(2016) 27: 145–154

______WHITE‐CROWNED (DIXIPHIA ) USE OF SPACE IN THE ECUADORIAN AMAZON ______Wendy P. Tori1 ∙ Kasun H. Bodawatta1 ∙ Kevin Tanager1,2 ∙ Erin L. Lewis1 ∙ Dörte S. Neumeister1 ∙ Joel Hogle1

1Biology Department, Earlham College, 801 National Rd. W, Richmond, IN 47374, USA. 2University of Michigan Medical School, 1301 Catherine St., Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA. E‐mail: Wendy P. Tori ∙ [email protected]

Abstract ∙ patterns of space use have fundamental consequences for ecological processes such as animal‐ mediated seed dispersal. This study examines the use of space of an understory frugivore: the White‐crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra) during the non‐breeding season at Tiputini Biodiversity Station in Ecuador. We radio‐tracked seven individuals of different sexes and ages. We found that White‐crowned have relatively small home ranges varying from 1.19 to 5.73 ha. The home ranges of male juveniles (2.25 ± 0.62 SE) were larger than that of the adult males (1.20 ± 0.01 SE), but smaller than those of females (4.27 ± 0.77 SE). Females and second‐year males were found to perform more long distance flights outside their core home range than hatch‐year or adult males. These movement differences suggest that they disperse seeds longer distances and more evenly across their home range. Adult and hatch‐year males appear to perform more short distance seed dispersal events. We argue that these contri‐ butions are complementary, and that White‐crowned Manakins are key dispersal agents that have a significantly influ‐ ence in the structure and composition of understory plant communities in the Amazon rainforest.

Resumen ∙ Uso de espacio del Saltarín Coroniblanco (Dixiphia pipra) en la Amazonía Ecuatoriana Los patrones de uso espacial utilizados por animales tienen consecuencias fundamentales en procesos ecológicos, tales como la dispersión de semillas. Este estudio examina el uso espacial de una especie frugívora de sotobosque: el Saltarín Coroniblanco (Dixiphia pipra) durante el ciclo no‐reproductivo en la Estación de Biodiversidad de Tiputini en Ecuador. Seguimos por radio telemetría a siete individuos de diferentes sexos y edades. Encontramos que el Saltarín Coroniblanco tiene un rango de hogar relativamente pequeño, variando entre 1,19 a 5,73 ha. El rango de hogar de los machos juveniles (2,25 ± 0,62 EE) es más grande que el de los machos adultos (1,20 ± 0,01 EE), pero más pequeño que el de las hembras (4,27 ± 0,77 EE). El estudio encontró que las hembras y los machos de segundo año realizaron vuelos más largos fuera del núcleo de su rango de hogar, comparado con machos juveniles del año o machos adultos. Estas diferencias sugieren que podrían dispersar semillas a mayores distancias y de manera más homogénea a lo largo de su rango de hogar. Los machos adultos y los juveniles del año parecen llevar a cabo eventos de dispersión de semillas a distancias más cortas. Nosotros argumentamos que estas contribuciones son complementarias y que los Saltarínes Coroniblancos son agentes de dispersión claves, influenciando considerablemente la estructura y la composición de las comunidades de plantas del sotobosque en el bosque tropical lluvioso de la Amazonía Ecuatoriana.

Key words: Ecuador ∙ Home range ∙ Lek ∙ Manakin ∙ Pipridae ∙ Radio telemetry ∙ Seed dispersal ∙ Space use

INTRODUCTION

Having knowledge of animal use of space can help us to better understand key ecological and evolutionary pro‐ cesses (Naef‐Denzer 1994), as well as identify habitat requirements for conservation planning and management (Davis et al. 2014). Animal movements can be influenced by many factors such as topography (Westcott 1994), food distribution (Pyke 1984), competition (MacArthur & Levins 1964), predation (Jefferies & Lawton 1984; Suhonen et al. 1994), mate availability (Bradbury & Gibson 1983), among others. These factors may play differ‐ ent roles for individuals of different ages and sexes (Westcott & Graham 2000), and could also vary between

Receipt 10 September 2015 ∙ First decision 1 June 2016 ∙ Acceptance 10 August 2016 ∙ Online publication 23 August 2016

