Visual Disorders of Higher Cortical Function

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Visual Disorders of Higher Cortical Function Visual disorders of higher cortical function 1Most optometrists are familiar with the myriad of visual percep- tion disorders occurring in infants and children. They are trained to administer and interpret tests that detect these congenital or SARA PENNER ROBERTS, O.D. developmental disorders occurring most frequently in children with ABSTRACT: Following stroke or other learning disability, attention deficit disorder, mental retardation. causes of brain damage, patients may demon- and cerebral palsy. 1 Within the optometric literature, little attention strate visual disorders of higher cortical func- has been given to acquired visual disorders of higher cortical func- tion. These defects involve visual attention, oc- tion that occur in the adult or elderly population as a result of brain ulomotor skills, visuospatial orientation and ob- ject recognition. It is important for optometrists damage from infarction, hemorrhage, trauma, tumor or senile de- to understand these conditions and consider mentia. It is the purpose of this paper to review these neurological their presence in individuals with persistent vis- syndromes of disturbed visual perception and to discuss the impor- ual complaints despite a normal exam, or in tance of their recognition. patients who fail to respond to magnification in Unilateral spatial inattention low vision rehabilitation. The diagnosis and management of these higher order visual dis- Unilateral spatial inattention or neglect is the involuntary failure turbances are discussed. to attend to or respond to meaningful sensory stimuli presented in the affected hemispace.' This phenomenon may occur with any KEY WORDS: hemi-inattention, visual agno- combination of visual, auditory and tactile stimuli. Although this is sia, alexia without agraphia, Balint's syndrome, higher order visual disturbances, rehabilitation, not caused by motor or sensory defects, it is frequently accompanied stroke, brain damage by hemianesthesias, hemiplegias and homonymous hemianopsias. The effect of hemispatial neglect can be devastating. In fact, some Roberts SP. Visual disorders of higher cortical func- tion. J Am Optom Assoc 1992: 63:723-32. researchers cite unilateral neglect as one of the main factors inter- fering with recovery and successful rehabilitation in stroke pa- tients' In severe forms, the patient demonstrates complete hemis- patial neglect to the extent that he may not dress or shave the Roberts, Sara (1992). Visual affected half of the body/ He may not recognize his arm and leg as disorders of higher cortical his own and object to their presence in his bed." Questions addressed function. Journal of the to the patient from the affected side may go unanswered and American Optometric navigation may be difficult if the patient collides with objects and is unable to orient himself to new surroundings.* In its most subtle Association, 63(10)724-732. forms, the patient demonstrates inattention only when faced with a complex task. In these cases, the hemispatial inattention may be detected by presenting the patient with symmetrical simultaneous stimuli (i.e., fingers presented to each side of the mid-line in the THIS IS superior visual field). Failure to detect both stimuli constitutes an COPYRIGHTED abnormal response known as the extinction phenomenon.5.6 Hemispatial inattention is usually the product of a right (non- MATERIAL. YOU dominant) parietal lobe lesion. Left sided parietal lesions are less MAY PRINT ONE commonly reported and are associated with less persistant and COPY FOR YOUR milder forms of inattention.''.7 Rarely there have been other reported OWN USE ONLY! cases of lesions occurring elsewhere in the brain including the frontal lobes, occipitoparietal lobes, cingulate gyrus, thalamus, pu- tamen, basal ganglia and mesencephalon.'J Infarction (stroke) is most commonly implicated, although the phenomenon has been reported in association with hemorrhage, tumor, and traumatic contusion.2 The exact mechanism is unclear. The examiner's assessment should begin with simple observa- tion of the patient. As mentioned earlier, patients with moderate to severe hemispatial neglect may demonstrate peculiar behavioral Volume 63. Number 10. 10/92 723 patterns as they orient their body and activities toward the unaffected hemispace (ipailateral to the side of the lesion). For example, they may comb only half of their hair, fill out only half of the insurance form or read only half of the letters on each line of the acuity chart. The patient's family members or case workers may also be able to provide a history of this type of behavior. A commonly used screening procedure for hemis- patial visual inattention is the double simultaneous stimulation test, which was first described by Oppen- heim using tactile stimuli.2 A standard confrontation visual field is first conducted using single stimuli pre- sented in each quadrant of the visual field, one at a time. If the responses are abnormal (i.e., failure to identify the stimulus), one cannot differentiate between a hemianopic visual field defect and a severe attentional disorder without further testing.