<<

p

CHAPTER 1

What Are the Origins of and Behavior?

Focus on Disorders: Closed Head Injury The of Brain and Behavior Defining Brain and Behavior Origin of Brain Cells and Classification Systems What Is the Brain? Evolution of Animals with Nervous Systems How Is the Structured? The Chordate Nervous System What Is Behavior? Evolution Perspectives on Brain and : Members of the Primate Order Behavior : Our Distant Ancestor Aristotle and Mind The First Humans Descartes and Dualism The Evolution of the Descartes’s Legacy Focus on Disorders: Linking Brain Function Studying Brain and Behavior to Brain Disease in Modern Humans Darwin and Materialism Human Brain-Size Comparisons Darwin’s Legacy Culture Focus on Disorders: Learning Disabilities

Crandall/The Image Works Micrograph: Oliver Meckes/Ottawa/Photo Researchers p

Twelve years ago, I survived a serious head injury. In the sec- significant event of my lifetime. The catastrophic effect of ond it took for my car to crash head-on, my was perma- my injury was such that I was shattered and then remolded nently changed, and I became another statistic in what has by the experience, and I emerged from it a profoundly been called “the silent epidemic.” different person with a different set of convictions, values, During the next months, my family and I began to un- and priorities. Above all, I have learned that there is no derstand something of the reality of the experience of head limit to the power of faith, hope, and love. With these, I injury. I had begun the painful task of recognizing and ac- made the journey out of the shadows into a larger, brighter cepting my physical, mental, and emotional deficits. I world than the one I had left behind before my injury. couldn’t taste or smell. I couldn’t read even the simplest (Linge, 1990) sentence without forgetting the beginning before I got to the end. I had a -trigger temper that could ignite instantly his description of what it is like to be brain injured into rage over the most trivial incident. During the first year, I could not take too much stimu- was written by Fred Linge, a clinical psychologist lation from other people. My brain would simply overload, Twith a degree in brain research. (For an explanation and I would have to go off into my room to get away. Noise of how the brain can be injured in an accident, see was hard for me to take, and I wanted the place to be kept “Closed Head Injury” on page 2.) In the years after his in- quiet, which was an impossibility in a small house with jury, Linge made an immense journey. He traveled from a three youngsters in it. I remember laying down some impos- time before the car crash, when he gave less thought to sible rules for all of us. For example, I made rules that the relation between his brain and his behavior than he everybody had to be in bed by 9:30 PM, that all lights had to be out, and that no noise of any kind was permitted after did to the way in which he dressed. At the end of the jour- that time. No TV, radios, or talking was allowed. Eventually ney, thoughts about his brain and his behavior dominated the whole family was in an uproar. his life. He became a consultant and advisor to many peo- Two years after my injury, I wrote a short article: ple who also had suffered brain injury. “What Does It Feel Like to Be Brain Damaged?” At that Most of you are like Fred Linge before he took that time, I was still intensely focusing on myself and my own journey. Your brain does its work so efficiently and unob- struggle. (Every head-injured survivor I have met seems to trusively that you hardly give it any thought. You may be go through this stage of narcissistic preoccupation, which creates a necessary shield to protect them from the painful unaware that the human brain has hundreds of parts, each realities of the situation until they have a chance to heal.) of which participates in certain tasks. You may have no I had very little sense of anything beyond the material world knowledge that the brain changes as you age, as you un- and could only write about things that could be described dergo major life events, and even as you engage in seem- in factual terms. I wrote, for example, about my various im- ingly trivial behaviors, such as reading the words on this pairments and how I learned to compensate for them by a page. In learning about the origins of the universe, the variety of methods. world, and human beings, you may have encountered no At this point in my life, I began to involve myself with other brain-damaged people. This came about in part after mention of the brain and its relation to behavior. Yet, if you the publication of my article. To my surprise, it was reprinted ever had first-hand experience with brain damage, you, in many different publications, copied, and handed out to too, would be confronted with the workings of this most thousands of survivors and families. It brought me an enor- wonderful and complex machine. mous outpouring of letters, phone calls, and personal visits The purpose of this book is to take you on a journey that continue to this day. Many were struggling as I had strug- not unlike the one that Fred Linge took. Through it you, gled, with no diagnosis, no planning, no rehabilitation, and too, will come to understand the link between brain and most of all, no hope. The far-ranging effects of head injury on the survivor’s behavior. Of course, we do not ask that you experience life and that of his or her family cannot be overemphasized. brain damage to undertake this journey. The road that we In my own case I realize that it was for me the single most offer is simply one of information and discovery. Yet, along

1 2 CHAPTER 1

Closed Head Injury Focus on Disorders Closed head injury results from a (A) (B)

blow to the head that subjects the A variety of mechanical brain to a variety of forces. First, the forces cause closed head injuries as a result of a force exerted on the skull at the site blow to the head. of the blow causes bruising (contu- sion) known as a “coup.” Second, the blow may force the brain The damage at the site of impact is called a coup (shown in pink). against the opposite side of the Direction of blow Direction of blow skull, producing an additional bruise called a “countercoup” (see The pressure resulting from a coup may produce a countercoup on the opposite the accompanying illustration). side of the brain (shown in blue).

Third, the movement of the brain Movement of the brain may shear nerve fibers, causing microscopic lesions, especially may cause a twisting or shearing of in frontal and temporal lobes. Blood trapped nerve fibers, causing microscopic in the skull (hematoma) and swelling (edema) cause pressure on the brain. lesions. Such lesions may be found throughout the brain, but they are most common in the Shading (pink and blue) indicates regions of the brain most frequently damaged in closed head injury. A blow can frontal and temporal lobes. Fourth, the bruises and strains produce a contusion both at the site of impact and at the caused by the impact may produce bleeding (hemorrhage). opposite side of the brain owing to compression of the Because the blood is trapped within the skull, it acts as a brain against the front (A) or back (B) of the skull. growing mass (hematoma), which exerts pressure on sur- rounding brain regions. Finally, like blows to other parts of severe because the head is moving when the blow is struck, the body, blows to the brain produce swelling (edema). This thereby increasing the velocity of the impact. swelling, which is a collection of fluid in and around dam- The diffuse effects of closed head injuries make diagno- aged tissue, is another source of pressure on the brain. sis very difficult, which is why these kinds of injuries have People who sustain closed head injury often lose con- been collectively called a “silent epidemic.” Victims of se- sciousness because the injury affects fibers in lower parts of vere closed head injury can suffer serious repercussions in the brain that are associated with waking. The severity of their everyday . Like Fred Linge, many have difficulty re- coma can indicate the severity of the injury. Closed head in- turning to their former levels of functioning, including carry- juries resulting from motor vehicle accidents are particularly ing out their previous jobs.

it, you will find that much of the evidence that we have In this chapter, we answer the question, What are the about the brain and behavior comes from the study of origins of brain and behavior? We begin by defining both changes in people who have suffered brain injury. At the the brain and behavior and outlining the nervous system’s same time, we are also learning more and more about basic structure. We then look at how people through his- how the brain works when we are healthy. This emerging tory have viewed the relation between brain and behavior, knowledge is changing how we think about ourselves, starting with the mentalistic perspective of Aristotle and how we structure education and our social interactions, progressing to the biological perspective of today. With and how we aid those with brain injury. this background in mind, we explore the evolution of WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 3 brain and behavior. Here we pay special attention to the matter of how our brains acquire the sophisticated skills of evolution of the human species, while still recognizing the human culture. In subsequent chapters, we will further de- many traits that we have in common with other animals. velop many of the ideas introduced here in addition to fill- Finally, we look at several matters concerning the study of ing in a great many details about brain anatomy and func- the brain and behavior in modern humans, including the tion and how behavior is organized.

DEFINING BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR Brain and behavior differ greatly but are linked. The brain is a physical object, a living tissue, a body . Behavior is action, momentarily observable, but fleeting. Yet one is responsible for the other, which is responsible for the other, which is responsible for the other, and so on, and so on. We begin by defining first the brain, then behavior, and finally their interrelation.

