Surprise As Vicarious Prediction Error

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Surprise As Vicarious Prediction Error Running head: SURPRISE AS VICARIOUS PREDICTION ERROR Surprisingly unsurprising! Infants’ looking time to improbable events is modulated by others’ expressions of surprise In preparation Yang Wu Hyowon Gweon Department of Psychology, Stanford University Keywords: cognitive development; social cognition; surprise; prediction error; emotion, statistical inference; looking time Address for correspondence: Corresponding Author Name: Yang Wu Department of Psychology Stanford University 450 Serra Mall, Jordan Hall 280 Stanford, CA 94305 E-mail: [email protected] 1 Abstract Research in diverse disciplines suggests that agents’ own prediction error enhances their learning. Yet, human learners also possess powerful capacities to learn from others. Here we ask whether infants can use others’ expressions of surprise as vicarious prediction error signals to infer hidden states of the world. First, we conceptually replicated Xu & Garcia (2008), showing that infants (12.0-17.9 months) looked longer at improbable than probable sampling outcomes (Experiment 1). Then we added emotional cues to the design (Experiment 2). Before revealing an outcome to an infant, the experimenter looked at the outcome and showed either an unsurprised or surprised emotional expression. While infants still looked longer at the improbable than the probable outcome following the experimenter’s unsurprised emotional expression, this trend was reversed following the experimenter’s surprised expression. Thus, although for decades infants’ looking time has been assumed to be longer for events that are unexpected or surprising, these results suggest that infants’ inferential abilities in social contexts are powerful enough to make surprising events “surprisingly unsurprising.” 2 Human learners accomplish remarkable achievements in their first years of life – understanding minds, numbers, and languages, to name a few (Carey, 2009; Gopnik, 2012). How children learn has inspired research in diverse scientific domains, and has been intensively studied for several decades. One challenge for learning, noted by this literature, is that our learning environment is often complex, dynamic and full of information. To make learning efficient, or even possible at all, learners must understand when to attend to some information, when to ignore it, and from whom to learn. One of the most powerful proposals for how learning occurs states that agents identify opportunities for learning based on prediction error (e.g., Kulkarni, Narasimhan, Saeedi, & Tenenbaum, 2016; Schultz, 2016; Silver et al., 2018). When there is a discrepancy between what an agent expects to happen and the actual outcome, the agent uses the discrepancy to modify its behavior. The neural and computational bases of such error-driven learning have been extensively studied in both animal models and human adults (see Schultz, 2016 for a review). This learning mechanism has also been a central notion of theoretical models in various domains, including reinforcement learning, perceptual inference, cognition, and decision-making (see Den Ouden, Kok, & De Lange, 2012). This type of error-driven, surprise-induced learning has also been found in human infants and children. When infants and children observe an event that is inconsistent with their predictions or expectations, they show a range of behavioral, neural and physiological changes. The most well-known behavior is that infants look longer at an event that violates their expectation than an event that does not. Accordingly, infants’ looking time to expected vs. unexpected events has been used in developmental research as a popular dependent measure (i.e., violation-of-expectation paradigm). Infants also show changes in pupil dilation (Jackson & Sirois, 2009), cerebral blood flow (Wilcox, Bortfeld, Woods, Wruck, & Boas, 2005) and brain electrical activity (Berger, Tzur, & Posner, 2006) following unexpected events. Critically, infants and older children also explore more when events are unexpected, and show enhanced 3 learning following surprising events (e.g., Bonawitz, van Schijndel, Friel, & Schulz, 2012; Chandler & Lalonde, 1994; Phelps & Woolley, 1994; Schulz, 2012; Stahl & Feigenson, 2018). Nonetheless, we often miss out on a lot of surprising events. Sometimes we fail to pay attention or lack visual access to those events. Critically, when one lacks the relevant world knowledge, one might fail to find an event surprising even when it is in principle surprising; for instance, if you know little about the weather in Lima (where it almost never rains), you won’t be surprised when it rains in Lima. Young learners, in particular, may often fail to detect surprising events. Despite their sophisticated, early- emerging knowledge of the physical and social world (e.g., core principles of