Extending Hidden Structure Learning: Features, Opacity, and Exceptions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Extending Hidden Structure Learning: Features, Opacity, and Exceptions University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations Dissertations and Theses November 2016 Extending Hidden Structure Learning: Features, Opacity, and Exceptions Aleksei I. Nazarov University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2 Part of the Computational Linguistics Commons, and the Phonetics and Phonology Commons Recommended Citation Nazarov, Aleksei I., "Extending Hidden Structure Learning: Features, Opacity, and Exceptions" (2016). Doctoral Dissertations. 782. https://doi.org/10.7275/9054817.0 https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2/782 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations and Theses at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EXTENDING HIDDEN STRUCTURE LEARNING: FEATURES, OPACITY, AND EXCEPTIONS A Dissertation Presented By ALEKSEI IOULEVITCH NAZAROV Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY September 2016 Linguistics © Copyright by Aleksei Ioulevitch Nazarov 2016 All Rights Reserved EXTENDING HIDDEN STRUCTURE LEARNING: FEATURES, OPACITY, AND EXCEPTIONS A Dissertation Presented By ALEKSEI IOULEVITCH NAZAROV Approved as to style and content by ___________________________________ Joe Pater, Co-Chair ___________________________________ Gaja Jarosz, Co-Chair ___________________________________ John McCarthy, Member ___________________________________ Kristine Yu, Member ___________________________________ David Smith, Member ___________________________________ John Kingston, Department Head Department of Linguistics DEDICATION For my parents, Elena and Yuli. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS First and foremost, I would like to thank my dissertation co-chairs, Gaja Jarosz and Joe Pater, for their patience, guidance, and support in the process of writing this dissertation. I am very grateful to Gaja for being willing to take the responsibility of becoming my co-chair in her first year of joining the UMass faculty, and my last year as a UMass graduate student. I would like to thank her for her enormous encouragement as well as her constructive criticism, and for opening my mind onto new perspectives and teaching me how to better articulate my thoughts. Without the many things that I learned from her in the course of this one year, this dissertation would not have taken the course it has taken now. I am especially thankful to Joe for helping me realize that I wanted to do computational phonology by encouraging me to explore this direction since my first year of graduate school. I am also very thankful to him as a teacher: the thoughts and ideas that came out of his courses will always stay with me; he has taught me to always be curious for the road not taken. His flexibility and guidance in the process of shaping this dissertation have been invaluable. Without exaggeration, I can say that without this, this dissertation would not exist at all. I would also like to thank John McCarthy for his patience, wisdom, and sense of humor. Working with him throughout my time as a graduate student has taught me to be infinitely more critical of my own thinking and writing. I will never forget the anecdotes and pieces of wisdom that he shared during meetings and classes, and I thank him for being generous in everything. v Kristine Yu deserves my special thanks for her encouragement and guidance, especially in the project that led to chapter 2 of this dissertation. I am very thankful to her for teaching me be more clear and precise, and for broadening my horizons with literature and perspectives I was completely unaware of before. I would like to thank David Smith for being very supportive and flexible, and for his very valuable comments on various instantiations of the projects explored in this dissertation. I learned many things about matters both computational and linguistic – things that will also help me to keep growing in the future. Brian Dillon and John Kingston have also been very important in the process of developing this dissertation, and I am very grateful for their help and support. Brian Dillon co-advised the project that led to chapter 2 of this dissertation. I would like to thank him for guiding me through my first steps in developing a computational learning project, for his support, for his inspiring ideas, and for all his help with both technical and linguistic questions in this project. In the early stages of exploring dissertation themes, I considered an experimental phonology project, with John Kingston as the prospective chair of the committee. I would like to thank John for his commitment and support, and for his help with experiment design and recording stimuli. I am very grateful for all that I learned from him during this process. I am very grateful to all of the UMass faculty for these 6 years of growth and learning. Apart from those whose name I