What Is the Brain? For his postgraduate research, our friend Harvey chose to study the electrical activity that the brain gives off. He said that he wanted to live on as a brain in a bottle after his body died. He expected that his research would allow his bottled brain to communi- cate with others who could “read” his brain’s electrical signals. Harvey failed in his ob- jective, in part because the goal was technically impossible but also because he lacked a full understanding of what “brain” means. Brain is the Anglo-Saxon word for the tissue that is found within the skull, and it is this tissue that Harvey wanted to put into a bottle. Figure 1-1 shows a typical hu- man brain oriented as in the skull of an upright human. The brain has two relatively symmetrical halves called hemispheres, one on the left and one on the right. So, just as your body is symmetrical, having two arms and two legs, so is the brain. If you make your right hand into a fist and hold it up, the fist can represent the positions of the brain’s hemispheres within the skull, with the thumb pointing toward the front. The entire outer layer of the brain consists of a folded tissue. The folds are called gyri (singular, ). This outer layer is known as the (usually re- ferred to simply as the cortex). The word cortex, which means “bark” in Latin, is aptly chosen both because of the cortex’s folded appearance and because it covers most of the rest of the brain. The cortex of each hemisphere is divided into four lobes, named after the skull bones beneath which they lie. The temporal lobe is located approxi- mately at the same place as the thumb on your upraised fist. Because it points for- ward, it is a good landmark for identifying which part of the brain is the front. The lobe lying immediately above the temporal lobe is called the because it is located at the front of the brain, beneath the frontal bone of the skull. The parietal lobe is located behind the frontal lobe, and the occipital lobe constitutes the area at the back of each hemisphere. It is clear that Harvey, who wanted to have his brain bottled after he died, wanted to preserve not just his brain but his self—his consciousness, his thoughts, all of his intelligence. This meaning of the term brain refers to something other than the organ found inside the skull. It refers to the brain as that which exerts control over behavior. On the CD, visit the module on This meaning of brain is what we intend when we talk of someone being “the brain the Central Nervous System. Look at behind the operation” or when we speak of the computer that guides a spacecraft as the 3-D view of the human cortex in being the vessel’s “brain.” The term brain, then, signifies both the organ itself and the the section on the overview of the fact that this organ controls behavior. Could Harvey manage to preserve his control- brain for a hands-on view of what this exerting self inside a bottle? Read on to learn the answer to this question. organ looks like. p

4 CHAPTER 1

(A) (B)(B) Lobes define broad The brain is made up Folds in the Cerebral cortex is YourYour rightright hand,hand, ifif mademade intointo divisions of the of two hemispheres, brain's surface the brain’s outer aa fist,fist, representsrepresents thethe positionspositions cerebral cortex. left and right. are called gyri. “bark” layer. ofof thethe lobeslobes ofof thethe leftleft hemispherehemisphere ofof youryour brain.brain. Top ParietalParietal lobelobe (knuckles)(knuckles) Parietal FrontalFrontal lobelobe OccipitalOccipital lobelobe lobe (fingers)(fingers) (wrist)(wrist)

Front Frontal Back lobe Occipital lobe Temporal lobe TemporalTemporal lobelobe (thumb)(thumb) Bottom

Sectional view Glauberman/Photo Researchers

Figure 1-1 (A) In this representation of the human brain, showing its orientation in the head, the visible part of the brain is the How Is the Nervous System Structured? cerebral cortex (cortex means “bark,” Just like every other organ of the body, the brain is composed of cells. These brain and the cortex resembles the bark of a cells come in a variety of shapes and sizes. One type of brain cell is the neuron tree). The cortex is a thin sheet of tissue (sometimes called nerve cell), which has fibers projecting from it that make contact that is folded many times so that it fits with other cells. These interconnecting fibers make the brain a sensing, integrating or- inside the skull. The folds are called gyri. The brain consists of two symmetrical gan that also instructs the body to move. halves called hemispheres. Each Most of the connections from the brain to the rest of the body are made through hemisphere is divided into four lobes: the spinal cord, which descends through a canal in the vertebrae (the bones that form frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital. the backbone). The spinal cord is in the same orientation as that of the upright arm (B) The fist of your hand can serve as a supporting your fist. The brain and spinal cord, which in mammals such as ourselves guide to the orientation of the brain are both protected by bones, together make up the central nervous system (CNS). and its lobes. The thumb represents The central nervous system is connected to the rest of the body through nerve the temporal lobe and points forward, fibers, as shown in Figure 1-2. To sense what goes on in the world around us and in the flexed fingers represent the frontal our own bodies, these nerve fibers are extensively connected to sensory receptors on lobe, the knuckles represent the parietal the body’s surface, to internal body organs, and to muscles. All of these nerve fibers lobe, and the wrist represents the radiating out beyond the brain and spinal cord as well as all the neurons outside the occipital lobe. brain and spinal cord are referred to as the peripheral nervous system (PNS). The central and peripheral nervous systems together make up the whole nervous system. Networks of sensory pathways made up of bundles of nerve fibers and motor pathways also made up of bundles of nerve fibers of the peripheral nervous system are connected to the central nervous system. Sensory nerves are those related to spe- cific senses, such as hearing, vision, and touch. They connect receptors for these p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 5

Figure 1-2 Central nervous system (CNS) Peripheral nervous system (PNS) The brain and spinal cord, those Parts of the nervous system that connect the brain The human nervous system receives parts of the nervous system that and spinal cord to the rest of the body, including: sensory information and produces are encased by the skull and behavior. The nervous system consists bones Sensory connections of the central nervous system (the brain to sensory receptors and spinal cord) and the peripheral in the skin nervous system (the many neurons that connect the brain and spinal cord to the Motor connections rest of the body). to body muscles

Autonomic connections to body organs

senses to sensory-processing areas of the brain. With information from sensory recep- Plug in the CD to look at how these tors, the brain constructs current images of the world, as well as memories of past nerve networks work in our brains. The events and expectations about the future. In addition, sensory information helps the overview of the brain in the module on brain to construct a self-identity, as will be considered shortly. The motor nerves con- the Central Nervous System includes a nect the brain and spinal cord to the body’s muscles. The movements produced by rotatable, 3-D view of the brain that motor pathways include the eye movements that you are using to read this book, the will help you visualize how all these hand movements that you make while turning the pages, and the posture that you parts fit together maintain as you read. Motor pathways are also used in the workings of your body’s organs, such as the beating of your heart, the contractions of your stomach, and the movement of your diaphragm, which inflates and deflates your lungs. These pathways are part of another subdivision, called the autonomic nervous system, of the nervous system. The auto- nomic nervous system consists of all the neurons that receive messages from or send commands to the various organs of the body, including the heart, digestive system, sex organs, excretory organs, blood vessels, and glands. Your autonomic system allows you, for instance, to feel both hunger before eating and satisfaction after a meal. It also enables you to digest and process food through your digestive tract. The auto- nomic nervous system, in short, regulates all your bodily functions, as well as the emotional responses associated with your voluntary actions. Knowing that the nervous system is far more than just a brain, we can return to the question of whether a brain kept alive in a bottle is a brain in the fullest sense of the word. What Harvey wanted to try in the “brain in a bottle experiment” and failed p

6 CHAPTER 1

to consider was whether his brain could sustain intelligent behavior in the absence of the sensations and movements provided by the brain’s connections to the rest of the body. We can probably never be really sure of the capacities of a bottled brain. Still, some research suggests that sensations and movements are essential for consciousness. In one study in the 1920s, Edmond Jacobson wondered what would happen if our muscles completely stopped moving. Jacobson believed that, even when we be- lieve that we are entirely motionless, we still make subliminal movements related to our thoughts. The muscles of the larynx subliminally move when we “think in words,” for instance, and we make subliminal movements of our eyes when we imagine a visual scene. So Jacobson had people practice “total” relaxation and later asked them what the experience was like. They reported a condition of “mental emptiness,” as if the brain had gone blank. In another study in the 1950s, Donald O. Hebb and his coworkers investigated the effects of sensory deprivation, as well as lack of movement, by having each subject lie on a bed in a soundproof room and remain completely still. Tubes covered the sub- jects’ arms so that they had no sense of touch, and translucent goggles cut off their vi- sion. The subjects reported that the experience was extremely unpleasant, not just be- cause of the social isolation, but also because they lost their normal focus in this situation. Some subjects even had hallucinations, as if their brains were somehow try- ing to create the sensory experiences that they suddenly lacked. Most asked to be re- leased from the study before it ended. These experiments suggest that the brain needs ongoing sensory and motor expe- rience if it is to maintain its intelligent activity. Thus, when we use the term brain to mean an intelligent, functioning organ, we should probably refer to a brain that is connected to the rest of the nervous system. It seems very unlikely that a “brain in a bottle” would continue to function in a normal way.