Communicated by W. Douglas Robinson © The Neotropical Ornithological Society

145 ORNITOLOGÍA NEOTROPICAL (2016) 27: 145–154 seasons (e.g., reproductive vs non‐reproductive sea‐ METHODS sons, Théry 1992). This is particularly true in species with lek mating systems, where males, females, and Study site. The research was conducted in a 100 ha juveniles play very different roles in reproduction. In plot (Harpia plot) at Tiputini Biodiversity Station these systems, adult males cluster in space and (hereafter TBS) located along the Tiputini River in the defend a small territory where they perform court‐ Orellana province in eastern Ecuador (~0°38'S, ship displays to prospective females with the purpose 76°08'W). TBS encompasses 650 ha of largely undis‐ of copulation. Females freely visit display territories turbed rainforest, located within the 1.5 million ha to choose mates, after which they perform all paren‐ Yasuní Biosphere Reserve. The Harpia plot elevation tal duties without help (e.g., Bradbury & Gibson ranges from 201 to 233 m a.s.l. (Durães et al. 2007). It 1983, Ligon 1999). In some lekking species, juvenile is gridded (100 x 200 m grid lines), and trails are geo‐ males have delayed plumage maturation (e.g., Foster referenced and marked every 50 m (for more infor‐ 1987, McDonald 1993, DuVal 2005, Ryder & Durães mation see Loiselle et al. 2007a). 2005, Doucet et al. 2007, Ryder & Wolfe 2009) and it can take them a few years to gain access to reproduc‐ Study species. White‐crowned Manakins are small tion (Tori 2008, Ryder et al. 2008, Duval & Kempe‐ in the family Pipridae. They are sexually naers 2008, but see Alatalo et al. 1996). Juvenile dimorphic; adult males are black with white crowns females typically join the reproductive pool faster and napes, and females are olive‐green with blue‐ (WPT pers. observ.). grey heads (del Hoyo et al. 2004). Juvenile males Differences in individual space use can have signif‐ fledge the nest with plumage coloration similar to icant effects in ecological processes such as seed dis‐ females, and they usually show some signs of definite persal. Seed dispersal is the movement of seeds away male plumage (i.e., white feathers in crown and black from a parental plant. It can be advantageous for feathers in body) after their first year (i.e., end of first many reasons such as escaping high mortality caused breeding season). Males acquire full adult plumage by distance/density dependence due to crowding or late in their second‐year (Ryder & Durães 2005). disease near the parental plant, by allowing the seed White‐crowned Manakins display on exploded leks, to arrive to rare sites with higher probability of sur‐ in which males are in auditory but not visual contact vival, or by finding suitable sites that are unpredict‐ (Castro‐Astor et al. 2007, Tori 2008). able in space and time (e.g., tree fall gaps; Wenney White‐crowned Manakins inhabit the understory. 2001). Typically most seeds are moved relatively They are mainly frugivorous (Worthington 1982, Tori short distances (Wilson 1993), however long‐distance 2008), but they also consume some insects (Fair et al. dispersal events are crucial for population spread, 2013, WPT pers. observ.). They eat a wide diversity of population dynamics, and for the maintenance of soft fleshy fruits and arillate seeds; consuming the genetic connectivity (Trakhtenbrot et al. 2005). pulp or aril and discarding the seed via regurgitation The objective of this study was to describe the use or passage through the gut. Loiselle et al. (2007b), in of space and the potential role as seed disperser of a a short‐term study conducted at our study site, lekking species of the family Pipridae, the White‐ reported that White‐crowned Manakins consumed crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra). To our knowledge, 44 different fruit species. Fruits from the families only one previous study examined movement pat‐ Melastomataceae, Rubiaceae, and Araceae make up a terns by this species using radio‐marked individuals large proportion of Manakin diets (Krijger et al. 1997, (Théry 1992), however, data available were collected Loiselle and Blake 1990). mostly during the breeding season and at one single location (French Guyana), despite its wide geographic Home range and spatial movement estimates. We range. Here we examined space use by individuals of set up mist nets to capture White‐crowned Manakin different sexes and ages during the non‐breeding sea‐ individuals around fruiting trees in previously known son at Tiputini Biodiversity Station in the Ecuadorian manakin capture locations within the Harpia plot dur‐ Amazon rainforest. We predicted that female space ing June–July 2009 and 2014, and June 2011 (non‐ use (measured as home ranges size, home range breeding season: when males are not fully active at overlap and distance traveled in 30 minutes) would leks; breeding season: November–April, Durães be greater than those of adult males, because adult 2009). Green individuals with unknown sex were males are constrained by their need to defend territo‐ molecularly sexed (Griffiths et al. 1998). All individu‐ ries which are a requirement for reproduction. More‐ als were tagged with a radio transmitter that over, we predicted that juvenile males would have weighted less than 5% of their body mass (models larger home ranges (hereafter HR) than adult males BD‐2N/0.51 g and BD‐2N/0.39 g, Hollohil Systems and females. Juvenile males are expected to visit mul‐ Ltd). Radio transmitters were attached using Rappole tiple leks to maximize their social affiliations with harnesses (Rappole & Tipton 1991). We employed tri‐ other males, and increase their chances to inherit a angulation telemetry and recorded simultaneously territory (Ryder et al. 2008) and become established the direction (i.e., bearings) of the with respect in a lek to gain potential access to reproduction. Ages to three observers in known locations (mean ± SE and sexes with larger HRs were predicted to perform time interval between recordings 14.99 ± 0.354 min‐ longer distance seed dispersal events. utes) for 5 to 9 days (until all areas known to be used

