Abstracts (PDF)

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Abstracts (PDF) Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 12 — November 2007 48th Annual Meeting — November 15–18, 2007 — Long Beach, California Papers 1–7 Friday Morning Implicit Memory is a widely used experimental procedure that has been modeled in Regency ABC, Friday Morning, 8:00–9:20 order to obtain ostensibly separate measures for explicit memory and implicit memory. Existing models are critiqued here and a new con- Chaired by Neil W. Mulligan fidence process-dissociation (CPD) model is developed for a modi- University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill fied process-dissociation task that uses confidence ratings in con- junction with the usual old/new recognition response. With the new 8:00–8:15 (1) model there are several direct tests for the key assumptions of the stan- Attention and Auditory Implicit Memory. NEIL W. MULLIGAN, dard models for this task. Experimental results are reported here that University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill—Traditional theorizing are contrary to the critical assumptions of the earlier models for the stresses the importance of attentional state during encoding for later process-dissociation task. These results cast doubt on the suitability memory, based primarily on research with explicit memory. Recent re- of the process-dissociation procedure itself for obtaining separate search has investigated the role of attention in implicit memory in the measures for implicit and explicit memory. visual modality. The present experiments examined the effect of divided attention on auditory implicit memory, using auditory perceptual iden- Attentional Selection and Priming tification, word-stem completion, and word-fragment completion. Par- Regency DEFH, Friday Morning, 8:00–10:00 ticipants heard study words under full attention conditions or while si- multaneously carrying out a distractor task. In Experiment 1, a distractor Chaired by Veronica J. Dark, Iowa State University task with low response frequency failed to disrupt later auditory prim- ing (but diminished explicit memory as assessed with auditory recog- 8:00–8:15 (5) nition). In Experiment 2, a distractor task with greater response fre- Semantic Selectivity Reflects Attention and Not “Just” Semantic quency disrupted priming on all three of the auditory priming tasks as Priming. VERONICA J. DARK & CHRISTOPHER M. MASCIOCCHI, well as the explicit test. These results imply that although auditory prim- Iowa State University—Selective attention is shown when some stim- ing is less reliant on attention than on explicit memory, it is still greatly uli undergo processing allowing identification, whereas other stimuli affected by at least some divided-attention manipulations. do not. Spatially precued items are selectively processed, as are items semantically (associatively) related to a prior context. Some have sug- 8:20–8:35 (2) gested that semantic selectivity just reflects benefit from semantic The Hebb Repetition Effect in Auditory Space. FABRICE B. PAR- priming, arguing that if attention (i.e., limited capacity resources) MENTIER, University of Plymouth, MURRAY T. MAYBERY & were involved, there also would be cost. We asked whether semantic MATTHEW HUITSON, University of Western Australia, & DYLAN M. selectivity would interfere with spatial selectivity and vice versa. Sub- JONES, Cardiff University—We examined the Hebb repetition effect jects reported either the spatially precued or the related word in for auditory spatial stimuli. Consistent with the proposition that the masked word pairs. In the spatial precue baseline, no related words ap- spatial scattering of auditory to-be-remembered stimuli hinders the peared. The addition of related words interfered with spatial selectiv- preattentive processing of order information by preventing the per- ity by inhibiting report of cued unrelated words and enhancing report ceptual organization of the stimuli into a coherent stream, two exper- of uncued related words. In the report-related baseline, no precues ap- iments using bursts of white noise showed no repetition learning for peared. The addition of precues interfered with semantic selectivity such stimuli. Further experiments produced a Hebb effect for non- primarily by inhibiting report of uncued related words. Selective at- words scattered across space, and for spatial locations when the stim- tention has both spatial and semantic components. uli were nonwords. This effect could not be explained on the basis of a binding account or a distinctiveness account. Rather, the data sug- 8:20–8:35 (6) gest that the presentation of stimuli spoken in a human voice offers a Spatial Attention and Masked Priming: Biological Relevance Mat- common carrier on the basis of which the locations