What Is Behavior? I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt began his textbook titled Ethology: The Biology of Behavior, pub- lished in 1970, with the following definition of behavior: “Behavior consists of pat- terns in time.” These patterns can be made up of movements, vocalizations, or changes in appearance, such as the color changes associated with blushing. The expression “patterns in time” can even include thinking. Although we cannot directly observe someone’s thoughts, there are techniques for monitoring changes in the brain’s elec- trical and biochemical activity that may be associated with thought. So thinking, too, forms patterns in time. A simpler definition of behavior is any kind of movement in a living organism. Such movements are limited to those that we can somehow see and measure. To dis- tinguish the movements of a living organism from the movements of other things, such as a falling leaf or waves rolling onto the seashore, we can add that the move- ments of a living organism have both a cause and a function. For some animals, most movements are inherited ways of responding; but, for others, movements entail both inherited and learned patterns of behavior. If all mem- bers of a species display the same behavior under the same circumstances, that species has probably inherited a nervous system designed to produce that behavior automati- cally. In contrast, if each member of a species displays a somewhat different response in a similar situation, that species has inherited a nervous system that is much more flexible and capable of allowing changes in behavior due to learning. An example of the difference between a relatively fixed behavior pattern and a more flexible one is seen in the eating behavior of two different animal species— crossbills and roof rats—as illustrated in Figure 1-3. Crossbills are birds with beaks p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 7

A crossbill's beak is A baby roof rat must Figure 1-3 specifically designed to learn from its mother Some animal behaviors are largely open pine cones. This how to eat pine cones. behavior is innate. This behavior is learned. innate, whereas others are largely learned. The eating of pine cones by crossbills is an example of an innate behavior made possible by this bird’s specially designed beak, the tips of which are crossed, as illustrated by the parrot crossbill on the left. In contrast, Rattus rattus, a roof rat that lives in pine trees in Israel, can become an effective pine- cone eater only if it learns the skill from its mother. This learning is a form of cultural transmission. that seem to be awkwardly crossed at the tips; yet this beak is exquisitely designed to Left: Adapted from The Beak of the Finch eat certain kinds of pine cones. When eating these pine cones, crossbills use largely (p. 183), by J. Weiner, 1995, New York: Vintage. Right: Adapted from “Cultural fixed behavior patterns that do not require much modification through learning. If a Transmission in the Black Rat: Pinecone crossbill’s beak is changed even slightly by trimming, the bird is no longer able to eat. Feeding,” by J. Terkel, 1995, Advances in Roof rats, in contrast, are rodents with sharp incisor teeth that appear to be designed the Study of Behavior, 24, p. 122. to cut into anything. Roof rats are also effective pine-cone eaters, but they can eat pine cones efficiently only if they are taught to do so by an experienced mother. For roof rats, then, in contrast with crossbills with their relatively fixed action pattern, pine-cone eating is an acquired skill made possible by a flexible nervous system that is open to learning. The behavior described here is limited to pine-cone eating, and we do not intend to imply that all behavior displayed by crossbills is fixed or that all be- havior displayed by roof rats is learned. A central goal of research is to distinguish be- tween behaviors that are inherited and those that are learned and to understand how the nervous system produces each type of behavior. The complexity of behavior varies considerably in different species, largely de- pending on the degree to which a species is capable of learning and has flexibility in its responses. Generally, animals with smaller, simpler nervous systems have a nar- rower range of behaviors that they can use to react to a situation. Animals with com- plex nervous systems have more behavioral options in any given situation. We hu- mans believe that we are the animal species with the most complex nervous system and the greatest capacity for learning new responses. Species that have evolved greater complexity have not thrown away their simpler nervous systems, however. Rather, complexity emerges in part because new nervous system structures are added to old ones. For this reason, although human behavior depends mostly on learning, we, like other species, still possess many inherited ways of responding.

In Review

The brain consists of two hemispheres, one on the left and one on the right. Each has a folded outer layer called the cortex, which is divided into four lobes: the temporal, the frontal, the parietal, and the occipital. The brain and spinal cord together make up the central nervous system, and all the nerve fibers radiating outward from the spinal cord to other parts of the body compose the peripheral nervous system. Networks of sensory and motor nerves span these two systems. A simple definition of behavior is any kind of movement in a living organism. Although all behaviors have both a cause and a function, behaviors vary in their complexity and the degree to which they depend on learning. p

8 CHAPTER 1

Mind. A nonmaterial entity that is pro- PERSPECTIVES ON BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR posed to be responsible for intelligence, attention, awareness, and consciousness. The central question in the study of brain and behavior is how the two are related. Fred Linge believed that both the behavioral problems that he suffered after his injury Mentalism. Of the mind; an explanation of behavior as a function of the mind. and the gradual recovery that he made in the months that followed were in some way related to changes that took place in his brain. This view has not always been generally accepted, however. More than 2000 years ago, Aristotle proposed that something called the mind, soul, or psyche produces behavior. In this section, we explore Aris- totle’s view and two other influential perspectives on how the brain and behavior are related. Certain older ideas that we will consider are still commonly held by contem- Link to a timeline on the history porary religions or by people who see them as “common sense.” Knowing the origin of brain research at www.worth of these ideas will allow you to see why some of them are useful to the modern science publishers.com/kolb/chapter1. of the brain and behavior, whereas others are not.

Aristotle and Mind The hypothesis that the mind, soul, or psyche is responsible for behavior can be traced more than 2000 years to ancient Greece. (The terms mind, soul, and psyche are often used interchangeably.) In classical mythology, Psyche was a maiden who became the wife of the young god Cupid. Venus, Cupid’s mother, opposed his marriage to a mortal, and so she harassed Psyche with a number of almost impossible tasks. Psyche Aristotle performed the tasks with such dedication, intelligence, and compassion that she was (384–322 BC) made immortal, thus removing Venus’s objection to her. The ancient Greek philoso- pher Aristotle was alluding to this story when he suggested that all human intellectual functions are produced by a person’s psyche. The psyche, Aristotle argued, was re- sponsible for life, and its departure from the body resulted in . Aristotle’s account of behavior had no role for the brain, which he thought ex- isted to cool the blood. To Aristotle, the psyche was nonmaterial. To him, the psyche

E. Lessing/Art Resource, NY was responsible for human thoughts, perceptions, and emotions and for such processes as imagination, opinion, desire, pleasure, pain, memory, and reason. The psyche was an entity, or “stuff,” as philosophers call it, that was independent of the body. Aristotle’s view that a nonmaterial psyche governs our behavior was adopted by François Gerard, Psyche and Cupid Christianity in its concept of the soul and has been widely disseminated throughout (1798). the Western world. Mind is an Anglo-Saxon word for memory and, when “psyche” was translated into English, it became mind. The philosophical position that a person’s mind, or psy- che, is responsible for behavior is called mentalism, meaning “of the mind.” Mental- ism is not a scientific perspective. Because the mind is nonmaterial, it cannot be stud- ied with the use of scientific methods. Furthermore, most modern scientists believe that, when we understand how the nervous system produces behavior, it will not be necessary to explain behavior by the actions of a mind. But, despite this scientific re- jection of mentalism, terms that are mentalistic—such as sensation, perception, atten- tion, imagination, emotion, motivation, memory, and volition—are still used in psy- chology textbooks today. These terms, however, are usually employed as labels for patterns of behavior, not in the Aristotelian sense of being products of some nonma- terial entity totally divorced from any part of the body.