physical objects; Spelke, Breinlinger, Macomber, & Jacobson, 1992), events that violate this knowledge are quite rare (e.g., a car floating in the air), making many potential opportunities for learning go unnoticed. Thus, although surprise-induced learning may be one fundamental mechanism that explains how agents learn broadly, it does not explain some critical aspects of human learning. However, learning does not occur in isolation; young learners benefit from others who already know much about the world (Csibra & Gergely, 2009; Boyd & Richerson, 1988). Here we propose that young children may not only learn from their own prediction error, but also use others’ expressions of surprise as vicarious prediction error signals to identify special opportunities for learning. Such cues are abundant in children’s everyday life: Parents, caregivers, and educators do not only express various emotional expressions when they interact with young children, but also tend to exaggerate them for pedagogical and communicative purposes. In particular, expressions of surprise are often used to attract children’s attention to an interesting (and potentially informative) event even when it may not necessarily be surprising for adults. This proposal is supported by the literature on early emotion understanding. From infancy on, children show remarkably early-developing sensitivity to others’ emotional expressions, and even represent others’ emotions as caused by events in the world and others’ goals, desires, and beliefs (see 4 Reschke, Walle, & Dukes, 2017 and Wellman, 2014 for reviews). Infants and children can also perform backward inferences, using observed emotional expressions to reason backward about their unobserved causes in the environment (Wu, Muentener, & Schulz, 2017) and hidden mental states of other people (Wu & Schulz, 2018). These studies suggest that even young children have a sophisticated understanding of others’ emotional expressions, and use those emotional expressions as powerful sources of information to guide their understanding of the world. Research on the emotional expression of surprise, however, has primarily studied this emotional expression as an affective consequence of one’s false beliefs (e.g., Hadwin & Perner, 1991; Ruffman & Keenan, 1996; Harris, 1989; Wellman & Banerjee, 1991; Scott, 2017). Additionally, a recent study found that 7-year-olds expect others to be surprised in response to a low-probability event (Doan, Friedman, & Denison, 2018). This work provides additional support that young children can represent others’ emotions as jointly caused by their internal mental states and external world states. However, a critical question about the relevance of surprise for learning remains open: Do young children use others’ expressions of surprise as signals of prediction error and use them to guide their own inferences about the world? To answer this question, we ask whether infants (12- to 17-month-olds) can use others’ expressions of surprise to form some expectations about an unobserved outcome. When an agent looks surprised after seeing the outcome of an event (but the outcome is not yet visible to the infant), it provides important information about the nature of the hidden outcome: it is unexpected. Thus, infants would therefore expect an unexpected outcome and look longer when the outcome is expected than unexpected. By contrast, if the agent looks unsurprised, this suggests that the hidden outcome is consistent with what she expected, leading to the usual pattern of looking time (i.e., longer when the outcome is unexpected than when it is expected; Aslin, 2007). Thus, our primary interest here is an interaction effect between the agent’s expression (Surprise vs. No Surprise) and the probability of the outcome (Probable vs. Improbable) in infants’ looking time. 5 We test these predictions using events adapted from a well-established prior study (Xu & Garcia, 2008). This study has shown that 8-month-olds expect randomly drawn samples to be representative of the population; when an agent randomly draws ping-pong balls from a box that contains mostly white and just a few red ones, infants look longer when the sample is mostly red (an improbable outcome) than when it is mostly white (a probable outcome). In our study, we add emotion information to the paradigm; before the agent reveals the outcome to the infant, she first looks at it herself and makes either a surprised or an unsurprised emotional response to it. Then she reveals the outcome to the infant. We predict that while previewing the agent’s unsurprised expression would result in a similar pattern of looking time as the original study, observing a surprised emotional expression would reverse the pattern
Recommended publications
  • Expectancy and Musical Emotion Effects of Pitch and Timing
