Computers in Human Behavior 46 (2015) 1–5

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

The delayed effect of treadmill desk usage on recall and attention ⇑ Élise Labonté-LeMoyne a, , Radhika Santhanam b, Pierre-Majorique Léger a, François Courtemanche a, Marc Fredette a, Sylvain Sénécal a a Tech3lab, HEC Montreal, 3000, chemin de la Côte-Sainte-Catherine, Montréal, Québec H3T 2A7, Canada b OU Price College of Business, Division of MIS, 307 W. Brooks, Norman, OK 73019, USA article info abstract

Article history: The treadmill desk is a new human–computer interaction (HCI) setup intended to reduce the time Available online 19 January 2015 workers spend . As most workers will not choose to spend their entire workday , this study investigated the short-term delayed effect of treadmill desk usage. An experiment was conducted in Keywords: which participants either sat or walked while they read a text and received emails. Afterward, all Treadmill desk participants performed a task to evaluate their attention and memory. Behavioral, neurophysiological, Cognition and perceptual evidence showed that participants who walked had a short-term increase in memory HCI and attention, indicating that the use of a treadmill desk has a delayed effect. These findings suggest that EEG the treadmill desk, in addition to having health benefits for workers, can also be beneficial for businesses Neurophysiology by enhancing workforce performance. Ó 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).

1. Introduction While the health benefits of the treadmill desk are well estab- lished (Levine & Miller, 2007), other types of impact relating to this ‘‘Sitting is the new smoking’’ (Merchant, 2013). Research shows model of HCI remain unknown. Proponents of the device claim that that some fundamentally negative effects stem from sitting for it is a much healthier alternative to the traditional form of HCI (i.e., long periods of time (Levine, 2010; Van der Ploeg, Chey, Korda, a regular computer desk) and can improve users’ health and Banks, & Bauman, 2012), such as increased risk of heart disease, well-being (Duvivier et al., 2013; Højbjerre et al., 2010; Koepp , and musculoskeletal pain (Duvivier et al., 2013; Hill & et al., 2013; Levine & Miller, 2007). Skeptics feel that, while it is Peters, 1998; Højbjerre et al., 2010). It has also been shown that a healthy alternative to the traditional desk, the downside is that sitting is a risk factor for death. Compared to more active people, it may create a context in which employees are not in optimal the all-cause mortality is increased by 15% for people who sit over conditions to perform their work (John, Bassett, Thompson, 8 h per day and by 40% for people who sit over 11 h per day (van Fairbrother, & Baldwin, 2009). der Ploeg et al., 2012). To date, three studies have used psychometric tests to investi- Thus, researchers and manufacturers are trying to design new gate cognitive processes during the use of a treadmill desk, includ- forms of human–computer interaction (HCI) devices that will help ing processing speed, attention, working memory, inhibition and workers to sit less and remain healthy while still being productive. spatial memory (Alderman, Olson, Mattina, Studies, & Brunswick, One proposed HCI device is the treadmill desk, invented in 1988 2014; Commissaris et al., 2014; Kline, Poggensee, & Ferris, 2014). (Edelson & Danoff, 1989) but only manufactured commercially All of these studies found that walking while working had no since 2007 (Levine & Miller, 2007). As Fig. 1 shows, a treadmill is impact on these cognitive processes, confirming the authors’ positioned underneath an elevated desk. A computer is placed hypothesis that the treadmill desk has no deleterious effects. This upon this desk with its mouse and keyboard. Users walk at low does not, however, demonstrate the absence of possible benefits. speed with their arms resting on the armrests to stabilize Any benefits could simply be canceled out by the cognitive themselves, enabling them to interact with the computer in a demands of the dual tasks of walking and cognitive evaluation normal way. The ideal speed has been identified as 2.25 km/h tests. Oppezzo and Schwartz (2014) found that creativity was (Funk et al., 2012). improved while walking, but that performance was diminished on a convergent thinking task that required more cognitive work- load. According to Oppezzo, creativity is generally considered not

