1 Rethinking the Ken Through the Lens of Psychological Science Jason M. Chin TC Beirne School of Law, University of Queensland W

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

1 Rethinking the Ken Through the Lens of Psychological Science Jason M. Chin TC Beirne School of Law, University of Queensland W 1 Rethinking the Ken Through the Lens of Psychological Science Jason M. Chin TC Beirne School of Law, University of Queensland William E. Crozier John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York *Forthcoming, Osgoode Hall Law Journal **This article deeply benefited from insights provided by Heather Douglas, Caitlin Goss, Saul Kassin, and John Monahan. We are also grateful for the incisive feedback of two anonymous reviewers and the Osgoode Hall Law Journal editors and editorial board. Conversations with Richard Kemp and Kristy Martire at the annual meeting of the Evidence-Based Forensics Initiative in Wollongong, New South Wales were critical in the early stages of this article. 2 Abstract Canadian courts are loathe to admit expert evidence from psychological scientists when that evidence does not concern a disposition, typically a psychological disorder. As a result, psychological evidence concerning the unconscious processes and situational forces that underlie mistaken eyewitness identifications and wrongful confessions are regularly excluded from courtrooms. Courts justify these exclusions on the basis that the evidence is not beyond the ken of the trier of fact – the psychologist would simply be describing an experience shared by judge and jury. This reasoning stands on a fundamental misunderstanding of psychology. In fact, psychological science finds that the situation drives behaviour in a manner that regularly evades the trier of fact’s ken. This is because these situational forces rely on unconscious cognitive processes, and humans rarely have introspective access to these processes. As a result, humans cling to several deep misconceptions about memory processes and confessions. In this Article, we first discuss the mechanics of the mischief: why humans fail to account for unconscious processes and the situation, and instead focus on disposition. Then we provide evidence for this type of reasoning in decisions to exclude expert evidence about eyewitness identifications and false confessions. We conclude with recommendations to manage prejudicial evidence in a manner that is based on a more nuanced understanding of human psychology. 3 Table of Contents Abstract ........................................................................................................................................... 2 Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ 3 Part I. Introduction .......................................................................................................................... 4 Part II. Unconscious Neglect and Dispositionism .......................................................................... 8 Unconscious versus Conscious Processes .................................................................................. 9 Social Psychology and the Nonintuitive Power of the Situation .............................................. 12 Applied Social and Cognitive Psychology on Eyewitness Memory and False Confessions .... 14 Part III. Unconscious Neglect and Dispositionism in Expert Evidence Law ............................... 20 Eyewitness Memory.................................................................................................................. 25 False Confessions and Mr. Big Operations............................................................................... 29 Part IV. Countering Unconscious Neglect and Dispositionism .................................................... 37 Part V. Conclusion ........................................................................................................................ 47 4 Part I. Introduction Outside consciousness there rolls a vast tide of life which is perhaps more important to us that the little isle of our thoughts which lies within our ken.1 Canada, like many jurisdictions, relies on a “beyond the ken” standard to determine if expert evidence is necessary and thus admissible in court.2 In other words, expert evidence may only be admitted if it is likely to be outside the experience of the trier of fact. Fields like forensic science and pathology usually meet this standard and thus experts in these fields regularly appear in court. However, psychological scientists – outside of some clinicians – face a good deal more difficulty.3 This tendency has received a great deal of academic criticism because these psychologists would often opine on psychological phenomena that contribute to wrongful convictions, especially eyewitness memory and false confessions.4 Our contribution to this field 1 Eneas Sweetland Dallas, The Gay Science (London: Chapman and Hall, 1866), as quoted in Timothy D Wilson, Strangers to Ourselves Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious (Cambridge: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, 2002) at 17 [Wilson]. 