are perceived as ters. MATTHEW FINKBEINER & ROMINA PALERMO, Macquarie part of a coherent stream, thereby affording the preattentive process- Centre for Cognitive Science—Theories of human cognition have long ing of order information and repetition learning. held that nonconscious processes are “automatic” insofar as they appear to be elicited independently of higher level cognitive mechanisms, such 8:40–8:55 (3) as attention. Recent findings have called this view into question by Implicit Motor Learning Depends on Task Structure: Evidence demonstrating that a masked visual word or number does not produce From the Chord Task. ELIOT HAZELTINE, TANA TRUELOVE, & priming unless it is attended. From these findings, it has been suggested KARL FRIEDRICHSEN, University of Iowa—Implicit learning of re- that nonconscious cognitive processes are not automatic because they are sponse contingencies is a robust phenomenon that has been demon- not elicited autonomously. Here, we show that the autonomy of non- strated even when attentional resources are diverted by a secondary conscious cognitive processes varies as a function of the masked prime’s task. In the present set of studies, we examined the effects of task biological relevance. Masked pictures of animals, tools, and vegetables structure on implicit learning using the chord task. In the chord task, produced priming, but, like words and numbers, only when attended. In individuals make two responses simultaneously, and learning is mea- contrast, masked pictures of faces produced priming both when attended sured as the performance cost associated with production of unprac- and not. Our results demonstrate that nonconscious processes may be ticed combinations compared to the production of practiced combi- elicited autonomously, but only when the to-be-processed information nations. In Experiments 1–4, chord learning occurred only when the has a high degree of biological and/or social relevance. stimuli for both tasks were visual and the responses for both tasks were manual. However, subsequent experiments showed that the over- 8:40–8:55 (7) lap in the stimulus and response modalities was not critical for learn- Feature Detection Requires Spatial Attention. JAN THEEUWES, ing, but the two tasks had to be conceptualized as related for practiced ERIK VAN DER BURG, & ARTEM V. BELOPOLSKY, Vrije Univer- response combinations to be performed better than unpracticed com- siteit—It has been claimed that the detection of a feature singleton can binations. This finding suggests that conceptual task boundaries play be based on activity in a feature map containing a coarse coding that an important role in implicit learning. something unique is present in the visual field. In the present experi- ment, participants detected the presence or absence of a color single- 9:00–9:15 (4) ton. Although it was irrelevant for the task, the color singleton was a Problems With Modeling Processes Underlying the Process- letter that could be repeated on the next trial. We showed that the iden- Dissociation Task. RICHARD A. CHECHILE & JESSICA R. tity of the singleton resulted in repetition priming on the next trial, sug- CHAMBERLAND, Tufts University—The process-dissociation task gesting that even in the simplest present–absent feature detection task, 1 Friday Morning Papers 8–14 focal attention is allocated to the feature singleton. These results are in- versity—A new approach to decision theory based on quantum infor- consistent with theories that claim that it is possible to detect a feature mation processing principles is used to explain some paradoxical phe- singleton without directing attention to the location of the singleton. nomena of human choice behavior. Quantum strategies were origi- nally used to explain the fact that humans prefer to cooperate rather 9:00–9:15 (8) than defect in a Prisoner Dilemma game. Here, we develop a quantum Numerical Format Effects in the Eriksen Task. PHILIP T. QUINLAN, model for the disjunction effect. This refers to a paradox in which (1) a University of York—Although this is not the only study to have ex- player prefers to defect when the player knows that an opponent will amined numerical versions of the Eriksen flanker task, it is (probably) defect, and (2) the player also prefers to defect when the player knows the only study with displays containing both Arabic and Chinese char- that an opponent will cooperate, but (3) the player reverses preference acters. Twenty-eight Chinese/English bilingual undergraduates were and cooperates when the opponent’s action is unknown. New experi- tested in two numerical versions of the task. On every trial, subjects mental findings on the disjunction effect are reported, and a quantum were timed to classify a central target—either four/six or five/seven— explanation for the findings is presented.
