A Case Study of Retrograde Amnesia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Case Study of Retrograde Amnesia BRAIN AND COGNITION 8, 3-20 (1988) Priming of Semantic Autobiographical Knowledge: A Case Study of Retrograde Amnesia ENDEL TULVING, DANIEL L. SCHACTER, DONALD R. MCLACHLAN, AND MORRIS MOSCOVITCH Unit for Memory Disorders, University of Toronto The case of a 36-year-old man who suffers dense retrograde and anterograde amnesia as a result of closed-head injury that caused extensive damage to his left frontal-parietal and right parieto-occipital lobes is described. Patient KC. has normal intelligence and relatively well-preserved perceptual, linguistic, short- term memory, and reasoning abilities. He possesses some fragmentary general knowledge about his autobiographical past, but he does not remember a single personal event or happening from any time of his life. He has some preserved expert knowledge related to the work he did for 3 years before the onset of amnesia, although he has no personal recollections from that period. Some features of K.C.‘s retrograde amnesia can be interpreted in terms of the distinction between episodic and semantic memory, and in terms of the distinction between episodic and semantic autobiographical knowledge. K.C.‘s semantic knowledge, but not his episodic knowledge, showed progressive improvement, or priming, in the course of the investigation. 0 1988 Academic Press. Inc. Much of the current knowledge and understanding of the amnesic syndrome is based on the study of anterograde impairment of memory function in brain-damaged patients. Less evidence is available about retrograde amnesia, that is, about retention of information acquired by the patients before the onset of amnesia. The present study makes a modest contribution to redressing the imbalance. It describes the case This research was supported by a Special Research Program Grant from the Connaught Fund, University of Toronto, and aided by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to Endel Tulving and Daniel Schacter, and by grants from the Medical Research Council of Canada and Ontario Mental Health Foundation to Morris Moscovitch. We thank K.C. for his participation in the study, Rick Julian for referring him to us, Janine Law for experimental and logistical assistance, Donald Stuss for consultation and testing of K.C., and Stephen Gamett for the positron emission tomography and analysis. We are especially grateful to the members of K.C.‘s family, and to his friends, CA., R.A., and J.S. for providing us with the vital intelligence without which the study could not have been undertaken. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Endel Tulving, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S IAI. 3 0278-2626188 $3.00 Copyright 0 1988 by Academic Press, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 4 TLJLVING ET AL. of a patient who, 5 years after an accident that caused massive closed- head injury, exhibits not only dense anterograde amnesia but also a severe impairment of autobiographical memory that covers his whole life. Traditionally, studies of retrograde amnesia have concentrated on the extent, severity, and temporal gradient of the impairment (e.g., Albert, Butters, & Levin, 1979; Butters & Albert, 1982; Marslen-Wilson & Teuber, 1975; Mayer-Gross, 1943; Russell & Nathan, 1946; Squire & Slater, 1975; Warrington & Sanders, 1971; Williams & Zangwill, 1952). More recently, interest has shifted to comparisons and contrasts of impaired and preserved memory functions in retrograde amnesia (e.g., Butters & Cermak, 1986; Cermak & O’Connor, 1983; Damasio, Eslinger, Damasio, Van Hoesen, & Cornell, 1985; Goldberg, 1987; Zola-Morgan, Cohen, & Squire, 1983). The present case study, too, was focused on dissociations in the patient’s premorbidly acquired knowledge. CASE HISTORY K.C. was born in 1951, the first of five children, and delivered during the seventh month of pregnancy. The postnatal period and infancy was uneventful. Developmental milestones were normal. However, at age 18, he received a blow to the head which resulted in admission to the Montreal Neurological Institute and observation for 3 days. He was discharged on phenytoin and phenobarbitol and remained on these anticonvulsants for 1 year. He had no seizures, but did not reenter school for 1 year. At age 24, he was involved in a traffic accident, without loss of con- sciousness, and sustained a fractured mandible. Although painful, neither of the head injuries appeared to have deleterious effects on K.C.‘s in- tellectual functioning. After graduating from high school, he enrolled in a 3-year course in business administration at a community college and graduated at age 25. In the spring of 1978, aged 27 years, he became employed at BE, an engineering and manufacturing company. His job consisted of two activities: driving a delivery and pick-up truck and doing quality control work. The accident responsible for K.C.