* In this case, the dou- ble stimulation test cannot be performed. If the re- sponses are normal, however, the examiner then pre- sents double simultaneous stimuli, placed on each side of the vertical midline. Extinction occurs when the patient fails to perceive the stimulus in the field con- tralateral to the lesion, while correctly perceiving the stimulus in the ipsilateral field. Although sensory ex- Figure 1: The results of the clock completion test, as per- tinction, like hemi-inattention, is typically a product of formed by a person with hemispatial visual inattention. right parietal lobe lesions, it is not pathognomonic for hemi-inattention disorders." In addition, the test sen- sitivity may be as low as 50 percent 9 Pursuit and saccade testing are also easy screening ments of the clock on the side ipsilateral to the lesion procedures that may alert one to the presence of lesions or they will complete only this half of the clock (Fig. that can produce hemispatial inattention. Affected pa- 1). This assymetry in performance allows the examiner tients may demonstrate difficulty when attempting to to differentiate this defect from other visuoperceptual track an object into the affected hemispace and main- or constructional difficulties, and placement of the test tain stable fixation. When asked to alternate fixation in the hemifield free of visual field defect reduces field between two targets on either side of the midline, they loss as a cause of poor performance. Similarly, the may fail to search or demonstrate very slow searching examiner may draw a series of horizontal lines and ask in the hemispace contralateral to the lesion. Optoki- the patient to draw an "X" at the center of each line." netic nystagmus (OKN) reflexes also may be of some Patients with hemispatial visual neglect may fail to benefit as they are typically decreased or absent when cross all the lines on the page, and of those lines which the drum is rotated towards the side of a parietal lobe are crossed, the "bisecting line" is skewed toward the lesion." side of attended space. A visual marker (such as a letter) For patients with concurrent hemianopic or quad- may be placed at the beginning and end of each line rantanopic visual field defects, the double simultaneous and the patient is then instructed to bisect the line only stimulation test cannot be performed, and pursuit/ after naming the letters associated with each line. This saccade testing may not produce reliable results since ensures that the entire line is seen, thereby eliminating abnormal responses may occur as a result of the visual any effect from visual field deficits. field defect alone. For these patients, the clock comple- The Rivermead Behavioral Inattention Test tion test and the line bisection tests are particularly (RBIT) has been developed as a useful method for useful. detecting visual neglect, as well as for estimating its In the clock completion test, the examiner draws a functional impact.'3 The RBIT differs from other tests circle (clock face), and then asks the patient to complete of visual inattention because it uses behavioral meas- the clock by drawing in the numbers and hands. The ures such as eating a meal, reading a menu, etc. ~ test material should be placed in the attended space, Rehabilitation of patients with bemispatial visual opposite the side of visual field loss. Patients with inattention is difficult. A common sense approach to hemispatial visual inattention will either place all ele- training, aa outlined by Bouska, et al., is found in Table 1.' One should keep in mind that although patients may be trained for specific tasks, they may find it hard to generalize and apply these techniques to untrained tasks' In addition, some patients manifest other cog- 724 Journal of the American Optometric Association Table I: Training guidelines for hemispatia visual inattention 1. Attempt to improve patient's awareness and understanding of inattention and/or visual field loss using examples in the environment. 2. Practice visual scanning skills, including ocular pursuit and saccades, into the unattended space. 3. Provide the patient with increasingly complex visual-perceptual and visual-motor tasks which require the integration of the above two points. Figure 2: Patient with apperceptive agnosia demostrating 4. Stress the importance of scanning during activ- inability to correctly match object shapes. (Drawing repro ities of daily living-, reminding the patient, duced with permission from Farah MJ, Visual Agnosia. "when something doesn't make sense, look into Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT press, 1990.) the unattended space and it usually will". altogether, claiming that all patients with visual agno- 5. Use visual markers at the end/beginning of the sias would show significant visual pereeptual difficulties page to decrease inattention while reading. if subjected to adequate study."'" For the sake of this discussion, however, we will continue to follow this classification scheme. nitive, motor or sensory deficits that may complicate or prohibit attempts at rehabilitation.
