion-induced sound is an intrinsic byproduct of essentially all animal behavior. Locomotion-induced sounds that have evolved specialization for commu- nication are termed sonations. The null hypothesis is that locomotion-induced sounds are noncommunicative (adventitious), produced by nonspecialized mor- phology, and are involuntary. A sound is a sonation if it is produced by specialized morphology, or is produced voluntarily. The production of locomotion-induced sound can be examined at two levels: the animal motions (kinematics) that lead to sound production and the physical acoustic mechanism(s) that generate(s) the sound itself. The physical acoustics of locomotion induced sound are diverse, with both aerodynamic and structural mechanisms, including aeroelastic flutter, percussion, stridulation, and presumably many other undescribed mechanisms. There is a direct sound–motion correspondence between aspects of an animal’s motions and ensuing locomotion-induced sounds, especially in the time domain. This correspondence has two implications. One is experimental: sound recordings are a useful and perhaps underutilized source of data about animal locomotion. The second is behavioral: locomotion-induced sounds intrinsically contain information about an animal’s motions (such as wingbeat frequencies) that may be of interest to other animals. Therefore, locomotion-induced sounds are intrinsically suited to be mechanisms by which animals directly evaluate the locomotor performance of other animals, such as during courtship. The sound–motion correspondence is also a constraint. Sonations seem less acoustically diverse than vocalizations. Because they require discrete behaviors to be produced, animals also have somewhat fewer opportunities to produce sonations strategically, and few sonations are frequency-­ modulated.
    [Show full text]
  • Ghost Stochastic Resonance with Distributed Inputs in Pulse-Coupled Electronic Neurons
    PHYSICAL REVIEW E 73, 021101 ͑2006͒ Ghost stochastic resonance with distributed inputs in pulse-coupled electronic neurons Abel Lopera,1 Javier M. Buldú,1,* M. C. Torrent,1 Dante R. Chialvo,2 and Jordi García-Ojalvo1,† 1Departamento de Física i Enginyeria Nuclear, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Colom 11, E-08222 Terrassa, Spain 2Department of Physiology, Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois 60611, USA ͑Received 29 September 2005; published 8 February 2006͒ We study experimentally the phenomenon of ghost stochastic resonance in pulse-coupled excitable systems, for input signals distributed among different elements. Specifically, two excitable electronic circuits are driven by different sinusoidal signals that produce periodic spikes at distinct frequencies. Their outputs are sent to a third circuit that processes these spiking signals and is additionally perturbed by noise. When the input signals are harmonics of a certain fundamental ͑that is not present in the inputs͒ the processing circuit exhibits, for an optimal amount of noise, a resonant response at the frequency of the missing fundamental ͑ghost frequency͒. In contrast with the standard case in which the signals being directly integrated are sinusoidal, this behavior relies here on a coincidence-detection mechanism. When the input signals are homogeneously shifted in frequency, the processing circuit responds with pulse packages composed of spikes at a frequency that depends linearly on the frequency shift. Expressions for the dependence of the package period and duration on the frequency shift and spike width, respectively, are obtained. These results provide an experimental verification of a recently proposed mechanism of binaural pitch perception. DOI: 10.1103/PhysRevE.73.021101 PACS number͑s͒: 05.40.Ϫa, 05.45.Ϫa I.
    [Show full text]
  • The Sounds of Music: Science of Musical Scales∗ 1
    SERIES ARTICLE The Sounds of Music: Science of Musical Scales∗ 1. Human Perception of Sound Sushan Konar Both, human appreciation of music and musical genres tran- scend time and space. The universality of musical genres and associated musical scales is intimately linked to the physics of sound, and the special characteristics of human acoustic sensitivity. In this series of articles, we examine the science underlying the development of the heptatonic scale, one of the most prevalent scales of the modern musical genres, both western and Indian. Sushan Konar works on stellar compact objects. She Introduction also writes popular science articles and maintains a Fossil records indicate that the appreciation of music goes back weekly astrophysics-related blog called Monday Musings. to the dawn of human sentience, and some of the musical scales in use today could also be as ancient. This universality of musi- cal scales likely owes its existence to an amazing circularity (or periodicity) inherent in human sensitivity to sound frequencies. Most musical scales are specific to a particular genre of music, and there exists quite a number of them. However, the ‘hepta- 1 1 tonic’ scale happens to have a dominating presence in the world Having seven base notes. music scene today. It is interesting to see how this has more to do with the physics of sound and the physiology of human auditory perception than history. We shall devote this first article in the se- ries to understand the specialities of human response to acoustic frequencies. Human ear is a remarkable organ in many ways. The range of hearing spans three orders of magnitude in frequency, extending Keywords from ∼20 Hz to ∼20,000 Hz (Figure 1) even though the sensitivity String vibration, beat frequencies, consonance-dissonance, pitch, tone.