    Manuscript Expectancy and musical emotion Effects of pitch and timing expectancy on musical emotion Sauvé, S. A.1, Sayed, A.1, Dean, R. T.2, Pearce, M. T.1 1Queen Mary, University of London 2Western Sydney University Author Note Correspondence can be addressed to Sarah Sauvé at [email protected] School of Electronic Engineering and Computer Science Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Road London E1 4NS United Kingdom +447733661107 Biographies S Sauve: Originally a pianist, Sarah is now a PhD candidate in the Electronic Engineering and Computer Science department at Queen Mary University of London studying expectancy and stream segregation, supported by a college studentship. EXPECTANCY AND MUSICAL EMOTION 2 A Sayed: Aminah completed her MSc in Computer Science at Queen Mary University of London, specializing in multimedia. R.T. Dean: Roger is a composer/improviser and researcher at the MARCS Institute for Brain, Behaviour and Development. His research focuses on music cognition and music computation, both analytic and generative. M.T. Pearce: Marcus is Senior Lecturer at Queen Mary University of London, director of the Music Cognition and EEG Labs and co-director of the Centre for Mind in Society. His research interests cover computational, psychological and neuroscientific aspects of music cognition, with a particular focus on dynamic, predictive processing of melodic, rhythmic and harmonic structure, and its impact on emotional and aesthetic experience. He is the author of the IDyOM model of auditory expectation based on statistical learning and probabilistic prediction. EXPECTANCY AND MUSICAL EMOTION 3 Abstract Pitch and timing information work hand in hand to create a coherent piece of music; but what happens when this information goes against the norm? Relationships between musical expectancy and emotional responses were investigated in a study conducted with 40 participants: 20 musicians and 20 non-musicians.
    [Show full text]
  • Probabilistic Models of Expectation Violation Predict Psychophysiological Emotional Responses to Live Concert Music
    Probabilistic models of expectation violation predict psychophysiological emotional responses to live concert music Hauke Egermann, Marcus T. Pearce, Geraint A. Wiggins & Stephen McAdams Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience ISSN 1530-7026 Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci DOI 10.3758/s13415-013-0161-y 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Psychonomic Society, Inc.. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be self-archived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com”. 1 23 Author's personal copy Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci DOI 10.3758/s13415-013-0161-y Probabilistic models of expectation violation predict psychophysiological emotional responses to live concert music Hauke Egermann & Marcus T. Pearce & Geraint A. Wiggins & Stephen McAdams # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2013 Abstract We present the results of a study testing the often- emotion induction, leading to a further understanding of the theorized role of musical expectations in inducing listeners’ frequently experienced emotional effects of music. emotions in a live flute concert experiment with 50 participants. Using an audience response system developed for this purpose, Keywords Emotion .
    [Show full text]
  • Surprise: a Belief Or an Emotion? in V.S
    Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This chapter was originally published in the book Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 202 published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who know you, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial From: Barbara Mellers, Katrina Fincher, Caitlin Drummond and Michelle Bigony, Surprise: A belief or an emotion? In V.S. Chandrasekhar Pammi and Narayanan Srinivasan, editors: Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 202, Amsterdam: The Netherlands, 2013, pp. 3-19. ISBN: 978-0-444-62604-2 © Copyright 2013 Elsevier B.V. Elsevier Author's personal copy CHAPTER Surprise: A belief or an emotion? 1 Barbara Mellers1, Katrina Fincher, Caitlin Drummond, Michelle Bigony Department of Psychology, Solomon Labs, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA 1Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1-215-7466540, Fax: þ1-215-8987301, e-mail address: [email protected] Abstract Surprise is a fundamental link between cognition and emotion. It is shaped by cognitive as- sessments of likelihood, intuition, and superstition, and it in turn shapes hedonic experiences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Pygmalion Effect: Proving Them Right