Click on the Web site to read a Descartes and Dualism detailed history of the origins of the Aristotle’s mentalistic explanation of behavior survived almost unquestioned until the mind–body question at www.worth 1500s. Then, in his book titled Treatise on Man, the first book on brain and behavior, publishers.com/kolb/chapter1. René Descartes (1596–1650), a French physiologist, mathematician, and philosopher, p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 9 proposed a new explanation of behavior in which the brain played an important role. Descartes placed the seat of the mind in the brain and linked the mind to the body. He saw mind and body as separate but interconnected. In the first sentence of Treatise on Man, he stated that people must be composed of both a mind and a body. Descartes wrote: I must first separately describe for you the body [which includes the brain]; René Descartes (1596–1650) then, also separately, the mind; and finally I must show you how these two natures would have to be joined and united to constitute people.... (Descartes, 1664, p. 1) To Descartes, most of the activities of the body, including sensation, motion, di- gestion, breathing, and sleep, can be explained by the mechanical principles by which the physical body and brain work. The mind, on the other hand, is nonmaterial, sepa- rate from the body, and responsible for rational behavior. Figure 1-4, an illustration from Descartes’s book, shows how the mind receives information from the body. When a hand touches a ball, for example, the mind learns through the brain that a ball exists, where the ball is located, and what its size and texture are. The mind also directs the body to touch the ball, but again it does so through the brain. The mind can command the brain to make the body carry out a great variety of actions, such as running, changing breathing rate, or throwing the ball across the room. The rational mind, then, depends on the brain both for information and for control of behavior. The age in which Descartes lived was exciting because scientists and engineers were making advances in understanding the universe, in constructing mechanical de- vices, and in comprehending the functions of the body. Descartes, who dissected ani- Figure 1-4 mals that he obtained from butcher shops, was recognized to be among the best Descartes proposed that humans have anatomists of his day. He was also aware of the many new machines being built, both a mind and a brain. He argued that including clocks, water wheels, and gears. He saw mechanical gadgets on public dis- the pineal body in the brain receives play in parks, such as those in the water gardens in Paris. One device caused a hidden different messages from a hand holding statue to approach and spray water when an unsuspecting stroller walked past it. a flute and from a hand touching a ball. The statue’s actions were triggered when the person stepped on a pedal hidden in the The mind, located in the pineal body, interprets these messages and so learns sidewalk. about the flute and ball. Influenced by these mechanical devices, Descartes proposed that the functions of From Treatise on Man, by R. Descartes, 1664. the body were produced by similar mechanical principles. For example, he used me- Reprint and translation (p. 60), 1972, chanical analogies both in describing how we automatically make decisions about dis- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. tances and angles on the basis of visual information and in trying to explain why, when we look at an object, we are less aware of what surrounds it. He also considered in detail “mechanical” physiological functions, such as digestion, respiration, and the Pineal body roles of nerves and muscles. To explain how the mind controls the body, Descartes suggested that the mind re- sides in a small part of the brain called the pineal body, which is located in the center of the brain beside fluid-filled cavities called ventricles. According to Descartes, the pineal body directs fluid from the ventricles through nerves and into muscles. When the fluid expands those muscles, the body moves. In Descartes’s theory, then, the Ventricles mind regulates behavior by directing the flow of ventricular fluid to the appropriate muscles. Note that, for Descartes, mind and body were separate entities and the pineal body was only a structure through which the mind works. Descartes’s proposal that an entity called the mind directs a machine called the Mind–body problem. The problem of body was perhaps the most influential idea ever proposed in philosophy or neuro- how to explain how a nonmaterial mind science. It was the first serious attempt to explain the role of the brain in controlling in- can command a material body. telligent behavior. The problem of how a nonmaterial mind and a physical brain might Dualism. A philosophical position that interact has come to be called the mind–body problem, and the philosophical posi- holds that both a nonmaterial mind and tion that behavior is controlled by two entities, a mind and a body, is called dualism. the material body contribute to behavior. p

10 CHAPTER 1

Descartes’s theory has many problems in its details and logic. With respect to its de- tails, we now know that people who have a damaged pineal body or even no pineal body at all still display normal intelligent behavior. The pineal body has a role in biological rhythms, not in governing all of human behavior. Furthermore, we now know that fluid is not pumped from the brain into muscles when they contract. Placing an arm in a bucket of water and contracting the arm’s muscles does not cause the water level in the bucket to rise, as it should if the volume of the muscle increased because fluid had been pumped into it. With respect to its logic, Descartes’s theory was also flawed. There is no obvious way that a nonmaterial entity could influence the body, because doing so would require the creation of energy, which would violate the laws of physics.

Descartes’s Legacy To determine if an organism possesses a mind, Descartes proposed two tests: the lan- guage test and the action test. To pass the language test, an organism must use lan- guage to describe and reason about things that are not physically present. The action test requires the organism to display behavior that is based on reasoning and is not just an automatic response to a particular situation. Descartes believed that, even if an engineer made a robot that appeared very human, it could be distinguished from a real human because it would fail these two tests. Descartes also assumed that animals are unable to pass the tests. A good deal of experimental work today is directed to- ward determining if he was right in this assumption. For example, studies of sign lan- guage taught to apes are partly intended to find out whether apes can describe and reason about things that are not present and so pass the language test. Descartes’s theory had a number of unfortunate results. On the basis of it, some people argued that young children and the mentally insane must lack human minds because they often fail to reason appropriately. We still use the expression “he’s lost his mind” to describe someone who is “mentally ill.” Some proponents of this view also reasoned that, if someone lacked a mind, that person was simply a machine and not due normal respect or kindness. Cruel treatment of animals, children, and the mentally ill was justified by Descartes’s theory. It is unlikely that Descartes himself intended these interpretations. He was reportedly very kind to his own dog, named Monsieur Grat. Without being aware of the source of their ideas, some people still hold a very dualistic notion about the mind and the body, including the brain. For instance, peo- ple still refer to the “mind’s ideals” on the one hand, and the body’s “animal instincts” on the other. John M. Harlow used such language to describe his now-famous brain- injured patient Phineas Gage, who lived a century ago. Gage was a 25-year-old dyna-

Department of Neurology and Image Analysis Facility, University of Iowa Department of Neurology and Image Analysis Facility, mite worker who survived an explosion that blasted an iron tamping bar (about a me- ter long and 3 centimeters wide) through the front of his head (Figure 1-5). Gage had Figure 1-5 been of average intelligence and very industrious and dependable. He was described as “energetic and persistent in executing all of his plans of operation.” But, after the When Phineas Gage died in 1861, no accident, his behavior changed completely. As Harlow wrote: autopsy was performed, but his skull was later recovered. Measurements from The equilibrium or balance, so to speak, between his intellectual faculties and Gage’s skull and modern imaging animal propensities seems to have been destroyed. He is fitful, irreverent, in- techniques were used to reconstruct the dulging at times in the grossest profanity, manifesting but little deference to accident and determine the probable his fellows, impatient of restraint or advice when it conflicts with his desires, location of the lesion. The frontal cortex of both hemispheres was damaged. at times perniciously obstinate, yet capricious and vacillating, devising many From “The Return of Phineas Gage: Clues plans of operation, which are no sooner arranged than they are abandoned in About the Brain from the Skull of a Famous turn for others appearing more feasible. A child in his intellectual capacity Patient,” by H. Damasio, T. Grabowski, R. Frank, A. M. Galaburda, and A. R. Damasio, and manifestations, he has the animal passions of a strong man. (Blumer and 1994, Science, 20, p. 1102. Benson, 1975, p. 153) p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 11

Linking Brain Function to Brain Disease Focus on Disorders With the growth of cities in Europe in the 1500s, there were too many mental patients to be accommodated in home care. The mentally ill were placed in hospitals, monasteries, and other buildings that had been converted into asylums.

