Dedicated to the memory of my mentor Conny Nordin, Professor of General Psychiatry and Royal Court Chaplain. Thank You for inspiring me to clinical research, for all the bad jokes and for challenging discussions on ethics.

“Seek and You will find” (Matthew 7:7)

List of Papers

This thesis is based on the following papers, which are referred to in the text by their Roman numerals.

I Wikström, S., Ley, D., Hansen-Pupp, I., Rosén, I., Hellström- Westas, L. (2008) Early amplitude-integrated EEG correlates with cord TNF-alpha and brain injury in very preterm infants. Acta Paediatrica, 97(7):915-9 II Palmu, K., Wikström, S., Hippeläinen, E., Boylan, G., Hell- ström-Westas, L., Vanhatalo, S. (2010) Detection of 'EEG bursts' in the early preterm EEG: visual vs. automated detection. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121(7):1015-22 III Wikström, S., Lundin, F., Ley, D., Hansen Pupp, I., Fellman, V., Rosén, I., Hellström-Westas, L. (2011) Carbon dioxide and glucose effects on EEG background in extremely preterm in- fants. Pediatrics, in press. IV Wikström, S., Ley, D., Rosén, I., Hansen Pupp, I., Norman, E., Fellman, V., Hellström-Westas, L. The predictive value of early single-channel aEEG/EEG in extremely preterm infants. Manuscript

Reprints were made with the permission of respective publishers.

Contents

Introduction ...... 11 Preterm birth ...... 11 Survival and outcome of very preterm infants ...... 11 Brain damage in preterm infants ...... 12 The electroencephalogram (EEG) ...... 14 The normal EEG of the preterm infant ...... 15 Abnormal preterm EEG ...... 17 Single-channel aEEG/EEG...... 17 The amplitude integrated EEG (aEEG) trend ...... 18 Monitoring EEG continuity measures ...... 22 Factors affecting preterm EEG and brain function ...... 23 Cerebral haemorrhagic lesions ...... 23 White matter damage ...... 23 Cerebral ischaemia...... 24 Carbon dioxide ...... 24 Glucose ...... 25 Medications ...... 25 Perinatal inflammation ...... 25 Aims ...... 27 Study I ...... 27 Study II ...... 27 Study III ...... 27 Study IV ...... 27 Patients and Methods ...... 28 Ethical considerations ...... 28 Clinical settings in the NICU ...... 29 Diagnosis of neonatal brain injury ...... 29 EEG recordings ...... 30 aEEG trend analysis ...... 30 Single-channel EEG analysis ...... 31 Follow up ...... 32 Patients and methods in study I ...... 33 Patients ...... 33 Methods ...... 33

Material and methods in study II ...... 34 Dataset ...... 34 Methods ...... 34 Patients and methods in study III ...... 35 Patients ...... 35 Methods ...... 35 Patients and methods in study IV ...... 36 Patients ...... 36 Methods ...... 36 Statistical analysis ...... 37 Results ...... 38 Results in study I ...... 38 Results in study II ...... 39 Results in study III ...... 40 Results in study IV ...... 41 Discussion ...... 43 Visual vs. Automated analysis of EEG background ...... 44 Early aEEG/EEG and brain damage ...... 46 Early aEEG/EEG and perinatal inflammation ...... 49 Carbon dioxide effects on EEG background ...... 49 Glucose effects on EEG background ...... 50 Improving long term monitoring of EEG in preterm infants ...... 52 Conclusions ...... 55 Future perspectives ...... 56 Swedish summary ...... 57 Acknowledgements ...... 59 References ...... 61

Abbreviations

aEEG Amplitude integrated EEG aEEG/EEG aEEG with corresponding EEG ASA Acute stage abnormalities BS Burst-suppression BSID-II Bayley scales of infant development II CBF Cerebral blood flow CPAP Continuous positive airway pressure cUS Cerebral ultrasonography DC Discontinuous EEG Electroencephalogram EPT Extremely preterm FFT Fast Fourier transformation GA Gestational age IBI Interburst interval IQR Interquartile range IVH Intraventricular haemorrhage ln Natural logarithm MDI Mental developmental index MRI Magnetic resonance imaging NDI Neurodevelopmental impairment NICU Neonatal intensive care unit NLEO Non linear energy operator NPV Negative predictive value PaCO2 Partial pressure of carbon dioxide in arterial blood PDI Psychomotor developmental index PNA Postnatal age PPV Positive predictive value PVL Periventricular leucomalacia SAT Spontaneous activity transient SWC Sleep-wake cycling TD Tracé discontinu VPT Very preterm WMD White matter damage

Introduction

This thesis work, which was performed with the overall aim to contribute to improved care of vulnerable preterm infants, is based on studies investigat- ing features of single-channel EEG background in infants born at 22-30 weeks gestational age (GA) in relation to perinatal factors and outcome.

Preterm birth According to the World Health Organization (WHO), preterm birth is de- fined as birth at less than 37 completed gestational weeks. Infants who par- ticipated in the studies in this thesis were born ‘very preterm’, before 32 gestational weeks or ‘extremely preterm’, i.e. before 28 completed gesta- tional weeks. Gestational age was, in almost all infants, estimated from fetal ultrasound at 17-18 gestational weeks which is standard in Sweden.

WHO has estimated that preterm birth constitute 10% of all births world- wide. Preterm birth is accordingly an important cause of neonatal morbidity in the world [1]. In Sweden 4.8% of all births were registered as preterm during 2008 [2] and 1% of the children are born very preterm, i.e. around 1000 infants in Sweden annually. About 20-30% of preterm births are attrib- uted to iatrogenic preterm delivery, i.e. due to maternal or fetal conditions such as preeclampsia, intrauterine growth restriction. Spontaneous delivery after preterm labour occurs in around 70-80% of preterm births. The cause is not always identified (idiopathic preterm birth) but especially in very early preterm labour and preterm premature rupture of the membranes, concurrent intrauterine infection and inflammation is common [1, 3]. Statistical risk factors linked to preterm birth include medical conditions of the mother or fetus, socioeconomic factors, genetic influence and infertility treatments [1].

Survival and outcome of very preterm infants During the 1980’s and 1990’s survival of extremely preterm infants (EPT) increased as a result of improvements in antenatal and neonatal care. In a large study cohort of extremely preterm infants, the EPICure study, includ- ing infants born in the UK and in Ireland during 1995, survival rates to dis-

11 charge of live born infants were: 11% at 23 weeks GA; 26% at 24 weeks and 44% at 25 weeks [4]. EXPRESS (EXtremely PREterm infants Study in Sweden), a more recent Swedish population based observational study of infants born at 22-26 weeks GA between 2004 and 2007, reported a 1-year survival in live born infants of 10% at 22 weeks GA; 53% at 23 weeks; 67% at 24 weeks, 82% at 25 weeks and 85% at 26 weeks GA [5].

The increased survival in EPT infants is associated with higher rates of ‘in- tact survivors’ and lowered prevalence of cerebral palsy [6-9], but also with an increase in the absolute number of surviving infants with neurodevelop- mental impairment (NDI) [6]. Attention is increasingly focused on the long- term neurocognitive morbidities in the extremely preterm survivors [10]. In the EPICure cohort 24% of the children had severe disabilities at 30 months [11], and when tested at 11 years age 45% of the children had serious func- tional disability compared with 1% of control classmates. Cerebral palsy occurred in 17%, but the most prevalent impairment was suboptimal cogni- tive function (in 40%) [10]. Reduced cognitive performance in extremely preterm born individuals seems to persist into late adolescence [12-13] and EPT born children are at increased risk for later psychiatric disorders, in- cluding attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), mood disorders, and autism spectrum disorders [13-15].

”Illness severity indices” or ”risk indices” are commonly used for assessing mortality or morbidity risks, especially for adjustments of study populations in quality of care assessment and clinical research. Such scores include vari- ous items of physiological risk factors that can be recorded during early postnatal life. Both the revised clinical risk index for babies (CRIB-II) and simplified version of score for neonatal acute physiology (SNAPPE-II) have primarily been validated as predictors of mortality risks, but also evaluated for predictors of NDI [16-18].

Brain damage in preterm infants Brain injury associated with haemorrhagic lesions and white matter damage (WMD) is common in very preterm infants. Periventricular and intraven- tricular haemorrhages (IVH) appear on neonatal cerebral ultrasonography (cUS) in 10-45% of EPT infants, most often during the first three postnatal days [5, 19-21]. The incidence of IVH increases with decreasing GA [5]. The “intraventricular haemorrhage” (IVH) originates in the highly vascular periventricular germinal matrix. The severity of IVH is usually classified according to Papile [22] (Figure 1): Grade I: a subependymal germinal ma- trix haemorrhage; Grade II: intraventricular progress of a germinal matrix bleeding without dilatation of the ventricle system; Grade III: intraventricu-

12 lar haemorrhage with ventricular dilatation; and Grade IV: refers to a haem- orrhagic periventricular infarction.

The long-term consequences of IVH’s are variable. Children with IVH grade I or II have, in some studies, no increased risks of handicap. However, EPT infants seem to have an additional risk of cerebral palsy in association with minor IVH due to their immaturity [19, 23]. Intraventricular haemorrhage grade III is overall associated with a 50% risk of neurodevelopmental handi- cap, and in IVH grade IV the risk is about 80%.

The risk of IVH is closely related to GA, but also to individual intrinsic fra- gility of the germinal matrix vasculature and to fluctuations in cerebral blood flow (CBF) [20, 24]. Fluctuations in CBF may be related to hypotension and hypertension, and loss of autoregulation, (‘pressure passive regulation’) which is common in unstable extremely preterm babies [25-26]. Further- more, hypercapnia during the first postnatal days is associated with an in- creased risk for severe IVH in preterm infants [27].

White matter damage (WMD) may be focal or diffuse (Figure 1), and the aetiology is multi-factorial. Cystic periventricular leucomalacia (cPVL), or focal WMD is associated with ischaemia and is nowadays quite rare; on histological evaluation it is recognized as necrosis and macroscopic cyst formation. Diffuse WMD is an ischaemic and/or inflammatory chronic dis- turbance involving myelination of the white matter, and is characterized by injury to the myelin producing oligodendrocytes, astrogliosis and infiltration of microglia [28]. Diffuse WMD is a common finding on magnetic reso- nance imaging (MRI) studies of extremely preterm infants [29]. The border zones between long penetrating arteries and the end zones of short penetrat- ing arteries (‘watershed areas’), where the circulation of the immature brain is not yet fully developed, are the most vulnerable regions for WMD [30]. Preterm infants with moderate-severe WMD on MRI at term equivalent age have a very high risk (50-70%) for developing later NDI [29].

Perinatal infection and fetal inflammatory response including the cytokines TNF-α, IL1-β and IL6 may initiate preterm labour and is also strongly asso- ciated with increased risk for WMD in preterm infants [31-32]. Cytokines act locally and have short half life after secretion [33]. In the brain they can be produced by microglia and astrocytes and especially interleukin-1 (IL-1) and Tumor Necrosis Factor-alfa (TNF-α) contribute directly to cytotoxic injury to immature oligodendrocytes and neurons [30].

13 IVH grade IV Diffuse WMD

cPVL

IVH grade I

IVH grade II-III

Figure 1. Illustration of IVH grades I, II-III and IV respectively, cPVL and diffuse WMD. Figure by Siw Wikström

Cerebral lesions can be diagnosed by cerebral ultrasonography (cUS) and MRI (and by autopsy in non-survivors). Repeated cUS examination is the standard bedside method for early neuroimaging of preterm infants in the NICU. Diffuse echodensity is often the first sign of WMD. Periventricular echodensities persisting for more than seven days are usually considered to be evidence of established WMD. Even though cUS is less reliable than MRI for detection of diffuse or subtle white matter damage [34-35], major lesions detected by cUS are associated with lower IQ and impairment at school age [35]. Among EPT infants without neonatal signs of major structural pathol- ogy on repeated cUS, neurodevelopmental impairment may still occur in as many as 30% of the infants [19, 36]. Optimal investigations for detection of WMD, as well as gray matter injury, are preferably performed at term equivalent age by using MRI [29].

The electroencephalogram (EEG) The electroencephalogram (EEG) is a recording over time of brain derived voltage gradients from electrodes placed over the scalp. The sources of the EEG signal are synchronized postsynaptic currents evoked mainly in the vertically oriented cortical pyramidal cells [37].

14 The EEG activity is modulated by interaction between cortical and subcorti- cal (e.g. thalamus) structures and thereby synchronized, which results in rhythms of the cortical activity. During the second trimester a structure called the subplate zone is present in the fetal brain. The subplate is the ori- gin of thalamo-cortical and cortico-cortical afferents that build up during development and is critical for modulation of preterm EEG activity [38].

The EEG, including continuous long-term monitoring, provides an approach to assessment of brain function and dysfunction in real time [39-40], and will thereby also provide additional information to structural cerebral imaging.

The normal EEG of the preterm infant For clinical purposes conventional neonatal EEG recordings are generally performed with 8-20 electrodes positioned according to the International 10- 20 System modified for neonates [41].

Interpretation of neonatal EEG demands knowledge of normal EEG devel- opment from early preterm to post term age. In general, neonatal EEG activ- ity has been discussed in terms of the development from a discontinuous to a continuous EEG [42-43]. The normal EEG background of extremely pre- terms, called tracé discontinu (TD) [43-44], shows a basically dichotomous pattern. Periods of low voltage activity, called interburst intervals (IBI), al- ternate with high voltage ‘bursts’ of activity with mixed frequency content. Several normal transients, e.g. frontal sharp transients and temporal theta (- bursts), have been identified in the preterm EEG, and can be useful for matu- rational assessment of the EEG [43, 45].

The frequency distribution of the discontinuous preterm EEG, is dominated by delta activity, with as much as 80% of the relative power within frequen- cies below 1 Hz in infants <32 weeks GA [46] while theta and alpha activity is increasingly recognized towards full term. A correlation between postcon- ceptional age and power of the delta and theta bands, respectively, has been reported. [46]

In the bursts of normal preterm EEG, fast activity is superimposed on slow waves of high amplitude [45]. Such complexes have also been named ‘spon- taneous activity transients’ (SATs) [47-48]. This spontaneous and intermit- tent activity has in animal models been recognized as a specific phenomenon that occurs during brain development [47, 49-51] and is suggested to be in- strumental for wiring neuronal networks [47, 49, 51].

15 It is important to recognize that ‘burst’, when mentioned in the context of bursts in the discontinuous preterm EEG, denote ‘physiological activity bursts’ or ‘SATs’. In contrast, ‘bursts’ as in ‘burst-suppression’ specify a different entity, a pathological EEG pattern with iso-electric IBI [43, 52] that can be seen in patients of all ages after major cerebral insults or during heavy sedation.

With increasing maturation, the duration of bursts increase and the IBIs be- come shorter, and there is also gradually more ongoing EEG activity during the IBIs [42-43, 45, 48]. In this way EEG background activity changes gradually towards higher continuity [43, 48]. It has been postulated that the gradual development of continuous activity is physiologically distinct from the development of bursts, and that it demands functional thalamocortical and cortico-cortical connections. These develop during the third trimester and consequently this activity is usually not appearing in EPT infants [47].

Several studies have provided reference data for IBIs at different matura- tional levels, based on visual quantification of preterm conventional EEG [42, 53-59]. Different definitions of bursts and IBI have been used, for ex- ample numerical criteria on maximum signal voltage during IBI varied be- tween 15 µV or 30 µV (Hahn, Vecchierini and Biagioni, Selton, Haya- kawa,Victor respectively). Hahn et al. consequently demonstrated a consid- erable difference in IBI values depending on which definition (i.e. thresh- olds) that was used [55]. Strict amplitude criteria are limited in that they are specific to the recording device, filter settings and electrode distances. Still, a summary of reference data show that in all studies the maximum IBI was below 60 seconds, and mean IBI was 6-14 seconds, in healthy infants at 25- 28 weeks GA [42, 53, 55-56, 58].

Changes in behavioural state, observed as alterations in eye movement, res- piration, heart rate, overall movements and muscle tone, are associated with alterations in EEG activity. At term age quiet sleep (QS) is characterized by high-voltage slow activity or discontinuous EEG (‘tracé alternant’) [43-44]. Active sleep is characterized by continuous EEG (and rapid eye movements, irregular breathing and some body movements) and wakefulness by continu- ous EEG (and by general body movements). Arousal and sleep states are regulated by brain stem and hypothalamic connections of the ascending re- ticular activating system [60]. In this manner sleep-wake cycling (SWC) as expressed in the EEG reflects specific physiological correlates of functional connectivity.