    The Pygmalion Effect: Proving Them Right getpocket.com/explore/item/the-pygmalion-effect-proving-them-right The Pygmalion Effect is a powerful secret weapon. Without even realizing it, we can nudge others towards success. In this article, discover how expectations can influence performance for better or worse. How Expectations Influence Performance Many people believe that their pets or children are of unusual intelligence or can understand everything they say. Some people have stories of abnormal feats. In the late 19th century, one man claimed that about his horse and appeared to have evidence. William Von Osten was a teacher and horse trainer. He believed that animals could learn to read or count. Von Osten’s initial attempts with dogs and a bear were unsuccessful, but when he began working with an unusual horse, he changed our understanding of psychology. Known as Clever Hans, the animal could answer questions, with 90% accuracy, by tapping his hoof. He could add, subtract, multiply, divide, and tell the time and the date. Clever Hans could also read and understand questions written or asked in German. Crowds flocked to see the horse, and the scientific community soon grew interested. Researchers 1/7 studied the horse, looking for signs of trickery. Yet they found none. The horse could answer questions asked by anyone, even if Von Osten was absent. This indicated that no signaling was at play. For a while, the world believed the horse was truly clever. Then psychologist Oskar Pfungst turned his attention to Clever Hans. Assisted by a team of researchers, he uncovered two anomalies.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Musical Anticipation: from the Time of Music to the Music of Time
    UC San Diego UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Modeling musical anticipation : from the time of music to the music of time Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4nv162ms Author Cont, Arshia Publication Date 2008 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Modeling Musical Anticipation: From the time of music to the music of time A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Music by Arshia Cont Committee in charge: Shlomo Dubnov, Chair Alain de Cheveigné Philippe Manoury Miller Puckette Lawrence Saul David Wessel 2008 Copyright Arshia Cont, 2008 All rights reserved. The dissertation of Arshia Cont is approved, and it is acceptable in quality and form for publication on microfilm and electronically: Chair University of California, San Diego 2008 iii DEDICATION !"#$%&'!()*+,-.&'!() /0123456!78. 9: CD JKL $ 3;<=>?@6AB 5EFGHI MN Dedicated to ones who dedicate... my parents Mansoureh Daneshkazemi and Gholamhossen Cont iv ! EPIGRAPH “I think that the search for a universal answer to the questions raised by musical experi- ence will never be completely fulfilled; but we know that a question raised is often more significant than the answer received. Only a reckless spirit, today, would try to give a total explanation of music, but anyone who would never pose the problem is even more reckless.” Remembering the future LUCIANO BERIO v TABLE OF CONTENTS Signature Page............................................ iii Dedication.............................................. iv Epigraph...............................................v Table of Contents.......................................... vi List of Figures............................................ xi List of Tables............................................ xiii Acknowledgements.......................................
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER 4  Anomie/Strainanomie/Strain Theorytheory Distribute
    CHAPTER 4 Anomie/StrainAnomie/Strain TheoryTheory distribute In April 1992, a young man from a well-to-do East Coast family hitchhiked to Alaska and walked alone into the wilderness north of Mt. McKinley. Four months later his decomposed bodyor was found by a party of moose hunters. His name turned out to be Christopher Johnson McCandless. He’d grown up, I learned, in an affluent suburb of Washington, D.C., where he’d excelled academically and had been an elite athlete. Immediately after graduation, with honors, from Emory University in the summer of 1990, McCandless dropped out of sight. He changed his name, gave the entire balance of a twenty-four-thousand-dollar savings account to charity, abandoned his car and most of his possessions, burned all the cash in his wallet. And then he invented a new life for himself, taking up residencepost, at the ragged margin of society, wandering across North America in search of raw, transcendent experience. His family had no idea where he was or what had become of him until his remains turned up in Alaska. — Jon Krakauer (1996), Into the Wild. Copyright 1996 by Jon Krakauer. Published by Anchor Books, a division of Random House, Inc. copy, Introduction Christopher McCandless grew up in a conforming, upper-middle-class family and seemed to be on the fast track to success. He graduated from Emory University with a 3.72 grade point average, and he spoke of going to law school. Instead, he turnednot his back on his family, adopted the new name of Alexander Supertramp, and set out to make his way alone in the wilderness.