⇑ Corresponding author. Tel.:+1 514 340 6000x2629. to engage cognitive workload, and thus the related benefits might E-mail address: [email protected] (É. Labonté-LeMoyne). have been immune from being canceled out by dual-task demands. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.12.054 0747-5632/Ó 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). 2 É. Labonté-LeMoyne et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 46 (2015) 1–5

Interestingly, Oppezzo and Schwartz (2014) is the only study (Leger et al., 2014). Self-report task attention scores were obtained whose results suggest a residual effect of walking on creativity using validated measurement scales (Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). after the treadmill usage. This result calls for further investigation Electroencephalography (EEG) recordings were made during the of the treadmill desk’s post-usage effect on subsequent tasks. Such recall task and questionnaire administration. research appears crucial, as more and more people will use this type of desk for short periods of time during a workday. 2.1. Participants Understanding the delayed effect might be even more important than understanding the concurrent effects. So far, no other studies Participants were students at local universities. There were 18 have looked at the delayed effects of the use of a treadmill desk on in total: 9 who performed the reading task while seated and 9 key cognitive processes. who performed it while walking at the treadmill desk. Seated par- In the present study, we investigate the presence of a ticipants who matched the walking participants for age (M = 23, short-term delayed effect after the use of a treadmill desk on SD = 3.97), gender (5 women, 4 men) and level of education two cognitive abilities that are important for work performance: (7 undergraduate students, 2 masters students) were chosen from Memory and attention. As so few studies have looked at the effects a larger group of participants from a parallel study using the of slow-speed walking on cognition, we chose to investigate two method advocated by Jafari, Hearne, and Churilov (2013). Partici- cognitive abilities that have been shown to receive a delayed pants had to be over 18 and have no known health issues. They benefit from exercise (McMorris, Tomporowski, & Audiffren, received a $20 gift card after participating. 2009). We used behavioral, neurophysiological, and perceptual measures to develop a rich understanding of those effects. We 2.2. Procedure hypothesized that the use of a treadmill desk would have a beneficial delayed effect on attention and memory. After obtaining permission for experimental procedures from the review board of our institution, participants were recruited through a specialized online platform. After providing their 2. Research methods informed consent, they were outfitted with EEG equipment in order to begin recording as soon as possible after they stopped The research questions were addressed using a laboratory- walking. Participants in the walking group had a five-minute per- based empirical study. Study participants engaged in a reading task iod to get used to walking on the treadmill. Then all participants while receiving emails. One group did this while walking at a performed the reading task while walking or sitting, followed by treadmill desk, while the control group was in a traditional HCI the recall task and questionnaires. EEG was recorded during the setup and sat at a regular desk. After the reading assignment, both recall task and questionnaire administration to measure the groups had to perform a memory and attentional task while seated. delayed effect. Fig. 2 illustrates the experimental procedure. Participants’ memory performance on the reading task (i.e., task 2.3. Reading task recall) was evaluated with a validated objective questionnaire The reading task was taken from Leger et al. (2014) and involves reading a lengthy text while periodically receiving emails. Partici- pants were told to picture themselves in a scenario where they would have to report to their superior in 40 min on the content of the text and some of the emails. Some emails were pertinent to the topic at hand while others were irrelevant (e.g., co-worker’s birthday party, cafeteria lunch menu, etc.). Participants had to opti- mize their use of time and decide whether or not to open and read each email depending on its subject line. The task ended after 40 min. Participants on the treadmill desk walked at 2.25 km/h, which is considered the least intrusive speed for this type of desk (Funk et al., 2012). Participants rested their forearms on the desk’s armrests and used the mouse to open and close the emails (see Fig. 1). When the time was up, participants stepped off the tread- mill and sat at the traditional desk to complete the rest of the experiment and measure the delayed effect. At this time, the EEG equipment was adjusted to ensure data recording quality and hence participants were given a 10-min break while the adjust- ment was made.

2.4. Recall task

In the recall task, participants were asked 33 true or false questions about the text and the relevant emails they had read (Leger et al., 2014). Performance was considered as the percentage of correctly answered questions.