Cyclic variation of the EEG background has been reported in infants from 25 gestational weeks, although more commonly appearing after 27-30 weeks [61-64]. It remains somewhat unclear whether very early appearing of

16 cyclicity in the EEG background is equivalent to later appearing sleep stages. It was previously validated in amplitude-integrated EEG (aEEG) that cyclicity at 32-34 gestational weeks corresponded to QS and active sleep/wakefulness, respectively [65].

Abnormal preterm EEG Abnormal features in the preterm EEG can be characterized as acute or chronic stage abnormalities. This categorization of the EEG has proved to be very useful since acute changes in the electrocortical activity recover in a similar way, also after severe insults. Persisting chronic stage abnormalities are commonly markers of established injury [66].

Acute stage abnormalities (ASA) appear during and after an insult, and in- clude changes in continuity (longer IBI and absence of continuous patterns), frequency (attenuation of faster frequencies) and amplitude (lowered ampli- tude range) with associated loss of SWC and reactivity [66]. Presence of such EEG features has been associated with haemorrhagic and ischaemic brain damage in preterm infants [67-70]. Maruyama et al showed, in very preterm infants, that the maximum degree of ASA during the first postnatal days was associated with the risk for later development of cerebral palsy (CP) and also correlated with the severity of the CP [71].

Chronic stage abnormalities denote persisting and characteristic EEG ab- normalities appearing when ASA have disappeared. These include dys- mature and disorganized patterns and have been associated with poor neuro- developmental outcome in very and moderately preterm infants [66, 72-74]. A dysmature pattern is characterized by a maturational delay of more than two weeks. Increased IBI alone is difficult to assign to either acute or chronic abnormalities as it may represent either an acute suppression or an immature pattern. Disorganized patterns are characterized by abnormal background patterns and waveforms, e.g. positive rolandic sharp waves (PRSW), which are markers of established WMD and predictive of cerebral palsy if frequent [74].

Single-channel aEEG/EEG Single- or double-channel aEEG/EEG have become readily available tech- niques for continuous non-invasive monitoring of brain function in both infants and adults [40, 75-76].

17 The amplitude integrated EEG (aEEG) trend The aEEG is a trend measure and a monitoring technique, derived from a single bipolar EEG montage. It has its’ origin in the Cerebral Function Monitor (CFM) designed in the 1960’s for continuous monitoring of resusci- tated adults [77]. A single- or two-channel EEG is recorded from a pair of bi-parietal electrodes or from two pairs of electrodes, one pair over each hemisphere (commonly frontal-parietal or frontal-central leads) [39]. The EEG processing to aEEG includes asymmetric band pass filtering which attenuates activity below 2 Hz and above 15 Hz in order to reduce artefacts and technical interference. Other important steps of aEEG processing is rec- tifying and smoothing of the signal, time compression and especially a semi logarithmic display (linear at 0-10 µV and logarithmic display above this voltage) in order to increase sensitivity for detection of changes in low am- plitude background activity [39, 78]. In this way the aEEG technique enables continuous assessment of long term changes in cortical background activity. Modern aEEG trend recording is always combined with display of the origi- nal EEG signal from the same recording channels. This facilitates discrimi- nation of true EEG findings from artefacts. The term ‘aEEG/EEG’ in this thesis denotes a single-channel EEG recording from which the aEEG trend or other measures may be derived, together with a possibility to assess the corresponding original EEG trace, while ‘aEEG’ indicates that only the aEEG trend was assessed or possible to assess.

The aEEG trend is, like the conventional EEG, mainly interpreted through visual pattern recognition, including assessment of continuity and disconti- nuity, appearance of sleep-wake cycling and identification of activ- ity. The aEEG’s sensitivity to changes in the low voltage range also makes this trend prone to external interference which often elevates the lower bor- der of the amplitude span when the electrocortical activity is depressed. Consequently, quantitative analysis of aEEG maximum and minimum ampli- tudes should be performed with caution. In the discontinuous preterm EEG, the lower border of the aEEG trend does normally define the peak-to-peak amplitude of the IBI, but may also be influenced by the duration of the IBIs [39]. The upper border on the other hand, represents the peak-to-peak ampli- tude of bursts. In some recent monitoring devices these amplitudes can also be directly quantified from the filtered signal. Artefacts, mainly from muscle activity and electrical interference, may disturb long term aEEG monitoring and reduce its’ clinical value [79-80], which is especially important to con- sider in the extremely preterm population, potentially vulnerable even to non-invasive EEG monitoring.

The very early aEEG background pattern has a very high predictive value in asphyxiated term infants [81]. Although the clinical value of aEEG and

18 aEEG/EEG is less established in preterm infants, several authors have re- ported reference values in stable preterm infants (Table 1 a and b). In paral- lel with EEG development, also aEEG background becomes increasingly continuous with maturation, and the most stable maturational feature seems to be increasing amplitude of the lower border during the most discontinuous part of the recording (i.e. QS periods in more mature preterm and term in- fants), if cycling is present. Furthermore, some investigators have also re- ported correlation between early aEEG abnormalities and preterm brain damage [61, 82-85]. Only a few studies have reported the prognostic value of early aEEG/EEG in VPT and EPT infants [61, 84-86], and to our knowl- edge only two previous studies reported outcomes beyond the neonatal pe- riod. There is, consequently, a need for more studies assessing the relation between long-term monitored aEEG/EEG background features, neonatal morbidity and long-term outcome. Furthermore, to aid the visual analyses there is also a need for studies applying quantitative measures reflecting the preterm discontinuous EEG.

Table 1 a and b. (Following pages). Summary of studies on aEEG and aEEG/EEG in stable preterm infants. LB = Lower border of the aEEG trend; UB = Upper border of the aEEG trend; DC = Discontinuous; PNA = Postnatal Age; PMA = Postmen- strual Age.

19

Results Rising LB = strongest maturational feature Increasing GA associated with gradual increase of LB; Average duration of quiet sleep 19-20 minutes SWC appeared at mean GA 28 weeks and median PNA 6 days correlated with GA total sum score Sub scores and and PCA; Cyclicity, continuity and bandwidth was indicators of maturation the most sensitive Higher GA associated with more continuous activity and fewer bursts/hour; Continuity and median amplitude increased for every day over the first week Trend for decrease in first week SEF during the aEEG matured with PMA; Development of continu- ity accelerated in EPT infants. li d S aEEG/EEG assessment assessment aEEG/EEG UB and LB aEEG amplitudes UB and LB am- Serial aEEGs (n=56); plitude, SWC Sleep wake cycling (SWC) for continuity, Serial aEEGs. Scores cyclicity, LB amplitude, bandwidth and sum score. Pattern: discon- aEEGs first 2 weeks; tinuous high/low voltage, continuous, bursts/h; aEEG daily first week; Automated quantification: Activity above different amplitude thresholds; Median aEEG Pattern: Conti- Serial aEEGs (n=119); nuity; SWC, Low/high base voltage, Upper high voltage, Span e g Term, preterm (n=10 and 19) Normal at 18 months GA <30w, n=38, No neonatal neurological complications GA 24-39 w, n=30, No no seda- HIE, IVH or PVL, tive, no malformation, GA 23-29 w, n=75, Stable, normal ultrasound and neonatal outcome GA 24-31 w, n=63; Neona- IVH2-4 or tal survival, no PVL GA 25-32 w, n=31, No PVL , IVH 3-4, Included infants and fol- Included infants and low up PCA 30-43 w, n=107, Normal at dischar

] 87 Author Viniker et al [ Thornberg, Thiringer[88] Kuhle et al[63] Burdjalov et al [89] Olischar et al [64] West et al [90] Sisman et al [91]

20 V µ < 5 < y e pattern g litude and % of activit p Increasing PMA associated with decreased IBI, Increasing PMA associated with decreased IBI, Higher GA and higher PNA associated with more con- tinuous activity Increasing maturation associated with decreased inter- cycle bandwidth and increased intercycle LB; No cycles at 24-30 w PCA Increasing GA/PMA was associated with increased con- tinuity and SWC, more mature bandwidth; At the same PNA: lower GA and higher PNA associated with more mature aEEG First week: GA correlated with aEEG lower margin amplitude and correlated negatively with % activity < 5 µV. GA and PNA contributed equally to lower margin am Results Higher GA and higher PNA associated with increased likelihood of continuous activity and lower likelihood of DC low volta V µ litude < 5 p 20 minutes ≥ in am g Repeated aEEGs (n=92); Pattern: con- tinuous; discontinuous high/low volt- age; nearly isoelectric Cycle character-Serial aEEGs (n=86); istics in developed SWC. weekly to Serial aEEGs first 72 h then discharge (n=624); Pattern: discon- tinuous high/ low voltage, continuous; SWC; Bandwidth Weekly aEEGs (n=79; 4-6/infant); Automated quantification of aEEG amplitudes and % of activity with lower mar aEEG/EEG assessment assessment aEEG/EEG Relative duration of aEEG patterns: discontinuous high/low voltage, con- tinuous; SWC Weekly single-channel EEGs from infant); Automated first week (4-6/ quantification of NLEO-based IBIs by detection algorithm. . y Normal at 1 ; GA <32 w , n=18, Neona- tally stable AGA, No: as- phyxia, IVH>1, sedative, or BPD Normal at 1 y. Included infants and Included infants and follow up GA 23-29 w, n=98, No ventilation, sepsis or hy- potension, no IVH/PVL, GA 24-34 w, n=56, No neona- normal IVH III-IV, tal outcomeno malforma- tion. No GA 24-35 w , n=32, IVH/PVL, sedation, or malformation; Normal neonatal outcome GA 25-34 w (PMA 25-42 w), n=96, Stable, no ultra- sound abnormality; Nor- mal neonatal outcome GA <32 w , n=18, Neona- tally stable AGA, No: as- phyxia, IVH>1, sedative, or BPD Author Klebermass et al [92] Herbertz et al [93] Kuint et al [94] Soubasi et al [95] Niemarkt et al [96] Niemarkt et al [97]

21 Monitoring EEG continuity measures Previously used methods for visual quantification of aEEG or long-term recorded limited-channel EEG include burst counts (aEEG), measurement of IBI (conventional EEG), and continuity assessments of the aEEG/EEG background, e.g. to measure the percentage of recording with amplitudes of the aEEG or original EEG above a certain voltage threshold IBI [53, 84-86, 90]. Just like the case of aEEG amplitudes, however, reliance on strict ampli- tude measures may be sensitive to interference “adding to” the EEG signal amplitude.

Based on physiological considerations [47-48], detection and long-term monitoring of bursts/SATs and IBIs in the preterm could provide useful in- formation for evaluation of functional maturation and overall brain activity [47].

Algorithms have been developed for detection of EEG bursts and interburst intervals utilising various signal features [98-100]. A segmentation algorithm by Särkelä et al [98] was developed for adult EEG, and software based on this algorithm is currently available in the Nervus/NicoletOne system (Care- Fusion, San Diego CA, USA). The basic idea of the segmentation algorithm is to use the output of a non-linear energy operator (NLEO), as presented by Plotkin and Swamy [101], which classifies electrocortical activity as either ‘burst’ or ‘interburst’. NLEO values reflect both amplitude and frequency content of the analysed signal. Absolute values of NLEO values are smoothed by a sliding window. If the smoothed NLEO value exceeds a burst threshold during a defined period of time, EEG is classified as burst. If the same value stays below another threshold for a certain period of time, then EEG is classified as ‘suppression’ (i.e. interburst, in a normal discontinuous preterm EEG). If neither of these conditions is met, or if the NLEO value exceeds an artefact threshold, the EEG is classified as artefact/continuous EEG. See Figure 2 for an example of NLEO-based segmentation.

22

Figure 2. Example of burst-interburst segmentation by NLEO algorithm. Above: Original single-channel EEG (P3-P4 lead) Middle: Detection status with output in relation to original EEG. 1 = IBI. 2 = Burst. Below: Smoothed NLEO-output, basis for burst detection status. Courtesy of Heidar Einarsson

Factors affecting preterm EEG and brain function Cerebral haemorrhagic lesions Development of an IVH is associated with acute EEG and aEEG depression, the degree and duration of the EEG depression associated with the severity of the IVH [67-69, 82, 84, 102]. It is also previously shown in children sur- viving with large IVH’s, that the number of bursts/hour in the aEEG trend during the first 48 hours is predictive of outcome [84]. Suspected seizures, and even status epilepticus, were frequently seen in association with IVHs in early aEEG recordings, however, without possibility of verification by the original EEG [61, 82]. Seizures were also detected in conjunction with echo- dense lesions (haemorrhages and/or WMD) in two-channel EEG recordings [103]. Absence of sleep wake cycling is usually associated with the overall depression of electrocortical activity in conjunction with development of haemorrhagic lesions.

White matter damage Hypoxic-ischaemic insults, including low cerebral blood flow associated with hypocarbia, may be associated with acute changes in the EEG. How- ever, the correlation between appearance of WMD and a previous possible insult is often difficult to establish. As a consequence, acute and early EEG changes during development of WMD are difficult to assess with certainty

23 due to the difficulty in establishing an early reliable diagnosis of WMD. Chronic changes appearing in the EEG, such as positive rolandic sharp waves (PRSW) are indicators of established brain injury, and especially presence of PRSW is regarded as a marker of white matter injury and predic- tive of an increased risk for cerebral palsy [74]. Disorganized and dysmature EEG patterns [70, 73] have also been identified as sensitive markers of WMD. A correlation between severity of WMD and decreased EEG Spectral Edge Frequency (SEF) was also found in one study, with some indication that EEG changes may be present early in WMD development [104]. In a recent study, the severity of both acute- and chronic stage abnormalities were correlated with severity of PVL, with the most important association during the first days and during the second week, respectively [70].

Cerebral ischaemia Low cerebral blood flow (CBF) has been coupled to transient EEG or aEEG background depression in preterm infants [105]. In preterm infants, low car- diac output and hypotension have been associated with EEG suppression [106-107]. At low mean arterial blood pressure (<23 mm Hg) relative delta power decreased, EEG amplitudes were lowered and IBIs were prolonged [107]. Gavilanes et al demonstrated very clearly in newborn piglets, how aEEG amplitudes decreased in conjunction with acute anemic hypotension and returned when blood pressure and blood volume were restored [108].

Carbon dioxide Carbon dioxide has a depressant effect on central nervous system activity, in its extreme form known as carbon dioxide narcosis. Three previous studies indicate that high arterial carbon dioxide levels in preterm infants are associ- ated with EEG background depression and changes in brain stem auditory responses [109-111]. The mechanisms for these reactions are not known in detail, although effects on ion-channels via intra- or extracellular cerebral pH have been suggested. A reversible association between increasing carbon dioxide levels and EEG, mainly slowing and amplitude depression, is well known from adult subjects and animal studies [112].

Carbon dioxide is also a powerful regulator of cerebral blood flow (CBF) [25, 113]. Hypocarbia may through vasoconstriction impair CBF and a clini- cal relevant association between early hypocapnia and ischaemic brain injury is established [114-115]. Hypercarbia on the other hand, cause vasodilatation and has been associated with increased risk of hemorrhagic damage [27]. Experimental data from neonatal rats also indicate that moderate hypercarbia could be neuroprotective, while extreme hypercarbia has been linked to in- creased risk for brain damage [116-117].

24 Glucose Both hypoglycaemia and hyperglycaemia are common in extremely preterm infants due to immature physiology and need for parenteral nutrition [118]. Although hypoglycaemia was previously considered to be the main risk, hyperglycaemia is currently increasingly recognized as a risk factor for brain damage and death in very preterm infants [119-120]. Experimental data also indicate that hyperglycaemia makes the brain more vulnerable to ischaemic insults [121-122], and this may consequently be a contributing factor to brain injury in unstable preterm babies.

Only a few studies have addressed neurophysiological effects from hypogly- caemia in newborns [123-125]. Koh et al showed that sensory evoked poten- tials were attenuated at blood glucose levels lower than 2.6 mmol/L in chil- dren (including five newborns). This level has since then been regarded as the lowest accepted level for neonatal blood glucose in many neonatal inten- sive care units, since it was also supported by epidemiological data [126].

Medications Administration of certain medications such as sedatives, opioids, antiepilep- tics and surfactant commonly depress electrocortical activity to an extent that the evaluation of EEG or aEEG is affected. A bolus dose of morphine can suppress an otherwise normal neonatal EEG, and especially in preterm infants turn a normal discontinuous pattern into burst-suppression [127]. Not only bolus doses, but also infusion of morphine may cause profound sup- pression of EEG background [128].