Recommended publications
  • Sciencedirect.Com Sciencedirect
    cortex 89 (2017) 135e155 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research report Agnosic vision is like peripheral vision, which is limited by crowding Francesca Strappini a,b,c, Denis G. Pelli d, Enrico Di Pace a and * Marialuisa Martelli a,b, a Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza, Rome, Italy b Neuropsychology Research Centre, IRCCS Foundation Hospital Santa Lucia, Rome, Italy c Neurobiology Department, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel d Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA article info abstract Article history: Visual agnosia is a neuropsychological impairment of visual object recognition despite Received 23 April 2014 near-normal acuity and visual fields. A century of research has provided only a rudimen- Reviewed 14 July 2014 tary account of the functional damage underlying this deficit. We find that the object- Revised 24 October 2014 recognition ability of agnosic patients viewing an object directly is like that of normally- Accepted 13 January 2017 sighted observers viewing it indirectly, with peripheral vision. Thus, agnosic vision is Action editor Jason Barton like peripheral vision. We obtained 14 visual-object-recognition tests that are commonly Published online 1 February 2017 used for diagnosis of visual agnosia. Our “standard” normal observer took these tests at various eccentricities in his periphery. Analyzing the published data of 32 apperceptive Keywords: agnosia patients and a group of 14 posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) patients on these tests, Visual agnosia we find that each patient's pattern of object recognition deficits is well characterized by one Crowding number, the equivalent eccentricity at which our standard observer's peripheral vision is like Object recognition the central vision of the agnosic patient.
    [Show full text]
  • Cory Newman Thesis 5-2021 Signed.Pdf (1.303Mb)
    DocuSign Envelope ID: 4851A328-CF63-4770-A833-B4246E7E5D23 Simulating Homonymous Hemianopsia for the Care Team by Cory Newman May 2021 Presented to the Division of Science, Information Arts, and Technologies University of Baltimore In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science 5/25/2021 Approved by: ________________________________ [name, Thesis Advisor] ________________________________5/25/2021 [name, Committee Member] i DocuSign Envelope ID: 4851A328-CF63-4770-A833-B4246E7E5D23 Abstract Homonymous hemianopsia is a visual impairment that involves the bilateral loss of a complete visual field. While research has been done to ascertain the details of cause and prognosis of homonymous hemianopsia, an obvious disparity arose in the research on how to educate the supporting care team of a person with homonymous hemianopsia to maximize the creation of educational and rehabilitation plans. This research presents two studies focused on closing that gap by providing an alternative method of understanding. In the initial study 16 participants with a confirmed caregiver relationship to one or more persons with homonymous hemianopsia were surveyed on their personal knowledge of the visual impairment. These participants were asked to express any visual obstacles they have encountered, and to ascertain the availability of a device or program that could provide an interactive interpretation of how their homonymous hemianopsia patient views their surroundings. Survey results confirmed the need for a program that could easily simulate homonymous hemianopsia for the care provider. An additional usability study was completed by eight of the 16 previous participants on a mobile homonymous hemianopsia simulation application prototype. User tests showed that participants gained a significant increase in understanding of how those with a homonymous hemianopsia visual impairment view the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Homonymous Hemianopsia
    Source: CLEVELAND CLINIC OHIO USA FACTSHEET Homonymous Hemianopsia Homonymous hemianopsia is a condition in which a person sees only one side - right or left of the visual world of each eye; results from a problem in brain function rather than a disorder of the eyes themselves. This can happen after a head or brain injury. What is homonymous hemianopsia? Homonymous hemianopsia is a condition in which a person sees only one side―right or left―of the visual world of each eye. The person may not be aware that the vision loss is happening in both eyes, not just one. Under normal circumstances, the left half of the brain processes visual information from both eyes about the right side of the world. The right side of the brain processes visual information from both eyes about the left side of the world. A visual world of someone with normal vision A visual world of someone with homonymous hemianopsia In homonymous hemianopsia, an injury to the left part of the brain results in the loss of the right half of the visual world of each eye. An injury to the right part of the brain produces loss of the left side of the visual world of each eye. This condition is created by a problem in brain function rather than a disorder of the eyes themselves. What causes homonymous hemianopsia? The most common cause of this type of vision loss is stroke. However, any disorder that affects the brain—including tumours, inflammation, and injuries--can be a cause. Source: CLEVELAND CLINIC OHIO USA It is estimated that 70% of the injuries leading to hemianopsias are due to an obstruction (blockage) of the blood supply (stroke).