2 R v Mohan, [1994] 2 SCR 9 at para 26, 114 DLR (4th) 419 [Mohan]. In the United States, the Federal Rules of Evidence overturned the common law beyond the ken standard. However, the authors of The New Wigmore: A Treatise on Evidence, 2d ed (Thomson Reuters) at s 2.1.3 note that “many-perhaps most” American courts continue to apply the old common law rule [New Wigmore]. Similarly, in Australia, the Uniform Evidence Law abolished the requirement that opinion evidence go beyond common knowledge. In New South Wales, for instance, see: Evidence Act 1995 (NSW) at s 80(b) “Ultimate issue and common knowledge rules abolished”. Still, common law States retain the beyond common knowledge requirement and regularly exclude psychological scientific evidence on that basis, R v LM [2004] QCA 192. And, as in the U.S., even states that have adopted the Uniform Evidence Law still retain the common knowledge distinction to some extent, see: JD Heydon, Cross on Evidence, 10th ed (Chatswood NSW: LexisNexis, 2015) at 237-239, 1018-1024. 3 For a review, see: David M Paciocco, “Coping with Expert Evidence About Human Behaviour” (1999) 26 Queen’s LJ 305-346 [Paciocco]; Gary T Trotter, “False Confessions and Wrongful Convictions” 35:2 Ottawa LR 179-210 [Trotter]; Timothy E Moore & Cindy R Wasser, “Social Science and Witness Reliability: Reliable Science Begets Reliable Evidence” 33 CR (6th) [Moore Reliable Science]; Jill Copeland, “Helping Jurors Recognize the Frailties of Eyewitness Identification Evidence” (2002) 46 Crim LQ 188-209 [Copleand]; Lisa Dufraimont, “Regulating Unreliable Evidence: Can Evidence Rules Guide Juries and Prevent Wrongful Convictions?” (2008) 33 Queen’s LJ 261-326 [Dufraimont]; Emma Cunliffe, “Without fear or favour? Trends and possibilities in the Canadian approach to expert human behaviour evidence” (2006) 10 International Journal of Evidence & Proof 280–315 [Cunliffe]. 4 Copeland, ibid; Trotter, ibid at 193; Dufraimont, ibid at 312, 321-326; Amy D Trenary, “State v. Henderson: A Model for Admitting Eyewitness Identification Testimony” (2013) 84 University of Colorado Law Review 1257- 1303. But for an argument that psychological scientific evidence is unnecessary, see: Lee Stuesser, “Experts on Eyewitness Identification: I Just Don’t See It” 31 Man LJ 543-553. 5 – the subject matter of this Article – is an explanation for why judges tend to find that psychological science is within the trier of fact’s ken and why that decision is mistaken. In determining whether evidence is beyond the ken, courts fall prey to two fundamental misunderstandings about human psychology. First, they neglect the unconscious processes5 that underlie our behaviours and emotions. Second, courts discount the power of the situation in favour of a dispositional model of human psychology. As a result, judges regularly exclude experts because of the assumption that jurors are fully equipped to understand those unconscious and situational forces. They reason that those psychological forces form part of the everyday human experience and thus jurors already know all they need to know. But, as E.S. Dallas noted in the quote that began this Article, this approach wrongly restricts their view of human nature to that which is within our conscious ken. To take an example from the eyewitness memory field, the leading Canadian judgment drew a strict line between evidence about the situational forces that cause misremembering and disorders like autism that impact memory.6 The situational evidence is inadmissible because jurors all have memories and have all experienced forgetting.7 Disposition is admissible because jurors have not experienced autism.8 This approach represents a facile and incorrect view of psychology. Rather, humans cannot report on what the unconscious parts of their brain are up to and are, in fact, oblivious to those processes. For similar reasons, they are often unaware of the situational forces that 5 In using the term unconscious, we are referring to the concept as it has been explored in decades of cognitive scientific research on mental processes that evade conscious reflection. This conception contrasts with the lay (and Freudian) meaning of the word often associated with sublimated desires. 6 R v McIntosh, 35 OR (3d) 97 at para 21, 117 CCC (3d) 385 [McIntosh]: “…expert evidence might be admissible to show that the witness is an autistic savant and that such exceptional memory feats are often associated with this syndrome.” 7 Ibid at para 20. 8 Ibid at para 21. 6 profoundly impact their lives. As hundreds of wrongful convictions demonstrate, people do not naturally think like psychological scientists and thus require