146 SPACE USE OF WHITE‐CROWNED MANAKIN by the bird were well sampled). In one case (adult Using these data, we ran a One‐way ANOVA compar‐ male 2) the battery of the transmitter died after 3 ing the average movement among individuals, and days and we were able to collect only two full days of we conducted Tukey HSD post hoc comparisons. Data telemetry data. In order to avoid triangulation errors were transformed to fulfill parametric assumptions. due to the movement of the bird, the three observers Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS (version took highly synchronized measurements (Schmutz & 23). White 1990) using two‐way radios. We ensured that all had telemetry recordings across the day RESULTS from dawn to dusk. We entered our data (observer locations and bearings) into the program LOASTM We estimated the HR of seven individuals: two males 4.0.3.8 (Ecological Software solutions LLC) and used in adult definitive plumage (A males), one second‐ the maximum likelihood estimator (accuracy year male (SY male), one hatch‐year male (HY male), 0.000001, 60 iterations) to calculate bird locations (x, and three females. The number of telemetry loca‐ y coordinates) for each telemetry recording. We dis‐ tions used to calculate HR estimates ranged from 73 carded all measurement sets that did not form trian‐ to 205 with a mean of 121.42 (± 18.81 SE). For all gles or had an error ellipse area greater than 5000 m2 individuals we used larger samples sizes than the (for details see LOASTM 4.0.3.8, Ecological Software minimum number of locations recommended by solutions LLC). We then used R (version 2.12), Geo‐ some authors in the literature (> 30 locations; Sea‐ spatial Model Environment (GME) and ArcGIS (ver‐ man et al. 1999). Moreover, our HR area accumula‐ sion 10.1, minimum bounding geometry ‐ convex tion curves suggest that our MCPs are good hull) to estimate the HR of all individuals. We calcu‐ representations of the HR during the non‐breeding lated HRs using two methods (1) 100% Minimum season, since all individuals reached a relatively sta‐ Convex Polygon (MCP), a widely used method that ble asymptote with the sample sizes used (Figure 1, draws a boundary polygon around all the known bird Table 1). locations (provides information about extent of an The mean (± SE) 100% MCP HR size was largest for individual’s HR and allows comparisons across stud‐ females (4.27 ± 0.77 ha), followed by juvenile males ies); and (2) Kernel‐density Estimators (50% and (2.25 ± 0.62 ha) and adult males (1.20 ± 0.01 ha). The 90%), which provide a HR estimate (probability den‐ second‐year male showed a larger MCP HR (2.87 ha) sity function) based on a probabilistic measure of ani‐ than the hatch‐year male (1.64 ha). An equivalent mal space use (provides information about utilization pattern but with smaller HR areas were found using of space within the HR). For the Kernel‐density Esti‐ 50% and 90% Kernel‐density Estimators (Table 1, mator we used the least‐squares cross‐validation Figure 2 A‐G). In all cases, individuals’ use of space smoothing parameters (LSCV, Worton 1995). To was very heterogeneous, spending 50% of their time examine if we had appropriate sampling effort to in an area as small as 9–26% of their HR. estimate accurate HRs using MCP, we created HR MCP HRs of White‐crowned Manakins of all ages area accumulation curves for each radio‐tracked indi‐ and sexes largely overlap with each other (Table 2, vidual. To do this, we randomly drew 5 locations at Figure 2H). Female HRs overlap with male HRs even the time (until all locations were included) and built outside the breeding season. Male HRs overlap more accumulation curves using HR area as the predictor with other individuals. Female HRs were not found to variable. We fitted a polynomial (second order) equa‐ overlap with each other, however we believe this lack tion and assumed appropriate sampling if the curve of overlap is influenced by chance due to small sam‐ reached a stable asymptote. ple sizes (population was not comprehensively sam‐ To compare HR overlap (100% MCP) between indi‐ pled). viduals we used ArcGIS (version 10.1). We created The straight‐line Euclidean distance traveled by raster layers for individual HRs (cell size 1 m2), and individuals during 30 minutes ranged from 3.53 to reclassified each layer giving a value of 1 to any cell 178.34 m for females (n = 104), from 2.02 to 242.36 m within the HR and a value of 0 to cells outside the HR. (n = 75) for juvenile males, and from 4.44 to 79.23 m Next we added each pair of layers using raster calcu‐ (n = 52) for adult males. We found a significant differ‐ lator and calculated the HR area overlap by adding ence in the distance traveled in 30 minutes among the total area of cells with a value of two. individuals of different sexes and ages (F6, 224 = 7.86, To get a better sense of the mobility of this spe‐ P < 0.001, Figure 3). Females and the second‐year cies and their potential for seed dispersal within their male tended to travel longer distances than adult and HR, we calculated the straight‐line Euclidean distance hatch‐year males. Statistical results for pairwise post each individual traveled in 30‐minute intervals. Time hoc comparisons are shown in Figure 3. Similar result intervals were selected to mimic the maximum gut‐ were found for 15‐ and 45‐minute intervals (data not transit time found for 14 species of labeled fruit (fam‐ shown). ilies: Araceae, Malpighiaceae, Cyclanthaceae, Dille‐ niaceae, Annonaceae, Salicaceae, and Rubiaceae) in DISCUSSION two closely related species (Red‐capped Manakin Ce‐ ratopipra mentalis, Golden‐collared Manakin Mana‐ Our results only partially support our predictions cus vitellinus; for details see Worthington 1989). about HR size among sex and age groups in White‐