‘s present condition occurred October 1981, at age 30. Riding his motorcyle home from work, he went off the road. No other vehicles were involved in the accident. He remained unconscious for approximately 72 hr, when a subdural hematoma was removed from the left cranium. He then became stuporous, but responded to commands for about the next 4-5 days. At approximately the seventh post-traumatic day he appeared to recognize his mother. The patient remained in an intensive care unit for 4 weeks. He was transferred to a rehabilitation hospital 6 weeks after the accident and exhibited a severe right hemiplegia. He was discharged home in July 1982, where he has remained in the care of his parents. PRIMING IN RETROGRADE AMNESIA 5 Neurological examination on December 12, 1986, revealed an alert, cooperative individual who responded appropriately to all questions. The most striking feature was a marked apathy, flattening of affect, and general indifference to his surroundings. Significant neurological signs included: bilateral anosmia, a right homonymous hemianopia with macular sparing (corrected visual acuity 52 on the right and 58 on the left) and bilateral optic disc pallor. The eye movements were full in the horizontal and vertical planes and no nystagmus was evoked. The left pupil measured 6 mm and responded poorly to light and accommodation whereas the right pupil measured 4 mm and responded more briskly to light and accommodation. The lower cranial nerves were intact. In the motor system there was ulnar deviation in the right hand, and in the foot lateral deviation of the great toe with marked restriction of movement of the small bones of the foot. The power was intact in upper and lower limbs and there was slight increase in tone at the elbow but no supinator spasticity. His coordination for fine finger movements and finger-nose testing were intact. There was no tremor or voluntary move- ments noted. Sensation was intact to pinprick, cold, light touch, and position sense. There was no sensory inattention elicited and two-point discrimination was equal on both sides of the body. In the finger tips, the threshold for two-point discrimination was approximately 2-3 mm. He was able to discriminate between fine and coarse sandpaper in both hands, and dermographia was intact bilaterally. Right hyperreflexia was present in upper and lower limbs and the plantar response was extensor on the right. The palmomental reflex was more pronounced on the right and the pout and snout reflexes were brisk. He exhibited a hemiplegic gait. Computerized axial tomography, Fig. 1, and magnetic resonance imaging scans of the head were performed. There is mild cerebral and cortical atrophy as evidenced by enlargement of the sulci, ventricles, and cerebellar folia. There is evidence of contusion in the deep white matter of the left frontal-parietal region, and right parieto-occipital region (magnetic res- onance scan). There is an infarct in the distribution of the left posterior cerebral artery and a large defect in the left frontal-parietal region sub- adjacent to the previous craniotomy. Positon emission tomography was performed and this revealed that glucose utilization was essentially normal in the regions of the left hemisphere which did not exhibit damage as revealed by the CAT-scan. K.C. has been observed and tested at our Unit for Memory Disorders since 1983. He participated in a 2-year longitudinal study of memory and cognitive performance of three groups of patients with memory disorders (Schacter, Harbluk, & McLachlan, 1984), as well as in a number of other experiments. For instance, a brief summary description of his state of 6 TULVING ET AL. FIG. 1. Computerized tomographic scan images of Patient K.C. The right side of the brain is to the reader’s left. PRIMING IN RETROGRADE AMNESIA 7 consciousness has been provided in Tulving (1985b), where he is identified by the code initials N.N. Also, under the code initials C.H. he participated in a study demonstrating amnesic patients’ ability to learn new computer- related vocabulary (Glisky, Schacter, & Tulving, 1986b) and computer programming (Glisky, Schacter, & Tulving, 1986a). For the purposes of the present study, K.C. was seen in four weekly sessions beginning in early October 1986, and continuing through January 1987. Each session typically lasted 24 or 3 hr. In addition, on two occasions we visited with K.C. the site of the BE plant where he had worked between 1978 and 1981, visited K.C.‘s present and previous homes, talked to his family, and interviewed three friends, all co-workers at BE at the time that K.C. worked there. Our knowledge of K.C.‘s past life in general and the 3 years at BE in particular is based on information provided by his parents, his brother, and his three friends and co-workers. CURRENT MENTAL STATUS K.C. is a quiet, polite, and cooperative person. He seldom complains about anything. In response to direct questions he says that his life is good, although he concedes that he has problems with his memory.