2.5. Measurement scales

Self-perceived on-task attention measuring focus was evaluated using a questionnaire adapted from Kanfer and Ackerman (1989). Fig. 1. Treadmill desk. The questionnaire includes 6 items answered with a 7-point Likert É. Labonté-LeMoyne et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 46 (2015) 1–5 3

Fig. 2. Experimental procedure. scale. The items were modified to apply to the task at hand and with amplitude exceeding ±75 lV were rejected, and recordings were translated in French by two bilingual researcher. were rereferenced to the common average reference. Epochs of one second were used for a Fast Fourier Transform. The first five 2.6. Electroencephalography minutes of the recall task and of the measurement scales were analyzed separately, minute by minute, and all epochs of each 2 EEG was recorded throughout except during walking. This data minute were averaged. The power (lV ) of each band of frequency was acquired with a 32-channel geodesic system (EGI, Eugene, OR, was obtained (Theta 4–7.5 Hz, Alpha 8–13 Hz, Beta 14–30 Hz, USA) and analyzed with Vision Analyzer software (Brain Products, Gamma 30–80 Hz), and relative power ratios of the different Munich, Germany). The recordings were filtered offline with a 0.5– frequencies were calculated as the band power divided by the total 100 Hz band pass filter and a 60 Hz notch filter; ocular corrections power of all four bands. were performed with an independent component analysis, epochs

2.7. Statistical analysis Table 1 Descriptive results. The recall scores were compared between groups with a logistic Recall performance On-task attention regression to estimate the probability of correctly answering a median (SD) median (SD) recall question. The measurement scale scores and the ratio of each Walking (n = 9) 0.75 (0.10) 6.33 (0.72) EEG frequency band for each electrode and for each minute were Control (n = 9) 0.70 (0.09) 5.50 (1.08) compared between groups using Mann–Whitney non-parametric SD – Standard deviation. tests. Statistical analysis was performed using the Statistical Pack- age for Social Sciences (SPSS, version 17.2; Chicago, IL, USA, 2009).

Table 2 Group differences for EEG band power for P4, P8, T7 and T8 electrodes.

Electrode P4 P8 Minute 1 2 1 2 Band Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma p Value 0.02 0.08 0.27 0.47 0.05 0.05 0.34 0.48 0.05 0.06 0.47 0.50 0.08 0.05 0.17 0.37

Electrode T7 T8 Minute 1 2 1 2 Band Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Theta Alpha Beta Gamma p Value 0.04 0.27 0.13 0.05 0.43 0.50 0.27 0.19 0.04 0.11 0.05 0.01 0.12 0.27 0.27 0.07

Fig. 3. Proportion of EEG frequency band power for electrodes with significant differences between groups (⁄: p < 0.05). 4 É. Labonté-LeMoyne et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 46 (2015) 1–5