have already mentioned, I would also like to especially thank Rajesh Bhatt, Lisa Green, Alice Harris, Kyle Johnson, Angelika Kratzer, Tom Roeper, and Ellen Woolford, who I have learned incredibly much from either in class or in person. I would also like to thank David Huber, Alexandra Jesse, Andrew vi McCallum, and Erik Learned-Miller for their courses that exponentiated my knowledge of modeling from various angles and perspectives, and that gave me the tools that make me not ashamed to call myself a computational phonologist. I would like to thank Marc van Oostendorp for being my first advisor at Leiden University. He has always supported and encouraged me to continue my path in linguistics, and I would like to thank him for being a true inspiration, both as a linguist and as a person. I would also like to thank all other linguistics faculty at Leiden University for all that I have learned from them and for their support, in particular, Lisa Cheng, Crit Cremers, and Claartje Levelt. I would like to thank all the wonderful colleagues who have commented on various parts of this work throughout the years, in particular, Adam Albright, Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero, Paul Boersma, Jeroen Breteler, Lisa Davidson, Brian Dillon, David Erschler, Silke Hamann, Ivy Hauser, Coral Hughto, Martin Krämer, John Kingston, John McCarthy, Marc van Oostendorp, Joe Pater, Presley Pizzo, Clàudia Pons-Moll, Olivier Rizzolo, Klaas Seinhorst, Robert Staubs, Peter Svenonius, and Kristine Yu, and audiences at UMass Amherst, the University of Tromsø, Leiden University, the University of Manchester, the LSA Annual Meeting in Washington. I would also like to thank the National Science Foundation for supporting the work that led to chapter 3 of this dissertation through grants BCS-0813829 and BCS- 1424007. For rescuing me time and time again, I am deeply indebted to Kathy Adamczyk, Sarah Vega-Liros, Tom Maxfield, and Michelle McBride. Thank you for keeping our vii department running, thank you for your company and moral support, and thank you for your incredible flexibility and resourcefulness. The UMass linguistics department has been a place of contemplation, ideas, and wonderful connections with people. I would like to thank all my amazing fellow graduate students, and I would especially like to thank my cohort – Tracy Conner, Nick LaCara, and Yangsook Park. I would also like to thank the community of phonologists at UMass for the stimulating discussions and fun get-togethers, especially Ivy Hauser, Coral Hughto, Claire Moore-Cantwell, Presley Pizzo, Brandon Prickett, Brian Smith, and Robert Staubs. I would like to thank David Erschler for being a true friend, and for putting up with my rants about cooking shows and pulp culture. I am also really thankful to Claire Moore-Cantwell for our conversations about life, music and phonology. I would like to thank Rodica Ivan, Claire Moore-Cantwell, Nick LaCara, Robert Staubs, and Katya Vostrikova for being amazing roommates and friends throughout the years. A big thank you also to Caroline Andrews, Thuy Bui, Seda Kan, Stefan Keine, Deniz Özyıldız, and Brian Smith for being kind, supportive, and otherwise wonderful human beings. I am also very grateful for the support I have gotten from my colleagues outside of this department. In particular, I would like to thank Michael Becker, Gašper Beguš, Clàudia Pons-Moll, Kevin Schluter, Xico Torres-Tamarit, Asia Zaleska for being truly supportive and kind, and generally amazing. I would like to thank Mirjam de Jonge not only for being an amazing friend in general, but also for being my longest-standing friend in linguistics – we met during a linguistics course that we took when we were still in secondary school, and we’re both still at it. viii I am especially indebted to all the friends that I was fortunate to make in this area outside of linguistics, in particular to Anthony, Bobby, Charlie, Ilya, Julian, Jordan, Megan, and Zarina. I would not have been able to do this without your support, and you know I mean it. Thank you for all you have taught me throughout these years. All my friends from elsewhere, you know who you are, thank you for always being there for me; and thank you especially to Bernard, Hannah, Ilia, Inas, Jacco, Jacob, James, Joram, Karima, Lai, Maarten, Nate, and Qimin. I am immensely grateful to Mallorie Chernin and the Amherst College Choral Society for teaching me so much as a human being and as a singer, and to Mark Swanson for giving me the opportunity to be a soloist in his Rodgers and Hammerstein project. I am also very thankful to the Amherst College Russian Club, and Maria, Nina, and Evgenia for organizing our weekly tea with music and singing. It has truly been an amazing time. My parents deserve much more gratitude than this page can contain. To my father, Yuli, I am incredibly thankful for teaching me how to think, to be critical, and to persevere; for always supporting me in my decision to go to the US, and for giving me advice from academic to academic.