Recommended publications
  • Semantic Memory - Psychology - Oxford Bibliographies
    1/16/2019 Semantic Memory - Psychology - Oxford Bibliographies Semantic Memory Michael N. Jones, Johnathan Avery LAST MODIFIED: 15 JANUARY 2019 DOI: 10.1093/OBO/9780199828340­0231 Introduction Semantic memory refers to our general world knowledge that encompasses memory for concepts, facts, and the meanings of words and other symbolic units that constitute formal communication systems such as language or math. In the classic hierarchical view of memory, declarative memory was subdivided into two independent modules: episodic memory, which is our autobiographical store of individual events, and semantic memory, which is our general store of abstracted knowledge. However, more recent theoretical accounts have greatly reduced the independence of these two memory systems, and episodic memory is typically viewed as a gateway to semantic memory accessed through the process of abstraction. Modern accounts view semantic memory as deeply rooted in sensorimotor experience, abstracted across many episodic memories to highlight the stable characteristics and mute the idiosyncratic ones. A great deal of research in neuroscience has focused on both how the brain creates semantic memories and what brain regions share the responsibility for storage and retrieval of semantic knowledge. These include many classic experiments that studied the behavior of individuals with brain damage and various types of semantic disorders but also more modern studies that employ neuroimaging techniques to study how the brain creates and stores semantic memories. Classically, semantic memory had been treated as a miscellaneous area of study for anything in declarative memory that was not clearly within the realm of episodic memory, and formal models of meaning in memory did not advance at the pace of models of episodic memory.
    [Show full text]
  • Brainnovations Mobiliser
    Converge. Discover. Deliver Brainnovations Mobiliser. Découvrir. Produire Funding provided, in part, by the Government of Ontario March 2014 - Volume 4 86 billion: that is the number of neurons, or cells, estimated to OBI Founders make up the human brain. Lawrence and Frances Bloomberg - Mount Sinai Hospital Within each cell there is a universe of complexity and activity. Sydney and Florence Cooper But the remarkable abilities of the brain to think, remember, - Baycrest respond, and feel, cannot be explained by just scaling up the Gerald and Geraldine Heffernan individual actions of each cell. - University of Toronto It comes down to the connections. No neuron works in William and Susanne Holland - Holland Bloorview isolation, in fact each cell is thought to make up to 10,000 Richard M. Ivey connections with other cells; the outcome of this - Western University interconnectivity, some 100 trillion connections, is the network Dr. Donald Stuss Robert and Linda Krembil of the human brain. - University Health Network President and Scientific Director Like the brain itself, the Ontario Brain Institute’s strength is its Arthur and Sonia Labatt - The Hospital for Sick Children Ontario Brain Institute ability to create connections. Joseph and Sandra Rotman Learn More - Ontario Brain Institute Lawrence and Judith Tanenbaum - Brain Canada Eli Lilly Canada Inc. Peering Into Dementia GE Healthcare Canada GlaxoSmithKline Inc. IBM Canada Ltd. The eyes are said to be the ‘windows into Medtronic of Canada Ltd. the soul’. Now a novel technique is allowing Nestlé Health Science, Canada researchers to take this romantic thought to a Pfizer Canada Ltd. new level and use the eye as a ‘window into the Valeant Canada LP brain’- to better understand and diagnose brain disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory
    How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory EXPLICIT MEMORY IMPLICIT MEMORY SEMANTIC MEMORY EPISODIC MEMORY EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCEDURAL MEMORY What It Is What It Is What It Is What It Is The memory of general knowledge and The autobiographical memory of an event The memory of the emotions you felt The memory of how to perform a facts. or experience – including the who, what, during an experience. common task without actively thinking and where. Example Example Example Example You remember what a bicycle is. You remember who was there and what When a wave of shame or anxiety grabs You can ride a bicycle automatically, with- street you were on when you fell off your you the next time you see your bicycle out having to stop and recall how it’s bicycle in front of a crowd. after the big fall. done. How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It Trauma can prevent information (like Trauma can shutdown episodic memory After trauma, a person may get triggered Trauma can change patterns of words, images, sounds, etc.) from differ- and fragment the sequence of events. and experience painful emotions, often procedural memory. For example, a ent parts of the brain from combining to without context. person might tense up and unconsciously make a semantic memory. alter their posture, which could lead to pain or even numbness. Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area The temporal lobe and inferior parietal The hippocampus is responsible for The amygdala plays a key role in The striatum is associated with producing cortex collect information from different creating and recalling episodic memory.