3. Results diminish in the beginning and improve later on. Various durations and the use over multiple days should be studied to investigate if This study found evidence of a delayed increase in cognitive the improved perceptions remain. A positive perception of a new performance for users of the treadmill desk. This can be seen habit is a strong determinant of its future adoption by an behaviorally, subjectively, and neurophysiologically. individual (Bandura, 1977). For the recall task performance, we found that the odds of Finally, our results showed a significant difference in the EEG of answering a question correctly were 34.9% higher in the walking people who walked versus people who sat. We found an effect of group (p = 0.05). Their self-perceived on-task attention was also the treadmill desk on brain activity that is associated with significantly higher (p = 0.007). Table 1 lists the medians and stan- increased memory and attentional abilities (Klimesch, 1999; dard deviations for these dependent variables. Müller et al., 2000). This is, however, limited to the first part of For the EEG, significant results were found for the recall task, the following task. Because our equipment required a 10-min specifically in the first half of the task. Significant results can be adjustment period between the walking and the evaluation period, seen in Table 2 and Fig. 3. The group differences appear mostly we do not know whether the difference between groups was also in the right temporo-parietal region. We observed significantly present during that time. The use of EEG allows us to identify that more theta activity in the seated group (p = 0.02–0.05) and more the brain is directly affected by the walking, but the causes of this alpha activity in the walking group (p = 0.05). Previous studies effect remain unknown. Future studies should use brain oxygena- had shown that good memory performance is correlated with a tion as a comparative measure to investigate the similarities and decrease in theta power and an increase in alpha power differences between the effects of walking at a treadmill desk (Klimesch, 1999). Past work also showed that an increase in and various intensities of exercise. gamma power in the parietal region is correlated with attentional Another limitation of our study is that the sample was small, processes (Müller, Gruber, & Keil, 2000) and memory (Lisman, but samples of this size are not uncommon for EEG studies and 2010). Our results are therefore in accordance with improved per- the presence of an effect with such small groups is a testament formance by the walking group on a task that requires memory to the strength of the effect. As well, control subjects were matched and attention. with walking subjects for age, gender and level of education. Future studies should be performed with larger and more diverse groups and must also observe the impact of the treadmill desk dur- 4. Discussion and concluding comments ing and immediately after its use in order to determine the specific duration of the effect. The purpose of this study was to investigate the impact of the In conclusion, our results suggest that the use of a treadmill use of a newer form of HCI, namely a treadmill desk, on cognitive desk can improve attention and memory after the user has stopped abilities that are necessary for work. We found delayed benefits for walking. It can also improve self-perceived attention, which could attention and memory after walking, once a person has sat down. lead to increased adherence to this new habit. Improvements in Our findings are supported by behavioral, neurophysiological, and work performance should be convincing for organizations that perceptual data. may be inclined to subsidize the use of treadmill desks. These find- These findings are important, as most users of a treadmill desk ings also contribute to the conversation on the use of alternate will not spend their entire workday walking. Workers might use kinds of desks in the popular media. While some have referred to their treadmill desks for some specific tasks and sit down for other sitting as the new smoking (Merchant, 2013) and praise the tread- specific tasks, which require more attention and memory, as sug- mill desk as a better alternative, the scientific evidence to support gested in this study. Many different types of office work can be per- these claims is limited so far. This study is a step in bridging the formed at this type of desk. As attention, memory and reading are gap between the strong incentive of the health benefits and the the building blocks of a variety of more complex office tasks, these fear of lost productivity. too could benefit in a similar manner. However, as other studies have found neutral or deleterious effects of the treadmill desk on Acknowledgements other cognitive and work abilities (Alderman et al., 2014; Commissaris et al., 2014; Kline et al., 2014), each type of task This research was partially funded by a grant from the Social should be studied independently. Future research should study a Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) to variety of HCI tasks to see whether the effect of walking while Sylvain Sénecal, Pierre-Majorique Léger and Marc Fredette, and working is specific to memory and attention or if it can be trans- by a grant from Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council ferred to more complex tasks requiring higher-level executive of Canada (NSERC) to Pierre-Majorique Léger. functions, such as complex problem-solving. In addition, other types of active workstations, such as a standing desk, could be References included in these studies. While the health benefits of this new practice are indisputable, Alderman, B. L., Olson, R. L., Mattina, D. M., Studies, S., & Brunswick, N. (2014). it will only be adopted if users feel they can accomplish their work Cognitive function during low intensity walking: A test of the treadmill workstation. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 11(4), 752–758. http:// as well as or better than with the use of a traditional desk. Our dx.doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2012-0097. results suggest that workers not only perform better on a recall Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. task, but they also perceive themselves to be more attentive to Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215. . Commissaris, D. A. C. M., Könemann, R., Hiemstra-van Mastrigt, S., Burford, E.-M., et al. and Straker et al. that participants perceived their perfor- Botter, J., Douwes, M., et al. (2014). Effects of a standing and three dynamic mance to be diminished while walking (Commissaris et al., 2014; workstations on computer task performance and cognitive function tests. Straker, Levine, & Campbell, 2009). This is probably because the Applied Ergonomics, 45(6), 1570–1578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2014. 05.003. tasks performed in these two studies were of short duration, and Duvivier, B. M. F. M., Schaper, N. C., Bremers, M. A., van Crombrugge, G., Menheere, because the perceptions were measured using only one question. P. P. C. A., Kars, M., et al. (2013). Minimal intensity physical activity (standing This emphasizes the importance of measuring the user’s percep- and walking) of longer duration improves insulin action and plasma lipids more than shorter periods of moderate to vigorous exercise (cycling) in sedentary tions in authentic contexts. There is probably an adaptation period subjects when energy expenditure is comparable. PLoS One, 8(2), e55542. to the use of a treadmill desk and users may feel their performance http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055542. É. Labonté-LeMoyne et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 46 (2015) 1–5 5