    [Show full text]
  • Part D: Characterizing Sound
    Part D : Characterizing Sound Part D: Characterizing Sound Chapter 36. Parameters and Perception Simple Harmonic Motion of an object is itself not sound, but a vibrating object can be a source of sound. Each of the physical parameters of SHM vibration is closely related to a perceptual characteristic of the sound produced. • The duration of a vibration, how long it lasts, is mainly determines the duration of the resulting sound. • Vibrations with larger amplitudes create sounds that are louder, all other things being equal. • Vibrations with higher frequencies cause sounds that are higher in pitch. • Since period is the reciprocal of frequency, it also relates directly to pitch. A longer period corresponds to a lower pitch. • The initial phase of a vibration has no bearing on the perception of the sound. Since the value of initial phase depends on a choice made by an observer (specifically, the choice of the time origin), it can’t have real-world consequences. Recall from Chapter 21 that phase differences can be physically meaningful, but those will only arise when multiple sounds combine. These relationships are not exclusive, as each of these perceptions (duration, loudness, and pitch) can be influenced by all of the physical parameters except initial phase. For instance, perceived loudness depends slightly on frequency. Nevertheless, the connections above are the principal ones. The characteristics of a sound can vary over time. Over the course of seconds, like notes in a song, you might consider that separate sounds, or one changing sound. On a more rapid scale, the exact variations over the first few milliseconds of a sound, called the attack in some contexts, can be very important for recognizing the source.
    [Show full text]
  • Tu.P3.1 II - 1739 Fundamental Frequencies Were Mistuned by ∆F Hz the Normalized ACF Calculated by In-Phase and Maintaining a Harmonic Relationship
    Multiple Missing Fundamental Phenomenon as a Monaural-Beat Perception Ryota Shimokura a and Yoichi Ando b Graduate School of Science and Technology, Kobe University Now at: a Department of Energy, Nuclear and Environmental Control Engineering, Bologna University [email protected] Now at: bDepartment of Architecture and Civil Engineering, Kumamoto University [email protected] Abstract phenomenon of periodicity pitch or residue pitch has played roles to explain the mechanism of pitch It is found that a monaural beat induced by two missing perception [1][2]. The fundamental frequency is fundamentals of complex tones (A and B) may be absent in the spectrum, however the pitch of it can be perceived, even in the condition without the envelope perceived. Schouten mixed a pure tone 206 Hz on a component of beat. This was realized by mixing the complex tone with fundamental frequency 200 Hz and complex tone A with the missing fundamental frequency indicated that the beat corresponding to 6 Hz did not f0a and the complex tone B with the missing occur [2]. In this experiment, we shall examine the fundamental frequency f0b. When we listen together, missing fundamental phenomenon perceived as a beat, then an additional missing fundamental frequency ∆f (= which was caused by two fundamental frequencies. Two f0b - f0a) may be perceived as the beat. The ∆f was different fundamental frequencies (f0a and f0b) by two changed to 2, 4, 8 and 16 Hz. The total tone mixed with different complex tones may induce an additional two complex tones has the envelope fluctuation fundamental frequency: ∆f (= f0a - f0b).
    [Show full text]
  • A. Acoustic Theory and Modeling of the Vocal Tract
    A. Acoustic Theory and Modeling of the Vocal Tract by H.W. Strube, Drittes Physikalisches Institut, Universität Göttingen A.l Introduction This appendix is intended for those readers who want to inform themselves about the mathematical treatment of the vocal-tract acoustics and about its modeling in the time and frequency domains. Apart from providing a funda­ mental understanding, this is required for all applications and investigations concerned with the relationship between geometric and acoustic properties of the vocal tract, such as articulatory synthesis, determination of the tract shape from acoustic quantities, inverse filtering, etc. Historically, the formants of speech were conjectured to be resonances of cavities in the vocal tract. In the case of a narrow constriction at or near the lips, such as for the vowel [uJ, the volume of the tract can be considered a Helmholtz resonator (the glottis is assumed almost closed). However, this can only explain the first formant. Also, the constriction - if any - is usu­ ally situated farther back. Then the tract may be roughly approximated as a cascade of two resonators, accounting for two formants. But all these approx­ imations by discrete cavities proved unrealistic. Thus researchers have have now adopted a more reasonable description of the vocal tract as a nonuni­ form acoustical transmission line. This can explain an infinite number of res­ onances, of which, however, only the first 2 to 4 are of phonetic importance. Depending on the kind of sound, the tube system has different topology: • for vowel-like sounds, pharynx and mouth form one tube; • for nasalized vowels, the tube is branched, with transmission from pharynx through mouth and nose; • for nasal consonants, transmission is through pharynx and nose, with the closed mouth tract as a "shunt" line.
    [Show full text]