Recommended publications
  • Opacity and Cyclicity
    Opacity and Cyclicity Paul Kiparsky Stanford University Phonological opacity and paradigmatic effects (“synchronic analogy”) have long been of interest in relation to change, naturalness, and the phonology/morphology interface. Their investigation has now acquired a new urgency, because they call into question OT’s postulate that constraints are evaluated in parallel. Conceptu- ally, parallelism is one of the basic and most interesting tenets of OT, and so there are good methodological reasons to try hard to save it in the face of such recal- citrant data. The price to be paid for it is the introduction of otherwise unneeded powerful new types of Faithfulness constraints, such as Output/Output (O/O) con- straints, Paradigm Uniformity constraints, and Sympathy constraints, which have turned out to compromise the OT program very severely. The alternative to this approach is to abandon full parallelism in favor of strat- ified constraint systems. This has the compensating advantage of maintaining a restrictive and well-defined constraint inventory, as originally envisaged in OT. More importantly, it achieves some genuine explanations by relating the strat- ification motivated by opacity and cyclicity to the intrinsic morphological and prosodic constituency of words and phrases, as characterized by the Stem, Word, and Postlexical levels of Lexical Phonology and Morphology (Booij 1996; 1997; Orgun 1996; Bermudez-Otero 1999). I shall refer to this approach as LPM-OT, and outline how it offers a superior account of the benchmark data that Kager 1999 discusses in Ch. 6 of his book.1 LPM-OT’s goal is to reduce cyclicity to I/O faithfulness, and opacity to inter- level constraint masking.
    [Show full text]
  • Transparency in Language a Typological Study
    Transparency in language A typological study Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6111 Trans 10 3512 JK Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover illustration © 2011: Sanne Leufkens – image from the performance ‘Celebration’ ISBN: 978-94-6093-162-8 NUR 616 Copyright © 2015: Sterre Leufkens. All rights reserved. Transparency in language A typological study ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit van Amsterdam op gezag van de Rector Magnificus prof. dr. D.C. van den Boom ten overstaan van een door het college voor promoties ingestelde commissie, in het openbaar te verdedigen in de Agnietenkapel op vrijdag 23 januari 2015, te 10.00 uur door Sterre Cécile Leufkens geboren te Delft Promotiecommissie Promotor: Prof. dr. P.C. Hengeveld Copromotor: Dr. N.S.H. Smith Overige leden: Prof. dr. E.O. Aboh Dr. J. Audring Prof. dr. Ö. Dahl Prof. dr. M.E. Keizer Prof. dr. F.P. Weerman Faculteit der Geesteswetenschappen i Acknowledgments When I speak about my PhD project, it appears to cover a time-span of four years, in which I performed a number of actions that resulted in this book. In fact, the limits of the project are not so clear. It started when I first heard about linguistics, and it will end when we all stop thinking about transparency, which hopefully will not be the case any time soon. Moreover, even though I might have spent most time and effort to ‘complete’ this project, it is definitely not just my work. Many people have contributed directly or indirectly, by thinking about transparency, or thinking about me.
    [Show full text]
  • Observations on the Phonetic Realization of Opaque Schwa in Southern French
    http://dx.doi.org/10.17959/sppm.2015.21.3.457 457 Observations on the phonetic realization of opaque * schwa in Southern French Julien Eychenne (Hankuk University of Foreign Studies) Eychenne, Julien. 2015. Observations on the phonetic realization of opaque schwa in Southern French. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology 21.3. 457-494. This paper discusses a little-known case of opacity found in some southern varieties of French, where the vowel /ə/, which is footed in the dependent syllable of a trochee, is usually realized as [ø], like the full vowel /Œ/. This case of opacity is particularly noteworthy because the opaque generalization is suprasegmental, not segmental. I show that, depending on how the phenomenon is analyzed derivationally, it simultaneously displays symptoms of counterbleeding and counterfeeding opacity. A phonetic analysis of data from one representative speaker is carried out, and it is shown that the neutralization between /ə/ and /Œ/ is complete in this idiolect. Implications for the structure of lexical representations and for models of phonology are discussed in light of these results. (Hankuk University of Foreign Studies) Keywords: schwa, Southern French, opacity 1. Introduction Opacity is one of the most fundamental issues in generative phonological theory. The traditional view of opacity, due to Kiparsky (1971, 1973) and framed within the derivational framework of The Sound Pattern of English (henceforth SPE, Chomsky and Halle 1968), goes as follows: (1) Opacity (Kiparsky 1973: 79) A phonological rule P of the form A → B / C__D is opaque if there are * An early version of this work was presented at the Linguistics Colloquium of the English Linguistics Department, Graduate School at Hankuk University of Foreign Studies.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling the Acquisition of Phonological Interactions: Biases and Generalization