The effect on the EEG is dependent on immaturity, dosage and timing of administration in relation to the recording [127, 129-131]. However, little is known about the duration of specific drug effects on the EEG, but recent studies indicated considerable inter-individual variation in recovery time after bolus doses of antiepileptics [132]. A study assessing effects of the ‘InSureE’ procedure (‘intubation, surfactant, extubation’) on aEEG in pre- term infants showed prolonged depression of the background activity, up to 24 hours after administration of 100 µg/kg morphine [133]. It is also shown that caffeine increase aEEG amplitudes throughout a 2-hour period after administration of a bolus dose [134].

Perinatal inflammation Beside the important role in the pathogenesis of WMD, TNF-α also modifies synaptic activity and may cause cell membrane hyperpolarisation [135], making it still more interesting in terms of acute neural dysfunction in rela-

25 tion to inflammatory response. One study has previously investigated possi- ble associations between perinatal inflammation, early EEG and brain injury in preterm infants [136]. However, that study could not demonstrate any association between endotoxin levels after birth, acute stage abnormalities in the EEG, or development of PVL, respectively. In contrast, data from fetal sheep suggested that changes in very slow EEG activity could be used to detect effects of inflammation on cerebral brain function [137]. Furthermore, a recent publication stated that EEG delta activity increased in association with chorioamnionitis in fetal sheep, and also reported a correlation between delta activity and cortical, as well as white matter, microglia activation [138].

26 Aims

The overall aim of this thesis was to characterize single-channel EEG and aEEG of very preterm infants in relation to specific influences on brain func- tion by using long-term monitoring techniques with focus on quantitative analysis of EEG background. The specific aims are listed below.

Study I To investigate if quantitative measures of the early single-channel aEEG/EEG are associated with neonatal brain injury, perinatal inflammation and long-term outcome in very preterm infants.

Study II To test the inter-rater reliability of visual burst detection in the early preterm EEG and to assess visual burst detection in the early preterm EEG in relation to an automated burst detector based on a non-linear energy operator (NLEO).

Study III To investigate if arterial carbon dioxide and plasma glucose levels are asso- ciated with EEG continuity and frequency content during the first days after birth in extremely preterm infants.

Study IV To describe early aEEG/EEG of very preterm infants in relation to two-year outcome and to identify aEEG/EEG features predictive of outcome. Fur- thermore to compare the prognostic information of single channel EEG monitored over several hours versus time restricted recording quantified with an automated algorithm for interburst interval detection.

27 Patients and Methods

Ethical considerations The clinical studies were performed at Lund University Hospital and study protocols approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board at Lund Univer- sity.

Any physical risks from EEG monitoring are considered to be very low and to our knowledge only minor skin irritation after adhesive tapes has been reported by others, although not to our awareness in the current studies. Very thin subdermal needle electrodes were used in study I. This type of electrode has been part of standard monitoring of infants, before the introduction of hydrogel electrodes, and they seem to cause very little discomfort. We have not recognized any infections or other side-effects of these electrodes. The hydrogel electrodes, used in studies II-IV, are gentle to the very sensitive skin of extremely preterms after soft preparation of the skin [39]. Still there are, of course, ethical issues that need to be addressed when setting up stud- ies involving newborn infants. The main ethical issue second to risk is the patients’ autonomy. The infants have not themselves decided on their par- ticipation in a study. In the present studies informed and written consent was obtained from the parents, in line with the Helsinki Declaration [139]. In the discussion of consent, an important aspect is the assumed position of de- pendence towards the clinicians, experienced by parents of extremely pre- term infants during the first postnatal days of their children. In this case, consent was performed prior to delivery, before the initiation of NICU care.

The studies are descriptive and will not benefit the participating individuals. The Helsinki Declaration states that incompetent subjects (e.g. preterm in- fants), ”must not be included in a research study that has no likelihood of benefit for them unless it is intended to promote the health of the population represented by the potential subject, the research cannot instead be per- formed with competent persons, and the research entails only minimal risk and minimal burden”. We propose that the potential gain of knowledge re- garding preterm neurophysiology, with the purpose of future improvements in neonatal care, meets this criterion.

28 Clinical settings in the NICU The infants in the included studies were all born at Lund University Hospi- tal, Sweden, a regional referral perinatal center with a tertiary level neonatal intensive care unit (NICU). Gestational age was in all cases determined by prenatal ultrasound, usually performed at 17-18 weeks of gestation accord- ing to Swedish routines. All included infants received antenatal steroids. According to Scandinavian tradition, early treatment with continuous posi- tive airway pressure (CPAP) was encouraged and extremely preterm infants were not routinely intubated after birth. Nevertheless, a majority of infants still needed early surfactant and were supported by mechanical ventilation. Moderate permissive hypercapnia with a PaCO2 goal of 5 to 7 kPa was prac- ticed during mechanical ventilation. Peripheral oxygen saturation was aimed at a range of 88 to 92% in extremely preterm infants, and mean arterial blood pressure at, or above, a level corresponding to the infant’s gestational age in weeks. Umbilical venous and arterial catheters as well as peripheral arterial catheters were inserted on clinical indication. Arterial blood gases and plasma glucose values were checked repeatedly as clinically indicated, and analysed in the NICU within a few minutes after sampling.

Most extremely preterm infants received oral feedings with donor breast milk from the first hours from birth and were also supported with i.v. 10% glucose infusion, with additional amino acid and lipid infusions after the first 24 hours [140]. An overall aim was to keep plasma glucose levels between 3 and 6 mmol/L. When study I was performed, the clinical standard of practice was to administrate continuous infusion of morphine (10-20 (µg/kg)/h) to mechanically ventilated infants. In the other studies, bolus doses of mor- phine (0.05-0.1 mg/kg), and sometimes continuous morphine infusion, were administered as indicated by validated pain scores to mechanically ventilated infants, but not routinely.

Diagnosis of neonatal brain injury Infants in the reported studies had repeated cUS performed on days 1, 3 and 7, at 3 and 6 weeks, and at term equivalent age. The examinations were per- formed by experienced neonatologists, and image data were also reviewed by paediatric radiologists. Germinal matrix and intraventricular haemor- rhages were classified according to Papile [22]. White matter damage was defined as periventricular echodensities persisting more than seven days, or periventricular cysts [141]. Cerebral MRI at term equivalent age was per- formed in infants participating in studies III and IV but data were not in- cluded in the present studies.

29 EEG recordings A single-channel EEG was the basis for analysis in all four studies. The EEG was recorded in the NICU at Lund University Hospital with a Ner- vus/NicoletOne EEG System with U16 amplifier, sampling rate 256 Hz, high pass filter at 0.16Hz and low pass filter at 500 Hz (Taugagreining HF, Reykjavik, Iceland/CareFusion, San Diego CA, USA). Electrodes were ap- plied at the P3 and P4 positions according to the International 10-20 System, plus a frontal reference electrode. In study I thin subdermal needle electrodes were used and in studies II, III and IV hydrogel electrodes (Ambu® Neu- roline, Ambu A/S, Ballerup, Denmark). All recordings were made during the first three days from birth, as soon as possible after initial stabilization at the NICU.

In studies I, III and IV, the aEEG trend was used at 6 cm/h for an initial identification of major artefacts, ongoing interference (e.g. from high fre- quency oscillatory ventilation) and seizure activity. Artefacts, interference and seizures activity thereafter confirmed in the single-channel original EEG were excluded from analysis. After selection of appropriate parts of the re- cordings (see below) quantitative aEEG and EEG output were exported to text files that were used for calculations. aEEG trend analysis In study IV, the aEEG trend, displayed at 6 cm/h, was visually rated in 4- hour blocks by two investigators together, blinded for the patients’ identity and clinical data. The assessment of the aEEG background pattern was re- peatedly aided by inspection of original single-channel EEG at 15 mm/s and 100 µV/cm sensitivity. The aEEG background pattern of every epoch was interpreted as burst-suppression, discontinuous or continuous according to Hellstrom-Westas et al, [142] and thereafter the activity dominating the 4-h epoch was further classified into 6 categories: 0 = Inactive/flat, 1 = Sparse BS, 2 = Dense BS, 3 =Mix of both BS and discontinuous (DC), 4 =Discontinuous (DC), 5 =Continuous (C). Cyclicity was rated in 4 catego- ries: 0 = Absent, 1 = Imminent, 2 = Established but immature, 3 = Devel- oped and mature.

In study I, minimum and maximum amplitudes of the aEEG [39] were iden- tified in 1-second epochs with the Nervus/NicoletOne software (exported to text files) and averaged during the whole study period.

30 Single-channel EEG analysis For visual detections of bursts in paper II, we selected an 11-minute epoch from each recording from places where the traces contained minimal arte- facts and consisted of clear epochs of tracé discontinu. Three investigators (SW, LHW, SV) chose visual criteria (see below) for burst identification and each reader marked the duration of all bursts in the selected EEG epochs. Raters used the same display settings: filtering 0.16-30 Hz, sensitivity 200 µV/cm, time base 15 mm/s. A burst was defined as a distinct occurrence of cerebral activity with a slow (< 2Hz) component and associated faster activ- ity. Burst onset was defined as a clear deviation from the relatively inactive background activity (i.e. interburst). Due to the filter-generated rebounds of the slow components in the preterm EEG [143], the end of the burst was defined by the visual appearance of the trace returning to baseline (i.e. IBI), rather than a strict return to zero line.

In study IV, seizure activity was identified in the original single-channel EEG by an experienced neurophysiologist (IR) who examined all recordings (in total 2614 hours) at 10 mm/s and 70 µV/cm sensitivity. The duration of each seizure was measured and it was also retrospectively determined whether the seizure was detectable in the aEEG trend at 6 cm/h. Seizure activity was characterized by: A sudden appearance of an abnormal electri- cal event; lasting 10 seconds or more; with evolving, repetitive waveforms that gradually built up and then declined in frequency, morphology, or am- plitude [144].

Bursts and IBI were detected using the segmentation algorithm implemented in the Nervus/NicOne software. In study I the interburst intervals were aver- aged over the whole study period 0-72 hours postnatal age. In study III, in- terburst intervals were averaged from 10-minute artefact free epochs of EEG during the interval between 15 to 5 minutes before each blood sample analy- sis. In study IV, median interburst intervals were calculated 1-hourly over the whole study period and over four time intervals (0-24 h, 24-48 h, 48-72 h and 0-72 h). Interburst% (percentage of recording detected as interburst, analogue to burst% in study II) was calculated over the same epochs.

In study I, the median non-filtered peak-to-peak amplitude in single-channel EEG was calculated for a comparison with the aEEG amplitude measures.

Power spectral analysis in study III was performed using Fast Fourier Trans- formation (FFT) with a time base of 10 seconds. Spectral power was ana- lysed from artefact-free 5-minute epochs, obtained between 10 and 5 min- utes before sample analyses. For every 5-minute epoch, total and relative

31 (%) band power was calculated within the following frequency bands: delta (0.5-4 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz), alpha (8-13 Hz) and beta (13-30 Hz).

For comparison of findings in long term continuous monitoring and an in- termittent time restricted recording, one hour of high quality single-channel EEG was selected from each of the original long term recordings in study IV. This data set was selected as closely before 48 hours age as possible and met the following criteria: No ongoing artefacts or technical interference as identified in P3-P4 original EEG at 70 µV/cm, no handling of the infant according to charts or obvious from EEG artefacts and no morphine adminis- tered during the previous three hours. If periodic cycling activity was visu- ally present in aEEG the 1 hour epoch was selected 30 minutes before to 30 minutes after maximum continuity according to the aEEG trend. For the selected one hour epochs, the same quantitative measures of EEG as above were calculated.

Follow up Surviving children had neurodevelopmental follow up at two years corrected age. A psychologist evaluated the children with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID-II) [145], including two subscales, Mental scale and Motor scale. Scores were transformed into Mental Developmental Index (MDI) and Psychomotor Developmental Index (PDI), both with mean indi- ces of 100 and SD of 15. Children with MDI or PDI scores below 50 were assigned a score of 50.

The infants were also examined by an experienced clinician, who was un- aware of the EEG findings, performing a standardized neurological evalua- tion, including the Neurologic Optimality Score [146]. The Neurologic Op- timality Score has a maximum score of 78 and numbers below 74 are con- sidered suboptimal.

In study I, the overall neurodevelopmental function was also classified ac- cording to Scheffzek, a composite outcome for classification of handicap level that can be scored from the neurological and cognitive tests [84, 147]. The Scheffzek classification includes five categories: the lowest category, 0, denotes a neurodevelopmentally healthy child while a severely multihandi- capped child will be classified in category 4. In study IV, neurodevelopmen- tal impairment (NDI) was defined as presence of cerebral palsy, or MDI <70, or PDI <70, or blindness, or deafness. Good outcome was defined as surviving without NDI, and poor outcome was defined as death or surviving with NDI at 2 years corrected age.

32 Patients and methods in study I Patients Sixteen preterm infants (GA median 25.5 w, range 24-28) were prospec- tively recruited between October 2001 and February 2003. All infants re- ceived surfactant and required mechanical ventilation during the first postna- tal days.

Nine infants had neonatally diagnosed brain damage; five developed IVH grade 1-3, three had IVH grade 2-3 with WMD, and one infant had isolated WMD. In the other seven infants no abnormalities were detected by cUS. All 16 infants survived and had neurodevelopmental follow-up at two years of corrected age.

Methods Single-channel aEEG/EEG-monitoring was initiated at a median (range) postnatal age of 36.2 h (11.5-69) and with duration 18.6 h (3.0-55.3). The duration of the analysed EEG from each infant was 16.6 h (3.0-50.3).

Blood samples were taken from the umbilical cord at birth and from arterial catheters at 6, 24 and 72 hours postnatal age. Nine cytokines (IL-1β, IL-2, IL-4, IL-6, IL-8, IL-10, IL-12, TNF-α and IFN-γ) were analysed in plasma using cytometric bead array and flow cytometry technique as previously described [148].

33 Material and methods in study II Dataset The dataset used in this methodological study consisted of 10-minute seg- ments of single-channel EEG tracings from 12 extremely preterm (EPT) and 6 very preterm (VPT) neonates randomly chosen from the cohort of 54 in- fants (without knowledge about clinical data) described in study IV. Gesta- tional ages among the EPT neonates were 23-27 weeks, and in the VPT group 28-30 weeks.

Methods Three independent raters marked the recordings for activity that was classi- fied as ‘bursts’, without knowledge about clinical data. Visual detections of the raters were compared at each sampled time point (256 points per sec- ond). The proportion of overall agreement was evaluated in order to obtain a single value describing the inter-rater agreement in each recording. Three descriptive parameters of bursts were derived for each recording from the markings of each rater: number of bursts, average burst duration and propor- tion of time covered by bursts (burst%). Additionally, the frequency spec- trum of epochs that were unanimously detected as either burst or interburst was calculated using Fast Fourier Transformation (FFT).

Two different versions of the NLEO algorithm for automated segmentation were implemented. The first version was constructed in MatLab to mimic the algorithm used in the NicOne software for burst detection (already de- scribed). In the second version modifications were done (see paper II) in order to further improve the accuracy of detection. Results of the automated detections were compared with a gold standard in which only epochs where all three raters agreed on the classification of the EEG (burst/interburst) were included (Figure 3).

Figure 3. Study work flow in paper II. With permission from the Publisher

34 Patients and methods in study III Patients Thirty-two extremely preterm infants with median (range) GA 25 w (22-27) were included between July 2005 and May 2007. The infants constitute a sub-group of the infants in study IV. Included infants had a least three paired samples including arterial blood gas/plasma glucose and good quality EEG preceding the 15 minutes before the blood gas analysis, with at least 2 hours since morphine administration. Out of the 45 extremely preterm infants de- scribed in study IV, 13 were excluded from this analysis due to lack of such data.

Twenty-seven of the 32 infants needed mechanical ventilation, and five were supported by CPAP. Twenty-nine of the infants survived the first week and in 26 of there were no signs of a large IVH (defined as grade III-IV) accord- ing to cUS. Furthermore, no infants had WMD diagnosed with cUS.

Methods In total 247 sets of samples were included in the analysis, each sample con- sisting of a measure of PaCO2, plasma glucose, IBI and EEG relative and total band power (n=185 for EEG power samples). The median (range) number of included samples per infant was 8 (3-17).

35 Patients and methods in study IV Patients Fifty-four infants were prospectively included between July 2005 and May 2007, 45 of the infants were born extremely preterm (EPT) at less than 28 weeks’ GA, and 9 were born very preterm (VPT) at 28-30 weeks’ GA. Dur- ing the first 72 hours from birth, infants were cared for according to clinical routines, except for recording of a single-channel EEG, and additional blood sampling of cytokines and growth factors [149].