    [Show full text]
  • Oct Institute
    Low Vision, Visual Dysfunction and TBI – Treatment, Considerations, Adaptations Andrea Hubbard, OTD, OTR/L, LDE Objectives • In this course, participants will: 1. Learn about interventions involving specialized equipment to adapt an environment for clients with low vision. 2. Learn about the most typical low vision presentations/conditions. 3. Gain increased knowledge of eye anatomy and the visual pathway. Overview of TBI Reference: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Overview of TBI Risk Factors for TBI Among non-fatal TBI-related injuries for 2006–2010: • Men had higher rates of TBI hospitalizations and ED visits than women. • Hospitalization rates were highest among persons aged 65 years and older. • Rates of ED visits were highest for children aged 0-4 years. • Falls were the leading cause of TBI-related ED visits for all but one age group. – Assaults were the leading cause of TBI-related ED visits for persons 15 to 24 years of age. • The leading cause of TBI-related hospitalizations varied by age: – Falls were the leading cause among children ages 0-14 and adults 45 years and older. – Motor vehicle crashes were the leading cause of hospitalizations for adolescents and persons ages 15-44 years. Reference: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Overview of TBI Risk Factors for TBI Among TBI-related deaths in 2006–2010: • Men were nearly three times as likely to die as women. • Rates were highest for persons 65 years and older. • The leading cause of TBI-related death varied by age. – Falls were the leading cause of death for persons 65 years or older.
    [Show full text]
  • Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-Inattention
    University of Puget Sound Sound Ideas School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects Occupational Therapy, School of 5-2016 Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi- inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting. Elizabeth Armbrust University of Puget Sound Domonique Herrin University of Puget Sound Christi Lewallen University of Puget Sound Karin Van Duzer University of Puget Sound Follow this and additional works at: http://soundideas.pugetsound.edu/ot_capstone Part of the Occupational Therapy Commons Recommended Citation Armbrust, Elizabeth; Herrin, Domonique; Lewallen, Christi; and Van Duzer, Karin, "Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting." (2016). School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects. 4. http://soundideas.pugetsound.edu/ot_capstone/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Occupational Therapy, School of at Sound Ideas. It has been accepted for inclusion in School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects by an authorized administrator of Sound Ideas. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INTERVENTIONS FOR HEMI-INATTENTION IN INPATIENT REHAB Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting. May 2016 This evidence project, submitted by Elizabeth Armbrust,
    [Show full text]
  • VISUAL FIELD Pathway Extends from the „Front‟ to the „Back‟ of the RETINA Brain
    NOTE: To change the image on this slide, select the picture and delete it. Then click the Pictures icon in the placeholde r to insert your own image. Visual Pathway Disorders Amr Hassan, MD, FEBN Associate professor of Neurology - Cairo University Optic nerve • Anatomy of visual pathway • How to examine • Visual pathway disorders • Quiz 2 Optic nerve • Anatomy of visual pathway • How to examine • Visual pathway disorders • Quiz 3 Optic nerve The Visual Pathway VISUAL FIELD Pathway extends from the „front‟ to the „back‟ of the RETINA brain. ON OC OT LGN OPTIC RADIATIONS ON = Optic Nerve OC = Optic Chiasm OT = Optic Tract LGN = Lateral Geniculate Nucleus of Thalamus VISUAL CORTEX 5 The Visual Pathway Eyes & Retina Light >> lens >> retina (inverted and reversed image). Eyes & Retina Eyes & Retina • Macula: oval region approximately 3-5 mm that surrounds the fovea, also has high visual acuity. • Fovea: central fixation point of each eye// region of the retina with highest visual acuity. Eyes & Retina • Optic disc: region where axons leaving the retina gather to form the Optic nerve. Eyes & Retina • Blind spot located 15° lateral and inferior to central fixation point of each eye. Object to be seen Peripheral Retina Central Retina (fovea in the macula lutea) 12 Photoreceptors © Stephen E. Palmer, 2002 Photoreceptors Cones • Cone-shaped • Less sensitive • Operate in high light • Color vision • Less numerous • Highly represented in the fovea >> have high spatial & temporal resolution >> they detect colors. © Stephen E. Palmer, 2002 Photoreceptors Rods • Rod-shaped • Highly sensitive • Operate at night • Gray-scale vision • More numerous than cons- 20:1, have poor spatial & temporal resolution of visual stimuli, do not detect colors >> vision in low level lighting conditions © Stephen E.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Problems After Brain Injury
    Visual problems after brain injury As a charity, we rely on donations from people like you to continue providing free information to people affected by brain injury. Donate today: www.headway.org.uk/donate Introduction Vision is the skill that allows us to see the world around us. When we look at the world, a complex series of processes takes place between the eyes and the brain. The eyes take in the information, while the brain (which is connected to the eyes by a nerve called the optic nerve) is responsible for processing and interpreting it. Through this system we are able to see things such as colours, shapes, movement, objects and people. When the brain is injured, the ability to interpret visual information can be affected in different ways. This factsheet has been written to explain how brain injury can affect vision and how to seek professional support with these issues. Tips for coping with visual problems are also offered. Words in bold are defined in a glossary at the end of the factsheet. What is vision? There are lots of different aspects of vision. Some of the things the brain needs to do to decode information that it receives from the eyes are: • process the shape and colour of objects • process and merge information received from both eyes • recall information from memory to recognise objects or places • process the movement of objects • process the location and position of an object in space • process information across the visual field (including peripheral vision) Generally, different parts of the brain are responsible for processing these different aspects of vision.