    [Show full text]
  • The Epistemic Impact of the Etiology of Experience
    The Epistemic Impact of the Etiology of Experience The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Siegel, Susanna. 2013. The epistemic impact of the etiology of experience. Philosophical Studies 162(3): 697-722. Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:5141367 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Open Access Policy Articles, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#OAP The Epistemic Impact of the Etiology of Experience* Susanna Siegel forthcoming in Philosophical Studies as a symposium Down in the basement of the mind, unconscious processes unfold that give rise to conscious experience. In this paper I argue that those processes can impact the rational role of experience. Suppose you want to know whether there’s any mustard in the fridge. You open the door to look. There’s the mustard, in a half-empty jar, sitting in the fridge door. So far, looking in the fridge has given you excellent reason to believe that there’s mustard in the fridge. Having a reason to believe is a generic notion, tied to what is epistemically appropriate to believe. Sticking with this generic normative notion, we can say that your perceptual experience provides justification, or equivalently, rational or evidential support for believing p.1 The very grammar of “perceptual experiences provide justification” can seem to presuppose that experiences do this all by themselves, or at least that they suffice to do so, absent defeaters.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotions and Decision Making, P. 1
    Emotions and Decision Making, p. 1 Emotion and Decision Making Jennifer S. Lerner Harvard University Ye Li University of California, Riverside Piercarlo Valdesolo Claremont McKenna College Karim Kassam Carnegie Mellon University Draft Date: 16 June 2014 Manuscript submitted for publication in the Annual Review of Psychology Emotions and Decision Making, p. 2 Contents I. Introduction ................................................................................................................................. 3 II. Emotional Impact on Judgment and Decision Making: Eight Major Themes ........................... 5 Theme 1: Integral emotions influence decision making ............................................................. 5 Theme 2: Incidental emotions influence decision making .......................................................... 7 Theme 3: Valence is not the only way that emotions influence decision making ...................... 9 Theme 4: Emotions shape decisions via the content of thought ............................................... 12 Theme 5: Emotions shape the depth of thought ........................................................................ 16 Theme 6: Emotions shape decisions via goal activation ........................................................... 18 Theme 7: Emotions influence interpersonal decision making .................................................. 20 Theme 8: Unwanted effects of emotion on decision making can sometimes be reduced ......... 23 III. General Model .......................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Expectation and Anticipation As Key Elements for the Constitution of Meaning in Music
    teorema Vol. XXXI/3, 2012, pp. 149-163 ISSN: 0210-1602 [BIBLID 0210-1602 (2012) 31:3; pp. 149-163] Expectation and Anticipation As Key Elements for the Constitution of Meaning in Music Elisa Negretto RESUMEN Utilizando un enfoque multidisciplinar que combina la fenomenología, la musico- logía y la psicología cognitiva de la música, abordo en el presente artículo los aspectos siguientes: ¿de qué manera los oyentes reconocen en su propia experiencia perceptiva musical un significado especial?, y, ¿cuáles son los aspectos principales que determinan el significado subjetivo que una experiencia musical adquiere en un contexto y en una situación específicos? Centrándome en la manera en que los oyentes perciben la música, mi objetivo principal es encontrar los elementos clave que influyen en la creación de significado de las experiencias musicales cotidianas. En particular, analizo un proceso cognitivo especialmente importante para la construcción del significado durante el desa- rrollo de un acto perceptivo: el proceso de la expectación. De este modo, propongo una distinción conceptual entre “expectativa” y “anticipación”, argumentando que una y otra influyen de manera diferente en la experiencia perceptiva de la música y, por tanto, en el significado que ésta adquiere para cada individuo. PALABRAS CLAVE: expectación, anticipación, constitución, significado, experiencia perceptiva. ABSTRACT Through an interdisciplinary approach involving phenomenology, musicology and cognitive psychology of music, this paper examines the following questions: how do listeners become aware of their musical perceptual experience as having a specific meaning? And, what are the main aspects constituting, within a particular context and a set of circumstances, the subjective meaning of a musical experience? Focusing on the way listeners perceive music, this paper aims to find the key elements that influ- ence how meaning is shaped in everyday musical experience.