Methods Single-channel EEG recording was initiated at a median postnatal age of 8 hours with median (range) duration 56 (14-71) hours per infant. Single- channel aEEG/EEG analysis was performed as already described. The Clini- cal Risk Index for Babies (CRIB-II) [16] was calculated from data obtained within the first 6 hours. Surviving infants had follow up at two years cor- rected age (Bayley II and neurological examination).

36 Statistical analysis For group comparisons in study I the two tailed Mann-Whitney U-test was used. Spearman rank correlation coefficient was used for correlations.

In study II the three raters proportion of overall agreement was calculated according to Fleiss [150]. Use of kappa statistics was avoided because it is not suited for dependent samples like EEG time series [151]. Confusion ma- trixes were calculated at ‘point-by-point’ level for detection algorithms ver- sus golden standard (i.e. points when all three raters agreed). Based on these, accuracy, sensitivity and specificity of the algorithms were evaluated.

In study III, data were analysed as cross-sectional time-series together with multivariate regression splines [152]. Regression splines are functions that do not require a particular model assumption (linear, quadratic, cubic) for the dependent variable. Instead a set of functions (spline bases) are chosen dur- ing estimation from a general, larger set of functions. IBI and EEG power variables were used as outcome variables in regression analyses and were transformed using the natural logarithm (ln) to obtain approximately normal distributions. PaCO2, plasma glucose, postnatal age and gestational age were entered as predictor variables in a backwards stepwise procedure. For com- parison with previously published data [111], we analysed a restricted data set, comprised of measurements at one particular point of time in every in- fant, namely the first collected sample after 24 hours age. This dataset of strictly independent observations was analysed by linear regression with multivariate regression splines. Plasma glucose levels were also divided in four categories, representing strategies of the clinical care. In order to test for differences in EEG variables between glucose categories we used panel-data regression with glucose category as a single predictor variable.

In study IV, non-parametric statistics were chosen for group comparisons and for comparisons of EEG measures between separate postnatal intervals. In the extremely preterm group IBI, interburst% and aEEG amplitudes of the respective intervals (also including the low artefact 1-hour recordings), CRIB-II scores and ultrasonographic findings were one at a time evaluated as predictors of outcome in logistic regression models. ROC-curves for pre- diction of outcome were created and optimal cut-off values were determined from the values of combined sensitivity and specificity (likelihood ratio). Sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative predictive values (PPV, NPV) for prediction of poor outcomes were calculated for aEEG/EEG fea- tures, CRIB-II scores and cerebral ultrasonography.

37 Results

Results in study I Clinical data are presented in Table 2. The IBI, averaged over the study pe- riod, wereas prolonged in the nine infants with neonatal brain injury (IVH of any degree and/or WMD) as compared to the seven infants without injury, median (range) 11.8 (9.6-23.2) sec versus 8.2 (7.1-11.6) sec (p = 0.005). Both the maximum and minimum aEEG amplitudes were lower in infants with neonatally diagnosed brain injury, median (range) 22.5 (12.5-30.9) µV versus 31 (20.7-43.5) µV (p = 0.013) and 11.1 (6.1-14.6) µV versus 15 (10.2-20) µV (p = 0.009), respectively.

Table2. Neonatal data for infants in study I. Values are medians (ranges). * p<0.05 difference between infants with and without handicap at 2-years corrected age. No handicap Handicap (Scheffzek cat. = 0) (Scheffzek cat. = 1-3) n=8 n=8 GA (weeks) 27 (24-28) 25 (24-28) Birth weight (g) 955 (660-1205) 881 (680-1205) Apgar score, 5 min 7 (3-9) 6 (5-8) IVH/WMD 2* 7 (any grade) (n) Caesarean section (n) 3 5 Antenatal steroids (n) 8 8

The eight infants with normal neurodevelopmental outcome had signifi- cantly shorter IBIs than infants with handicaps, median (range) 8.7 (8.1- 23.2) seconds vs 11.8 (9.6-14.1) seconds, respectively (p=0.036). The maximum and minimum aEEG amplitudes also differed according to Schef- fzek category, Neurological Optimality Score and PDI. There were no asso- ciations between peak-to-peak EEG amplitude and brain injury or neurode- velopmental outcome.

Cord blood TNF-α showed a correlation with IBI (rs = 0.595; p = 0.025). We recognized no other direct correlations between cytokine levels and aEEG/EEG measures.

38 Results in study II Overall agreement of burst detections, at point-by-point level, between raters was 86% and 81% (EPT and VPT neonates respectively). See Figure 4 for an example of visual detection. Burst% showed a significant pair wise corre- lation between all raters and in both groups of infants. In manual markings, the burst duration increased with increasing maturation: Median burst dura- tion by the three raters were 4.3, 3.1 and 2.9 s, respectively, in extremely preterm infants, and 4.4, 4.8 and 3.3 s, respectively, in very preterm babies. Although very few bursts were longer than 10 s, burst lengths were variable between 1-10 s with a non-Gaussian positively skewed distribution. There was a peak in the middle frequency range (4-8 Hz) in bursts only.

The time points classified the same way by the NLEO algorithm and in agreed visual assessment were 87% and 79% of all sampled time points (EPT and VPT infants, respectively). The sensitivity of the original algo- rithm was 64% and 69% (EPT and VPT infants, respectively) and the aver- age specificity was 96% and 88%.

Figure 4. Detection of bursts in a VPT infant. Red bars indicate detections in the P3-P4 derivation by three raters. Unanimous detections in pink (‘burst’) and blue (“interburst”). Courtesy of Sampsa Vanhatalo.

39 Results in study III

There was a positive association between PaCO2 and IBI; higher PaCO2 was associated with longer IBI. This association was only present in samples from the 26 infants with good outcome. A 1 kPa increase in PaCO2 was overall associated with a 9% increase in IBI but the association between PaCO2 and IBI was stronger in the subset of data restricted to the first sam- ple after 24 postnatal hours, a 1 kPa increase in PaCO2 was there associated with a 16% increase in IBI.

There were significant non linear associations between PaCO2 and EEG power in all frequency bands, but only in the 157 samples from infants with good outcome. The association was strongest in the delta band. Total EEG power reached a maximum at a PaCO2 of 5.1 kPa and was attenuated both at higher and lower PaCO2. There were no associations between PaCO2 and relative band power.

Corrected for carbon dioxide effects, plasma glucose had a U-shaped asso- ciation with IBI in infants with good outcome. The lowest IBI appeared at a plasma glucose level of 4 mmol/L. There were no correlations between plasma glucose and EEG band power measures. Figure 5 shows the associa- tion between categorized plasma glucose and IBI.

Figure 5. Box plot demonstrating interburst intervals in relation to plasma glucose, but only from samples where simultaneous PaCO2 was within the intended range (5-7 kPa).

40 Results in study IV Twenty-two EPT and all eight VPT infants survived without NDI at two years of age (good outcome) while 8 EPT infants died in the neonatal period and 11 survived with NDI, i.e. 19 EPT infants had poor outcome.

5

4

3 GA 28 w+

GA<28 w good 2 GA<28 w poor

1

0

0-4 4-8 8-12 12-16 16-20 20-24 24-28 28-32 32-36 36-40 40-44 44-48 48-52 52-56 56-60 60-64 64-68 68-72

Figure 6. Background (BG) score development during 0-72 postnatal hours. Back- ground activity increased significantly from 0-24 hours to 24-48 hours but only in infants with good outcome.

Figure 6 shows the trends in background pattern over the study period. Some degree of cyclicity was identified in 25 out of 41 EPT infants, more often in infants with good rather than poor outcome (p = 0.029). Seizures were identified in the single-channel EEG from 21 infants (43%). In total 89 seizures, duration 12 seconds to 6 hours 10 minutes (median 60 s) were identified. Seizures were associated with neonatal brain damage but not with outcome.

Significant predictors of outcome in EPT infants included: presence or ab- sence of BS (aEEG background score ≤2) (p<0.001), continuous activity (aEEG background score = 5) (p = 0.038) and EEG cyclicity (p = 0.029).

The highest combined sensitivity and specificity for prediction of poor out- come was obtained from interburst% in the 1-hour low artefact recordings with a cut-off interburst% value >50% (Figures 7 and 8). A 10%-units in- crease in interburst% was associated with an odds ratio 2.0 for poor outcome (p = 0.005).

Findings of IVH or PVL during the first three days were not overall predic- tive of outcome. CRIB-II scores were lower in EPT infants with good versus poor outcome (p = 0.036). When studying survivors only, there were differ- ences in both visual and quantitative EEG background measures but not regarding CRIB-II scores, depending on outcome (NDI) at follow up.

41

Figure 7. Interburst% during the 1 hour low artefact EEG recordings close to 48 hours age. Data shown for EPT infants with good and poor outcome (n = 22 and n = 18 respectively), and VPT infants (n = 7), all with good outcome.

Figure 8. Interburst% in 1-hour low artefact EEG recordings, close to 48 hours age. Data shown for EPT survivors with and without NDI (i.e. presence of cerebral palsy, or MDI <70, or PDI <70, or blindness, or deafness) at two years corrected age.

42 Discussion

The results of the present thesis, obtained from analysis of early single- channel aEEG/EEG recorded over many hours, demonstrate that the electro- cortical background activity is an early marker of brain damage in very pre- term infants. In line with results in previous studies, the present data support the use of single-channel aEEG/EEG in preterm infants for monitoring aim- ing at early identification of brain damage. The early aEEG/EEG back- ground is also associated with other factors, including perinatal inflamma- tion since increased levels of TNF-α in cord blood were associated with aEEG/EEG depression. No previous study has reported such an association between pro-inflammation at birth and EEG activity. It was also demon- strated that PaCO2 and glucose levels considered to be within physiological ranges were associated with the most continuous EEG background in ex- tremely preterm infants. Study III also showed that increased EEG disconti- nuity may occur at carbon dioxide levels that are used in many NICUs for mechanical ventilation as part of lung protection strategies.

Study IV demonstrates that although the accuracy of prediction is moderate, early aEEG/EEG may be a better predictor of long-term outcome than cra- nial ultrasound abnormalities during the first week, or a clinical risk index such as the CRIB-II. The predictive value of 1-hour low artefact recordings at 48 hours age was at least equal to that of long term monitoring. This find- ing points out the importance of high recording quality and absence of drug induced EEG suppression during recording. Furthermore, we also demon- strate that brief epileptic seizures are common in extremely preterm infants during the first postnatal days, and that their presence is not invariably asso- ciated with a poor prognosis.

Besides common pattern recognition of aEEG background, automated detec- tion of bursts and interburst intervals was performed in these studies. Study II includes a detailed technical analysis of this method and also shows that inter-rater reliability is confidently high in visual detection of bursts in the preterm EEG. We further conclude that the evaluated NLEO-based segmen- tation algorithm is capable of accurate detection of bursts in early preterm EEG and that it therefore is valid as a scientific tool in clinical studies.

43 Visual vs. Automated analysis of EEG background All aEEG/EEG assessments begin with a visual inspection of the recording, and include an evaluation of the quality of the recording and presence of possible artefacts. This evaluation is the beginning of a structured evaluation of the aEEG/EEG trace, which is essential both for clinical monitoring and for research.

The aEEG/EEG recordings can be assessed according to several scoring systems, but in the current studies we have classified aEEG background pat- terns corresponding to established EEG terminology in order to be able to investigate and compare physiological findings. Furthermore, we repeatedly inspected the original single-channel EEG trace in order to confirm that the aEEG pattern was consistent with the original EEG background, since arte- facts such as high-frequency ventilation may severely influence the appear- ance of the aEEG trend. For seizure assessment, we analysed the original single-channel EEG trace since previous studies have shown that brief sei- zures will not appear in the aEEG trend.

As expected, most very preterm infants had discontinuous background pat- terns during the first postnatal days, although more continuous activity was seen in a few infants and associated with good long-term outcome. We also showed that BS was associated with poor outcome. However, this basis of evaluation probably works best in a one-way direction: the absence of BS supports a favourable prognosis in the same way as ‘better than expected’ presence of continuous background pattern. Although pattern recognition is an accepted method that has proven to be useful both for the clinical situa- tion and for research, additional quantitative measures (e.g. burst or inter- burst measures) may provide a more precise identification of brain damage and prediction of neurodevelopmental outcome. This is likely to be espe- cially important for analysis of long term recordings where detailed inspec- tion is time consuming and investigation of specific transients and symmetry is limited or lost due to the single-channel mode of recording.

The results of the presented studies support that automated quantitative analyses of EEG continuity (e.g. burst or interburst measures) are valuable in single-channel EEG monitoring, and that the analyses are in line with visual assessment by pattern recognition of the aEEG trend. Study I was, to our knowledge, the first to utilise automated analysis of IBIs over prolonged periods of time. Previous studies have manually quantified IBIs in very pre- term infants after visual analysis of the EEG recordings [42, 53, 55-59]. A disadvantage with manual estimation of IBI is that it is extremely cumber- some, and consequently this has previously not been done in long-term re- cordings. However, the present detector algorithm also has limitations when

44 data are collected over several hours, since it requires visual exclusion of EEG epochs with major technical interference.

The NLEO-based burst detector that was used in these studies already per- forms relatively well in the youngest age groups. However, as the neonatal EEG matures to a less dichotomous form [43-45] bursts become less promi- nent. Consequently, it can be expected that the distinction between bursts and interbursts is progressively more difficult with increasing GA, and our results may be applicable to earliest preterm ages only. The method has not yet been validated in term burst-suppression EEG.

The mean values of IBIs during the whole recording period of studies I and IV were 8.7 and 7.4 seconds, respectively, in infants with normal follow up. The obtained IBIs are generally comparable with the 6-18 seconds mean IBIs previously reported in healthy preterms of the same GA interval [42, 53, 55, 58]. However, as already pointed out, detailed comparisons may be difficult since studies using manual measurements of IBIs have applied dif- ferent amplitude and/or duration criteria regarding burst and interburst inter- vals. This ‘thresholding’ is likely to impact burst/interburst durations similar to the differences between raters in the thesis study II.

In contrast to results from studies using quantitative surrogate measures of continuity (i.e. derived from EEG amplitudes) [85-86], we used EEG meas- ures directly related to preterm developmental physiology and neurobiology, i.e. detection of bursts and IBI (or ‘SATs’ and ‘interSAT’), as suggested by some investigators [47, 86]. It is reasonable to hypothesize that the develop- mental burst/SATs events and/or IBIs are suitable markers of the brain’s well-being in preterm infants. The duration of averaged IBIs is consequently associated with outcome, as shown in the present studies I and IV. However, the most accurate quantitative EEG measure for prediction in study IV was interburst%, which is the percentage of the recording that is detected as IBI, and thus also reflecting the duration of burst activity. In study II we showed that burst% (complementary of interburst%) is less sensitive to detection thresholds and reflect discontinuity more consistently than the number of, or duration of, bursts and IBI.

The upper and lower borders of aEEG amplitudes in discontinuous traces reflect the amplitudes of bursts and IBI, respectively, but the amplitude of the lower border also reflects the length of IBIs [39]. Many studies in pre- term infants have applied manual quantification of aEEG amplitudes, and in study I we found that suppressed aEEG amplitudes were associated with brain damage and handicap. However, the amplitude values obtained by exporting 1-second epochs of aEEG in the NicoletOne software for further analysis are due to the short epoch lengths not comparable to visual analysis

45 since these amplitude measures actually represent an unselected mixture of amplitudes during both bursts and interburst. Niemarkt et al used the same software and exported longer epochs (10-15 s) to obtain amplitudes that were more comparable to the visual on-screen appearance of the aEEG trend [96].

Early aEEG/EEG and brain damage Healthy preterm infants seem to have increasing electrocortical activity dur- ing the first days after birth, and our results are in line with a few previous reports although none has evaluated this phenomenon specifically [58, 85, 106]. It is difficult to assess whether these early changes represent matura- tional changes, or a recovery from the birth process. Interestingly, the early improvement of EEG background was present only in infants with good outcome. Hence, absence of background improvement may be indicative of brain dysfunction with long term importance.

A majority of IVHs develop during the first three postnatal days [153], and our findings that IVHs are accompanied by transient aEEG/EEG background depression is also in accordance with previous reports [61, 82-85, 102, 154]. Some investigators have noted that aEEG or EEG background suppression sometimes is observed prior to the time point when brain injury is demon- strated by cUS [68, 82, 85, 103]. If such early markers in the aEEG/EEG could be identified with high accuracy, this would open up for neuro- protective strategies and consequently the time course of injury and the sen- sitivity/specificity of the above findings have to be further addressed.