There, patients were little more than prisoners. They lived Giraudon/Art Resource, NY under appalling conditions and were often chained and placed on public view. They were poorly treated in part be- cause many people at the time accepted Descartes’s belief Philippe Pinel supervising the unchaining of the insane in La that an entity called the mind controls rational behavior. Bicêtre asylum in Paris in 1793. Having “lost their minds,” these patients were considered lit- tle more than animals. At the end of the 1700s, the French Revolution brought others in the hospital and published them in his textbooks in a new social order and new attitudes toward freedom, in- and journals. Many scientists, attracted by Charcot’s approach, cluding the idea that all people were equal before the law, visited and studied with him, including Sigmund Freud, the even those in asylums. The French physician Philippe Pinel founder of psychoanalysis. They, in turn, began to apply his reformed two large asylums in Paris—La Bicêtre and La method in other countries and in other ways. Many of Char- Salpêtrière. He released many of the patients and greatly im- cot’s coworkers are well known today for the diseases that proved conditions there for those too ill to leave. Neverthe- they identified, many of which were named after them. less, as cities continued to grow, so too did the size of asy- The method of relating nervous system abnormalities to lums. For example, La Salpêtrière (which was originally a behavioral abnormalities contributed greatly to scientific saltpeter factory) was the size of a small city, housing as knowledge. It led to the understanding that an intact brain is many as 5000 female patients. essential for normal behavior. It also showed that, when the When Jean Charcot came to La Salpêtrière Hospital in link between symptoms and nervous system pathology is the 1860s, he and his staff began to document the symptoms known, patients can be diagnosed and given more intelli- of the patients. Then, when the patients later died, the scien- gent treatment. At the same time, systematic study linking tists examined their nervous systems and correlated the brain pathology to behavioral symptoms contributed to the abnormalities that they found with the patients’ previous downfall of Descartes’s view that the pineal body was cen- behavior. Charcot described the results in his lectures to tral to the control of behavior.

This description embraces the idea that we humans are composed of both a mind that controls our rational behavior and a body that expresses our animal passions. The description would be more accurate, though perhaps less colorful, without refer- ring to this dualism of mind and body. After all, people are animals, so all of their be- haviors are animal behaviors. Some behaviors are “high minded,” whereas others are lower, or “animalistic.” Furthermore, this appeal to dualism detracts from the most important aspect of Harlow’s findings. Because Gage’s brain damage was in the frontal lobes, Harlow was providing evidence that the frontal lobes were locations of foresight and planning. “Linking Brain Function to Brain Disease,” above, describes the beginnings of the important discovery that particular parts of the brain regulate specific kinds of behaviors. p

12 CHAPTER 1 Darwin and Materialism By the middle of the nineteenth century, the beginnings of another theory of the brain and behavior were emerging. This theory was the modern perspective of mate- rialism—the idea that rational behavior can be fully explained by the working of the brain and the rest of the nervous system, without any need to refer to a mind that Alfred Wallace controls our actions. This perspective had its roots in the evolutionary theories of Al- (1823–1913) (1809–1892) fred Russel Wallace and Charles Darwin. Wallace and Darwin independently arrived at the same conclusion—the idea that all living things are related. Each outlined this view in a paper presented at the Link to more research about Linnaean Society of London in July 1858. Darwin further elaborated on the topic in Charles Darwin on the Web site at his book titled by Means of , published in www.worthpublishers.com/kolb/chapter1. 1859. This book presented a wealth of supporting detail, which is why Darwin is men- tioned more often as the founder of modern evolutionary theory. Both Darwin and Wallace had looked carefully at the structure of plants and ani- mals and at animal behavior. Despite the diversity of living organisms, they were struck by the number of characteristics common to so many species. For example, the skeleton, muscles, and body parts of humans, monkeys, and other mammals are re- markably similar. These observations led first to the idea that living organisms must be related, an idea widely held even before Wallace and Darwin. But, more impor- tantly, these same observations led to Darwin’s explanation of how the great diversity in the biological world could have come from common ancestry. Darwin’s principle of natural selection proposes that animals have traits in common because traits are passed from parents to their offspring. Darwin believed that all organisms, both living and extinct, are descended from some unknown ancestor that lived in the remote past. In Darwin’s terms, all living things are said to have . As the descendants of that original organism spilled into various habitats over millions of years, they developed different structural and behavioral that made them suited for specific ways of life. But, at the same time, they retained many similar traits that reveal their relatedness to each other. Brain cells are one such characteristic common to animal species. Brain cells are an that emerged only once in animal evolution. Consequently, all brain cells that living animals possess are descendants of that first brain cell. Natural selection is Darwin’s way of explaining how new species evolve and ex- isting species change over time. A species is a group of organisms that can breed among themselves, but not with members of other species. Individual organisms within any given species vary extensively in their characteristics, with no two mem- bers of the species being exactly alike. Some are big, some are small, some are fat, some are fast, some are lightly colored, and some have large teeth. Those individual organisms whose characteristics best help them to survive in their environment are Materialism. The philosophical position that holds that behavior can be explained likely to leave more offspring than are less-fit members. This unequal ability of indi- as a function of the nervous system with- vidual members to survive and reproduce leads to a gradual change in a species’ pop- out explanatory recourse to the mind. ulation, with characteristics favorable for survival in that particular habitat becoming Common descent. Refers to individual more prevalent over generations. If some part of a species population then becomes organisms or families that descend from reproductively isolated—say, because it is separated by a physical barrier such as an the same ancestor. ocean or a mountain range—that subgroup over time could evolve into a new Natural selection. Differential success species, different from the species from which it originated. For example, imagine a in the reproduction of different pheno- chubby primate living in an environment of dense vegetation that is faced with grad- types resulting from the interaction of or- ual drying of the climate so that its environment eventually becomes a savanna with ganisms with their environment. Evolution takes place when natural selection causes only scattered trees. Those members of the species that are thinnest and so can run changes in relative frequencies of alleles quickly, are lightly colored and so blend with the grass, and have fine teeth, which are in the gene pool. better for eating insects, will prosper and so leave the most descendants. p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 13

Neither Darwin nor Wallace understood the basis of the great variation in plant and animal species. The underlying principles of that variation were discovered by another scientist, Gregor Mendel, beginning about 1857, through experiments that he did with pea plants. Mendel deduced that there are heritable factors, which we now call genes, re- lated to the various physical traits displayed by the species. Members of a species that have a particular gene or combination of genes will express that trait. If the genes for a trait are passed on to offspring, the offspring also will have the same characteristic. New traits appear because genes combine in new ways, because existing genes change or mu- tate, or because new genes are formed. Thus, the unequal ability of individual organisms to survive and reproduce is related to the different genes that they inherit and pass on to their offspring. By the same token, similar characteristics within or between species are usually due to similar genes. For instance, genes that produce the nervous system in dif- ferent kinds of animal species tend to be very similar to one another.

Darwin’s Legacy Darwin’s theory of natural selection has three important implications for the study of the brain and behavior. First, because all animal species are related, so too must be their brains. Darwin himself did not say much about this topic, but contemporary scientists do. Today, brain researchers study a wide range of animals, including slugs, fruit flies, rats, and monkeys, knowing that they can often extend their findings to hu- man beings. Second, because all species of animals are related, so too must be their behavior. Darwin was particularly interested in this subject. In his book titled On the Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, he argued that emotional expressions are similar in humans and other animals because we inherited these expressions from a common ancestor. Evidence for such inheritance is illustrated in Figure 1-6, which shows that smiling is common to people throughout the world. That people in differ- ent parts of the world display the same behavior suggests that the trait is inherited rather than learned. The third implication of Darwin’s theory is that both the brain and behavior were built up bit by bit in animals that evolved to greater complexity, as humans obviously did. Later in this chapter, we will trace how the human nervous system evolved from a simple net of nerves, to a spinal cord connected to that net, and finally to a nervous system with a brain that controls behavior. Evidence that the brain controls behavior is today so strong that the idea has the status of a theory: the brain theory. Donald O. Hebb in his influential book titled The Organization of Behavior, published in 1949, described the brain theory as follows: Modern psychology takes completely for granted that behavior and neural function are perfectly correlated, that one is completely caused by the other. There is no separate soul or life force to stick a finger into the brain now and then and make neural cells do what they would not otherwise. (Hebb, 1949, p. xiii)

Figure 1-6 Darwin proposed that emotional expression is inherited. Part of the J. Tisne/Stone Images J. Tisne/Stone O. Benn/Stone Images

A. Cassidy/Stone Images evidence supporting this suggestion is the finding that people from all parts of J. Greenberg/Visuals Unlimited J. Greenberg/Visuals the world use the same emotional expressions that they also recognize in others, as is illustrated by these smiles. p

14 CHAPTER 1

Millions of years ago 4500 3500 2500

PRECAMBRIAN

Origin Origin of life First of single-celled earth animals

Some people reject the idea that the brain is responsible for behavior, because they think it denies religion. Their thinking, however, is mistaken. The biological explanation of brain and behavior is neutral with respect to religious beliefs. Fred Linge, the brain- injured man described in the beginning of this chapter, has strong religious beliefs, as do the other members of his family. They used their religious strength to aid in his re- covery. Yet, despite their religious beliefs, they realized that Linge’s brain injury was the cause of his change in behavior and that the process of recovery that his brain under- went was the cause of his restored health. Similarly, there are many behavioral scientists with strong religious beliefs who see no contradiction between those beliefs and their use of the scientific method to examine the relations between the brain and behavior.