    [Show full text]
  • PSYC20006 Notes
    PSYC20006 BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY PSYC20006 1 COGNITIVE THEORIES OF MEMORY Procedural Memory: The storage of skills & procedures, key in motor performance. It involves memory systems that are independent of the hippocampal formation, in particular, the cerebellum, basal ganglia, cortical motor sites. Doesn't involve mesial-temporal function, basal forebrain or diencephalon. Declarative memory: Accumulation of facts/data from learning experiences. • Associated with encoding & maintaining information, which comes from higher systems in the brain that have processed the information • Information is then passed to hippocampal formation, which does the encoding for elaboration & retention. Hippocampus is in charge of structuring our memories in a relational way so everything relating to the same topic is organized within the same network. This is also how memories are retrieved. Activation of 1 piece of information will link up the whole network of related pieces of information. Memories are placed into an already exiting framework, and so memory activation can be independent of the environment. MODELS OF MEMORY Serial models of Memory include the Atkinson-Shiffrin Model, Levels of Processing Model & Tulving’s Model — all suggest that memory is processed in a sequential way. A parallel model of memory, the Parallel Distributed Processing Model, is one which suggests types of memories are processed independently. Atkinson-Shiffrin Model First starts as Sensory Memory (visual / auditory). If nothing is done with it, fades very quickly but if you pay attention to it, it will move into working memory. Working Memory contains both new information & from long-term memory. If it goes through an encoding process, it will be in long-term memory.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae
    CURRICULUM VITAE STEFAN KÖHLER, PH.D. CURRENT ADDRESS The Brain and Mind Institute Western International Research Building University of Western Ontario London, Ontario, Canada N6A 5B7 phone: (519) 661-2111 ext. 86364 email: [email protected] CURRENT AND PAST POSITIONS 2014 – present Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Brain and Mind Institute & Graduate Program in Neuroscience, University of Western Ontario 2006 – 2014 Associate Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Brain and Mind Institute & Graduate Program in Neuroscience, University of Western Ontario 2008 – present Associate Scientist, Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre, Toronto 2000 – 2006: Assistant Professor, Dept. of Psychology & Graduate Program in Neuroscience, University of Western Ontario 1998 – 2000: Research Associate, Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University 1995 – 1998: Post-Doctoral Research Fellow, Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto UNIVERSITY EDUCATION 1991 – 1995: Ph.D., Psychology, University of Toronto in addition: Completion of the Collaborative Program in Neuroscience at the Ph.D. level, University of Toronto Thesis: Visual long-term memory for spatial location and object identity in humans: Neural correlates and cognitive processes Supervisor: Morris Moscovitch 1985 – 1991: Diplom, Psychology, Universität Bielefeld, Germany Thesis: Memory deficits in patients with Alzheimer's disease Supervisor: Wolfgang Hartje 2 AREAS OF RESEARCH INTEREST General: Cognitive neuroscience Specific: Memory & amnesia Visual cognition
    [Show full text]
  • Introduction
    Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-86288-2 - Memory in Autism Edited by Jill Boucher and Dermot Bowler Excerpt More information Part I Introduction © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 978-0-521-86288-2 - Memory in Autism Edited by Jill Boucher and Dermot Bowler Excerpt More information 1 Concepts and theories of memory John M. Gardiner Concept. A thought, idea; disposition, frame of mind; imagination, fancy; .... an idea of a class of objects. Theory. A scheme or system of ideas or statements held as an explan- ation or account of a group of facts or phenomena; a hypothesis that has been confirmed or established by observation or experiment, and is propounded or accepted as accounting for the known facts; a statement of what are known to be the general laws, principles, or causes of some- thing known or observed. From definitions given in the Oxford English Dictionary The Oxford Handbook of Memory, edited by Endel Tulving and Fergus Craik, was published in the year 2000. It is the first such book to be devoted to the science of memory. It is perhaps the single most author- itative and exhaustive guide as to those concepts and theories of memory that are currently regarded as being most vital. It is instructive, with that in mind, to browse the exceptionally comprehensive subject index of this handbook for the most commonly used terms. Excluding those that name phenomena, patient groups, parts of the brain, or commonly used exper- imental procedures, by far the most commonly used terms are encoding and retrieval processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Semantic Priming in Schizophrenia: a Review and Synthesis
    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society (2002), 8, 699–720. Copyright © 2002 INS. Published by Cambridge University Press. Printed in the USA. DOI: 10.1017.S1355617702801357 CRITICAL REVIEW Semantic priming in schizophrenia: A review and synthesis MICHAEL J. MINZENBERG,1 BETH A. OBER,2 and SOPHIA VINOGRADOV1 1Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco and Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, San Francisco, California 2Department of Human and Community Development, University of California, Davis and Department of Veterans Affairs Northern California Health Care System, Martinez, California (Received October 9, 2000; Revised June 4, 2001; Accepted June 5, 2001) Abstract In this paper, we present a review of semantic priming experiments in schizophrenia. Semantic priming paradigms show utility in assessing the role of deficits in semantic memory network access in the pathology of schizophrenia. The studies are placed in the context of current models of information processing. In this review we include all English-language reports (from peer-reviewed journals) of single-word semantic priming studies involving participants with schizophrenia. The studies to date show schizophrenic patients to exhibit variable semantic priming effects under automatic processing conditions, and consistent impairments under controlled0attentional conditions. We also describe associations with other neurocognitive dysfunction, neurochemical and electrophysiological disturbances, and clinical manifestations (such as thought disorder). (JINS, 2002, 8, 699–720.) Keywords: Semantic priming, Schizophrenia, Semantic memory, Language, Information processing INTRODUCTION Semantic Memory and Spreading Schizophrenia is primarily a disorder of thinking and lan- Activation Network Models guage. Indeed, investigators have suggested that a defect in All information processing models posit the existence of a language information processing may be pathognomonic of long-term memory system.