147 ORNITOLOGÍA NEOTROPICAL (2016) 27: 145–154

Figure 1. Home range area accumulation curves (using 100% Minimum Convex Polygons) and polynomial second order func‐ tions for three White‐crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra) females (grey, black and white circles respectively), two adult males (white and black squares) and 2 juvenile males (hatch‐year male: grey triangles and second‐year male: white triangles) at Ti‐ putini Biodiversity Station, eastern Ecuador. crowned Manakins. We found that female HRs are (Pipridae: erythrocephala, 3.0–22.7 ha), about 3.5 times larger than those of adult males, but White‐fronted Manakin (Pipridae: Lepidothrix serena, juvenile males have intermediate HR sizes (1.9 times 2.8–24.1 ha), White‐bearded Manakin (Pipridae: smaller than females). In lek‐mating systems, males Manacus manacus, 2.1–20.3 ha), White‐throated are expected to have high territory attendance to Manakin (Pipridae: Corapipo gutturalis, 2.0–10.0 ha), maximize reproductive success (e.g., Fiske et al. 1998, and Brown‐winged Schiffornis (Tityridae: Schiffornis Karubian et al. 2012). We found that males are more turdina, 14.3–20.1 ha) (Théry 1992, but see Durães et sedentary maintaining a close watch of their territo‐ al. 2007). Our HR estimates are relatively consistent ries even outside the breeding season. Females are with the only other study that estimated HRs in usually solitary (WPT pers. observ.) and their move‐ White‐crowned Manakins in French Guiana (Théry ments are not constrained by territorial defense, so 1992, see Table 1). However, Théry found that imma‐ they are able to move more freely among resources ture males had the largest HRs (Table 1), and the HR (e.g., food, bathing sites, mates, etc.) and have larger estimates (100% MCP) resulting from his study were HRs. Second‐year males were found to have larger substantially larger than ours, exceeding our mean HRs and longer trip distances than hatch‐year males. HR estimates for all sexes and ages by more than 45% As a consequence females and second‐year males (female: 4.27 vs. 9.0 ha, male: 1.20 vs. 2.2 ha, and move more widely and are expected to perform second‐year male: 2.25 vs. 19.8 ha). These differ‐ more long distance seed dispersal events. Our results ences could be the result of many reasons. First, both suggests that differences in age and sex have a strong studies used telemetry methods that could have effect on individual movement patterns that can be resulted in different levels of accuracy when estimat‐ translated into important consequences in seed‐dis‐ ing bird locations. In Théry’s study, bird locations (i.e., persal patterns. fixes) were recorded by a single observer from sta‐ tions placed every 20 m in the field. This methodol‐ Home ranges and space use. We found that White‐ ogy does not account for triangulation errors due to crowned Manakins have a relatively small HR fast movements by the radio‐tracked bird (see (1.19–5.73 ha) compared with many other tropical Schmutz & White 1990). In our study, we recorded understory frugivores such as Ochre‐bellied Fly‐ bird locations by three observers simultaneously. catcher (Tyrannidae: Mionectes oleaginous, 28.4 ± Durães et al. (2007), using a similar synchronized 3.7 ha; Wescott & Graham 2000), Swallow‐tailed telemetry technique in a closely related manakin spe‐ Manakin (Pipridae: Chiroxiphia caudata, 1.6–15.6 ha; cies (Blue‐crowned Manakin, Lepidothrix coronata), Hansbauer et al. 2008), Golden‐headed Manakin found similar female HR areas than ours (100% MCP:

148 SPACE USE OF WHITE‐CROWNED MANAKIN for and

failure.

showed sizes

HR Polygon

battery

Females

Convex

published

Ecuador.

transmitter

(NBS)

of

(BS)

Minimum 30 reported 1992 (BS)

previously

Na 20.9 9.2 Station,

(ha)

(BS),

Théry list

(100% because 8.8,

2.3

by 18.7, sizes

we

2.1, HR days

full Biodiversity hectares

2 in

1.8 90% 0.63 2.70 0.75 3.08 3.20 0.92 (ha) for

comparison, sizes Tiputini

of only

at Kernel HR

50% 0.17 0.52 0.81 0.16 0.82 1.01 0.26

sake

and

the season

(ha) 1.19 2.87 3.09 1.64 4.00 5.73 1.20 MCP conducted

For

was

P breeding 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 ‐

information, adult).

< < < < < < <

=

non

(A

Telemetry model df

the

* 2,13 2,16 2,27 2,32 2,16 2,38 2,15

males

during 1992).