Recommended publications
  • Malayalam Noun and Verb Morphological Analyzer: a Simple Approach
    Malayalam Noun and Verb Morphological Analyzer: A Simple Approach Nimal J Valath1, Narsheedha Beegum2 M.Tech Student1, M.Tech Student2 ABSTRACT Malayalam is a language of the Dravidian family and is one of This paper discusses the methods involved in the the four major languages of this family with a rich literary development of a Simple Malayalam Verb and Noun tradition. It is very close to Tamil, one of the major languages of Morphological Analyzer. Since in Malayalam, words can be the same family. This was due to the extensive cultural derived from a root word, a purely dictionary based approach synthesis that took place between the speakers of the two for Morphological analysis is not practical. Hence, a ‘Rule-cum- languages. The origin of Malayalam as a distinct language may Dictionary’ based approach is followed along with the Suffix be traced to the last quarter of 9th Century A.D. Throughout its Stripping concept. The grammatical behavior of the language, gradual evolution Malayalam has been influenced by the the formation of words with multiple suffixes and the various circumstances prevailed on different periods. preparation of the language are dealt with here, with examples of noun and verb forms in detail. Mainly Malayalam was influenced by Sanskrit and Prakrit brought into Kerala by Brahmins. After the 11th century a unique mixture of the native languages of Kerala and Sanskrit Keywords known as Manipravalam served as the medium of literary Morphological Analyzer, Malayalam, Suffix stripping, expression. Malayalam absorbed a lot from Sanskrit, not only in Transliteration, Retransliteration, Verb and Noun, Sandhi rules, the lexical level, but also in the phonemic, morphemic and Word Formation, Noun Cases, Algorithm.
    [Show full text]
  • University of Groningen the Long and the Short of Priming in Visual Search
    University of Groningen The long and the short of priming in visual search Kruijne, Wouter; Meeter, Martijn Published in: Attention, Perception & Psychophysics DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0860-2 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2015 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Kruijne, W., & Meeter, M. (2015). The long and the short of priming in visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 77(5), 1558-1573. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0860-2 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.
    [Show full text]
  • Unsupervised Separation of Transliterable and Native Words For
    Unsupervised Separation of Transliterable and Native Words for Malayalam Deepak P Queen’s University Belfast, UK [email protected] Abstract names that need to be transliterated than translated to correlate with English text. On a manual analy- Differentiating intrinsic language words sis of a news article dataset, we found that translit- from transliterable words is a key step erated words and proper nouns each form 10-12% aiding text processing tasks involving dif- of all distinct words. It is useful to transliterate ferent natural languages. We consider such words for scenarios that involve processing the problem of unsupervised separation of Malayalam text in the company of English text; transliterable words from native words for this will avoid them being treated as separate index text in Malayalam language. Outlining a terms (wrt their transliteration) in a multi-lingual key observation on the diversity of char- retrieval engine, and help a statistical translation acters beyond the word stem, we develop system to make use of the link to improve effec- an optimization method to score words tiveness. In this context, it ia notable that there has based on their nativeness. Our method re- been recent interest in devising specialized meth- lies on the usage of probability distribu- ods to translate words that fall outside the core vo- tions over character n-grams that are re- cabulary (Tsvetkov and Dyer, 2015). fined in step with the nativeness scorings In this paper, we consider the problem of sepa- in an iterative optimization formulation. rating out such transliterable words from the other Using an empirical evaluation, we illus- words within an unlabeled dataset; we refer to trate that our method, DTIM, provides sig- the latter as “native” words.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuro-Opthalmology (Developments in Ophthalmology, Vol
    Neuro-Ophthalmology Developments in Ophthalmology Vol. 40 Series Editor W. Behrens-Baumann, Magdeburg Neuro- Ophthalmology Neuronal Control of Eye Movements Volume Editors Andreas Straube, Munich Ulrich Büttner, Munich 39 figures, and 3 tables, 2007 Basel · Freiburg · Paris · London · New York · Bangalore · Bangkok · Singapore · Tokyo · Sydney Andreas Straube Ulrich Büttner Department of Neurology Department of Neurology Klinikum Grosshadern Klinikum Grosshadern Marchioninistrasse 15 Marchioninistrasse 15 DE–81377 Munich DE–81377 Munich Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Neuro-ophthalmology / volume editors, Andreas Straube, Ulrich Büttner. p. ; cm. – (Developments in ophthalmology, ISSN 0250-3751 ; v. 40) Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 978-3-8055-8251-3 (hardcover : alk. paper) 1. Neuroophthalmology. I. Straube, Andreas. II. Büttner, U. III. Series. [DNLM: 1. Eye Movements–physiology. 2. Ocular Motility Disorders. 3. Oculomotor Muscles–physiology. 4. Oculomotor Nerve-physiology. W1 DE998NG v.40 2007 / WW 400 N4946 2007] RE725.N45685 2007 617.7Ј32–dc22 2006039568 Bibliographic Indices. This publication is listed in bibliographic services, including Current Contents® and Index Medicus. Disclaimer. The statements, options and data contained in this publication are solely those of the individ- ual authors and contributors and not of the publisher and the editor(s). The appearance of advertisements in the book is not a warranty, endorsement, or approval of the products or services advertised or of their effectiveness, quality or safety. The publisher and the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to persons or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content or advertisements. Drug Dosage. The authors and the publisher have exerted every effort to ensure that drug selection and dosage set forth in this text are in accord with current recommendations and practice at the time of publication.