    [Show full text]
  • 5/14/19 1 PATRICIA ANN REUTER-LORENZ Chair and Professor of Psychology University of Michigan Department of Psychology 53
    5/14/19 PATRICIA ANN REUTER-LORENZ Chair and Professor of Psychology University of Michigan Department of Psychology 530 Church Street Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-1043 Phone: (764) 764-7429 Fax: (764) 763-7480 Email Address: [email protected] PARL Lab Web Site Education: Ph.D. 1987 University of Toronto (Psychology) M.A. 1981 University of Toronto (Psychology) B.A. 1979 State University of New YorK, Purchase (Psychology, with Honors) Professional Experience: 2016-present Michael I. Posner Collegiate Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience 2015-present Chair, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan 2002-present Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan 2012-present Faculty Associate, Survey Research Center, Institute for Social Research 2008-present Co-Director, International Max PlancK Research School on the LIFE course 2012 (Winter) Visiting Scientist, Center for Vital Longevity, UT Dallas 2009 (Spring) Visiting Scholar, Wales Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience & University of Wales, Bangor 2002-2005 Chair, Cognition and Cognitive Neuroscience Area, University of Michigan 1997-2002 Associate Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan 1992-1997 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan 1988-1991 Research Assistant Professor, Program in Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, Dartmouth Medical School, Hanover, NH 1989-1991 Adjunct Assistant Professor, Dept. of Psychology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH 1986-1988 Postdoctoral Fellow, Cognitive Neuroscience, Cornell
    [Show full text]
  • Session 5. Learning and Memory
    Picton Session 5: Learning and Memory Brain and Mind Learning and Memory … those short, plump little cakes called ‘petites madeleines,’ which look as though they had been moulded in the fluted scallop of a pilgrim’s shell. … I raised to my lips a spoonful of the tea in which I had soaked a morsel of the cake. No sooner had the warm liquid, and the crumbs with it, touched my palate than a shudder ran through my whole body, and I stopped, intent upon the extraordinary changes that were taking place. An exquisite pleasure had invaded my senses but individual, detached, with no suggestion of its origin (Marcel Proust, In Search of Lost Time, 1913) Lulu Durand, 2012 René Depasse This is the famous quotation about memory by Marcel Proust. It describes how the past can be re-experienced. How simple sensory triggers can bring forth complex memories. How these memories are at first indistinct and mysterious and only later become clear. How emotions are the glue that ties memories together. Where to get madeleines in Toronto? Try Madeleines bespoke pastry http://www.madeleines.ca/ For green-tea madeleines – Uncle Tetsu’s at Bay and Dundas. Brain and Mind: Course Outline 1. Introduction. Brain anatomy. 5. Learning and Memory. Synaptic Stroke. Neurons. Excitation. Action changes. Motor skills. Priming. potentials. Synaptic transmission.. Episodic vs semantic memory. Body sensations. Braille. Amnesia. Alzheimer’s Disease. 2. Moving to the Music. Muscles. 6. Language and Emotion. Language. Stretch reflexes. Basal ganglia. Humans vs chimps. Aphasia. Dyslexia. Cerebellum. Parkinson’s Disease. Basic emotions. Autonomic Nervous Balance.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Therapy for Traumatic Brain Injury
    SELF-THERAPY FOR TRAUMATIC BRAIN INJURY: TEACHING YOURSELF TO PREVENT HEAD-INJURED MOMENTS Release 3.3 Larry E. Schutz, Ph.D., ABPP copyright 2006, 2008 TABLE OF CONTENTS page chapter The Basic Program 1 Introduction: Start Here! 3 1. Step One : Learning About the Injury 4 2. Head-Injured Moments 5 3. Learning How to Recognize Head-Injured Moments 7 4. Learning Where to Look to Find Head-Injured Moments 8 5. Figuring Out What Went Wrong 9 6. Memory Issues 10 7. Figuring Out How Big the Problem Is 12 8. At the Crossroads of Recovery 13 Cheat sheet for watching for head-injured moments 14 9. Step Two : Taking Control of My Life--The Action Window 15 10. Keeping Appointments and Arrangements Yourself—The Appointment Book 16 11. Structure and Productivity—The Activity Routine 17. 