    [Show full text]
  • 26 Aphasia, Memory Loss, Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders - - 8/4/17 12:21 PM )
    1 Aphasia, Memory Loss, 26 Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders M.-Marsel Mesulam CHAPTER The cerebral cortex of the human brain contains ~20 billion neurons spread over an area of 2.5 m2. The primary sensory and motor areas constitute 10% of the cerebral cortex. The rest is subsumed by modality- 26 selective, heteromodal, paralimbic, and limbic areas collectively known as the association cortex (Fig. 26-1). The association cortex mediates the Aphasia, Memory Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders Loss, integrative processes that subserve cognition, emotion, and comport- ment. A systematic testing of these mental functions is necessary for the effective clinical assessment of the association cortex and its dis- eases. According to current thinking, there are no centers for “hearing words,” “perceiving space,” or “storing memories.” Cognitive and behavioral functions (domains) are coordinated by intersecting large-s- cale neural networks that contain interconnected cortical and subcortical components. Five anatomically defined large-scale networks are most relevant to clinical practice: (1) a perisylvian network for language, (2) a parietofrontal network for spatial orientation, (3) an occipitotemporal network for face and object recognition, (4) a limbic network for explicit episodic memory, and (5) a prefrontal network for the executive con- trol of cognition and comportment. Investigations based on functional imaging have also identified a default mode network, which becomes activated when the person is not engaged in a specific task requiring attention to external events. The clinical consequences of damage to this network are not yet fully defined. THE LEFT PERISYLVIAN NETWORK FOR LANGUAGE AND APHASIAS The production and comprehension of words and sentences is depen- FIGURE 26-1 Lateral (top) and medial (bottom) views of the cerebral dent on the integrity of a distributed network located along the peri- hemispheres.
    [Show full text]
  • Absence of Hemispatial Neglect Following Right Hemispherectomy in a 7-Year-Old Girl with Neonatal Stroke
    Neurology Asia 2017; 22(2) : 149 – 154 CASE REPORTS Absence of hemispatial neglect following right hemispherectomy in a 7-year-old girl with neonatal stroke *1Jiqing Qiu PhD, *2Yu Cui MD, 3Lichao Sun MD, 1Bin QiPhD, 1Zhanpeng Zhu PhD *JQ Qiu and Y Cui contributed equally to this work Departments of 1Neurosurgery, 2Otolaryngology and 3Emergency Medicine, The First Hospital of Jilin University, Changchun, Jilin, China Abstract Neonatal stroke leads to cognitive deficits that may include hemispatial neglect. Hemispatial neglect is a syndrome after stroke that patients fail to be aware of stimuli on the side of space and body opposite a brain lesion. We report here a 7-year-old girl who suffered neonatal right brain stroke and underwent right hemispherectomy due to refractory epilepsy. Post-surgical observation of the child’s behavior and tests did not show any signs of hemispatial neglect. We concluded the spatial attention function of the child with neonatal stroke might be transferred to the contralateral side during early childhood. Key words: Epilepsy; hemispatial neglect; hemispherectomy; neonatal stroke; child. INTRODUCTION CASE REPORT Approximately one in every 4,000 newborns suffer The patient was a 7-year-old right-handed girl. from neonatal stroke, with a high mortalityrate.1-3 She was delivered with normal body weight. The survivors may develop varying degrees Gestation and delivery were uneventful. There of hemiparesis and low intelligence quotient was no family history of epilepsy. She had coma (IQ). Children with neonatal stroke, especially a few days after birth and was diagnosed with children with hemiplegia, have a high risk of neonatal stroke (Figure 1A-D).