In Review

We have considered three perspectives on how behavior arises. Mentalism is the view that behavior is a product of an intangible entity called the mind, implying that the brain has little importance. Dualism is the notion that the mind acts through the brain to pro- duce language and rational behavior, whereas the brain alone is responsible for the “lower” kinds of actions that we have in common with other animal species. Finally, materialism is the view that all behavior, language and reasoning included, can be fully accounted for by brain function. Materialism is the perspective that guides contemporary research on the brain and behavior.

THE EVOLUTION OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR The popular interpretation of is that we are descended from apes. Ac- tually, apes are not our ancestors, although we are related to them. To demonstrate the difference, consider the following story. Two people named Joan Campbell were intro- duced at a party, and their names afforded a good opening for a conversation. Although both belonged to the Campbell lineage (family line), one Joan was not descended from the other. The two women lived in different parts of North America. One was from Texas and the other was from Ontario, and both their families had been in those locations for many generations. But, after comparing family histories, the two Joans discovered that they had ancestors in common. The Texas Campbells were descended from Jeeves Camp- bell, brother of Matthew Campbell, from whom the Ontario Campbells were descended. Common ancestor. An ancestor from which two or more lineages or family Jeeves and Matthew had both boarded the same fur-trading ship when it stopped for wa- groups arise and so is ancestral to both ter in the Orkney Islands north of Scotland before sailing to North America. The Joan groups. Campbells’ common ancestors, then, were the mother and father of Jeeves and Matthew p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 15

1500 500CENOZOIC 0 PALEOZOIC MESOZOIC

First First First simple nervous brains human systems brains

Campbell. Both the Texas Campbell family line and the Ontario one were descended Figure 1-7 from this same man and woman. In much the same way, humans and apes are descended Relative to the origin of the earth 4500 from common ancestors. But, unlike the Joan Campbells, we do not know who those dis- million years ago, the first single-celled tant relatives were. By comparing the characteristics of humans and related animals, animals 3500 million years ago, the however, scientists are tracing our lineage back farther and farther. In this way, they can origin of neurons 700 million years ago, piece together the story of the origin of the human brain and behavior. and the first brain about 250 million years ago, the appearance of the first human brain within about the past 4 Origin of Brain Cells and Brains million years is a very recent event. The brain and brain cells go back a very long time. The earth originated about 4500 million years ago, and the first life forms arose about 3500 million years in the past. About 700 million years ago, animals evolved the first brain cells, and, by 250 million years ago, the first brain had evolved. A humanlike brain, however, first developed only about 3 million to 4 million years ago, and our modern human brain has been around for only the past 100,000 to 200,000 years. As evolutionary history goes, that is a rather short amount of time. Although life evolved very early in the history of our planet, brain cells and the brain are more recent adaptations, and large complex brains, such as ours, evolved only very, very recently. Figure 1-7 shows this evolution- ary time line.

Classification Systems Since the first appearance of a living organism, the diversity of life on earth has been enormous. Millions of species have evolved, some of which have become extinct. As many as 30 million to 100 million species currently inhabit the planet. Scientists have described only a small number of these species, about 1.5 million. The rest remain to be found, named, and classified. Taxonomy, the branch of biology concerned with naming and classifying species, groups organisms according to their common characteristics and their relationships to one another. As shown in Figure 1-8, the broadest unit of classification is a king- dom, with more subordinate groups being a phylum, class, order, family, genus, and species. We humans belong to the animal kingdom, the chordate phylum, the mam- malian class, the primate order, the family, the genus, and the sapi- ens species. Animals are usually identified with a first and second name, their genus and species name. So we humans are called Homo sapiens, meaning “wise humans.” This hierarchy of categories helps us trace the evolutionary history of our human brain and behavior. Brain cells and muscles first evolved in animals; the brain as an Taxonomy. The branch of biology con- organ first evolved in chordates; a large brain with many different functions first cerned with naming and classifying the evolved in mammals; a brain capable of producing complex tools first evolved in diverse forms of life. p

16 CHAPTER 1

Figure 1-8 Living organisms Taxonomy classifies animals into groups subordinate to more comprehensive groups. (From bottom to top) Modern humans are the only surviving species of the genus that included numerous extinct species of humanlike animals. Kingdom: Animals Humans belong to the ape (Hominidae) Characteristics: Neurons family, which includes a number of living and muscles used for members, among them chimpanzees. locomotion Hominidae are but one of many families of the primate order. The primates are Phylum: Chordates members of the class of mammals, which Characteristics: Brain in turn belongs to the chordate phylum, and spinal cord which in turn belongs to the animal kingdom. A characteristic feature of Class: Mammals animals is a nervous system, and in Characteristics: chordates the nervous system includes a Large brains and brain and spinal cord. A distinctive social behavior feature of mammals is a large brain, and, among the mammals, the primates are a Order: Primates particularly large-brained order, with the Characteristics: Visual largest brains being found in humans. control of hands

Family: Hominidae Characteristics: Tool use

Genus: Human Characteristics: Language

Species: Modern human Characteristics: Complex culture

apes; and a brain capable of language and culture first evolved in Homo sapiens.Al- though the most complex brain and patterns of behavior have evolved in the human lineage, large brains and complex behaviors have also evolved in some other lineages. Some birds, such as the Galápagos woodpecker finch, use simple tools, and dolphins have surprisingly large brains.

Galápagos woodpecker finch Evolution of Animals with Nervous Systems A nervous system is not essential for life. In fact, most organisms in both the past and Cladogram. A that the present have done without one. Of the five major taxonomic kingdoms, only one branches repeatedly, suggesting a classification of organisms based on the contains species with nervous systems. Figure 1-9 shows these five kingdoms in a time sequence in which evolutionary chart called a cladogram (from the Greek word clados, meaning “branch”). A clado- branches arise. gram displays groups of organisms as branches on a tree in such a way that branch p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 17

Brain cells, nervous systems, and Figure 1-9 muscles first evolved in animals. In this cladogram of the five kingdoms of living organisms, the line connecting the kingdoms represents the sequence in which the kingdoms evolved. Animals are members of the most recently evolved kingdom, and their characteristics include brain cells, nervous systems, and muscles. Monera Protista Plantae Fungi Animalia (bacteria) (single cells) (plants) (fungi) (animals)

Muscles and neurons

Multicells

True cells (nuclei and organelles) Common ancestor order represents how the groups are related. The five kingdoms shown are: Monera (simple cells, such as bacteria), Protista (more complex cells, such as protozoa), Plan- tae (plants), Fungi, and Animalia (animals). Looking at this chart tells you at a glance how uncommon the nervous system is in the living world: it is found only in animals. You can also see that neurons are associated with muscles, suggesting that the func- tions of both were to enable movement. This cladogram also reinforces our earlier point that the nervous system is a late evolutionary development. Life first appeared with Monera, and it thrived for millions of years with nothing but single-celled or- ganisms. The first multicellular organisms arose in Plantae and Fungi. Brain cells, ner- vous systems, and muscles did not appear in Animalia until much later. They are structures that life did without for eons. Brain cells, nervous systems, and muscles are what give animal species their char- acteristic feature: the ability to move. This ability has enabled animals to occupy many different biological niches, where they use their movements for food gathering, repro- duction, and self-preservation. The animal kingdom contains a great many species. Taxonomists have so far identified about 1 million animal species and organized them into 15 phyla (phyla is the plural of phylum). Figure 1-10 shows the evolution of the nervous system in animal phyla. The nervous Bilateral symmetry. Refers to organs or system in species in older phyla, such as jellyfishes and sea anemones, is extremely sim- parts that are present on both sides of the ple. It consists of a , with no structure that resembles a brain. (The net looks a body and very similar in appearance on little like a human nervous system from which the brain and spinal cord have been re- each side (For example, the hands are bi- moved). Species in somewhat more recent phyla, such as flatworms, are more complexly laterally symmetrical, whereas the heart is structured. These organisms have heads and tails, are bilaterally symmetrical (one-half not.) of the body is the mirror image of the other), and are segmented (the body is composed Segmentation. Refers to animals that of similarly appearing segments). They also have segmented nervous systems that re- can be divided into a number of parts that are similar; also refers to the idea that semble the human nervous system, with sensory and motor neurons projecting from many animals, including , are each segment. Bilateral symmetry and segmentation are two important structural composed of similarly organized body features of the human nervous system. For example, just as our body is bilaterally segments. p