    [Show full text]
  • Acta Psychologica 137 (2011) 138–150
    Acta Psychologica 137 (2011) 138–150 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy Looking, language, and memory: Bridging research from the visual world and visual search paradigms Falk Huettig a,b,⁎, Christian N.L. Olivers c, Robert J. Hartsuiker d a Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands b Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands c VU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands d Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium article info abstract Article history: In the visual world paradigm as used in psycholinguistics, eye gaze (i.e. visual orienting) is measured in order Received 3 March 2010 to draw conclusions about linguistic processing. However, current theories are underspecified with respect Received in revised form 22 July 2010 to how visual attention is guided on the basis of linguistic representations. In the visual search paradigm as Accepted 23 July 2010 used within the area of visual attention research, investigators have become more and more interested in Available online 3 September 2010 how visual orienting is affected by higher order representations, such as those involved in memory and language. Within this area more specific models of orienting on the basis of visual information exist, but they PsycINFO classification: 2323 need to be extended with mechanisms that allow for language-mediated orienting. In the present paper we 2326 review the evidence from these two different – but highly related – research areas. We arrive at a model in 2340 which working memory serves as the nexus in which long-term visual as well as linguistic representations 2346 (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Benefits of Being Bilingual
    The Cognitive Benefits of Being Bilingual By Viorica Marian, Ph.D., and Anthony Shook Editor’s note: Today, more of the world’s population is bilingual or multilingual than monolingual. In addition to facilitating cross-cultural communication, this trend also positively affects cognitive abilities. Researchers have shown that the bilingual brain can have better attention and task-switching capacities than the monolingual brain, thanks to its developed ability to inhibit one language while using another. In addition, bilingualism has positive effects at both ends of the age spectrum: Bilingual children as young as seven months can better adjust to environmental changes, while bilingual seniors can experience less cognitive decline. We are surrounded by language during nearly every waking moment of our lives. We use language to communicate our thoughts and feelings, to connect with others and identify with our culture, and to understand the world around us. And for many people, this rich linguistic environment involves not just one language but two or more. In fact, the majority of the world’s population is bilingual or multilingual. In a survey conducted by the European Commission in 2006, 56 percent of respondents reported being able to speak in a language other than their mother tongue. In many countries that percentage is even higher—for instance, 99 percent of Luxembourgers and 95 percent of Latvians speak more than one language.1 Even in the United States, which is widely considered to be monolingual, one-fifth of those over the age of five reported speaking a language other than English at home in 2007, an increase of 140 percent since 1980.2 Millions of Americans use a language other than English in their everyday lives outside of the home, when they are at work or in the classroom.
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Possessive Structures
    Ling 404 Lecture Notes No.4 Synchronic Clines in Morphology Productivity Clines: Japanese, Navajo, Mohawk, Eskimo, vs. Spanish, Italian, English One of the questions will be asking throughout is where do the above languages fall on the productivity cline and why (providing data and analyses.) One question to ask is to what degree is the lexicon ‘morpheme-based’ or ‘word-based’ (Chapters 3-4)? ‘Working Memory’ is implicated in the choice. For instance, are speakers all equally productive with their morpheme-units as tucked within word, a [-Fusion] language, or do some languages rather require morphemes within words to be memorized and incorporated as part of the lexical item, a [+Fusion] language? In addition to labeling languages as [+]Synthetic (as in Turkish, Hungarian, Spanish, English, Mohawk) or [-]Synthetic (as in Chinese, Vietnamese), the following data provide an additional exercise in teasing out what might be going on in languages which carry such a large number of embedded morphemes. Question leading to mid-term: How might such morphemes in Polysynthetic-type languages be stored and processed? The notion of [+/-Fusion] will be the central question here as we move to our mid-term material of this class. Typically, agglutinative morphemes are considered to be ‘loosely’ structured in that one morpheme has a one-to-one meaning. But the question might rather be how productive are the morphemes in isolation—viz., do they allow for movement? The question of movement, both at the word-level and at the morpheme-level will allow us to determine the nature of +/- productivity and +/- fusion. Regarding Movement.