    Modeling the Acquisition of Phonological Interactions: Biases and Generalization Brandon Prickett and Gaja Jarosz1 University of Massachusetts Amherst 1 Introduction Phonological processes in a language have the potential to interact with one another in numerous ways (Kiparsky 1968, 1971). Two relatively common interaction types are bleeding and feeding interactions, with (1) showing hypothetical examples of both (for a similar hypothetical example, see Baković 2011). (1) Example of Bleeding and Feeding Interactions Bleeding Feeding Underlying Representation (UR) /esi/ /ise/ [-Low] → [αHigh] / [αHigh]C_ (Vowel Harmony) ese isi [s] → [ʃ] / _[+High] (Palatalization) - iʃi Surface Representation (SR) [ese] [iʃi] In the bleeding interaction above, the underlying form /esi/ undergoes harmony, since the /e/ and /i/ have different values for the feature [High]. Since the harmony is progressive, the /i/ assimilates to the /e/ and can no longer trigger the palatalization process (which occurs after harmony), resulting in a surface form of [ese]. The feeding interaction has the opposite effect, with an underlying /e/ becoming a high vowel due to harmony and then triggering the palatalization process to create the SR [iʃi]. The crucial difference between the two interaction types in this example is whether the UR undergoes lowering or raising due to the harmony process. Two other interactions can be produced using these same URs by applying the two rules in the opposite order. These are called Counterbleeding and Counterfeeding interactions and are shown in (2). (2) Example of Counterbleeding and Counterfeeding Interactions Counterbleeding Counterfeeding UR /esi/ /ise/ Palatalization eʃi - Vowel Harmony eʃe isi SR [eʃe] [isi] In the counterbleeding interaction, both palatalization and harmony are able to change the underlying representation, but the motivation for palatalizing (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi*
    This is a printout of the final PDF file and has been approved by me, the author. Any mistakes in this printout will not be fixed by the publisher. Here is my signature and the date 06/10/2018 Speakers Treat Transparent and Opaque Alternation Patterns Differently — Evidence from Chinese Tone Sandhi* Jie Zhang 1. Introduction 1.1 Opacity The study of opacity has had a long tradition in generative phonology. Kiparsky (1973: p.79) defined two types of opacity for a phonological rule, as in (1). The first type is also known as underapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-true; the second type is also known as overapplication opacity, and the opaque rule is non-surface-apparent (e.g., Bakovic 2007). (1) Opacity: A phonological rule P of the form A → B / C __ D is opaque if there are surface structures with any of the following characteristics: (a) Instance of A in the environment C __ D; (b) Instance of B derived by P in environments other than C __ D. In the rule-based framework, opacity can generally be derived by ordering the opaque rule before another rule that could either set up or destroy the application of the opaque rule (but see Bakovic 2007, 2011, who advocates the decoupling of opacity and rule ordering). In a surface-oriented theory like Optimality Theory (OT; Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004), however, opacity poses significant challenges. Without modification to the theory, neither underapplication nor overapplication opacity can be straightforwardly derived. A number of general solutions for opacity have been proposed within OT, including the Sympathy Theory (McCarthy 1999), Stratal OT (Kiparsky 2000, Nazarov and Pater 2017), OT with Candidate Chains (OT-CC; McCarthy 2007), and Serial Markedness Reduction (Jarosz 2014, 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • The Acquisition of Phonological Opacity
    The acquisition of phonological opacity Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero University of Newcastle upon Tyne Abstract. This paper argues that Stratal OT is explanatorily superior to alternative OT treatments of phonological opacity (notably, Sympathy Theory). It shows that Stratal OT supports a learning model that accounts for the acquisition of opaque grammars with a minimum of machinery. The model is illustrated with a case study of the classical counterbleeding interaction between Diphthong Raising and Flapping in Canadian English. 1. Phonological opacity: Stratal OT vs Sympathy Theory Following the appearance of Prince & Smolensky (1993), phonologists were quick to realize that, in its original version, Optimality Theory (OT) was unable to describe a large set of phonological phenomena previously modelled by means of opaque rules. Ten years later, opacity remains the severest challenge confronting OT phonology. The problem is crucial because opacity effects constitute one of the clearest instances of Plato’s Problem (Chomsky 1986) in phonology: learners face the task of acquiring generalizations that are not true on the surface. The ability to explain the acquisition of opaque grammars should accordingly be regarded as one of the main criteria by which generative theories of phonology are to be judged. Among the variants of OT phonology currently on offer, two claim to provide a comprehensive solution to the problem of opacity: Sympathy Theory (McCarthy 1999, 2003) and Stratal OT (Bermúdez-Otero 1999b; Kiparsky 2000, forthcoming). In this paper, I compare the strategies whereby these two phonological models seek to achieve —and indeed transcend— explanatory adequacy: • I shall first consider the formal restrictions that each theory imposes on the complexity of opaque effects (§2).