18 CHAPTER 1

Nerve net: Simple nervous Segmented nerve trunk: Ganglia: Structures that Brain: True system, organized as a net, Bilaterally symmetrical resemble and function brain and with no brain organization somewhat like a brain spinal cord

Sea Flatworm Squid Frog anemone

Ganglia

Common ancestor symmetrical, our brain has two bilaterally symmetrical hemispheres. The brain Figure 1-10 itself evolved by growth in the most anterior (front) segments of the nervous system. Species in still more recently evolved phyla, such as clams, snails, and octopuses, have This cladogram illustrates the an additional feature in their nervous systems: collections of neurons called ganglia (sin- evolutionary relationship of the 15 animal phyla, showing the evolution of gular, ganglion), some of which are in the region of the animal’s head. Ganglia resemble a the nervous system from a nerve net, to brain and function somewhat like one, but, unlike a brain, they are not a central struc- a segmented nervous system, to a ture coordinating all of the animal’s behavior. Only species in one phylum, the chor- nervous system consisting of ganglia and dates, have a true spinal cord and brain. Chordates get their name from the notochord, a nerve trunks, and finally to a nervous flexible rod that runs the length of the back. In humans, the notochord is present only in system featuring a brain. an ; by birth, it has been replaced by vertebrae that encase the spinal cord. There are several important differences between the chordate nervous system and the nervous system in older phyla. Whereas the chordate nervous system is located along the back, in other animals the nervous system is located below the gut. In addi- tion, the brain is a central processing structure in the chordate nervous system. It is lo- cated in the animal’s head (the body part that arrives first as the animal moves). It is also in close proximity to the animal’s many sensory-receptor systems, such as those for vi- sion, hearing, taste, and smell. This positioning of the brain makes it quick to receive and respond to sensory information as the animal travels from one place to another.

The Chordate Nervous System Although much variation exists in the nervous systems of chordates, the basic pattern of a brain attached to a spinal cord is common to all of them. This pattern is found even in the earliest chordate species. Figure 1-11 shows seven of the nine classes of chordates to which the approximately 38,500 chordate species belong. In each class, the nervous system consists p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 19

Agnatha Chondrichthyes Osteichthyes Amphibia Reptilia Aves Mammalia (lampreys and hagfish) (sharks and rays) (bony fishes) (frogs and salamanders) (reptiles) (birds) (mammals)

Large brains

Figure 1-11 Limbs This cladogram illustrates the evolutionary relationship between classes of chordates, animals having a brain and a spinal cord. Common increased with the development of limbs ancestor in amphibia. Birds and mammals are the most recently evolved chordates, and large brains are found in both classes. of a brain and a spinal cord as well as sensory and motor connections, but, in each class, there is an enormous range of brain sizes. Distinguishing features of chordates are limbs for locomotion along with some very large-brained species, especially the birds and mammals. Turn on the CD to view a close-up The relative difference in brain size in different classes of chordates is illustrated of the in the module on the in Figure 1-12, which shows representative brains of a fish, an amphibian, a bird, and Central Nervous System in the section on a mammal (in this case, a human). The , of which the cerebral cortex is the the brainstem and subcortical structures. external structure (described earlier as being folded and covering most of the rest of the brain in humans), is proportionately small and smooth in the earliest-evolved chordate shown in Figure 1-12. In later-evolved chordates with larger brains, the cor- Figure 1-12 tex is disproportionately larger and begins to cover other structures. Finally, in large- The brains of representative chordates brained mammals, the cortex is folded. By folding, the cortex is able to greatly in- have many structures in common. These crease its size while still fitting into a small skull (just as a folded piece of paper can side views of the brains of some repre- occupy a small container). Toward the back of the brain, a structure called the cere- sentative chordates show that the cere- brum and cerebellum account for most bellum (which means “little brain” in Latin) also has increased in size, taking on the of the increase in brain size. The human appearance of a little cortex. Figure 1-12 shows the human brain cut longitudinally brain has been cut through the center to through its center to illustrate how the cortex has grown to cover the rest of the brain, illustrate how the cerebral cortex enfolds including the cerebellum. The evolution of more complex behavior in chordates is the rest of the brain. The similar parts of closely related to the evolution of both the cerebral cortex and the cerebellum. The the brain found in these diverse animal large differences in brain size that exist in the 4000 species of mammals are due species also illustrate that there is a basic mainly to differences in the size of these two brain regions. brain plan.

Cerebrum Cerebellum Cerebrum Cerebellum Cerebrum Cerebellum Cerebrum Cerebellum

Fish FrogBird Human p

20 CHAPTER 1

In Review

Brain cells and the nervous system are relatively recent developments in the evolution of life on this planet. Because they evolved only once in the animal kingdom, a similar basic nervous system pattern exists in all animals. The nervous system did become more complex in certain groups of animals, and this increase in complexity closely parallels increasingly complex behavior. Particular lineages of animals, such as the lineage to which humans belong, are characterized by especially large brains and complex behav- ior. These evolutionary developments are closely tied to the growth of the cerebral cortex and cerebellum.

HUMAN EVOLUTION Although everyone can see similarities among humans, apes, and monkeys, many Visit the Web site for links to a people once believed that humans are far too different from monkeys and apes to tutorial about human evolution at have had a common ancestor with them. These skeptics also reasoned that the ab- www.worthpublishers.com/kolb/ sence of a “missing link,” or intermediate form of ancestor, further argued against the chapter1. possibility of common descent. In the past century, however, so many intermediate forms between humans and other apes have been found in the record that entire books are required to describe them. Here we consider only some of the more promi- nent ancestors that link apes to ourselves. Figure 1-13 This cladogram illustrates a hypothetical Humans: Members of the Primate Order relationship between the members of The human relationship to apes and monkeys places us in the primate order, a subcat- the primate order. Humans are members of the family of apes. In general, brain egory of mammals that includes not only apes and monkeys, but lemurs, tarsiers, and size increases across the groupings, with marmosets as well. In fact, we humans are only 1 of about 275 species in the primate humans having the largest brains. order, some of which are illustrated in Figure 1-13. Primates have excellent vision— including color vision and eyes in the front of the face to enhance depth perception—

Lemurs Tarsiers New World Old World Gibbons Orangutans Gorillas Chimpanzees Humans and lorises monkeys monkeys

Apes

Common ancestor p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 21 and they use this excellent vision to deftly guide their hand movements. Female pri- Hominid. General term referring to pri- mates usually have only one infant per pregnancy, and they spend a great deal more mates that walk upright, including all time caring for their young than most other animals do. Associated with their skillful forms of humans, living and extinct. movements and their highly social nature, primates on average have larger brains than animals in other orders of mammals, such as rodents (mice, rats, beavers, squir- rels) and carnivores (wolves, bears, cats, weasels). Humans are members of the suborder apes, which in addition to us includes gib- bons, orangutans, gorillas, and chimpanzees. Apes are arboreal animals, with limber shoulder joints that allow them to brachiate (swing from one handhold to another) in trees. Among the apes, we are most closely related to the chimpanzee, having had a common ancestor between 5 million and 10 million years ago. The family to which humans belong is called Hominidae. In the past 5 million years, there have been many hominids, or humanlike animals that are members of the Hominidae family. Some extinct hominid species lived at the same time as one another. At present, however, we are the only surviving hominid species.