    [Show full text]
  • Carly J. Leonard Curriculum Vitae
    Carly J. Leonard curriculum vitae Department of Psychology Email: [email protected] University of Colorado, Denver Office: 5005G North Classroom Campus Box 173, PO Box 173364 Phone: (303) 315-7068 Denver, CO 80217 Fax: (303) 315-7072 Education 2003 - 2008 Johns Hopkins University Psychological and Brain Sciences M.A., 2005; Ph.D., 2008 Advisor: Dr. Howard Egeth 1998 - 2002 Rutgers University (Rutgers College) summa cum laude: B.A. in Psychology; B.A. in Economics Minor in Cognitive Science Advisor: Dr. Zenon Pylyshyn Positions Held 2016 - Assistant Professor, University of Colorado Denver 2014 - 2016 Project Scientist, University of California, Davis 2008 - 2014 Postdoctoral Fellow, University of California, Davis Training with Dr. Steven Luck and Dr. James Gold of the Maryland Psychiatric Research Center, University of Maryland School of Medicine 2003 - 2008 Graduate Research Fellow, Johns Hopkins University Graduate Teaching Assistant, Johns Hopkins University 2002 - 2003 Research Assistant, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science 2000 - 2002 Undergraduate Research Assistant, Rutgers Center for Cognitive Science Professional Affiliations American Psychological Association, Member Cognitive Neuroscience Society, Member Psychonomic Society, Fellow (Full member) Vision Sciences Society, Member Publications Hahn, B., Robinson, B. M., Leonard, C. J., Luck, S. J., & Gold, J. M. (in press). Posterior parietal cortex dysfunction is central to working memory storage and broad cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience. Lee, J., Leonard, C. J., Luck, S. J., & Geng, J. J. (in press). Dynamics of feature-based attentional selection during color-shape conjunction search. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Bansal, S., Robinson, B. M., Geng, J. J., Leonard, C. J., Hahn, B., Luck, S.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 13 — November 2008 49Th Annual Meeting — November 13–16, 2008 — Chicago, Illinois
    Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 13 — November 2008 49th Annual Meeting — November 13–16, 2008 — Chicago, Illinois Papers 1–7 Friday Morning Motion and Attention motion is mixed. In the present study, target stimuli were associated with Grand Ballroom, Friday Morning, 8:00–9:40 objects that loomed, receded or remained static in arrays of varying size. Results showed that both motion types received equal prioritization, as Chaired by Gary Chon-Wen Shyi, National Chung Cheng University evidenced by their parallel search slopes, yet looming targets benefited from an overall reduction in reaction time (RT). Further investigation 8:00–8:15 (1) ruled out possible confounding explanations for this RT advantage, Differential Distribution of Visuospatial Attention in Tracking Mul- while a perceptual measure of performance confirmed the attentional tiple Moving Objects. GARY C.-W. SHYI & SUNG-EN CHIEN, Na- equivalence of the two motion types. Taken together, these results in- tional Chung Cheng University—Paying attention to a relatively com- dicate that looming and receding objects receive equal prioritization plex object has shown evidence for differential distribution within the during attentional selection. However, it may be that postattentional pro- object. Here, we explored differential distribution of attention when mul- cesses, possibly those involved in motor preparation, facilitate responses tiple moving objects were visually tracked. In Experiments 1 and 2, we to looming motion. not only replicated the findings reported by Alvarez and Scholl (2005), demonstrating both attentional concentration and attentional amplifica- 9:20–9:35 (5) tion, but also generalized the effects to uniformed circular shapes. In Individual Differences in Voluntary Visual Attention.