Recommended publications
  • David A. Reinhard
    David A. Reinhard University of Massachusetts Amherst Psychology of Peace and Violence Program Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences 135 Hicks Way, Tobin Hall, Room 627 Amherst, MA 01003 [email protected] | www.david-reinhard.com Education Ph.D. University of Virginia Social Psychology, 2017 Dissertation: The Influence of Temporal Group Identities on Goal-Pursuit Committee: Benjamin Converse (Chair), Gerald Clore, Shigehiro Oishi, Kieran O’Connor M.A. University of Virginia Social Psychology, 2014 Thesis: Activating Mental Representations of Rivals Increases Motivation Committee: Benjamin Converse (Chair), Gerald Clore, Timothy Wilson B.A. University of Michigan Honors Program Psychology (High Honors), 2010 Thesis: The Influence of Weight Cues on Product Perceptions Advisor: Norbert Schwarz Academic and Professional Appointments University of Massachusetts Amherst Psychology of Peace and Violence Program, Department of Psychology Postdoctoral Research Associate, 2017-Present PI: Bernhard Leidner University of Michigan Research Center for Group Dynamics, Institute for Social Research Research Associate (Lab Manager), 2010-2011 PI: Stephanie Brown, Sara Konrath Research Interests Motivation and Goal Pursuit, Rivalry, Temporal Collective Identities, Social Cognition, Emotion Grants and Fellowships Society for the Study of Peace, Conflict, and Violence APA Division 48, Small Grants Program for Peace Psychology ($500). “Destructive or Constructive International Conflict: Enemies or Rivals? An Empirical Investigation and Intervention in Israel and Iran”. 2018 August 2018 University of Massachusetts Amherst, Campus Climate Improvement Grants ($2,500). “Bridging the Great Divide: Democrat-Republican Rivalry and Its Consequences for Diversity, Disparity, and Inclusion on Campus.” 2018. The Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues, Grants-in-Aid ($1,998). “De-escalating Conflict in International Rivalries”.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Idea and Literature Review on the Effect of Affective Forecasting on Emotional Experience
    International Journal of New Developments in Engineering and Society ISSN 2522-3488 Vol. 4, Issue 4: 123-132, DOI: 10.25236/IJNDES.040411 Research Idea and Literature Review on the Effect of Affective Forecasting on Emotional Experience Sophie Liu Waterloo Collegiate Institute, Waterloo, N2L3P2, Canada ABSTRACT: According to the study by Timothy Wilson and Daniel Gilbert on affective forecasting (2003), people’s prediction of their emotional reactions towards future events could influence their actual emotional responses. The aim of this research idea is to further investigate the impact of affective forecasting on the intensity of affective experience by conducting two experiments in which participants would be divided into two groups based on their predictions of the intensity of their emotional experience towards a fear-inducing event and a happiness-inducing event, and the intensity of the actual emotional experience of the two groups would be recorded and compared. Several related studies on affective forecasting are reviewed in the introduction section. KEYWORDS: Affective Forecasting, Affective Experience, Expectation Effect, Literature Review, Research Proposal Introduction Affective forecasting [1] is the prediction of one’s future affect/feelings when experiencing certain events. The concept was first investigated by Kahneman and Snell as “hedonic forecasts” when they examined it impact on decision making in 1990 [2]. In 2003 psychologists Timothy Wilson and Daniel Gilbert coined it with the name of “affective forecasting” [1]. While earlier researches focused on the emotional forecasts rather than the actual response, other researchers (e.g. Baron, 1992; Gilbert, Driver-Linn, & Wilson, 2002; Gilbert & Wilson, 2000; Gilbert, Wilson, 2003; Loewenstein & Frederick, 1997) later moved on to examine the accuracy of forecasts by measuring both predicted and experienced emotional response.