Edelson, N., & Danoff, J. (1989). Walking on an electric treadmill while performing Leger, P.-M., Senecal, S., Courtemanche, F., Ortiz de Guinea, A., Titah, R., Fredette, M., VDT office work. SIGCHI Bulletin, 21(1), 72–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/ et al. (2014). Precision is in the eye of the beholder: Application of eye fixation- 67880.67890. related potentials to information systems. Journal of the Association for Funk, R. E., Taylor, M. J. L., Creekmur, C. C., Ohlinger, C. M., Cox, R. H., & Berg, W. P. Information Systems, 15, 1–53. (2012). Effect of walking speed on typing performance using an active Levine, J. A. (2010). Health-chair reform: Your chair: Comfortable but deadly. workstation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 115(1), 309–318. http://dx.doi.org/ Diabetes, 59(11), 2715–2716. http://dx.doi.org/10.2337/db10-1042. 10.2466/06.23.26.PMS.115.4.309-318. Levine, J. A., & Miller, J. M. (2007). The energy expenditure of using a ‘‘walk-and- Hill, J. O., & Peters, J. C. (1998). Environmental contributions to the work’’ desk for office workers with obesity. British Journal of Sports Medicine, epidemic. Science, 280, 1371–1374. 41(9), 558–561. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.2006.032755. Højbjerre, L., Sonne, M. P., Alibegovic, A. C., Dela, F., Vaag, A., Meldgaard, J. B., et al. Lisman, J. (2010). Working memory: The importance of theta and gamma (2010). Impact of physical inactivity on subcutaneous adipose tissue oscillations. Current Biology, 20(11). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ in healthy young male offspring of patients with type 2 diabetes. j.cub.2010.04.011 [R490-2]. Diabetes, 59(11), 2790–2798. http://dx.doi.org/10.2337/db10-0320. McMorris, T., Tomporowski, P., & Audiffren, M. (Eds.). (2009). Exercise and Cognitive Jafari, N., Hearne, J., & Churilov, L. (2013). Why caution is recommended with post- Function (pp. 386). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.. . group randomized controlled trials: The case of acute stroke trials. Statistics in Merchant, N. (2013). Nilofer merchant: Got a meeting? Take a walk [Video file] Medicine. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/sim.5862. John, D., Bassett, D., Thompson, D., Fairbrother, J., & Baldwin, D. (2009). Effect of (retrieved 15.09.14). using a treadmill workstation on performance of simulated office work tasks. Müller, M. M., Gruber, T., & Keil, A. (2000). Modulation of induced gamma band Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 6(5), 617–624. activity in the human EEG by attention and visual information processing. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38(3), 283–299. http://dx.doi.org/ integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00171-9. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74(4), 657–690. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0021- Oppezzo, M., & Schwartz, D. L. (2014). Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect 9010.74.4.657. of walking on creative thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory Memory, and Cognition, 40(4), 1142–1152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036577. performance: A review and analysis. Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews, Straker, L., Levine, J., & Campbell, A. (2009). The effects of walking and cycling 29(2-3), 169–195. . computer workstations on keyboard and mouse performance. Human Factors: Kline, J. E., Poggensee, K., & Ferris, D. P. (2014). Your brain on speed: Cognitive The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 51(6), 831–844. http:// performance of a spatial working memory task is not affected by walking speed. dx.doi.org/10.1177/0018720810362079. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(May), 288. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/ Van der Ploeg, H. P., Chey, T., Korda, R. J., Banks, E., & Bauman, A. (2012). Sitting time fnhum.2014.00288. and all-cause mortality risk in 222–497 Australian adults. Archives of Internal Koepp, G. A., Levine, J. A., Manohar, C. U., McCrady-Spitzer, S. K., Ben-Ner, A., Medicine, 172(6), 494–500. Hamann, D. J., et al. (2013). Treadmill desks: A 1-year prospective trial. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.), 21(4), 705–711. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/oby.20121.