    [Show full text]
  • 2Ndind PAGE.Indd
    Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society — Volume 11 — November 2006 47th Annual Meeting — November 16–19, 2006 — Houston, Texas Papers 1–8 Friday Morning Semantic Priming within-language associative priming effects were quite similar. Fi- Grand Ballroom JKL, Friday Morning, 8:00–9:20 nally, Experiment 3 showed a significant translation priming effect for noncognates. Thus, highly fluent bilinguals do develop between- Chaired by David A. Balota, Washington University language links with noncognates at the conceptual level. We examine the implications of these results for models of bilingual memory. 8:00–8:15 (1) Effects of Semantic Priming and Stimulus Quality: Insights From Judgment and Decision Making 1 RT Distributional Analyses. DAVID A. BALOTA & MELVIN J. YAP, Grand Ballroom ABC, Friday Morning, 8:00–10:00 Washington University—In a series of experiments, reaction time (RT) distribution analyses were used to examine the nature of the in- Chaired by Christine Ruva, University of South Florida fluence of semantic priming on lexical decision and word-naming per- formance. In contrast to variables that both shift and skew the RT dis- 8:00–8:15 (5) tributions (e.g., word frequency and lexicality), semantic relatedness Judgments of Functional Relationships in Systems of Three Vari- produces primarily a shift in the RT distribution, with no increase in ables. KENT L. NORMAN & BENJAMIN K. SMITH, University of skewing. In the standard clear target conditions, the shifting of the RT Maryland, College Park—The participants were told the relationship distribution suggests a constant influence of priming on lexical pro- between variables A and B and between B and C and then asked to cessing across all targets.
    [Show full text]
  • A Psychophysical Approach to Test: “The Bignetti Model”
    PSYCHOLOGY AND COGNITIVE SCIENCES ISSN 2380-727X http://dx.doi.org/10.17140/PCSOJ-3-121 Open Journal Research A Psychophysical Approach to Test: “The *Corresponding author Bignetti Model” Enrico Bignetti, MD Professor of Clinical Biochemistry and Molecular Biology Department of Veterinary Sciences Enrico Bignetti, MD1*; Francesca Martuzzi, MD2; Angelo Tartabini, MD3 University of Parma Via del Taglio 10 Parma 43126, Italy 1Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Parma, Via del Taglio 10, Parma 43126, Tel. +39 0521 032710 Italy [email protected]; E-mail: 2Department of Food Science, University of Parma, Via del Taglio 10, Parma 43126, Italy [email protected] 3Department of Neurosciences, University of Parma, Via del Taglio 10, Parma 43126, Italy Volume 3 : Issue 1 Article Ref. #: 1000PCSOJ3121 ABSTRACT The cognitive “Bignetti Model” (TBM) thoroughly discussed elsewhere, shares a strong anal- Article History ogy with “Learning Through Experience” (LTE) and Bayesian Learning Process (BLP). Here, Received: December 24th, 2016 TBM’s theory is challenged by means of a psychophysical press/no-press decision task (DT). Accepted: February 2nd, 2017 Participants must press a computer key in response to sweet food image (SWEET) or refrain Published: February 3rd, 2017 from doing it with a salted food image (SALTED) (24 trials each, mixed at random in a 48-trial DT). Reaction times (RT) plotted as a function of trials decrease exponentially according to a Citation well-known “intertrial priming” effect. When 1 SWEET is repeated 24 times per DT, RTs tend Bignetti E, Martuzzi F, Tartabini A. to a minimal value that corresponds to the fastest, instinctive RT the participant can exhibit A psychophysical approach to test: when engaged in a traffic light-based task.
    [Show full text]
  • Teap 2018 Abstracts
    60. TeaP 2018 Abstracts of the 60h Conference of Experimental Psychologists Edited by Alexander C. Schütz, Anna Schubö, Dominik Endres, Harald Lachnit March, 11th to 14th Marburg, Germany This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in other ways, and storage in data banks. The use of registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. The authors and the publisher of this volume have taken care that the information and recommendations contained herein are accurate and compatible with the standards generally accepted at the time of publication. Nevertheless, it is difficult to ensure that all the information given is entirely accurate for all circumstances. The publisher disclaims any liability, loss, or damage incurred as a consequence, directly or indirectly, of the use and application of any of the contents of this volume. © 2018 Pabst Science Publishers, 49525 Lengerich, Germany Printing: KM-Druck, 64823 Groß-Umstadt, Germany Contents Keynote lectures __________________________________________________________ 5 Contributions ___________________________________________________________ 11 Author index ___________________________________________________________ 307 Keynote lectures 5 Keynote lectures Control of Attention in Natural Environments Mary Hayhoe Center for Perceptual Systems, The University of Texas at Austin [email protected] In the context of natural behavior, humans must allocate attention and select information from the visual scene to satisfy behavioral goals.
    [Show full text]