The usefulness of EEG for prognostic purposes in newborn infants has been suggested since the 1960’s [155], and both IBIs and other measures of in- creased discontinuity have been related to adverse outcome in very preterm infants. In 1988 Connell et al reported on the long-term prognostic value of continuous two-channel EEG during the first 72 hours in infants of 34 weeks or less GA [68]. Prolonged IBI was associated with severe IVH and the prognostic values of EEG and repeated cUS were very similar. However, an additional prognostic value of the EEG was noted in infants with IVH grades II-III, since outcome was diverse in these infants and not predicted by cUS alone. In study IV, only presence of IVH grades III-IV predicted outcome, and the aEEG/EEG measures were overall better than cUS in the context of outcome prediction. Visually assessed aEEG continuity during the first three days was likewise associated with severity of IVH and two-year outcome in a cohort of preterm infants with mean GA 25 weeks [61]. In another cohort of very preterm infants with large IVHs, the maximum number of bursts/hour, manually counted in the aEEG trend during the first 24-48 hours

46 of from birth was predictive of long-term outcome [84]. More recently, both Bowen et al [85] and West et al [86] studied quantitative measures derived from the amplitude of two-channel EEG during the first 48 hours among infants born at less than 29 weeks GA. Bowen et al analysed three 2-h EEG epochs and found that EEG activity <80% at the 10 µV-level was a marker of poor short-term outcome, with PPV 73% and NPV 86% for death or any IVH. West et al assessed 1-hour of aEEG and quantitative measures (activity at 25 µV-level) of two-channel EEG recording in 76 infants and reported PPV 41% and NPV 84% for prediction of poor outcome until a median age of 15 months.

Burst-suppression is associated with poor outcome in term infants. We showed in study IV that presence of BS is associated with poor outcome also in extremely preterm infants. A closer assessment of this predictor shows that the sensitivity for prediction of poor outcome was relatively high (89%), but that specificity was lower (64%), with PPV 68% and NPV 88%. This is in line with overall findings on outcome prediction based on aEEG/EEG in preterm infants where NPVs for prediction of adverse outcome are generally are higher than PPVs, i.e. a ‘normal’ aEEG/EEG pattern strongly suggests a good outcome. [66, 85-86, 156].

One reason behind the modest specificity of BS as predictor of poor outcome is that transient BS may develop after administration of medications such as morphine or other opioids, also in preterm infants with good outcome [129]. It was also recently shown that BS patterns in aEEG are common in preterm infants developing sepsis but does not seem to have any influence on long- term aEEG maturation or short term outcome [157]. In the aEEG, a tracé discontinu (TD) pattern shows some variability of the lower border ampli- tude, while a BS is usually very flat without variation. However, the impor- tant distinction between the normal TD [43] in preterm infants and BS may be difficult , especially in the aEEG trend [158]. This is a main reason to repeatingly confirm aEEG classification by inspection of the single-channel EEG.

Seizures and brief rhythmic discharges are markers of abnormal brain func- tion [159]. In previous studies, suspected seizures in aEEG were detected in 60-75% of preterm infants with IVH. [61, 82]. However, presence of sus- pected seizures was not predictive of outcome in infants with large IVHs [84]. Later aEEG/EEG studies (including study IV) have confirmed the as- sociation between brain injury, mainly presence of IVH, and seizures. Shah et al recently presented data on seizures, diagnosed in aEEG tracings with secondary confirmation by simultaneous two-channel EEG, in 51 preterm infants (median recording duration 74 h) [160]. Seizures were identified in 11 (22%) infants and associated with adverse neonatal outcome (death or

47 abnormal MRI) [160]. However, three out of 11 infants (27%) with seizures had no IVH. In a study by West et al [86], five out of 76 preterm infants had seizures in a 1-hour recording of two-channel EEG, and all five infants had a poor outcome.

A majority of preterm seizures are very brief; mean durations of less than one minute [161] and two minutes [162], respectively, have previously been reported and were also confirmed by the findings in study IV showing a median seizure duration of 60 seconds. The incidence of seizures among infants in study IV, despite the single-channel mode of recording, was 43% which is considerably higher than previously reported [160]. However, no previous study has assessed more than 2600 h of single-channel EEG during the first postnatal days in very preterm infants (median 56 h per infant). Al- though brief seizure events were common in our population, their presence did not correlate with long-term outcome. About half of the brief seizures (49%) were possible to detect in the aEEG indicating, in line with previous studies [163-165], that the aEEG trend severely underestimates the amount of seizures.

Presence of cyclicity in extremely preterm infants during the first postnatal days is associated with good outcome, as shown both in study IV and in a few other studies [61, 63, 85]. Crude sleep-wake cycling has previously been identified in aEEG and EEG from 25 gestational weeks, although more evi- dent after 27-30 weeks [57, 62-63]. Our findings show that cyclic variations in cortical activity may be visible very early in preterm life, although there is currently no data available demonstrating that this is an immature equivalent of sleep-wake cyclicity. The presence of early preterm cycling activity indi- cates the importance of considering activity states in the interpretation of even extremely preterm aEEG/EEG.

In summary, studies I and IV were, to our knowledge, the first to demon- strate that quantitative measures of EEG background, continuously moni- tored over several days, are associated with brain damage and predictive of two-year outcome in VPT and EPT infants. However, the predictive accu- racy of the very early aEEG/EEG is moderate and much lower than in full term asphyxiated infants [81]. A probable reason for this is that intercurrent illnesses, e.g. late onset sepsis, and other complications of prematurity may affect long-term outcomes in the preterm infants. Term infants, however, who have recovered from initial illness more often have an uncomplicated and stable course.

48 Early aEEG/EEG and perinatal inflammation TNF-α is considered a key cytokine in injury to neurons and oligodendrocyte precursors [30-31]. Data supporting a causal link between inflammatory response and cortical activity is scarce, but experimental studies show that administration of TNF-α to isolated newborn rat myenteric neurons leads to cell membrane hyperpolarization [135]. If similar mechanisms are present within the immature brain this could explain the association between in- creased levels of TNF-α and the EEG depression observed in our preterm infants in study I. A limitation of the study is the small number of infants. It should be regarded as a pilot study.

Results from studies using isolated hippocampal slices also indicate that glial TNF-α directly modifies synaptic activity through AMPA and GABA cir- cuits [166-168], but the relevance for this action in relation to EEG and pre- term brains is not known.

Carbon dioxide effects on EEG background The results in study III confirm findings of two earlier studies in preterm infants, also showing that increasing blood levels of carbon dioxide are asso- ciated with EEG depression [109, 111]. Victor et al only studied infants without large IVHs. We also included data from infants with severe ultra- sound abnormalities, but the association between carbon dioxide and elec- trocortical activity was only present in infants who survived the first week without major brain damage. These findings imply that the EEG response to carbon dioxide, in parallel with cerebral blood flow (CBF) autoregulation, may be disrupted in infants with brain injury [25, 113].

In vitro studies verify decreased neuronal excitability at hypercapnia, where decreased field excitatory postsynaptic potentials were found dependent on changes in intra- or extracellular pH, respectively [169-170]. Recently, spon- taneous network events in neonatal rat neurons were effectively suppressed by increased carbon dioxide levels. The effect was proportional to, and it was concluded that it was also mediated by, intracellular neuronal acidosis [171]. Even small acid load affected the spontaneous networks events, indi- cating that neonatal neurons are very sensitive to changes in intracellular pH [171].

It is unlikely that the suppression of cortical activity in study III was medi- ated by changes in cerebral blood flow. The CO2-CBF-reactivity will in- crease CBF with increasing CO2 [25] while decreased neuronal excitability occurs also in vitro where the CBF variable has been eliminated [172].

49

Murdoch Eaton et al reported that both respiratory and metabolic acidosis was associated with EEG changes [109]. In contrast, Victor et al found no relation between blood pH and EEG. Experimental data show that hydrogen ions not readily diffuse over the blood brain barrier [173] and carbon dioxide may thus have larger effect on cerebral pH and neuronal excitability. We found in study III that pH was to 80% determined by PaCO2. We had very few samples with metabolic acidosis, and since base deficit is a calculated value, rather than directly measured, we abstained from analysing effects from metabolic acidosis on EEG. However, if the EEG suppression is medi- ated by cerebral pH it would be highly relevant to further study the duration of EEG depression at hypercarbia, since neuronal intracellular pH is likely to be rapidly corrected [169] and changes in excitability may therefore depend on both the extent of carbon dioxide fluctuation and the rate of CO2 change.

Permissive hypercapnia is commonly used as a lung protection strategy dur- ing mechanical ventilation of preterm infants. By acceptance of mild to moderate hypercapnia (6-7 kPa) high tidal volumes and pulmonary over distension can to some extent be avoided. A certain degree of hypercarbia may also reduce the risk of unintended hypocarbia which is associated with risk of WMD [174]. Several RCTs and retrospective studies have investi- gated the safety of permissive hypercapnia and found no increased risk of IVH or adverse neurodevelopmental outcome related to hypercarbia [175- 177] although one study indicated that large fluctuations of PaCO2 were predictive of lower BSID results [178]. The increased discontinuity at high PaCO2 (and high plasma glucose) levels should be interpreted as a sign of functional neuronal impairment rather than established neuronal damage, at least initially but any neurodevelopmental consequences of the association between hypercapnia and EEG suppression is currently not known. These findings emphasize the monitoring properties of the single-channel aEEG/EEG as compared to its use for diagnostic or prognostic purposes.

Glucose effects on EEG background The depletion of substrate needed to maintain neuronal function may well explain any EEG depression during hypoglycaemia. In rodent hippocampal neurons, glucose depletion effectively blocked spontaneous network events [171]. However, neurophysiological evidence demonstrating that hypogly- caemia adversely affects brain function or EEG in newborn infants is, as already discussed, very limited [123-124]. Stenninger et al evaluated effects of hypoglycaemia on the aEEG in 12 term infants of diabetic mothers. Hy- poglycaemia (mean blood glucose 1.5 mmol/L) was associated with a subtle, but statistically significant reduction in maximum aEEG amplitude [124].

50 Furthermore, a few case reports have shown transient flattening of the aEEG during severe hypoglycaemia [39]. In contrast, hypoglycaemia was not asso- ciated with measurable changes in various EEG parameters (aEEG minimum amplitude and continuity, and spectral edge frequency) in newborn lambs and in preliminary data from newborn infants above 32 gestational weeks [125, 179]. Moderately preterm and term infants, as well as newborn lambs, have more continuous EEG than the extremely preterm infants in the present study. Consequently, measures of EEG continuity (such as IBI) may be more sensitive for evaluating preterm infants than term infants. Furthermore, given the normal EEG discontinuity in preterm infants, measures of continuity may be more directly correlated to brain function than power measures. Compa- rable to the studies by Harris et al, we did not find any changes in relative band power related to glucose levels, indicating that we would also not have been able to demonstrate major changes in spectral edge frequency.

Hyperglycaemia is common in preterm infants where immature physiology and need for parenteral nutrition predispose for high plasma glucose concen- trations. No previous studies have, to our knowledge, investigated possible EEG changes during neonatal hyperglycaemia. Hyperglycaemia might act as an oxidative factor with negative effects on brain cells, especially oligoden- trocytes, which may be an important explanation of the long term conse- quences of hyperglycaemia [120], but unlikely of any importance as con- cerns rapid fluctuations in cortical activity along with changes in plasma glucose.

Skov and Pryds measured cerebral blood flow (CBF) in preterm infants with low blood glucose and found a rapid decrease in CBF after i.v. infusion of glucose [180]. They suggested the presence of a central glucose sensor which increases CBF at low glucose levels in order to provide sufficient amounts of substrate for brain function. Such mechanisms may explain the relation between moderate hyperglycaemia and EEG continuity in study III. The present findings are also supported by preliminary data in a group of 24 infants with GA <28 weeks where higher blood glucose concentrations dur- ing the first three days were associated with suppressed aEEG background [181].

A limitation of study III is that data consist of time series, but not with a natural course. Carbon dioxide and plasma glucose were measured on clini- cal indications and actions were taken to correct values towards the intended ranges. This compresses the range of observations around values that have been regarded as ‘optimal’. Samples therefore include relatively few very low or high values of both carbon dioxide and plasma glucose, and conse- quently the results should be interpreted with caution outside the clinically intended ranges. Any interaction between the effects of carbon dioxide, pH

51 and plasma glucose on the immature brain may be further investigated. Con- cerning plasma glucose it could be speculated that very high glucose concen- trations may indicate a more severe general illness, which in turn may con- ceal physiological associations between glucose and electrocortical activity.

Improving long term monitoring of EEG in preterm infants Artefacts are common in neonatal aEEG/EEG monitoring, and automated EEG analysis may be sensitive to such artefacts, e.g. movements or care procedures [79-80]. Some artefacts may be compensated for in automated algorithms, for example by baseline subtraction [182]. Seizure activity might also interfere with automated analysis such as burst detection, because both seizures and normal bursts have multifrequency, complex waveform pat- terns. In the present studies II, III and IV, epochs containing seizure activity were excluded from analysis. In future neonatal brain monitors, coincident and independent detection of seizures should be included in addition to background measures.

There are currently several lines of ongoing research and development of tools for automated background continuity analysis [100, 182-183]. The optimal strategies (for today’s technical solutions) may vary depending on aim: identifying BS or DC patterns, in contrast to continuous activity [184], segmentation of BS patterns in asphyxiated term infants [100], or quantifica- tion of discontinuous patterns in preterm infants [182]. For clinical purposes the ideal automated segmentation algorithm detector should have both diag- nostic (regarding acute brain damage) and prognostic value. Current litera- ture, including the first and last study of this thesis, suggests that quantifica- tion of bursts or IBIs in the youngest preterm population can provide valu- able information about both diagnosis and prognosis [68, 84]. For the future, improvements in burst/SAT detection may be achieved by development of algorithms independent of absolute EEG amplitude thresholds [182]. For example this could be done by employing supervised tech- niques where patterns are ‘learned’ through extraction of suitable classifica- tion features from samples of manually classified training data [100]. Ide- ally, the detector would be capable of distinguishing bursts/SATs from the ongoing oscillations that increase with GA in the preterm EEG.

A further question, partly addressed in study IV, is which indices derived from burst detection that gives the most valuable clinical information over longer periods of time (e.g. number of bursts, burst durations, IBIs, burst%, interburst%). Also, the statistical measures of burst occurrence (e.g. mean,

52 median, percentiles or variability measures), with the highest discriminatory properties for identification of abnormal brain activity have to be evaluated. In clinical use, the different combinations of predictive values, sensitivity and specificity of different methods must be balanced against the intended aims: High sensitivity measures will be needed for identification of infants at high risk of brain damage, where more specific investigation and closer sur- veillance may then be initiated. Further, high NPV of EEG measures may put more confidence behind a reassuring message in the situation of parent’s counselling when EEG indicates no abnormality. Additionally, any active intervention with potentially serious adverse effects will rather demand a high PPV and high specificity when unfavourable EEG measures are identi- fied. Also when non-survivors were excluded from analysis of study IV, the early aEEG/EEG (but not CRIB-II score or cUS findings) differed between infants who developed NDI and infants who had a normal outcome at two years. Beside this important indication on long term importance in compari- son with standard clinical data, the overall gain in predictive values from single-channel EEG compared to a combination of clinical data in the CRIB- II was small. In clinical use, the importance of improved prognostication must be balanced against any potential harm from the introduction of addi- tional procedures in the care of extremely preterm infants.

During early brain development, bursts/SATs are first focal events [47]. Hence, single-channel recordings may have low sensitivity to detect early SATs, and cannot assess the extent to which cerebral cortex is engaged by a single event. Also, burst waveforms in single-channel recordings depend on the locations of cortical events in relation to the electrode position. The am- plitude of some bursts is, for example, much higher than others just because of the distance to the recording electrode. This will together affect both vis- ual and automated burst detection. Therefore multi-channel EEG may be the choice of basic research on burst/SAT occurrence in relation to brain devel- opment. However, for clinical purposes it is reasonable to expect an accept- able trade-off with single-channel EEG in extremely preterm and extremely vulnerable infants.