F

adult 91.96

311.98 505.14 491.23 228.40 453.49 202.78 polynomial

Théry

than

2 model R

093 0.98 0.97 0.97 0.97 0.91 0.96 monitored

HRs (NBS,

2

2 2 2 x

calculations,

2

2

larger 2

Polynomial 7.56 seasons 1.81x 1.40x 1.78x

(HR) – 1.45x – individuals 3.32x – –

‐ ‐ 0.93x ) ‐

showed range pipra

592.14x breeding 377.35x 432.57x

258.05x

‐ 1170.04x

+ + + 670.870x 145.04x

+

+

year)

Equation + + ‐

non home

Dixiphia for 2537 ( 547.48 =8.54 and

1073.83 6676.54

= 5478.89 5682.176 = y ‐ ‐

second

‐ y = = y =

= =

y y

y (BS) used

y

SY

Manakin

year, ‐ # 87 86 73 93 breeding 165 205 141 locations

locations of the

crowned

hatch ‐

=

* days (HY during 5 9 9 5 7 8

White

3

number

of

followed # males seven

3 2 1 1 2

of

measured followed,

male male

male male Young

SY Sex/age HY A A Female Female Female days

of

males. Manakins

Estimators) ‐ than

Number

density crowned HRs ‐ 1. ‐

White Table larger Kernel

149 SPACE USE OF WHITE‐CROWNED MANAKIN

Figure 2. White‐crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra) home ranges (HRs) during the non‐breeding season at Tiputini Biodiversity Station, eastern Ecuador. Panels A‐G show individual HRs using a 100% Minimum Convex Polygons (dotted line) and Kernel‐ density Estimators (50%: double hatched line; and 90%: solid line). White circles depict bird locations. Panels are drawn to the same scale for comparisons. Panel H shows HR overlaps for 100% Minimum Convex Polygons for females (female 1–3), adult (A male 1–2), hatch‐year (HY male) and second‐year (SY male) males. Note the change in scale for panel H.

4 ha, 95% confidence intervals 2–6 ha). Second, HR across seasons, because individuals need to fulfil dif‐ estimates can be influenced by small sample sizes ferent life‐history needs throughout the year. For and the statistical properties of the HR estimator example, female 3 HR did not overlap with any lek used. Minimum Convex Polygon estimates have the (WPT pers. observ.), thus she will need to extend her limitation that the size of the HR tends to increase as HR during the breeding season to get access to sample size increases (Boulanger & White 1990). This mates, and could also potentially alter her movement is an unlikely explanation for the differences found, patterns due to parental activities (e.g., feeding since our cumulative HR area results suggest that our chicks, traveling with fledglings). In this sense, our HR estimates reached a relatively stable asymptote female HR estimates are probably conservative, since and therefore should be good estimates of HR sizes. females might travel large distances outside their Third, the HR estimations reported by Théry were normal ranges to fulfill reproductive needs. Lastly, done mostly during the breeding season, while our the differences found between studies might be estimates were done in the non‐breeding season. It is explained by differences in bird movements due to likely that bird movements significantly change variations in habitat characteristics or bird behaviors

150 ORNITOLOGÍA NEOTROPICAL (2016) 27: 145–154

Table 2. Pair‐wise comparison matrix of the percent of area of overlap between 100% Minimum Convex Polygon home ranges (HRs) of White‐crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra) females , adult (A), hatch‐year (HY) and second‐year (SY) males in eastern Ecuador. Cells below the diagonal report the presence or absence of HR overlaps between pairs of individuals. Cells above the diagonal report the percent HR overlap (% area of the HR) of individuals in the row, followed by the percent of HR overlap of the individuals in the column.