    [Show full text]
  • David A. Reinhard
    David A. Reinhard University of Massachusetts Amherst Psychology of Peace and Violence Program Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences 135 Hicks Way, Tobin Hall, Room 627 Amherst, MA 01003 [email protected] | www.david-reinhard.com Education Ph.D. University of Virginia Social Psychology, 2017 Dissertation: The Influence of Temporal Group Identities on Goal-Pursuit Committee: Benjamin Converse (Chair), Gerald Clore, Shigehiro Oishi, Kieran O’Connor M.A. University of Virginia Social Psychology, 2014 Thesis: Activating Mental Representations of Rivals Increases Motivation Committee: Benjamin Converse (Chair), Gerald Clore, Timothy Wilson B.A. University of Michigan Honors Program Psychology (High Honors), 2010 Thesis: The Influence of Weight Cues on Product Perceptions Advisor: Norbert Schwarz Academic and Professional Appointments University of Massachusetts Amherst Psychology of Peace and Violence Program, Department of Psychology Postdoctoral Research Associate, 2017-Present PI: Bernhard Leidner University of Michigan Research Center for Group Dynamics, Institute for Social Research Research Associate (Lab Manager), 2010-2011 PI: Stephanie Brown, Sara Konrath Research Interests Motivation and Goal Pursuit, Rivalry, Temporal Collective Identities, Social Cognition, Emotion Fellowships and Grants University of Virginia Presidential Fellowship in Data Science ($28,000). “The role of emotions in political discourse”. 2016-2017 December 2017 The Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Small Research Grant ($1,500). “De- escalating Conflict in International Rivalries”. 2018 Publications Clore, G.L. & Reinhard, D.A. (in press). Emotional intensity: It’s the thought that counts. In R. Davidson, A. Shackman, A. Fox, & R. Lapate (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: A volume of short essays addressing fundamental questions in emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Show full text]
  • Consuming Experiences Shift Standards Through Attentional Collapse Carey K
    ASSOCIATION FOR CONSUMER RESEARCH Labovitz School of Business & Economics, University of Minnesota Duluth, 11 E. Superior Street, Suite 210, Duluth, MN 55802 Consuming Experiences Shift Standards Through Attentional Collapse Carey K. Morewedge, Princeton University, USA Daniel T. Gilbert, Harvard University, USA Kristian O. R. Myerseth, University of Chicago, USA Timothy Wilson, University of Virginia, USA In five studies, participants forecasted how much they would enjoy a future experience (e.g., eating potato chips) or had that experience (e.g., ate potato chips). Hedonic forecasts were strongly affected by the presence of a superior alternative (e.g., chocolate) or an inferior alternative (e.g., sardines), but hedonic experiences were unaffected in three studies—experiencers were happy eating potato chips irrespective of the present alternative. In our forth and fifth studies, hedonic experiences were affected by present alternatives only when hedonic experiences required few attentional resources. The results suggest that forecasters underestimate the extent to which hedonic experiences "consume" attention and render alternatives irrelevant. [to cite]: Carey K. Morewedge, Daniel T. Gilbert, Kristian O. R. Myerseth, and Timothy Wilson (2008) ,"Consuming Experiences Shift Standards Through Attentional Collapse", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 35, eds. Angela Y. Lee and Dilip Soman, Duluth, MN : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 89-92. [url]: http://www.acrwebsite.org/volumes/13278/volumes/v35/NA-35 [copyright notice]: This work is copyrighted by The Association for Consumer Research. For permission to copy or use this work in whole or in part, please contact the Copyright Clearance Center at http://www.copyright.com/. SYMPOSIA SUMMARY Erroneous Lay Theories of Future Affect: Processes and Consequences Cecile Cho, Columbia University Carey Morewedge, Princeton University SESSION OVERVIEW program viewing.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring Unconscious Bias
    Testing Your Own Unconscious Bias The most effective tool available for Exploring Unconscious Bias testing one’s own unconscious bias is the Implicit Association Test (IAT), created and maintained by Project by Howard Ross, Founder & Chief Learning Officer, Cook Ross, Inc. Implicit, a consortium made up of researchers from Harvard University, the University of Virginia, and the University of Washington. The IAT was created more than 10 years ago and has now been used by millions of people in over 20 countries. Researchers at these three schools, as well as others, have used the test to study many aspects of organizational and social performance, ranging from healthcare decisions to the operations Consider this: Less than 15% of American men are over six foot tall, yet almost of the criminal justice system. To take 60% of corporate CEOs are over six foot tall. Less than 4% of American men the IAT, without charge, go to https:// implicit.harvard.edu/implicit/. are over six foot, two inches tall, yet more than 36% of corporate CEOs are over six foot, two inches tall.1 Why does this happen? Clearly corporate boards of directors do not, when conducting a CEO search, send out a message to “get us a tall guy,” and yet the numbers speak for themselves. In fact, when corrected for age and gender, an inch of height is worth approximately $789 per year in salary!2 Similar patterns are true for Generals and Admirals in the Military, and even for Presidents of the United States. The last elected President whose height was below average was William McKinley in 1896, and he was “ridiculed in the press as ‘a little boy.’” 3 It seems not only unfair, but patently absurd to choose a CEO because of height, just like it is unfair and absurd to give employees lower performance evaluations solely because they are overweight.