12. Memory for Daily Events—The Activity Diary 18 13 Using Your Daily Schedule as a Planning Technique 20 A sample daily schedule blank 22 14. Step Three : Controlling Overstimulation 24 15. Step Four : Increasing Mental Effort 26 16. Step Five : Better Living Habits to Help My Brain Work Better 27 17. Booze, Dope, Caffeine, Nicotine, and Other Drugs 28 18. Dealing With Sleep Problems 29 19. Step Six : Full Analysis of Your Head-Injured Moments 31 The Analysis Form 32 20. Summary of How You Fix Your Brain 33 21. Home Therapies for Basic Cognitive Control 34 Newspaper Search 35 Number Search 36 Search A 37 Advanced Number Search 38 Key to Advanced Number Search A 39 Word Searches 40 Mind Control Jigsaw Exercise 41 Therapeutic Video Games 42 Slapjack 43 Twenty Questions 44 Therapeutic Taboo 45 Room Search 46 Therapeutic Jackstraws and Jenga 47 Memory Challenges 48 22.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflective Imagination: the Cognitive Process Underlying the Experience of Meaning Through Music
    journal of interdisciplinary music studies 2019, volume 9, art. #19091203, pp. 33-48 Reflective Imagination: The Cognitive Process Underlying the Experience of Meaning through Music Alejandra Wah Department of Arts, Culture and Media Studies, University of Groningen Background in philosophy. Over the past few decades several philosophers have drawn parallels between the engagement with music and imagination. For instance, Kendall L. Walton (1994; 1999) maintains that the same kind of imagining is involved in experiencing music as in experiencing narratives. Andrew Kania (2015) underlines that our experience of musical space and movement, much like our experience of fiction, is imaginative. Jerrold Levinson (1996) suggests that listeners experience music imaginatively—specifically, imagining others expressing emotions through the music (Gendler 2018). Background in cognitive science. Anna Abraham and Andreja Bubic (2015) have recently argued that semantic memory is the root of all aspects of human imagination. Abraham (2016) proposes five categories to analyze the human imaginative mind: (i) perceptual/motor related mental imagery, (ii) recollective or intentionality processing, (iii) generative or novel combinatorial processing, (iv) exceptional phenomenology in the aesthetic response, and (v) altered psychological states such as dreams, hallucinations, and delusions (2016: 4197). Aims. To shed light on the experience of meaning through music, I expand upon the assertions of philosophers who argue that the experience of music is underlain by imagination—but miss the role played by the development of memory retrieval mechanisms on this experience—and upon the findings of cognitive scientists who—while recognizing how memory retrieval mechanisms help to explain imagination—confuse aesthetic processing with artistic experience.
    [Show full text]
  • Corteza Prefrontal, Funciones Ejecutivas Y Regulación De La Conducta
    Corteza prefrontal, funciones ejecutivas y regulación de la conducta J. Tirapu Ustárroz A. García Molina P. Luna Lario A. Verdejo García M. Ríos Lago Corteza prefrontal, funciones ejecutivas y regulación de la conducta J. Tirapu Ustárroz A. García Molina P. Luna Lario A. Verdejo García M. Ríos Lago Introducción ciones, al escoger para la supervivencia a los portadores de los comportamientos mejor adaptados. Como señala Ralph Adolphs: ‘Los organismos complejos han El segundo nivel o escalafón lo constituyen las criaturas desarrollado cerebros que construyen modelos internos del skinnerianas, llamadas así en honor al psicólogo conductista mundo para interaccionar de manera flexible con un entorno estadounidense Burrhus F. Skinner. Las criaturas skinnerianas cambiante’ [1]. Para Daniel Dennett [2], los organismos vivos presentan la novedad de poseer cierta flexibilidad en su com- que pueblan la Tierra se pueden dividir en tres tipos de criatu- portamiento. Ante un problema dado, pueden ir probando a ras: darwinianas, skinnerianas y popperianas. Las criaturas ciegas las distintas variantes de conducta que son capaces de darwinianas son los organismos más sencillos desde el punto generar (es como disponer de un juego de llaves e ir introdu- de vista del comportamiento. Su gama de conductas se reduce ciendo una tras otra en la cerradura), hasta que por casualidad a estímulo-respuesta, es decir, respuestas simples y extremada- dan con una que funciona y dispara el efecto deseado. Esto por mente rígidas, pero si sirven, entonces sobreviven; en caso con- sí solo ya constituye cierta ventaja, pero es que además las cria- trario, mueren. Estas respuestas estarían grabadas en los genes turas skinnerianas cuentan con un sistema de refuerzo que de los individuos de esa especie.
    [Show full text]