Sex/age Female 1 Female 2 Female 3 A male 1 A male 2 HY male SY male

Female 1 ‐ 0 0 0 0 0 47.8,51.3 Female 2 No ‐ 0 18.8, 62.6 4.1, 13.8 37.9, 92.8 0 Female 3 No No ‐ 1.8, 8.8 8.1, 39.0 0 0 A male 1 No Overlap Overlap ‐ 46.8, 47.2 68.1, 50.0 0 A male 2 No Overlap Overlap Overlap ‐ 16.6, 12.1 49.7, 20.6 HY male No Overlap Overlap Overlap Overlap ‐ 0 SY male Overlap No No Overlap Overlap No ‐ between study sites. Regardless of these differences, prevalent plant families in the Neotropics (e.g., both studies are consistent with the hypothesis that Wheelwright et al. 1984, Loiselle & Blake 1999, Loi‐ adult males are more stationary than females selle & Blake 2002). As lek breeding birds, White‐ because they need to defend territories and display crowned Manakins of different sexes (Karubian et al. for females in order to have access to reproduction 2012) and ages are expected to have different repro‐ (territoriality appears to be a pre‐requisite for repro‐ ductive strategies that can lead to different effective‐ duction in this species, Tori 2008). Interestingly, our ness as seed dispersers (Cestari & Pizo 2013). Males results differ significantly from Théry’s HR sizes for have been suggested to be more constrained than males during the non‐breeding season. Théry argued females and juveniles in the potential for seed move‐ that male HRs during the non‐breeding season are ment (long‐distance dispersal) because they are unstable, and can cover up to 30 ha in 5 days. To the required to spend considerable time at aggregated contrary, our results suggest that male White‐ display sites to get access to reproduction (Loiselle et crowned Manakins stay close to their established ter‐ al. 2007b). Our findings are consistent with these ritories year round, probably defending their display arguments. We found that females and second‐year sites from potential contesters even outside the males had larger HRs, performed more long‐distance breeding season. flights outside their core HR, and traveled farther dis‐ Our HR results also show that males visit other tances during 30‐minute intervals (representative neighboring territories within their lek, but they do gut‐passage time). The mean seed dispersal distance not perform long‐distance flights to visit other leks for females (55.47 ± 7.10 m) and the second‐year during the non‐breeding season. We also found that male (57.40 m) in 30 minutes is about 1.8 times female, male, and juvenile HRs overlap in space. larger than the one found for resident males (29.77 ± Although our data provides no information on the 2.55 m). Our study suggests that the majority of social interactions between individuals, it demon‐ seeds ingested by males are deposited in the lek strates space‐use sharing among individuals of differ‐ neighborhood (HR core area). Surprisingly, the only ent ages and sexes. This information is consistent hatch‐year male we followed showed similar seed‐ with previous reports that suggest that manakins can dispersal potential as adult males (31.74 m). Females feed at fruiting trees (Leck 1969, Lill 1976) and inter‐ and second‐year males generate a more extensive act at bathing sites (Théry 1992) with little to no and even distribution of seeds, and are more likely to aggression among conspecifics. Tolerance of other be responsible for long distance dispersal events. individuals across the HR is not expected to be the Adult and hatch‐year males appear to provide com‐ same for individuals of all sexes. Adult males for plementary dispersal services, dispersing seed at instance have antagonistic interactions with other smaller distances and in a more clumped distribution. adult males within their territories (about 4.7% of Radio‐tracking revealed that adult males spent large their HR area) at leks during the breeding season (Tori amounts of their time in very small core areas around 2008), and they have been found to respond to play‐ leks. It is possible that leks could act as ecological back and bird dummies within their territories during seed traps, where clumped seeds within territories the non‐breeding season (WPT pers. observ.). As far (e.g., area under favorite display perches) might have as we know, there is no evidence that females or lower survival probabilities due to crowding (Cestari hatch‐year males would engage in antagonistic inter‐ & Pizo 2013). However, Ryder et al. (2006) reported a actions with conspecifics within their HRs. higher incidence of fruiting trees and fruit biomass within White‐crowned Manakin leks at TBS. Other Seed dispersal potential. Manakins have been authors have also reported higher density of seeds reported as important seed dispersers for some with no apparent effect on seedling survival in leks of

151 SPACE USE OF WHITE‐CROWNED MANAKIN

Figure 3. Mean distance traveled (± SE) by White‐crowned Manakin (Dixiphia pipra) individuals during 30‐minute intervals. Bars with similar letters are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s HSD, P > 0.05). Females and second‐year males (SY male) tended to travel farther distances than males. Surprisingly the hatch‐year male (HY male) moved similar distances as the adult males (A male). the Long‐wattled Umbrellabird (Cephalopterus pen‐ ferent environmental characteristics that affect the duliger) in Ecuador (Karubian et al. 2012). This sug‐ probability of plant recruitment. gests that even sedentary displaying males could play an important role in seed dispersal, and have a large ACKNOWLEDGMENTS influence in the structure and composition of plant communities. Special thanks to M. Hennessey, Z. Silberberg, and M. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study Ventura for their help collecting and analyzing data. that provides significant insight into how differences We are deeply indebted to B. Loiselle and J. Blake for in life history strategies of White‐crowned Manakins their collaboration. Special thanks to J. I. Pareja for can drive important consequences for seed dispersal his insights, helpful comments, and support along the even outside the breeding season. Our results suggest study. We are grateful to J. H. Tori for help with trans‐ that individuals of different sexes and ages are not lation of the abstract. We are grateful to W. Robin‐ equivalent in their ecological role as seed dispersers. son, K. Delhey, and one anonymous reviewer for Females and second‐year males make longer move‐ helpful comments on the manuscript. We also thank ments, thus distributing relatively few seeds at lek D. and C. Romo, K. Swing, J. Guerra, D. Mosquera, sites. In contrast, hatch‐year and adult males act T. B. Ryder, J. R. Hidalgo, D. McCartney, K. Wills, mainly as short‐distance seed dispersers within their E. Butler, J. Iverson, C. Smith, E. Pareja, A. Otero, J. HR core area. We argue that these contributions are Logan, and all the Tiputini Biodiversity Station staff complementary and that White‐crowned Manakins for their support. We would like to thank N. and S. are key dispersal agents for many understory plants in Test, Earlham College's Faculty‐Student Collaborative the Amazon rainforest. We acknowledge that our Research Project Fund, and the Biology Department small sample sizes limit the level of our inference, and at Earlham College for funding this project. Thanks to we understand that this study is not capturing all the Ministerio del Ambiente, Distrito Forestal Napo, variability in space use for different sexes and ages. Tena, Ecuador for providing permits (Permits 13‐IC‐ Further studies are required to provide insight on FAU‐DFN, 041‐2014‐FAU‐MAE‐DPAO‐PNY). This re‐ individual movement patterns during the breeding search was carried out in accordance with Institu‐ season, as well as to examine whether individuals of tional Animal Care and Use Committee protocol different ages and sexes bring seeds to sites with dif‐ number 20140429 – 1WT at Earlham College.