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Field Recovery - Case Report of a Patient with Left Homonymous Hemianopsia Post-Stroke
    CopyrightMedical © Marsha & Davis Surgical Benshir CRIMSON PUBLISHERS C Wings to the Research Ophthalmology Research ISSN 2578-0360 Case Report Visual Field Recovery - Case Report of a Patient with Left Homonymous Hemianopsia Post-Stroke Marsha Davis Benshir* Center for Vision Development, USA *Corresponding author: Marsha Davis Benshir, Center for Vision Development, 164 W Main St Ste. B New Market, MD 21774, USA Submission: February 06, 2018; Published: March 23, 2018 Abstract have spontaneous recovery and others will adapt through physical and occupational therapy. Vision rehabilitation modeled on other widely used types of therapyVisual post-strokefield loss from has stroke been demonstratedoccurs in up to to 50% restore of patients peripheral requiring vision hospitalization in some patients for beyond rehabilitation the expected after stroke window [1]. for Of spontaneousthese patients recovery many will of peripheral vision. Introduction Optometrists specializing in Neuro-optometric rehabilitation study in Rotterdam, Netherlands, glaucoma was the leading cause have been working with physiatrists, neurologists and other falling. In patients over 85 the incidence is closer to 1:6. In one rehabilitation specialist for decades, and have demonstrated that [5]. of visual field loss followed by other optic nerve disease and stroke expected time for recovery. The research proving that therapy can Case Report visual field recovery is possible for many patients beyond the be effective is being done by neurologists who have demonstrated This patient was a previously healthy 60 y.o. right-handed male re-wiring of neural pathways with improved performance after who developed sudden left side facial weakness, confusion and repetitive visual-motor tasks and recovery of somatosensory and disorientation on the way home from work.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Impairment Age-Related Macular
    VISUAL IMPAIRMENT AGE-RELATED MACULAR DEGENERATION Macular degeneration is a medical condition predominantly found in young children in which the center of the inner lining of the eye, known as the macula area of the retina, suffers thickening, atrophy, and in some cases, watering. This can result in loss of side vision, which entails inability to see coarse details, to read, or to recognize faces. According to the American Academy of Ophthalmology, it is the leading cause of central vision loss (blindness) in the United States today for those under the age of twenty years. Although some macular dystrophies that affect younger individuals are sometimes referred to as macular degeneration, the term generally refers to age-related macular degeneration (AMD or ARMD). Age-related macular degeneration begins with characteristic yellow deposits in the macula (central area of the retina which provides detailed central vision, called fovea) called drusen between the retinal pigment epithelium and the underlying choroid. Most people with these early changes (referred to as age-related maculopathy) have good vision. People with drusen can go on to develop advanced AMD. The risk is considerably higher when the drusen are large and numerous and associated with disturbance in the pigmented cell layer under the macula. Recent research suggests that large and soft drusen are related to elevated cholesterol deposits and may respond to cholesterol lowering agents or the Rheo Procedure. Advanced AMD, which is responsible for profound vision loss, has two forms: dry and wet. Central geographic atrophy, the dry form of advanced AMD, results from atrophy to the retinal pigment epithelial layer below the retina, which causes vision loss through loss of photoreceptors (rods and cones) in the central part of the eye.
    [Show full text]
  • BJNN Neglect Article Pre-Proof.Pdf
    Kent Academic Repository Full text document (pdf) Citation for published version Gallagher, Maria and Wilkinson, David T. and Sakel, Mohamed (2013) Hemispatial Neglect: Clinical Features, Assessment and Treatment. British Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, 9 (6). pp. 273-277. ISSN 1747-0307. DOI https://doi.org/10.12968/bjnn.2013.9.6.273 Link to record in KAR https://kar.kent.ac.uk/37678/ Document Version UNSPECIFIED Copyright & reuse Content in the Kent Academic Repository is made available for research purposes. Unless otherwise stated all content is protected by copyright and in the absence of an open licence (eg Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher, author or other copyright holder. Versions of research The version in the Kent Academic Repository may differ from the final published version. Users are advised to check http://kar.kent.ac.uk for the status of the paper. Users should always cite the published version of record. Enquiries For any further enquiries regarding the licence status of this document, please contact: [email protected] If you believe this document infringes copyright then please contact the KAR admin team with the take-down information provided at http://kar.kent.ac.uk/contact.html 1 Hemispatial Neglect: Clinical Features, Assessment and Treatment Maria Gallagher1, David Wilkinson1 & Mohamed Sakel2 1School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK. 2East Kent Neuro-Rehabilitation Service, East Kent Hospitals University NHS Foundation Trust, UK. Correspondence and request for reprints to: Dr. David Wilkinson, School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP, UK. Email: [email protected].
    [Show full text]