    [Show full text]
  • Role of Phonological Opacity and Morphological Knowledge in Predicting Reading
    Role of Phonological Opacity and Morphological Knowledge in Predicting Reading Skills in School-Age Children A dissertation presented to the faculty of the College of Health Sciences and Professions of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy Gayatri Ram December 2013 © 2013 Gayatri Ram. All Rights Reserved. 2 This dissertation titled Role of Phonological Opacity and Morphological Knowledge in Predicting Reading Skills in School-Age Children by GAYATRI RAM has been approved for the School of Rehabilitation and Communication Sciences and the College of Health Sciences and Professions by Sally A. Marinellie Associate Professor of Health Sciences and Professions Randy Leite Dean, College of Health Sciences and Professions 3 Abstract RAM, GAYATRI, Ph.D., December 2013, Speech-Language Science Role of Phonological Opacity and Morphological Knowledge in Predicting Reading Skills in School-Age Children Director of Dissertation: Sally A. Marinellie The aim of the present study was to investigate different aspects of morphological knowledge for phonologically transparent and opaque derived words in young school-age children. We were also interested in identifying morphological predictors of decoding and reading comprehension in school-age children. A total of 53 typically developing children in grade 3 participated in the project. All children completed three experimental tasks measuring different aspects of morphological knowledge for both kinds of derived words as well as standardized measures for decoding, passage comprehension and phonological awareness. Results indicated that children performed better on phonologically transparent derived words as compared to opaque derived words on all three morphological knowledge tasks. Further, results of hierarchical regression analyses revealed that both phonological awareness and morphological knowledge accounted for unique variance in reading skills in children.
    [Show full text]
  • Handouts for Advanced Phonology: a Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL
    Handouts for Advanced Phonology: A Course Packet Steve Parker GIAL and SIL International Dallas, 2016 Copyright © 2016 by Steve Parker and other contributors Page 1 of 304 Preface This set of materials is designed to be used as handouts accompanying an advanced course in phonology, particularly at the graduate level. It is specifically intended to be used in conjunction with two textbooks: Phonology in generative grammar (Kenstowicz 1994), and Optimality theory (Kager 1999). However, this course packet could potentially also be adapted for use with other phonology textbooks. The materials included here have been developed by myself and others over many years, in conjunction with courses in phonology taught at SIL programs in North Dakota, Oregon, Dallas, and Norman, OK. Most recently I have used them at GIAL. Many of the special phonetic characters appearing in these materials use IPA fonts available as freeware from the SIL International website. Unless indicated to the contrary on specific individual handouts, all materials used in this packet are the copyright of Steve Parker. These documents are intended primarily for educational use. You may make copies of these works for research or instructional purposes (under fair use guidelines) free of charge and without further permission. However, republication or commercial use of these materials is expressly prohibited without my prior written consent. Steve Parker Graduate Institute of Applied Linguistics Dallas, 2016 Page 2 of 304 1 Table of contents: list of handouts included in this packet Day 1: Distinctive features — their definitions and uses -Pike’s premises for phonological analysis ......................................................................... 7 -Phonemics analysis flow chart ..........................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Overapplication Opacity in Phonological Acquisition
    Overapplication Opacity in Phonological Acquisition Olga Urek CASTL/University of Tromsø Abstract Phonological opacity is a challenge for parallel OT, which does not allow for intermediate levels of representation. Several modifications of the theory have been proposed over the years to incorporate opacity, all of them falling short of accounting for spontaneous opacity effects in developing grammars. In this paper I demonstrate that if certain independently motivated adjustments are made to the recent OT- based theory of opacity called Optimality Theory with Candidate Chains (OT-CC, see McCarthy 2007), it can successfully deal with spontaneous opacity effects. 1. Opacity in Optimality Theory The term ‘phonological opacity’ refers to cases where a surface form of some language L appears to have exceptionally undergone (overapplication or counterbleeding opacity) or failed to undergo (underapplication or counterfeeding opacity) a certain phonological process active in language L. (1) Opacity (Kiparsky 1973) A phonological rule P of the form A → B / C_D is opaque if there are surface structures with either of the following characteristics: a. instances of A in the environment C_D. b. instances of B derived by P that occur in environments other than C_D. Serialist accounts of opacity crucially rely on the existence of some intermediate form that deviates from the input and from the actual output. According to Prince & Smolensky (1993:6), in ‘classic’ Optimality Theory (OT) the “Input à Output map has no internal structure: all possible variants are produced by Gen in one step and evaluated in parallel”. Precisely this property makes opacity a challenging issue for OT. There have been many attempts to incorporate opacity in OT, which can be roughly subdivided into two large (and partly overlapping) groups: those that rely on expanding some basic assumptions about the nature of underlying representations or the constraint component Con and those that make reference to forms distinct from the input and the actual output.