The optimal number of electrodes for seizure identification in preterm in- fants is also unknown. A single- or limited-channel montage will not be able to detect all seizure activity [41, 158, 185]. Several authors have shown that about 80% of all seizures may be identified in aEEG with guidance of sin- gle/two-channel original EEG [41, 158, 186]. The aEEG trend in itself (without guidance of original EEG) on the other hand, seems to severely underestimate seizure occurrence [163-165], which is also very obvious from the present study IV.

53 Study IV addressed the clinically important questions of how (intermittently or continuous) and when (how early) to record single channel EEG/aEEG in extremely preterm infants. The time restricted (1-hour) recordings without any knowledge of the infant’s status, had low artefact burden as compared to the bulk of long term recordings and were chosen so that no morphine had been administered during the last three hours. The equal predictive value of short but low artefact recordings and data obtained by long term monitoring possibly suggests that detailed analyses of repetitive and attended conven- tional EEG should be performed for the purpose of optimal prediction. The feasibility of full-scale conventional EEG in early neonatal care can however be seriously questioned and detailed analysis of extremely preterm conven- tional EEG is certainly an expert issue.

Also, the value of very early (within hours from birth) aEEG/EEG is impor- tant to assess in high-risk very preterm infants. In study IV significant differ- ences in background activity between infants with good and poor outcome were at first identified very closely before 24 hours age, although some stud- ies report differences between infants with normal and adverse outcome also during the first postnatal day [71, 85]. Accordingly, it is important to further investigate the value of very early recordings as our results do not support the use of aEEG/EEG for predictive purposes on the first postnatal day in vulnerable extremely preterm infants.

Finally, I want to call attention to the separate purposes of EEG/aEEG re- cording: for prognostic use time-limited recordings of high quality may work well, but as indicated by study IV, still be insufficient for detection of sei- zure activity. Detection of developing background abnormalities in real- time, related to unexpected events also demands long-term monitoring.

54 Conclusions

The presented studies demonstrate that:

Automated NLEO-based detection of bursts or IBI has acceptable discrimi- natory properties and clinical relevance making it a useful scientific tool and a promising technique for long term monitoring of discontinuous EEG back- ground.

The aEEG trend and quantification of IBI in single-channel EEG provide moderate prediction of outcome in extremely preterm infants already after 24 postnatal hours, and also in the absence of IVH.

Awareness of recording quality is very important in single-channel aEEG/EEG analysis.

Background suppression according to single-channel aEEG/EEG monitoring is an early marker of brain damage.

Fetal inflammatory response, as signalled by increased level of umbilical cord TNF-α, may be associated with acute depression of neural function detectable in early EEG background.

Increasing EEG discontinuity occurs at carbon dioxide levels often accepted as part of lung protection strategies.

Plasma glucose concentrations are also associated with electrocortical activ- ity in extremely preterm infants.

The results may, in the future be used for early identification of infants at high risk of brain damage and adverse outcome and could also have implica- tions for early intervention.

55 Future perspectives

Monitoring brain function by aEEG/EEG may, already today, aid immediate detection of altered cerebral function secondary to other physiological dis- turbances. In this way monitoring may guide the care and indicate need for closer surveillance or specific investigations. To extend this possibility in preterms, more knowledge of specific pathophysiological and pharmacologi- cal effects on EEG background is needed. Furthermore, automated seizure detection may also be valuable tools for early detection of brain dysfunction.

The possibilities of direct action targeting the harmful processes behind pre- term brain damage and neuro-developmental impairment are small today. Early intervention programs for preterms seek to take advantage of the plas- ticity of the immature brain, in order to compensate for established damage [187]. In the future however, specific neuroprotective interventions may be developed and hence, also the possibilities to act when an unfavourable con- dition is recognized. In this perspective, further development of brain moni- toring and better methods for prediction of neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants will be vital for clinical action. Specifically, detection of neural dysfunction reflecting yet not established brain damage is requested. The neurophysiological time course of brain lesions should consequently be further addressed in this population, with focus on the early time-limit of prediction - when there still is time to act. Prognostic applications of early preterm aEEG/EEG are however, less probable to have implications on end- of-life decisions (withdrawal of care) in preterms as compared to asphyxi- ated term infants where the prognostic value is considerably higher.

From a future psychiatrist’s point of view, enhancement of early infant- parental attachment is interesting. In situations where parents trust and hope for the future of their child (and in worst case also their early psychological attachment) is failing, a reassuring counselling may be confidently based on prognostic favourable EEG findings when available.

56 Swedish summary

Avhandlingsarbetets övergripande syfte var att karaktärisera en-kanaligt EEG och amplitudintegrerat EEG (aEEG) hos mycket för tidigt födda barn i relation till hjärnskador, utveckling och fysiologi. I fokus var kvantitativa mått i långtidsövervakning av det omogna barnets diskontinuerliga elektro- kortikala bakgrundsaktivitet.

I arbete I undersöktes om kvantitativa mått i tidigt aEEG/EEG hade samband med strukturella hjärnskador synliga på ultraljud, utveckling vid två års ålder eller perinatal inflammation. Sexton barn med genomsnittlig gestationsålder på 25 veckor ingick i studien. EEG’ts interburstintervall (IBI) var längre hos barn med neonatala hjärnskador och hos barn som utvecklade handikapp än hos friska. TNF-α i navelsträngsblod korrelerade med IBI.

I arbete II undersöktes överensstämmelse mellan bedömare vid visuell detek- tion av “burstar” i EEG. Detta jämfördes sedan med en algoritm för automa- tiserad analys av IBI. Datamaterialet bestod av 10-minutersavsnitt av en- kanaligt EEG från tolv extremprematura och sex mycket prematura barn. Tre oberoende bedömare markerade “burstar” i EEG’t med en “overall- agreement” punkt-för-punkt på 86% hos de extremprematura och 81% hos de mycket prematura barnen. Sensitiviteten hos detektionsalgoritmen var 64% för extremprematura respektive 69% för mycket prematura barn. Speci- ficiteten var 96% respektive 88%. Algoritmen modifierades i nästa steg med syfte att förbättra precisionen.

I arbete III undersöktes om arteriella koldioxidnivåer (PaCO2) respektive plasmaglukos korrelerade med kortikal bakgrundsaktivitet 32 extremt pre- maturfödda barn.Totalt analyserades 247 blodprover med avseende på koldi- oxidnivå och glukoshalt, och simultant med detta kvantifierades IBI i enka- naligt EEG. Vi fann att längden på IBI ökade med stigande PaCO2. Detta samband förelåg dock endast hos barn som överlevde den första veckan utan hjärnskada. Vid ett dygns ålder innebar 1 kPa ökning i PaCO2 en förlängning av IBI med 16%. Korrigerat för koldioxidnivåer uppvisade plasmaglukos ett U-format samband med IBI som var kortast vid plasmaglukos på 4 mmol/l.

I arbete IV undersöktes det prediktiva värdet av tidigt en-kanaligt aEEG/EEG. Både visuell skattning och kvantitativ analys av EEG utfördes. I

57 studien inkluderades 41 barn med gestationsålder 22-27 veckor (extremt prematurfödda) och 8 barn med gestationsålder 28-30 veckor (mycket pre- maturfödda), och dessa följdes upp med utvecklingsbedömning hos psykolog och läkare vid två års korrigerad ålder. Förekomst av burst- suppressionmönster, förekomst av kontinuerlig bakgrundsaktivitet och före- komst av sömn-vakenhetscykler identifierades som prognostiskt betydelse- fulla i visuell bedömning av aEEG-trenden. Kvantifierade interburstintervall och interburst-% (den andel av registreringen som utgörs av IBI) var även de prediktiva för två-årsutfall och interburst% hade studiens högsta kombinera- de sensitivitet och specificitet. Epileptiform aktivitet sågs hos 21 barn (43%) och var vanligare hos barn med påvisbar hjärnskada. Sådan anfallsaktivitet hade dock ingen koppling till handikapp vid två års ålder.

Sammanfattningsvis visades att bakgrundsaktiviteten i enkanaligt aEEG/EEG är en tidig markör för hjärnskada och prediktiv för utveckling till två års ålder hos extremt för tidigt födda barn. Den elektrokortikala bak- grundaktiviteten uppvisar även samband med perinatal inflammation samt blodets koldioxidnivåer och glukoshalt. Den testade detektionsalgoritmen för automatiserad mätning av interburstintervall har prestanda som gör den an- vändbar i klinisk forskning. De aktuella resultaten gör det även troligt att kvantitativ analys av det prematura EEG’ts bakgrundsaktivitet (så som be- stämning av IBI) kan bli kliniskt användbara.

En utvecklad övervakning av det prematurfödda barnets hjärnaktivitet kan i framtiden bli användbar i syfte att tidigt identifiera barn med hög risk för hjärnskada och utveckling av handikapp. Att identifiera dessa barn kan idag ha betydelse för tidig riktad utredning men framgent kan detta även få bety- delse för aktiv intervention, om möjligheterna till specifik neuroprotektion utvecklas.

58 Acknowledgements

I want to express my warmest gratitude to all those who have supported this thesis work and provided the data for the studies, and especially:

My supervisor Professor Lena Hellström-Westas at the Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, Uppsala University. You have guided me through science with never failing enthusiasm, six years of patience and an extraordinary daily presence in my PhD project, despite physical distance.

My co-supervisor professor emeritus Ingmar Rosén at the Department of Clinical Neurophysiology in Lund, for sharing your knowledge in the field of neonatal neurophysiology and for supportive method criticism. It is an honour to be adviced by the Nestor.

My co-authors and collaborators at the Department of Pediatrics in Lund: Vineta Fellman, David Ley, Ingrid Hansen Pupp and Elisabeth Norman for sharing the data you have sampled and for valuable methodological input from varying angles.

My co-authors Sampsa Vanhatalo for mind challenging discussions and ex- ploring new roads. Kirsi Palmu for fruitfully bridging engineering and medi- cine. Fredrik Lundin for help matching data with methods in biostatistics, and also for hands-on computational work. Geraldine Boylan and Eero Hip- peläinen for valuable methodological input and interpretation of data.

The children who participated in our studies, and of course all their parents.

The NICU staff, including psychologists at Lund University Hospital, and especially research nurses Ann-Cathrine Berg and Eva Hammarstrand, cru- cial for carrying out the clinical studies.

Lars-Johan Ahnlide (Cephalon A/S), Heidar Einarsson, Gardar Thorvardson and associate professor Frank Wikström for valuable technical and computa- tional support.

Neonatologist and neurologist Erik Stenninger, Örebro, for introducing me into aEEG and for the recommendation to read Lenas publications.

59 My colleagues at the Department of Pediatrics in Karlstad, especially Karl- Gustav Ellström and Christina Lindblad for teaching me neonatology, Gun- nar Carlsson for a schedule that made research possible, Anna Hallström- Schönberg, Eva Albinsson and Maria Forssberg for great friendship.

The Department of Psychiatry, Karlstad: My senior colleagues Edvard Smith, Hans Lind and Rune Andersson and my boss Ingegerd Lindqvist for accepting, and even supporting, my rather split interests.

Dr. Mikael Hasselgren and the primary care R&D unit in Karlstad for an office and a research environment. The staff of Vårdcentralen Kronoparken for flexibility and very supportive attitude.

Göran Hellström, Lena’s husband for an understanding attitude when I called during weekends.

My parents Siw and Stig that supported the career decision I made at the age of three, for playing with Ivar and Alrik when I had to work during week- ends (and for Figure 1). My sister Cajsa for being my sister, for creativity and for camel-related topics. My parents-in-law, Monica and Bengt.

Angelica, my loved wife for patience with my absent-mindedness as well as my physical absence, and for Power-point assistance. My boys Ivar and Alrik - nu ska jag hinna leka med er igen!

The studies of this thesis were supported by grants from:

The Center for Clinical Research and County council of Värmland with per- sonal financial support that made my doctoral studies possible. Swedish Research Council. Linnéa and Josef Carlsson Foundation.

60 References

1. Beck S, Wojdyla D, Say L, et al. The worldwide incidence of preterm birth: a systematic review of maternal mortality and morbidity. Bull World Health Organ. Jan 2010;88(1):31-38. 2. Socialstyrelsen. Graviditeter, förlossningar och nyfödda barn - Medicinska födelseregistret 1973-2008. Stockholm: 978-91-86301- 73-6; 2009. 3. Goldenberg RL, Culhane JF, Iams JD, Romero R. Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. Lancet. Jan 5 2008;371(9606):75-84. 4. Costeloe K, Hennessy E, Gibson AT, Marlow N, Wilkinson AR. The EPICure study: outcomes to discharge from hospital for infants born at the threshold of viability. Pediatrics. Oct 2000;106(4):659- 671. 5. Fellman V, Hellstrom-Westas L, Norman M, et al. One-year survival of extremely preterm infants after active perinatal care in Sweden. JAMA. Jun 3 2009;301(21):2225-2233. 6. Wilson-Costello D, Friedman H, Minich N, Fanaroff AA, Hack M. Improved survival rates with increased neurodevelopmental disability for extremely low birth weight infants in the 1990s. Pediatrics. Apr 2005;115(4):997-1003. 7. Wilson-Costello D. Is there evidence that long-term outcomes have improved with intensive care? Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. Oct 2007;12(5):344-354. 8. Platt MJ. Long-term outcome for very preterm infants. Lancet. Mar 8 2008;371(9615):787-788. 9. Himmelmann K, Hagberg G, Uvebrant P. The changing panorama of cerebral palsy in Sweden. X. Prevalence and origin in the birth- year period 1999-2002. Acta Paediatr. Sep 2010;99(9):1337-1343. 10. Johnson S, Fawke J, Hennessy E, et al. Neurodevelopmental disability through 11 years of age in children born before 26 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics. Aug 2009;124(2):e249-257. 11. Wood NS, Costeloe K, Gibson AT, Hennessy EM, Marlow N, Wilkinson AR. The EPICure study: associations and antecedents of neurological and developmental disability at 30 months of age following extremely preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Mar 2005;90(2):F134-140. 12. Hallin AL, Hellstrom-Westas L, Stjernqvist K. Follow-up of adolescents born extremely preterm: cognitive function and health at 18 years of age. Acta Paediatr. Sep 2010;99(9):1401-1406.

61 13. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet. Jan 19 2008;371(9608):261-269. 14. Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, Hennessy E, Wolke D, Marlow N. Psychiatric disorders in extremely preterm children: longitudinal finding at age 11 years in the EPICure study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. May 2010;49(5):453-463 e451. 15. Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Adult outcome of extremely preterm infants. Pediatrics. Aug 2010;126(2):342-351. 16. Parry G, Tucker J, Tarnow-Mordi W. CRIB II: an update of the clinical risk index for babies score. Lancet. May 24 2003;361(9371):1789-1791. 17. Gagliardi L, Cavazza A, Brunelli A, et al. Assessing mortality risk in very low birthweight infants: a comparison of CRIB, CRIB-II, and SNAPPE-II. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Sep 2004;89(5):F419-422. 18. Lodha A, Sauve R, Chen S, Tang S, Christianson H. Clinical Risk Index for Babies score for the prediction of neurodevelopmental outcomes at 3 years of age in infants of very low birthweight. Dev Med Child Neurol. Nov 2009;51(11):895-900. 19. Patra K, Wilson-Costello D, Taylor HG, Mercuri-Minich N, Hack M. Grades I-II intraventricular hemorrhage in extremely low birth weight infants: effects on neurodevelopment. J Pediatr. Aug 2006;149(2):169-173. 20. Ballabh P. Intraventricular hemorrhage in premature infants: mechanism of disease. Pediatr Res. Jan 2010;67(1):1-8. 21. Larroque B, Marret S, Ancel PY, et al. White matter damage and intraventricular hemorrhage in very preterm infants: the EPIPAGE study. J Pediatr. Oct 2003;143(4):477-483. 22. Papile LA, Burstein J, Burstein R, Koffler H. Incidence and evolution of subependymal and intraventricular hemorrhage: a study of infants with birth weights less than 1,500 gm. J Pediatr. Apr 1978;92(4):529-534. 23. Ancel PY, Livinec F, Larroque B, et al. Cerebral palsy among very preterm children in relation to gestational age and neonatal ultrasound abnormalities: the EPIPAGE cohort study. Pediatrics. Mar 2006;117(3):828-835. 24. Perlman JM. The relationship between systemic hemodynamic perturbations and periventricular-intraventricular hemorrhage--a historical perspective. Semin Pediatr Neurol. Dec 2009;16(4):191- 199. 25. Greisen G. Autoregulation of cerebral blood flow in newborn babies. Early Hum Dev. May 2005;81(5):423-428. 26. Soul JS, Hammer PE, Tsuji M, et al. Fluctuating pressure-passivity is common in the cerebral circulation of sick premature infants. Pediatr Res. Apr 2007;61(4):467-473.