    [Show full text]
  • DIALOGUE Page 1
    DIALOGUE Page 1 Volume 18, No. 2 DIALOGUE Dialogue — Fall, 2003 The Official Newsletter of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Chris Crandall & Monica Biernat, Co-Editors 2004 Convention: Looking Inside the Forward to Austin, Texas Current Issue: meeting in this new property, one of a dozen By Rick Hoyle State of the Society; New 2, 3 we anticipate an enthusiastic preconferences ranging in Arrangements for the 2004 reception by hotel focus from the latest Executive Director chosen convention, to be held management and staff. research findings in specific areas of personality and Student Pub/TIP Winners; 4 January 29-31 in Austin, Carlston on Writing Texas, are nearly complete. The program committee, led social psychology, to career Construction of the new by Mark Leary, has put development, to the art and The first Summer Institute 5, Hilton Austin, convention together the most varied and science of teaching in Social Psychology 16 headquarters, is ahead of extensive SPSP program psychology. schedule. ever (see p. 8 for an article PSPB News; call for Editor 6 from the Program Chair The convention officially Nominations Hotel management projects a about how the decisions opens with a welcome were made). reception at 5:30 on Report from the 7 “soft opening” by November th and a full opening by early Thursday the 29 . As a Publications Committee January. As one of the first If you can make it to Austin reward for our willingness a day early, then you’ll have to commit to holding our SPSP Convention 8 organizations to stage a Programming the option of participating in (Continued on page 3) President’s Column: Jim 9 SPSP Election Results: New Officers Chosen Blascovich on Rumors SPSP Elections were held Blascovich will become Past- term, and represents the Dan Wegner on Discovery 10 and Debate in Science this spring; the Society President.
    [Show full text]
  • Researchers Overturn Landmark Study on the Replicability of Psychological Science
    Researchers overturn landmark study on the replicability of psychological science By Peter Reuell Harvard Staff Writer Category: HarvardScience Subcategory: Culture & Society KEYWORDS: psychology, psychological science, replication, replicate, reproduce, reproducibility, Center for Open Science, Gilbert, Daniel Gilbert, King, Gary King, Science, Harvard, FAS, Faculty of Arts and Sciences, Reuell, Peter Reuell Summary: A 2015 study claiming that more than half of all psychology studies cannot be replicated turns out to be wrong. Harvard researchers have discovered that the study contains several statistical and methodological mistakes, and that when these are corrected, the study actually shows that the replication rate in psychology is quite high – indeed, it is statistically indistinguishable from 100%. RELATED LINKS: According to two Harvard professors and their collaborators, a 2015 landmark study showing that more than half of all psychology studies cannot be replicated is actually wrong. In an attempt to determine the replicability of psychological science, a consortium of 270 scientists known as The Open Science Collaboration (OSC) tried to replicate the results of 100 published studies. More than half of them failed, creating sensational headlines worldwide about the “replication crisis” in psychology. But an in-depth examination of the data by Daniel Gilbert (Edgar Pierce Professor of Psychology at Harvard University), Gary King (Albert J. Weatherhead III University Professor at Harvard University), Stephen Pettigrew (doctoral student in the Department of Government at Harvard University), and Timothy Wilson (Sherrell J. Aston Professor of Psychology at the University of Virginia) has revealed that the OSC made some serious mistakes that make this pessimistic conclusion completely unwarranted: The methods of many of the replication studies turn out to be remarkably different from the originals and, according to Gilbert, King, Pettigrew, and Wilson, these “infidelities” had two important consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • DIALOGUE Page 1
    DIALOGUE Page 1 Volume 21, No. 1 DIALOGUE Dialogue—Spring, 2006 The Official Newsletter of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Chris Crandall & Monica Biernat, Co-Editors Society News from Palm Springs: Inside the Bigger, Better, With Less Support Current Issue: The SPSP Executive Committee met on PSPR, and the move of the executive office th, Sunday, January 29 after the Palm Springs from Rochester to Ithaca, the Executive Publication Committee Report 2 convention. The blue skies and desert of Palm Committee planned for a one-time budget Springs were reflected in the meeting—the deficit of about $55K. Instead, last year finances and membership growth of SPSP ended with a budget surplus. Baumeister & Vohs on 3 corresponded to blue skies, but the funding Behaving Ourselves future for the discipline seems a closer match Sage has increased the amount of royalties to the parched desert that surround the city. from PSPB from 25% to 40%. Starting Penner on SISP 2006 in Ann 4 gently in 2007, but to a larger extent in the Arbor Membership. At the end of 2005, the size of 2008 calendar year, the income from Sage the Society was 4,683 members. Of this 2,034 will increase substantially. are students (43%), and 2,649 are full (voting) SPSP Convention News; Call 2, 5 members. The Society’s growth cannot go on The SPSP meeting is bigger every year, and for Proposals at this pace forever, as the total size of the field this means a growing income. It also means Graduate Student Committee 6, 28, is not growing at the same rate as the Society.