    [Show full text]
  • The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory Current Issues
    This article was downloaded by: 10.3.98.104 On: 26 Sep 2021 Access details: subscription number Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG, UK The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory S. J. Hannahs, Anna R. K. Bosch Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory Publication details https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9781315675428-3 Martin Krämer Published online on: 29 Dec 2017 How to cite :- Martin Krämer. 29 Dec 2017, Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory from: The Routledge Handbook of Phonological Theory Routledge Accessed on: 26 Sep 2021 https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9781315675428-3 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR DOCUMENT Full terms and conditions of use: https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/legal-notices/terms This Document PDF may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproductions, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The publisher shall not be liable for an loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. 3 Current issues and directions in Optimality Theory Constraints and their interaction Martin Krämer 3.1 Introduction The first manuscripts on Optimality Theory (henceforth OT; Prince & Smolensky 1993/2004) were circulated in the early nineties, which is roughly a quarter of a century ago.
    [Show full text]
  • Vowel Harmony, Opacity, and Finite-State OT
    Vowel Harmony, Opacity, and Finite-State OT Samuel R. Bowman The University of Chicago July 22, 2011 Abstract: In this paper I propose cyclic and stratal extensions to Finite- State Optimality Theory as treatments for vowel harmony and for opaque phenomena more generally. I develop a model of grammar and an ac- companying set of constraints within a software model built on the open- source PyPhon toolkit—a computational implementation of OT’s Gen and EVAL algorithms. 0 Constraint-based grammars and typology Since their introduction in the early nineteen-nineties, constraint-based grammars have been widely used in generative phonology for their ability to describe complex patterns in a way that is simple, cognitively plausible, and easily generalized across languages. In any constraint-based grammar, the great bulk of the generative labor is carried out by violable constraints. In an idealized picture, these constraints are clear and principled descriptions of phenomena that are widely dispreferred across languages, like Dep: do not epenthesize segments. The job of the language user is, for every underlying form, to find a surface form that best satisfies the constraints that matter to their language, and to do so directly, without a sequential derivation. Under the associated richness of the base hypothesis (Prince and Smolensky, 1993/2004), constraint-based grammars are expected to be able to produce an ac- ceptable output for any input, even if (as in crosslinguistic borrowing) that input flagrantly violates basic structural principles of the target language. Because of this, it is quite common for analyses to focus on inputs for which no output satisfies all of the constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • Sympathy and Phonological Opacity* John J
    Sympathy and phonological opacity* John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts, Amherst 1 Statement of the problem A central idea in rule-based phonology is the serial derivation (Chomsky and Halle 1968). In a serial derivation, an underlying form passes through a number of intermediate representations on its way to the surface: (1) Serial Derivation Underlying representation = UR UR transformed by rule 1 = output1 Output1 transformed by rule 2 = output2 ... Outputn-1 transformed by rule n = Surface representation Implementational details can differ: the order of rules might be stipulated or it might be derived from universal principles; the steps might be called "rules", "cycles", or "levels"; the steps might involve applying rules or enforcing constraints. But, details aside, the defining characteristic of a serial derivation, in the sense I will employ here, is the preeminence of the chronological metaphor: the underlying form is transformed into a succession of distinct, accessible intermediate representations on its way to the surface. I will call any theory with this property "serialism". *This work was supported by the National Science Foundation under grant SBR-9420424. Versions of it were presented at MIT (4/4/97), the Hopkins Optimality Workshop/Maryland Mayfest ‘97 (5/11/97), the LSA Linguistic Institute (7/22/97), the University of Amsterdam (9/25/97), Ohio State University (10/24/97), UT Austin (11/7/97), and the Rutgers Optimality Research Group (5/15/98). I acknowledge with particular gratitude the comments of Alan Prince;
    [Show full text]