62 27. Kaiser JR, Gauss CH, Pont MM, Williams DK. Hypercapnia during the first 3 days of life is associated with severe intraventricular hemorrhage in very low birth weight infants. J Perinatol. May 2006;26(5):279-285. 28. Volpe JJ. The encephalopathy of prematurity--brain injury and impaired brain development inextricably intertwined. Semin Pediatr Neurol. Dec 2009;16(4):167-178. 29. Woodward LJ, Anderson PJ, Austin NC, Howard K, Inder TE. Neonatal MRI to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. N Engl J Med. Aug 17 2006;355(7):685-694. 30. Volpe JJ. Neurobiology of periventricular leukomalacia in the premature infant. Pediatr Res. Nov 2001;50(5):553-562. 31. Duggan PJ, Maalouf EF, Watts TL, et al. Intrauterine T-cell activation and increased proinflammatory cytokine concentrations in preterm infants with cerebral lesions. Lancet. Nov 17 2001;358(9294):1699-1700. 32. Romero R, Gotsch F, Pineles B, Kusanovic JP. Inflammation in pregnancy: its roles in reproductive physiology, obstetrical complications, and fetal injury. Nutr Rev. Dec 2007;65(12 Pt 2):S194-202. 33. Goldsby R. Immunology. 5th ed. ed. New York: W. H. Freeman ; Basingstoke : Palgrave Macmillan; 2002. 34. Leijser LM, de Vries LS, Cowan FM. Using cerebral ultrasound effectively in the newborn infant. Early Hum Dev. Dec 2006;82(12):827-835. 35. Rademaker KJ, Uiterwaal CS, Beek FJ, et al. Neonatal cranial ultrasound versus MRI and neurodevelopmental outcome at school age in children born preterm. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Nov 2005;90(6):F489-493. 36. Laptook AR, O'Shea TM, Shankaran S, Bhaskar B. Adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes among extremely low birth weight infants with a normal head ultrasound: prevalence and antecedents. Pediatrics. Mar 2005;115(3):673-680. 37. Steriade M, Gloor P, Llinas RR, Lopes de Silva FH, Mesulam MM. Report of IFCN Committee on Basic Mechanisms. Basic mechanisms of cerebral rhythmic activities. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Dec 1990;76(6):481-508. 38. Kostovic I, Jovanov-Milosevic N. The development of cerebral connections during the first 20-45 weeks' gestation. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. Dec 2006;11(6):415-422. 39. Hellström-Westas L, De Vries LS, Rosén I. Atlas of amplitude- integrated EEGs in the newborn. 2nd ed. ed. London: Informa Healthcare; 2008. 40. Toet MC, Lemmers PM. Brain monitoring in neonates. Early Hum Dev. Feb 2009;85(2):77-84.

63 41. Shellhaas RA, Clancy RR. Characterization of neonatal seizures by conventional EEG and single-channel EEG. Clin Neurophysiol. Oct 2007;118(10):2156-2161. 42. Hayakawa M, Okumura A, Hayakawa F, et al. Background electroencephalographic (EEG) activities of very preterm infants born at less than 27 weeks gestation: a study on the degree of continuity. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. May 2001;84(3):F163-167. 43. Andre M, Lamblin MD, d'Allest AM, et al. in premature and full-term infants. Developmental features and glossary. Neurophysiol Clin. May 2010;40(2):59-124. 44. Lamblin MD, Andre M, Challamel MJ, et al. [Electroencephalography of the premature and term newborn. Maturational aspects and glossary]. Neurophysiol Clin. Apr 1999;29(2):123-219. 45. Vecchierini MF, Andre M, d'Allest AM. Normal EEG of premature infants born between 24 and 30 weeks gestational age: terminology, definitions and maturation aspects. Neurophysiol Clin. Oct-Nov 2007;37(5):311-323. 46. Bell AH, McClure BG, McCullagh PJ, McClelland RJ. Variation in power spectral analysis of the EEG with gestational age. J Clin Neurophysiol. Jul 1991;8(3):312-319. 47. Vanhatalo S, Kaila K. Development of neonatal EEG activity: from phenomenology to physiology. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. Dec 2006;11(6):471-478. 48. Tolonen M, Palva JM, Andersson S, Vanhatalo S. Development of the spontaneous activity transients and ongoing cortical activity in human preterm babies. . Mar 30 2007;145(3):997- 1006. 49. Ben-Ari Y. Excitatory actions of gaba during development: the nature of the nurture. Nat Rev Neurosci. Sep 2002;3(9):728-739. 50. Khazipov R, Luhmann HJ. Early patterns of electrical activity in the developing cerebral cortex of humans and rodents. Trends Neurosci. Jul 2006;29(7):414-418. 51. Khazipov R, Sirota A, Leinekugel X, Holmes GL, Ben-Ari Y, Buzsaki G. Early motor activity drives spindle bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature. Dec 9 2004;432(7018):758-761. 52. Steriade M, Amzica F, Contreras D. Cortical and thalamic cellular correlates of electroencephalographic burst-suppression. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Jan 1994;90(1):1-16. 53. Connell JA, Oozeer R, Dubowitz V. Continuous 4-channel EEG monitoring: a guide to interpretation, with normal values, in preterm infants. Neuropediatrics. Aug 1987;18(3):138-145. 54. Benda GI, Engel RC, Zhang YP. Prolonged inactive phases during the discontinuous pattern of prematurity in the electroencephalogram

64 of very-low-birthweight infants. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Mar 1989;72(3):189-197. 55. Hahn JS, Monyer H, Tharp BR. Interburst interval measurements in the EEGs of premature infants with normal neurological outcome. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Nov 1989;73(5):410-418. 56. Selton D, Andre M, Hascoet JM. Normal EEG in very premature infants: reference criteria. Clin Neurophysiol. Dec 2000;111(12):2116-2124. 57. Vecchierini MF, d'Allest AM, Verpillat P. EEG patterns in 10 extreme premature neonates with normal neurological outcome: qualitative and quantitative data. Brain Dev. Aug 2003;25(5):330- 337. 58. Victor S, Appleton RE, Beirne M, Marson AG, Weindling AM. Spectral analysis of electroencephalography in premature newborn infants: normal ranges. Pediatr Res. Mar 2005;57(3):336-341. 59. Biagioni E, Frisone MF, Laroche S, et al. Maturation of cerebral electrical activity and development of cortical folding in young very preterm infants. Clin Neurophysiol. Jan 2007;118(1):53-59. 60. Fuller PM, Gooley JJ, Saper CB. Neurobiology of the sleep-wake cycle: sleep architecture, circadian regulation, and regulatory feedback. J Biol Rhythms. Dec 2006;21(6):482-493. 61. Hellström-Westas L, Rosén I, Svenningsen NW. Cerebral function monitoring during the first week of life in extremely small birthweight (ESLBW) infants. Neuropediatrics. 1991;22(1):27-32. 62. Scher MS, Johnson MW, Holditch-Davis D. Cyclicity of neonatal sleep behaviors at 25 to 30 weeks' postconceptional age. Pediatr Res. Jun 2005;57(6):879-882. 63. Kuhle S, Klebermass K, Olischar M, et al. Sleep-wake cycles in preterm infants below 30 weeks of gestational age. Preliminary results of a prospective amplitude-integrated EEG study. Wien Klin Wochenschr. Apr 17 2001;113(7-8):219-223. 64. Olischar M, Klebermass K, Kuhle S, et al. Reference values for amplitude-integrated electroencephalographic activity in preterm infants younger than 30 weeks' gestational age. Pediatrics. Jan 2004;113(1 Pt 1):e61-66. 65. Greisen G, Hellstrom-Vestas L, Lou H, Rosen I, Svenningsen N. Sleep-waking shifts and cerebral blood flow in stable preterm infants. Pediatr Res. Nov 1985;19(11):1156-1159. 66. Watanabe K, Hayakawa F, Okumura A. Neonatal EEG: a powerful tool in the assessment of brain damage in preterm infants. Brain Dev. Sep 1999;21(6):361-372. 67. Clancy RR, Tharp BR, Enzman D. EEG in premature infants with intraventricular hemorrhage. Neurology. May 1984;34(5):583-590. 68. Connell J, de Vries L, Oozeer R, Regev R, Dubowitz LM, Dubowitz V. Predictive value of early continuous electroencephalogram monitoring in ventilated preterm infants with intraventricular hemorrhage. Pediatrics. Sep 1988;82(3):337-343.

65 69. Aso K, Abdab-Barmada M, Scher MS. EEG and the neuropathology in premature neonates with intraventricular hemorrhage. J Clin Neurophysiol. Jul 1993;10(3):304-313. 70. Kidokoro H, Okumura A, Hayakawa F, et al. Chronologic changes in neonatal EEG findings in periventricular leukomalacia. Pediatrics. Sep 2009;124(3):e468-475. 71. Maruyama K, Okumura A, Hayakawa F, Kato T, Kuno K, Watanabe K. Prognostic value of EEG depression in preterm infants for later development of cerebral palsy. Neuropediatrics. Jun 2002;33(3):133-137. 72. Biagioni E, Bartalena L, Biver P, Pieri R, Cioni G. Electroencephalographic dysmaturity in preterm infants: a prognostic tool in the early postnatal period. Neuropediatrics. Dec 1996;27(6):311-316. 73. Okumura A, Hayakawa F, Kato T, Kuno K, Watanabe K. Developmental outcome and types of chronic-stage EEG abnormalities in preterm infants. Dev Med Child Neurol. Nov 2002;44(11):729-734. 74. Marret S, Parain D, Menard JF, et al. Prognostic value of neonatal electroencephalography in premature newborns less than 33 weeks of gestational age. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Mar 1997;102(3):178-185. 75. de Vries LS, Hellstrom-Westas L. Role of cerebral function monitoring in the newborn. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. May 2005;90(3):F201-207. 76. Rundgren M, Rosen I, Friberg H. Amplitude-integrated EEG (aEEG) predicts outcome after cardiac arrest and induced hypothermia. Intensive Care Med. Jun 2006;32(6):836-842. 77. Maynard D, Prior PF, Scott DF. Device for continuous monitoring of cerebral activity in resuscitated patients. British Medical Journal. 1969;4:545-546. 78. Rosen I. The physiological basis for continuous electroencephalogram monitoring in the neonate. Clin Perinatol. Sep 2006;33(3):593-611, v. 79. Suk D, Krauss AN, Engel M, Perlman JM. Amplitude-integrated electroencephalography in the NICU: frequent artifacts in premature infants may limit its utility as a monitoring device. Pediatrics. Feb 2009;123(2):e328-332. 80. Hagmann CF, Robertson NJ, Azzopardi D. Artifacts on electroencephalograms may influence the amplitude-integrated EEG classification: a qualitative analysis in neonatal encephalopathy. Pediatrics. Dec 2006;118(6):2552-2554. 81. Spitzmiller RE, Phillips T, Meinzen-Derr J, Hoath SB. Amplitude- integrated EEG is useful in predicting neurodevelopmental outcome in full-term infants with hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy: a meta- analysis. J Child Neurol. Sep 2007;22(9):1069-1078.

66 82. Greisen G, Hellstrom-Westas L, Lou H, Rosen I, Svenningsen NW. EEG depression and germinal layer haemorrhage in the newborn. Acta Paediatr Scand. May 1987;76(3):519-525. 83. Olischar M, Klebermass K, Waldhoer T, Pollak A, Weninger M. Background patterns and sleep-wake cycles on amplitude-integrated electroencephalography in preterms younger than 30 weeks gestational age with peri-/intraventricular haemorrhage. Acta Paediatr. Dec 2007;96(12):1743-1750. 84. Hellstrom-Westas L, Klette H, Thorngren-Jerneck K, Rosen I. Early prediction of outcome with aEEG in preterm infants with large intraventricular hemorrhages. Neuropediatrics. Dec 2001;32(6):319- 324. 85. Bowen JR, Paradisis M, Shah D. Decreased aEEG continuity and baseline variability in the first 48 hours of life associated with poor short-term outcome in neonates born before 29 weeks gestation. Pediatr Res. May 2010;67(5):538-544. 86. West CR, Harding JE, Williams CE, Nolan M, Battin MR. Cot-side electroencephalography for outcome prediction in preterm infants: observational study. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Mar 2011;96(2):F108-113. 87. Viniker DA, Maynard DE, Scott DF. Cerebral function monitor studies in neonates. Clin Electroencephalogr. Oct 1984;15(4):185- 192. 88. Thornberg E, Thiringer K. Normal pattern of the cerebral function monitor trace in term and preterm neonates. Acta Paediatr Scand. Jan 1990;79(1):20-25. 89. Burdjalov VF, Baumbart S, Spitzer AR. Cerebral Function Monitoring: A New Scoring System for the Evaluation of Brain Maturation in Neonates. Pediatrics. 2004;112:855-861. 90. West CR, Harding JE, Williams CE, Gunning MI, Battin MR. Quantitative electroencephalographic patterns in normal preterm infants over the first week after birth. Early Hum Dev. Jan 2006;82(1):43-51. 91. Sisman J, Campbell DE, Brion LP. Amplitude-integrated EEG in preterm infants: maturation of background pattern and amplitude voltage with postmenstrual age and gestational age. J Perinatol. Jun 2005;25(6):391-396. 92. Klebermass K, Kuhle S, Olischar M, Rucklinger E, Pollak A, Weninger M. Intra- and extrauterine maturation of amplitude- integrated electroencephalographic activity in preterm infants younger than 30 weeks of gestation. Biol Neonate. 2006;89(2):120- 125. 93. Herbertz S, Pulzer F, Gebauer C, Panhofer M, Robel-Tillig E, Knupfer M. The effect of maturation and sedation on amplitude- integrated electroencephalogram of the preterm neonate: results of a prospective study. Acta Paediatr. Nov 2006;95(11):1394-1399.

67 94. Kuint J, Turgeman A, Torjman A, Maayan-Metzger A. Characteristics of amplitude-integrated electroencephalogram in premature infants. J Child Neurol. Mar 2007;22(3):277-281. 95. Soubasi V, Mitsakis K, Nakas CT, et al. The influence of extrauterine life on the aEEG maturation in normal preterm infants. Early Hum Dev. Dec 2009;85(12):761-765. 96. Niemarkt HJ, Andriessen P, Peters CH, et al. Quantitative analysis of amplitude-integrated electroencephalogram patterns in stable preterm infants, with normal neurological development at one year. Neonatology. 2010;97(2):175-182. 97. Niemarkt HJ, Andriessen P, Peters CH, Pasman JW, Zimmermann LJ, Bambang Oetomo S. Quantitative analysis of maturational changes in EEG background activity in very preterm infants with a normal neurodevelopment at 1 year of age. Early Hum Dev. Apr 2010;86(4):219-224. 98. Sarkela M, Mustola S, Seppanen T, et al. Automatic analysis and monitoring of burst suppression in anesthesia. J Clin Monit Comput. Feb 2002;17(2):125-134. 99. Pfurtscheller K, Muller-Putz GR, Urlesberger B, Muller W, Pfurtscheller G. Relationship between slow-wave EEG bursts and heart rate changes in preterm infants. Neurosci Lett. Sep 9 2005;385(2):126-130. 100. Lofhede J, Lofgren N, Thordstein M, Flisberg A, Kjellmer I, Lindecrantz K. Classification of burst and suppression in the neonatal electroencephalogram. J Neural Eng. Dec 2008;5(4):402- 410. 101. EI. Plotkin MS. Nonlinear based on parameter invariant moving average modelling. 1992. 102. Watanabe K, Hakamada S, Kuroyanagi M, Yamazaki T, Takeuchi T. Electroencephalographic study of intraventricular hemorrhage in the preterm newborn. Neuropediatrics. Nov 1983;14(4):225-230. 103. Connell J, Oozeer R, Regev R, De Vries LS, Dubowitz LM, Dubowitz V. Continuous four-channel EEG monitoring in the evaluation of echodense ultrasound lesions and cystic leucomalacia. Arch Dis Child. Oct 1987;62(10):1019-1024. 104. Inder TE, Anderson NJ, Spencer C, Wells S, Volpe JJ. White matter injury in the premature infant: a comparison between serial cranial sonographic and MR findings at term. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. May 2003;24(5):805-809. 105. Greisen G, Pryds O. Low CBF, discontinuous EEG activity, and periventricular brain injury in ill, preterm neonates. Brain Dev. 1989;11(3):164-168. 106. West CR, Groves AM, Williams CE, et al. Early low cardiac output is associated with compromised electroencephalographic activity in very preterm infants. Pediatr Res. Apr 2006;59(4 Pt 1):610-615. 107. Victor S, Marson AG, Appleton RE, Beirne M, Weindling AM. Relationship between blood pressure, cerebral electrical activity,