    [Show full text]
  • Revised 6/21 TIMOTHY D. WILSON CURRICULUM
    Revised 6/21 TIMOTHY D. WILSON CURRICULUM VITAE CONTACT INFORMATION Department of Psychology (434) 924-0674 2594 Clinical Dept Wing, West Complex [email protected] P.O. Box 400400 https://uva.theopenscholar.com/timothy-wilson University of Virginia Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400 EDUCATION Ph.D. University of Michigan, 1977 (Psychology) M.A. University of Michigan, 1975 (Psychology) B.A. Hampshire College, 1973 (Psychology) PROFESSIONAL AFFILIATIONS National Academy of Sciences (Elected Member) American Academy of Arts and Sciences (Elected Member) American Psychological Society (Fellow) Society for Experimental Social Psychology (Fellow) Society of Personality and Social Psychology (Fellow) EMPLOYMENT HISTORY 2011 - present Affiliated Faculty Member, Batten School of Leadership & Public Policy 2001 - present Sherrell J. Aston Professor of Psychology, University of Virginia 2008, Spring Visiting Faculty Member, University of British Columbia 2001 - 2004 Chair, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia 1993 - 2001 Professor, University of Virginia 1995 - 1997, 1998 - 1999 Director of Graduate Studies, University of Virginia 1984 - 1993 Associate Professor, University of Virginia 1992, Spring Visiting Scholar, Department of Psychology, Stanford University 1987 - 1988 Associate Chair, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia 1986, Spring Visiting Scholar, Department of Psychology, University of Washington 1979 - 1984 Assistant Professor, University of Virginia 1977 - 1979 Assistant Professor, Duke University 1974 - 1977 Research Assistant, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan 1975 - 1976 Teaching Assistant, University of Michigan 1974 Research Assistant, Highway Safety Research Institute, University of Michigan ACADEMIC AND PROFESSIONAL HONORS Elected to the National Academy of Sciences (2020) Member, Virginia Academy of Sciences (2020) Research Award, 2020, University of Virginia Winner of the 2019 Daniel M.
    [Show full text]
  • Mid-Course Retreat
    TEMPLETON SCIENCE OF PROSPECTION AWARDS MID-COURSE RETREAT August 8-10, 2015 Union League of Philadelphia A special thank you to The John Templeton Foundation TABLE OF CONTENTS Sponsors 1 Steering Committee 3 Agenda Overview 7 Presentation Schedule 9 Project Descriptions 11 SPONSORS The Prospective Psychology Project Supported by a grant from the John Templeton Foundation, the University of Pennsylvania Positive Psychology Center has established the Prospective Psychology Project to advance the scientific understanding of prospection, or the mental representation of possible futures. To foster this new field of research, the Prospective Psychology Project announced the Templeton Science of Prospection Awards competition in 2013. The awards encourage research aimed at understanding prospection, particularly how prospection is measured, mechanisms of prospection, and how prospection can be improved and applied in a broad array of disciplines. To learn more about Prospective Psychology, please visit www.prospectivepsych.org. The University of Pennsylvania Positive Psychology Center The Positive Psychology Center promotes research, training, education, and the dissemination of Positive Psychology. This field is founded on the belief that people want to lead meaningful and fulfilling lives, to cultivate what is best within themselves, and to enhance their experiences of love, work, and play. Positive Psychology has three central concerns: positive emotions, positive individual traits, and positive institutions. Understanding positive emotions entails the study of contentment with the past, happiness in the present, and hope for the future. Understanding positive individual traits consists of the study of the strengths and virtues, such as the capacity for love and work, courage, compassion, resilience, creativity, curiosity, integrity, self-knowledge, moderation, self- control, and wisdom.