    [Show full text]
  • Preschoolers' Developing Ability to Reason About Others' Emotions
    Developmental Psychology © 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 55, No. 8, 1680–1693 0012-1649/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000749 Integrating Expectations and Outcomes: Preschoolers’ Developing Ability to Reason About Others’ Emotions Mika Asaba Desmond C. Ong STAR Artificial IntelligenceءStanford University Stanford University and A Initiative, Agency for Science, Technology and Research, Singapore Hyowon Gweon Stanford University People’s emotional experiences depend not only on what actually happened, but also on what they thought would happen. However, these expectations about future outcomes are not always communicated explicitly. Thus, the ability to infer others’ expectations in context and understand how these expectations influence others’ emotions is an important aspect of our social intelligence. Prior work suggests that an abstract understanding of how expectations modulate emotional responses may not emerge until 7 to 8 years of age. Using a novel paradigm that capitalizes on intuitive physics to generate contextually plausible expectations, we present evidence for expectation-based emotion inference in preschool-aged children. Given two bowlers who experienced identical final outcomes (hitting 3 of 6 pins), we varied the trajectory of their balls such that one would initially expect to hit all pins (high-expectation), while the other would expect to hit none (low-expectation). In Experiment 1, both 4- and 5-year-olds appropriately adjusted characters’ happiness ratings upward (low-expectation) or downward (high-expectation) relative to their initial emotions; however, only 5-year-olds made adjustments robust enough to manifest as higher final ratings for the low-expectation than the high-expectation character. In Experiments 2–3, we replicate these results and show that 5-year-olds reliably differentiate the characters’ emotions even when their expectations must be inferred from context.
    [Show full text]
  • Surprise As Shortcut for Anticipation: Clustering Mental States in Reasoning ∗
    Surprise as Shortcut for Anticipation: Clustering Mental States in Reasoning ∗ Michele Piunti, Cristiano Castelfranchi and Rino Falcone Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies - CNR {michele.piunti, c.castelfranchi, r.falcone}@istc.cnr.it Abstract level in goal deliberation, planning, intention reconsideration, learning and action control. To enhance effectiveness in real world applications, In addition, expectations have a foundational role in emo- autonomous agents have to develop cognitive com- tion life-cycle and expectation enabled agents are lean- petencies and anticipatory capabilities. Here we ing to be ’surprised’ according to a human-like behavioral point out their strong liaison with the functional metaphor. Defining surprise as a function of the experienced roles of affective mental states as those of human- mismatch between what is expected and the perceived data (at like metaphor: not only the root elements for both a given level of representation), expectations become ”prereq- surprise and anticipation are expectations, but also uisites” for surprise, thus different kinds of expectations holds part of the effects of the former elicit efforts on the to different kinds of surprise. Here we point out that sources latter. By analyzing different kinds of expectations, of surprise (generally speaking, the ”unexpected” signals) can we provide a general architecture enhancing prac- have either negative or positive consequences on purposive tical reasoning with mental states, describing and behaviors when they are considered in terms of penalties, empirically evaluating how mental and behavioral costs rather than benefits, advantages. attitudes, emerging from mental states, can be ap- In sections 2 and 3 we give a reformulation of the prob- plied for augmenting agent reactivity, opportunism lem from a cognitive perspective, in terms of Mental States.
    [Show full text]
  • Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: the Free Press Table of Contents in Word
    Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: The Free Press Table of contents in word Merton, Robert K. (1968) Social Theory and Social Structure. New York: The Free Press1 Table of contents in word....................................................................................................1 Note on layout .....................................................................................................................2 SOCIAL THEORY AND SOCIAL STRUCTURE............................................................2 PREFACE TO THE 1968 ENLARGED EDITION ...........................................................3 PREFACE TO THE 1957 REVISED EDITION ................................................................5 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS...................................................................................................6 CONTENTS -AN OVERVIEW..........................................................................................7 Part I ON THEORETIC SOCIAL THEORY AND SOCIAL STRUCTURE..................23 I ON THE HISTORY AND SYSTEMATICS OF SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY........23 II ON SOCIOLOGICAL THEORIES OF THE MIDDLE RANGE ............................59 III MANIFEST AND LATENT FUNCTIONSTOWARD THE CODIFICATION OF FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS IN SOCIOLOGY FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS............92 iv THE BEARING OF SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY ON EMPIRICAL RESEARCH .158 V THE BEARING OF EMPIRICAL RESEARCH ON SOCIOLOGICAL THEORY174 Part II STUDIES IN SOCIAL AND CULTURAL STUCTURE INTRODUCTION....189 VI SOCIAL STRUCTURE AND ANOMIE ..............................................................198
    [Show full text]