68 cerebral fractional oxygen extraction, and peripheral blood flow in very low birth weight newborn infants. Pediatr Res. Feb 2006;59(2):314-319. 108. Bunt JE, Gavilanes AW, Reulen JP, Blanco CE, Vles JS. The influence of acute hypoxemia and hypovolemic hypotension of neuronal brain activity measured by the cerebral function monitor in newborn piglets. Neuropediatrics. Oct 1996;27(5):260-264. 109. Eaton DG, Wertheim D, Oozeer R, Dubowitz LM, Dubowitz V. Reversible changes in cerebral activity associated with acidosis in preterm neonates. Acta Paediatr. May 1994;83(5):486-492. 110. Friss HE, Wavrek D, Martin WH, Wolfson MR. Brain-stem auditory evoked responses to hypercarbia in preterm infants. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. May 1994;90(5):331-336. 111. Victor S, Appleton RE, Beirne M, Marson AG, Weindling AM. Effect of carbon dioxide on background cerebral electrical activity and fractional oxygen extraction in very low birth weight infants just after birth. Pediatr Res. Sep 2005;58(3):579-585. 112. Forslid A. Transient neocortical, hippocampal and amygdaloid EEG silence induced by one minute inhalation of high concentration CO2 in swine. Acta Physiol Scand. May 1987;130(1):1-10. 113. Pryds O. Control of cerebral circulation in the high-risk neonate. Ann Neurol. Sep 1991;30(3):321-329. 114. Greisen G, Vannucci RC. Is periventricular leucomalacia a result of hypoxic-ischaemic injury? Hypocapnia and the preterm brain. Biol Neonate. 2001;79(3-4):194-200. 115. Dammann O, Allred EN, Kuban KC, et al. Hypocarbia during the first 24 postnatal hours and white matter echolucencies in newborns < or = 28 weeks gestation. Pediatr Res. Mar 2001;49(3):388-393. 116. Vannucci RC, Towfighi J, Heitjan DF, Brucklacher RM. Carbon dioxide protects the perinatal brain from hypoxic-ischemic damage: an experimental study in the immature rat. Pediatrics. Jun 1995;95(6):868-874. 117. Vannucci RC, Towfighi J, Brucklacher RM, Vannucci SJ. Effect of extreme hypercapnia on hypoxic-ischemic brain damage in the immature rat. Pediatr Res. Jun 2001;49(6):799-803. 118. Hey E. Hyperglycaemia and the very preterm baby. Semin Fetal Neonatal Med. Aug 2005;10(4):377-387. 119. Heimann K, Peschgens T, Kwiecien R, Stanzel S, Hoernchen H, Merz U. Are recurrent hyperglycemic episodes and median blood glucose level a prognostic factor for increased morbidity and mortality in premature infants

69 121. Rehncrona S, Rosen I, Siesjo BK. Brain lactic acidosis and ischemic cell damage: 1. Biochemistry and neurophysiology. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1981;1(3):297-311. 122. Gisselsson L, Smith ML, Siesjo BK. Hyperglycemia and focal brain ischemia. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. Mar 1999;19(3):288-297. 123. Koh TH, Aynsley-Green A, Tarbit M, Eyre JA. Neural dysfunction during hypoglycaemia. Arch Dis Child. Nov 1988;63(11):1353- 1358. 124. Stenninger E, Eriksson E, Stigfur A, Schollin J, Aman J. Monitoring of early postnatal glucose homeostasis and cerebral function in newborn infants of diabetic mothers. A pilot study. Early Hum Dev. Apr 2001;62(1):23-32. 125. Harris DL, Weston PJ, Spooner CV, Battin MR, Harding JE. Cot- Side EEG Monitoring Does Not Detect Changes That Are Clinically Useful in the Management of Neonatal Hypoglycaemia. PAS 20102010. 126. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ. Adverse neurodevelopmental outcome of moderate neonatal hypoglycaemia. BMJ. Nov 19 1988;297(6659):1304-1308. 127. Bell AH, Greisen G, Pryds O. Comparison of the effects of phenobarbitone and morphine administration on EEG activity in preterm babies. Acta Paediatr. Jan 1993;82(1):35-39. 128. Young GB, da Silva OP. Effects of morphine on the electroencephalograms of neonates: a prospective, observational study. Clin Neurophysiol. Nov 2000;111(11):1955-1960. 129. Nguyen The Tich S, Vecchierini MF, Debillon T, Pereon Y. Effects of sufentanil on electroencephalogram in very and extremely preterm neonates. Pediatrics. Jan 2003;111(1):123-128. 130. Hellstrom-Westas L, Westgren U, Rosen I, Svenningsen NW. Lidocaine for treatment of severe seizures in newborn infants. I. Clinical effects and cerebral electrical activity monitoring. Acta Paediatr Scand. Jan 1988;77(1):79-84. 131. van Leuven K, Groenendaal F, Toet MC, et al. Midazolam and amplitude-integrated EEG in asphyxiated full-term neonates. Acta Paediatr. Sep 2004;93(9):1221-1227. 132. Shany E, Benzaquen O, Friger M, Richardson J, Golan A. Influence of antiepileptic drugs on amplitude-integrated electroencephalography. Pediatr Neurol. Dec 2008;39(6):387-391. 133. van den Berg E, Lemmers PM, Toet MC, Klaessens JH, van Bel F. Effect of the "InSurE" procedure on cerebral oxygenation and electrical brain activity of the preterm infant. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Jan 2010;95(1):F53-58. 134. Supcun S, Kutz P, Pielemeier W, Roll C. Caffeine increases cerebral cortical activity in preterm infants. J Pediatr. Mar 2010;156(3):490- 491. 135. Rehn M, Hubschle T, Diener M. TNF-alpha hyperpolarizes membrane potential and potentiates the response to nicotinic

70 receptor stimulation in cultured rat myenteric neurones. Acta Physiol Scand. May 2004;181(1):13-22. 136. Okumura A, Hayakawa F, Kato T, Kuno K, Watanabe K. Correlation between the serum level of endotoxin and periventricular leukomalacia in preterm infants. Brain Dev. Sep 1999;21(6):378-381. 137. Thordstein M, Lofgren N, Flisberg A, Bagenholm R, Lindecrantz K, Kjellmer I. Infraslow EEG activity in burst periods from post asphyctic full term neonates. Clin Neurophysiol. Jul 2005;116(7):1501-1506. 138. Gavilanes AW, Gantert M, Strackx E, et al. Increased EEG delta frequency corresponds to chorioamnionitis-related brain injury. Front Biosci (Schol Ed). 2010;2:432-438. 139. World Medical A. World Medical Association declaration of Helsinki : ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects ; Edinburgh 2000. Guildford: Canary Publications; 2000. 140. Bellander M, Ley D, Polberger S, Hellstrom-Westas L. Tolerance to early human milk feeding is not compromised by indomethacin in preterm infants with persistent ductus arteriosus. Acta Paediatr. Sep 2003;92(9):1074-1078. 141. de Vries LS, Eken P, Dubowitz LM. The spectrum of leukomalacia using cranial ultrasound. Behav Brain Res. Jul 31 1992;49(1):1-6. 142. Hellström-Westas L, Rosén I, de Vries LS, Greisen G. Amplitude- integrated EEG Clasification and Interpretation in Preterm and Term Infants. NeoReviews. 2006;7(2):e76-e87. 143. Vanhatalo S, Kaila K. Generation of 'positive slow waves' in the preterm EEG: by the brain or by the EEG setup? Clin Neurophysiol. Jun 2008;119(6):1453-1454; author reply 1454-1455. 144. El-Dib M, Chang T, Tsuchida TN, Clancy RR. Amplitude-integrated electroencephalography in neonates. Pediatr Neurol. Nov 2009;41(5):315-326. 145. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2 ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1993. 146. Haataja L, Mercuri E, Regev R, et al. Optimality score for the neurologic examination of the infant at 12 and 18 months of age. J Pediatr. 1999;135:153-161. 147. Scheffzek A, Stahl M, von Toenges V. [The prognosis of the very small premature infant. Catamnestic studies of premature infants with a birth weight up to 1,000 grams]. Monatsschr Kinderheilkd. Jan 1989;137(1):42-48. 148. Hansen-Pupp I, Harling S, Berg AC, Cilio C, Hellstrom-Westas L, Ley D. Circulating interferon-gamma and white matter brain damage in preterm infants. Pediatr Res. Nov 2005;58(5):946-952. 149. Hansen-Pupp I, Hovel H, Hellstrom A, et al. Postnatal Decrease in Circulating Insulin-Like Growth Factor-I and Low Brain Volumes in Very Preterm Infants. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. Feb 2 2011.

71 150. Fleiss JL. Measuring nominal scale agreement among many raters. Psychological Bulletin. 1971;76(5):378-382. 151. Norman RG, Scott MA. Measurement of inter-rater agreement for transient events using Monte Carlo sampled permutations. Stat Med. Feb 20 2007;26(4):931-942. 152. Royston P, Sauerbrei W. Multivariable modeling with cubic regression splines: A principled approach. The Stata Journal. 2007;7(1):45-70. 153. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the newborn. 5th ed. ed. Philadelphia, Pa. ; London: Saunders; 2008. 154. van de Bor M, van Dijk JG, van Bel F, Brouwer OF, van Sweden B. Electrical brain activity in preterm infants at risk for intracranial hemorrhage. Acta Paediatr. Jun 1994;83(6):588-595. 155. Monod N, Ducas P. The prognostic value of the EEG during the first two years of life. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. Jul 1969;27(1):107. 156. Tharp BR, Scher MS, Clancy RR. Serial EEGs in normal and abnormal infants with birth weights less than 1200 grams--a prospective study with long term follow-up. Neuropediatrics. May 1989;20(2):64-72. 157. Helderman JB, Welch CD, Leng X, O'Shea TM. Sepsis-associated electroencephalographic changes in extremely low gestational age neonates. Early Hum Dev. Aug 2010;86(8):509-513. 158. Toet MC, van der Meij W, de Vries LS, Uiterwaal CS, van Huffelen KC. Comparison between simultaneously recorded amplitude integrated electroencephalogram (cerebral function monitor) and standard electroencephalogram in neonates. Pediatrics. May 2002;109(5):772-779. 159. Oliveira AJ, Nunes ML, Haertel LM, Reis FM, da Costa JC. Duration of rhythmic EEG patterns in neonates: new evidence for clinical and prognostic significance of brief rhythmic discharges. Clin Neurophysiol. Sep 2000;111(9):1646-1653. 160. Shah DK, Zempel J, Barton T, Lukas K, Inder TE. Electrographic seizures in preterm infants during the first week of life are associated with cerebral injury. Pediatr Res. Jan 2010;67(1):102-106. 161. Clancy RR, Legido A. The exact ictal and interictal duration of electroencephalographic neonatal seizures. Epilepsia. Sep-Oct 1987;28(5):537-541. 162. Scher MS, Hamid MY, Steppe DA, Beggarly ME, Painter MJ. Ictal and interictal electrographic seizure durations in preterm and term neonates. Epilepsia. Mar-Apr 1993;34(2):284-288. 163. Hellstrom-Westas L. Comparison between tape-recorded and amplitude-integrated EEG monitoring in sick newborn infants. Acta Paediatr. Oct 1992;81(10):812-819. 164. Shellhaas RA, Soaita AI, Clancy RR. Sensitivity of amplitude- integrated electroencephalography for neonatal seizure detection. Pediatrics. Oct 2007;120(4):770-777.

72 165. Rennie JM, Chorley G, Boylan GB, Pressler R, Nguyen Y, Hooper R. Non-expert use of the cerebral function monitor for neonatal seizure detection. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. Jan 2004;89(1):F37-40. 166. Beattie EC, Stellwagen D, Morishita W, et al. Control of synaptic strength by glial TNFalpha. Science. Mar 22 2002;295(5563):2282- 2285. 167. Stellwagen D, Beattie EC, Seo JY, Malenka RC. Differential regulation of AMPA receptor and GABA receptor trafficking by tumor necrosis factor-alpha. J Neurosci. Mar 23 2005;25(12):3219- 3228. 168. Stellwagen D, Malenka RC. Synaptic scaling mediated by glial TNF-alpha. Nature. Apr 20 2006;440(7087):1054-1059. 169. Lee J, Taira T, Pihlaja P, Ransom BR, Kaila K. Effects of CO2 on excitatory transmission apparently caused by changes in intracellular pH in the rat hippocampal slice. Brain Res. Jan 15 1996;706(2):210- 216. 170. Dulla CG, Dobelis P, Pearson T, Frenguelli BG, Staley KJ, Masino SA. Adenosine and ATP link PCO2 to cortical excitability via pH. Neuron. Dec 22 2005;48(6):1011-1023. 171. Ruusuvuori E, Kirilkin I, Pandya N, Kaila K. Spontaneous network events driven by depolarizing GABA action in neonatal hippocampal slices are not attributable to deficient mitochondrial energy metabolism. J Neurosci. Nov 17 2010;30(46):15638-15642. 172. Balestrino M, Somjen GG. Concentration of carbon dioxide, interstitial pH and synaptic transmission in hippocampal formation of the rat. J Physiol. Feb 1988;396:247-266. 173. Wagerle LC, Kumar SP, Belik J, Delivoria-Papadopoulos M. Blood- brain barrier to hydrogen ion during acute metabolic acidosis in piglets. J Appl Physiol. Aug 1988;65(2):776-781. 174. Thome UH, Carlo WA. Permissive hypercapnia. Semin Neonatol. Oct 2002;7(5):409-419. 175. Mariani G, Cifuentes J, Carlo WA. Randomized trial of permissive hypercapnia in preterm infants. Pediatrics. Nov 1999;104(5 Pt 1):1082-1088. 176. Carlo WA, Stark AR, Wright LL, et al. Minimal ventilation to prevent bronchopulmonary dysplasia in extremely-low-birth-weight infants. J Pediatr. Sep 2002;141(3):370-374. 177. Hagen EW, Sadek-Badawi M, Carlton DP, Palta M. Permissive hypercapnia and risk for brain injury and developmental impairment. Pediatrics. Sep 2008;122(3):e583-589. 178. McKee LA, Fabres J, Howard G, Peralta-Carcelen M, Carlo WA, Ambalavanan N. PaCO2 and neurodevelopment in extremely low birth weight infants. J Pediatr. Aug 2009;155(2):217-221 e211. 179. Harris DL, Battin MR, Williams CE, Weston PJ, Harding JE. Cot- side electro-encephalography and interstitial glucose monitoring

73 during insulin-induced hypoglycaemia in newborn lambs. Neonatology. 2009;95(4):271-278. 180. Skov L, Pryds O. Capillary recruitment for preservation of cerebral glucose influx in hypoglycemic, preterm newborns: evidence for a glucose sensor? Pediatrics. Aug 1992;90(2 Pt 1):193-195. 181. Shany E, Granot S, Meledin I, Richardson J, Bassan H, Friger M. Effect of Respriatory Acidosis on Cerebral Function. Paper presented at: The 6th International Conference on Brain Monitoring and Neuroprotection in the Newborn2011; Amsterdam. 182. Palmu K, Stevenson N, Wikstrom S, Hellstrom-Westas L, Vanhatalo S, Palva JM. Optimization of an NLEO-based algorithm for automated detection of spontaneous activity transients in early preterm EEG. Physiol Meas. Nov 2010;31(11):N85-93. 183. Lofhede J, Degerman J, Lofgren N, et al. Comparing a supervised and an unsupervised classification method for burst detection in neonatal EEG. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2008;2008:3836- 3839. 184. Lofhede J, Thordstein M, Lofgren N, et al. Automatic classification of background EEG activity in healthy and sick neonates. J Neural Eng. Feb 2010;7(1):16007. 185. Tekgul H, Bourgeois BF, Gauvreau K, Bergin AM. Electroencephalography in neonatal seizures: comparison of a reduced and a full 10/20 montage. Pediatr Neurol. Mar 2005;32(3):155-161. 186. Shah DK, Mackay MT, Lavery S, et al. Accuracy of bedside electroencephalographic monitoring in comparison with simultaneous continuous conventional electroencephalography for seizure detection in term infants. Pediatrics. Jun 2008;121(6):1146- 1154. 187. Spittle AJ, Orton J, Doyle LW, Boyd R. Early developmental intervention programs post hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2007(2):CD005495.

74