    [Show full text]
  • The Interpretive-Sensory Access Theory of Self-Knowledge: Empirical Adequacy and Scientific Fruitfulness
    Contents lists available at Vilnius University Press Problemos ISSN 1392-1126 eISSN 2424-6158 2020, vol. 97, pp. 150–163 DOI: https://doi.org/10.15388/Problemos.97.12 Pažinimo ir sąmonės filosofija / Philosophy of Mind The Interpretive-Sensory Access Theory of Self-Knowledge: Empirical Adequacy and Scientific Fruitfulness Paulius Rimkevičius Institute of Philosophy Vilnius University E-mail [email protected] ORCID https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6172-3275 Abstract. The interpretive-sensory access theory of self-knowledge claims that we come to know our own minds by turning our capacities for knowing other minds onto ourselves. Peter Carruthers argues that two of the theory’s advantages are empirical adequacy and scientific fruitfulness: it leaves few of the old discoveries unexplained and makes new predictions that provide a framework for new discoveries. A decade has now passed since the theory’s introduction. I review the most important developments during this time period regarding the two criteria: whether the theory’s six main predictions were supported, and whether the theory’s predictions contributed to new empirical studies. I argue that the interpretive-sensory access theory of self-knowledge remains empirically adequate and scientifically fruitful. Keywords: self-knowledge, Peter Carruthers, interpretive-sensory access, empirical adequacy, scientific fruitfulness Interpretacinės-sensorinės prieigos savižinos teorija: empirinis adekvatumas ir mokslinis vaisingumas Santrauka. Interpretacinės-sensorinės prieigos savižinos teorija teigia, kad mes sužinome savo pačių mintis nukreipdami į save pačius savo gebėjimus, skirtus kitų žmonių mintims sužinoti. Peteris Carruthersas įrodinėja, kad du iš šios teorijos pranašumų yra jos empirinis adekvatumas ir mokslinis vaisingumas: ji paaiškina ligtolinius atradimus ir pateikia naujų numatymų, kurie suteikia pagrindą naujiems atradimams.
    [Show full text]
  • Risk Realization, Emotion, and Policy Making
    Missouri Law Review Volume 69 Issue 4 Fall 2004 Article 8 Fall 2004 Risk Realization, Emotion, and Policy Making Chris Guthrie Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarship.law.missouri.edu/mlr Part of the Law Commons Recommended Citation Chris Guthrie, Risk Realization, Emotion, and Policy Making, 69 MO. L. REV. (2004) Available at: https://scholarship.law.missouri.edu/mlr/vol69/iss4/8 This Conference is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at University of Missouri School of Law Scholarship Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Missouri Law Review by an authorized editor of University of Missouri School of Law Scholarship Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Guthrie: Guthrie: Risk Realization, Emotion, and Policy Making Symposium: Risk Realization, Emotion, and Policy Making Chris Guthrie* We live in scary times. We face the prospect of such fearful events as the spread of SARS and other dangerous diseases;' repeated terrorist attacks on American soil; 2 the loss of loved ones in war;3 the tainting of our food supply with mad cow disease and other toxins;4 and so on. As Professor Fei- genson and his colleagues put it, "[t]hreats seem to abound." 5 The anticipation of such threats evokes powerful emotion. In their study of terrorism and SARS, Professor Feigenson and his colleagues report "sig- nificant positive correlations between people's risk perceptions and their negative affect."6 In their review of the judgment and decision-making litera- ture, Professor Slovic and his colleagues document the interplay between reason and emotion in assessing risk.
    [Show full text]