152 ORNITOLOGÍA NEOTROPICAL (2016) 27: 145–154

REFERENCES Krijber, CL, M Opdam, M Théry & F Bongers (1997) Courtship behavior of manakins and seed bank composition in a Alatalo, RV, T Burke, J Dann, O Hanotte, J Höglund, A Lundberg, French Guianan Rain Forest. Journal of Tropical Ecology 13: R Moss & PT Rintamäki (1996) Paternity, copulation distur‐ 631–636. bance and female choice in lekking Black Grouse. Animal Leck, CF (1969) Observations of birds exploiting a Central Behaviour 52: 861–873. American fruit tree. Wilson Bulletin 81: 264–269. Boulanger, JG & GC White (1990) A comparison of home‐range Ligon, D (1999) The evolution of avian breeding systems. estimators using Monte Carlo simulation. The Journal of Oxford Ornithology Series. Oxford Univ. Press Inc., New Wildlife Management 54: 310–315. York, New York, USA. Bradbury, J & R Gibson (1983) Leks and mate choice. Pp Lill, A (1976) Lek behavior in the Golden‐headed Manakin, Pipra 109–138 in Bateson, P (ed). Mate choice. Cambridge Univ. erythrocephala, in Trinidad (West Indies). Paul Parey, Berlin Press, New York, New York, USA. and Hamburg, Germany. Castro‐Astor, IN, MA Alves & RB Cavalcanti (2007) Display Loiselle, BA & JG Blake (1990) Diets of understory fruit‐eating behavior and spatial distribution of the White‐crowed birds in Costa Rica. Studies in Avian Biology 13: 91–103. Manakin in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil. The Condor 109: Loiselle, BA & JG Blake (1999) Dispersal of melastome seeds by 155–166. fruit‐eating birds of tropical forest understory. Ecology 80: Cestari, C & MA Pizo (2013) Seed dispersal by the lek‐forming 330–336. White‐bearded Manakin (Manacus manacus, Pipridae) in Loiselle, BA & JG Blake (2002) Potential consequences of the Brazilian Atlantic Forest. Journal of Tropical Ecology 29: extinction of frugivorous birds for shrubs of a tropical wet 381–389. forest. Pp. 397–406 in Levey, DJ, WR Silva & M Galetti (eds). Davis, DM, KP Reese & SC Gardner (2014) Diurnal space use Seed dispersal and frugivory: ecology, evolution and con‐ and seasonal movement patterns of Greater Sage‐grouse servation. CAB International, Wallingford, UK. in northeastern California. Wildlife Society Bulletin 38: Loiselle, BA, JG Blake, R Duraes, TB Ryder & WP Tori (2007a) 710–720. Environmental and spatial segregation of leks among six del Hoyo, J, A Elliott & DA Christie (2004) Handbook of the birds co‐occuring species of manakins (Aves: Pipridae) in eastern of the world. Volume 9: Cotingas to pipits and wagtails. Ecuador. The Auk 124: 420–431. Lynx Edicions, Barcelona, Spain. Loiselle, BA, PG Blendinger, JG Blake & TB Ryder (2007b) Eco‐ Doucet, SM, DB McDonald, MS Foster & RP Clay (2007) Plum‐ logical redundancy in seed dispersal systems: a comparison age development and molt in Long‐tailed Manakins (Chi‐ between manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Pp 178–195 in Dennis, roxiphia linearis) variation according to sex and age. The AJ, EW Schupp, R Green & D Westcott (eds). Seed dispersal: Auk 124: 29–43. theory and its application in a changing world. CAB Inter‐ Durães, R, BA Loiselle & JG Blake (2007) Intersexual spatial rela‐ national, Wallingford, UK. tionships in a lekking species: Blue‐crowned Manakins and MacArthur, R & R Levins (1964) Competition, habitat selection female hot spots. Behavioral Ecology 18: 1029–1039. and character displacement in a patchy environment. Pro‐ Durães, R (2009) Lek structure and male display repertoire of ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 51: Blue‐crowned Manakins in eastern Ecuador. The Condor 1207–1210. 111: 453–461. McDonald, DB (1993) Delayed plumage maturation and orderly DuVal, EH (2005) Age‐based plumage changes in the Lance‐ queues for status: a manakin mannequin experiment. tailed Manakin: a two‐year delay in plumage maturation. Ethology 94: 31–45. The Condor 107: 915–920. Naef‐Denzer, B (1994) Radiotracking of Great and Blue Tits: DuVal, EH & B Kempenaers (2008) Sexual selection in a lekking new tools to assess territoriality, home‐range use and bird: the relative opportunity for selection by female resource distribution. Ardea 82: 335–347. choice and male competition. Proceedings of the Royal Pyke, GH (1984) Optimal foraging theory: a critical review. Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 275: Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15: 523–575. 1995–2003. Rappole, J & A Tipton (1991) New harness design for attach‐ Fair, JM, TB Ryder, BA Loiselle, JG Blake, TE Larson, P Davis, J ment of radio transmitters to small passerines. Journal of Syme, GB Perkins & JM Heikoop (2013) Estimates of dietary Field Ornithology 62: 335–337. overlap for six species of Amazonian manakin birds using Ryder, TB & R Durães (2005) It’s not easy being green: using stable isotopes. Isotopes in Environmental and Health molt and morphological criteria to age and sex green‐ Studies 49: 420–435. plumage manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Ornitología Neotropi‐ Fiske, P, PT Rintamaki & E Karvonen (1998) Mating success in cal 16: 481–491. lekking species: a meta‐analysis. Behavioral Ecology 9: Ryder, TB, JG Blake & BA Loiselle (2006) A test of the hotspot 328–338. hypothesis for three species of manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Foster, MS (1987) Delayed plumage maturation, neoteny, and The Auk 123: 247–258. social system differences in two manakins of the Chi‐ Ryder, TB, DB McDonald, JG Blake, PG Parker & BA Loiselle roxiphia. Evolution 41: 547–558. (2008) Social networks in the lek‐mating Wire‐tailed Griffiths, R, MC Double, K Orr & JG Dawson (1998) A DNA Manakin (Pipra filicauda). Proceedings of the Royal Society test to sex most birds. Molecular Ecology 7: 1071–1075. of London Series B, Biological Sciences 275: 1367–1374. Hansbauer, MM, I Storch, RG Pimentel & JP Metzger (2008) Ryder, TB & JD Wolfe (2009) The current state of knowledge on Comparative range use by three Atlantic Forest understory molt and plumage sequences in selected Neotropical bird bird species in relation to forest fragmentation. Journal of families: a review. Ornitología Neotropical 20: 1–18. Tropical Ecology 24: 291–299. Schmutz, JA & GC White (1990) Error in telemetry studies: Jefferies, JM & JH Lawton (1984) Enemy free space and the effects of animal movement on triangulation. The Journal structure of ecological communities. The Biological Journal of Wildlife Management 54: 506–510. of the Linnean Society 23: 263–286. Seaman, DE, JJ Millspaugh, BJ Kernohan, GC Brundige, KJ Rae‐ Karubian, J, R Durães, JL Storey & TB Smith (2012) Mating be‐ deke & RA Gitzen (1999) Effects of sample size on kernel havior drives seed dispersal by the Long‐wattled Umbrella‐ home range estimates. The Journal of Wildlife Manage‐ bird Cephalopterus penduliger. Biotropica 44: 689–698. ment 63: 739–747.

153 SPACE USE OF WHITE‐CROWNED MANAKIN

Suhonen, J, K Norrdahl & E Koepimäki (1994) Avian predation seed dispersal of a tropical frugivore. Oecologia 122: risk modifies breeding bird community on a farmland area. 249–257. Ecology 75: 1626–1634. Wheelwright, NT, WA Haber, KG Murray & C Guindon (1984) Théry, M (1992) The evolution of leks through female choice: Tropical fruit‐eating birds and their food plants: a survey of differential clustering and space utilization in six sympatric a Costa Rican lower montane forest. Biotropica 16: manakins. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30: 173–192. 227–237. Willson, MF (1993) Dispersal mode, seed shadows, and coloni‐ Tori, WP (2008) Sexual selection in a species with exploded zation patterns. Vegetatio 108: 261–280. leks: the White‐crowned Manakin (Pipra pipra). Ph.D. diss., Worthington, A (1982) Population size and breeding rhythms of Univ. of Missouri, St. Louis, Missouri, USA. two species of manakins in relation to food supply. Pp Trakhtenbrot, A, R Nathan, G Perry & D Richardson (2005) The 213–225 in Leigh, EG, AS Rand & DM Windsor (eds). The importance of long‐distance dispersal in biodiversity con‐ ecology of a tropical forest: seasonal rhythms and long‐ servation. Diversity and Distributions 11: 173–181. term changes. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, Wenny, DG (2001) Advantages of seed dispersal: a re‐evalua‐ D.C., USA. tion of directed dispersal. Evolutionary Ecology Research 3: Worthington, AH (1989) Adaptations for avian frugivory: assim‐ 51–74. ilation efficiency and gut transit time of Manacus vitellinus Westcott, DA (1994) Leks of leks: a role for hotspots in lek evo‐ and Pipra mentalis. Oecologia 80: 381–389. lution? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Worton, BJ (1995) Using Monte Carlo simulations to evaluate Biological Sciences 258: 281–286. kernel‐based home range estimators. The Journal of Wild‐ Westcott, DA & DL Graham (2000) Patterns of movement and life Management 59: 794–800.

154