Australopithecus: Our Distant Ancestor One of our hominid ancestors is probably Australopithecus (Australo meaning “southern,” pithecus meaning “ape”) or a primate very much like it. A reconstruction of what the animal looked like is shown in Figure 1-14. The name Australopithecus was coined by an Australian, Raymond Dart, for the skull of a child that he found in a box of fossilized remains from a limestone quarry near Taung, South Africa, in 1924. (The choice of a name to represent his native land is probably not accidental.) We now know that there were many species of Australopithecus, some of which existed at the same time. The skull of the “” did not belong to the earliest species, which lived about 4 million years ago. These early australopiths are the first primates to show a distinctly human charac- teristic: they walked upright. Scientists have deduced their upright posture from the shape of their back, pelvic, knee, and foot bones and from a set of 3.6-million- to 3.8-million-year-old fossilized footprints that a family of them left behind when walk- Figure 1-14 ing through freshly fallen ash from a volcano. The footprints feature a well-developed arch and a big toe that was more like that of humans than of apes. The most complete Reconstruction of Australopithecus, one of the oldest known species in the Australopithecus skeleton yet found is that of a young female, popularly known as hominid family, to which modern humans “Lucy.” This skeleton was 40 percent complete even after having been buried for 3 mil- belong. Australopithecus walked upright lion years. Lucy was only about 1 meter tall and had a brain about the size of a modern with free hands, as do modern humans, chimpanzee’s brain, which is about one-third the size of a modern human brain. but its brain was the size of an ape’s, The evolutionary sequence from Australopithecus to humans is not known pre- about one-third the size of our brain. cisely, in part because there were a number of species of Australopithecus alive at the same time. One possible lineage is shown in Figure 1-15. A common ancestor gave rise to Australopithecus, and one member of this group gave rise to the Homo lineage. The last of the australopith species disappeared from the fossil record about 1 million years ago after coexisting with hominids for some time. Also illustrated in Figure 1-15 is the large increase in brain size that evolved in the hominid lineage.

The First Humans The oldest to be designated as Homo, or human, are those found by Mary and Louis Leakey in the Olduvai Gorge in Tanzania in 1964, dated at about 2 million years. The primates that left these skeletal remains had a strong resemblance to Australopithecus, from which they were thought to descend. But Mary Leakey argued that their dental pat- tern is more similar to that of modern humans than to that of australopiths and, more p

22 CHAPTER 1

1600 A. robustus H. neanderthalensis A. afarensis 1400 H. sapiens

1200 A. africanus H. erectus 1000 Common ancestor 800 H. habilis H. habilis 600 Common A. africanus H. erectus ancestor Brain size (in cubic centimeters) 400 H. neanderthalensis 200 H. sapiens 0 43210 Millions of years ago

Figure 1-15 The origins of humans. The human lineage and a lineage of extinct Australopithecus likely arose from a important, that they apparently made simple stone tools, which were found near their common ancestor about 4 million years bones. The Leakeys named the species (meaning “handy human”) to sig- ago. Thus the ancestor of the human nify that its members were tool users. Again, the precise relationships in the Homo lineage lineage Homo was likely an animal are not known, because there were a number of species of Homo alive at the same time. similar to Australopithecus africanus. The The first humans whose populations spread beyond Africa migrated into Europe probable sequence of human evolution and into Asia. This species was (“upright human”), so named because was from Homo habilis to Homo erectus of the mistaken notion that its predecessor, Homo habilis, had a stooped posture. to Homo sapiens. Connecting lines are not shown, because a number of species Homo erectus first shows up in the fossil record about 1.6 million years ago and lasts of each group were alive at the same until perhaps as recently as 100,000 to 30,000 years ago. Homo erectus has a pivotal time. Homo neanderthalensis is position in our evolutionary history. Its brain was bigger than that of any previous considered a subspecies of Homo sapiens, hominid, overlapping in size the measurements of present-day human brains. Homo but how closely they were related is erectus also made more sophisticated tools than did Homo habilis. unclear. There was a very large increase Modern humans, Homo sapiens, appeared in Asia and North Africa about 200,000 to in brain size in this proposed lineage. 100,000 years ago and in Europe about 40,000 years ago. Most anthropologists think that they migrated from Africa. Until about 60,000 years ago, perhaps even as recently as 30,000 years ago, they coexisted with other hominid species in Africa, Europe, and Asia. For example, in Europe they coexisted with , named after Neander, Ger- many, where the first skulls were found. Neanderthals had brains as large as or larger than those of modern humans, used tools similar to those of early Homo sapi- ens, and possibly had a similar hunting culture. We do not know how Homo sapiens com- pletely replaced other human species, such as Neanderthals. Homo sapiens may have been more aggressive and killed off competing species. Or they may have been more skilled at toolmaking and so were better at getting food. Or perhaps the various human species in- terbred so completely that characteristics of all but ourselves simply disappeared. Homo sapiens “Lucy” The Evolution of the Human Brain Scientists who study the evolution of the brain propose that a relative increase in the size and complexity of the brain in different species is what enabled the evolution of AFRICA more complex behavior. In this section, we consider the relation between brain size and behavior across different species. We also consider a number of hypotheses about how the human brain became so large. p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 23

BRAIN SIZE AND BEHAVIOR Principle of proper mass. The idea In his book titled The Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence, published in 1973, H. J. that complex behavioral functions are Jerison uses the principle of proper mass to sum up the relation between increased produced by a larger brain or brain region size of the nervous system and increased complexity of behavior. This principle than that in which simple behavioral states that the amount of neural tissue responsible for a particular function is equiv- functions are produced; usually used to refer to the idea that the brain size of alent to the amount of processing that the function requires. So, as behaviors be- an animal species is proportional to its come more complex and require more neural processing, a greater amount of neural behavioral complexity. tissue must be allocated to them. It follows that species exhibiting more complex behaviors will possess relatively larger brains than those of species whose behaviors are simpler. Jerison found support for the principle of proper mass by comparing brain size in a wide variety of animal species. Jerison calculated that, as body size increases, the size of the brain increases at about two-thirds the increase in body weight. With the use of this formula, plus an average brain-volume-to-body-weight ratio as a base, it is possible to determine the expected brain size for a mammal of any given weight. This expected brain size is plotted in the diagonal line shown in Figure 1-16, a graph in which body size is on the x-axis and brain size on the y-axis. The diagonal line repre- sents the expected increase in brain size as body size increases. The polygon sur- rounding the diagonal line encompasses the brain and body sizes of all mammals. The actual brain and body sizes of a number of representative animals also are plotted on this graph. Animals that lie below the diagonal line have brains that are below average for an animal of that size, whereas animals that lie above the diagonal line have brains that are larger than expected for an animal of that size. Notice that the rat has a brain that is a little smaller and the elephant a brain that is a little larger than expected. No- tice also that a modern human is located farther to the upper left than any other ani- mal, indicating a brain that is relatively larger for its body size than that of any other animal. To further illustrate the relative sizes of brains, Jerison developed a numerical system that eliminates body weight as a factor and adjusts brain sizes according to a

10,000 The position of the modern Homo sapiens Porpoise Elephant Figure 1-16 5000 human brain, at the farthest upper left, indicates it has the Blue Brain and body sizes of some common largest relative brain size. whale mammals. The axes are in logarithmic 1000 Gorilla units to represent the wide range of 500 Australopithecus Chimpanzee body and brain sizes. The polygon Baboon Lion includes the brain and body sizes of all 100 Wolf 50 mammals. The line through the polygon Cat illustrates the expected increase in brain 10.0 size as body weight increases. The 5.0 labeled dots show the brain and body

Brain weight (in grams) Vampire bat Opossum sizes of a number of representative 1.0 Rat species. Animals that lie above the 0.5 Deviation from the diagonal diagonal line have brain sizes that are Mole The average brain size line indicates either larger larger than would be expected for an (above) or smaller (below) 0.1 relative to body weight animal of that size. Modern humans brain size than average, 0.05 is located along the have the largest brain relative to body diagonal line. relative to body weight. size of all mammals. Adapted from The Evolution of the Brain and 0.001 0.01 0.1 1 10 100 1000 10,000 100,000 Intelligence (p. 175), by H. J. Jerison, 1973, New Body weight (in kilograms) York: Academic Press. p

24 CHAPTER 1

scaling factor. He calls this system an encephalization quotient (EQ). Figure 1-17 Encephalization quotient (EQ). A measure of brain size obtained from the (top) lists the EQs for several common animals. Notice that a rat has an EQ about ratio of actual brain size to the expected one-half that of a cat, which is representative of the average mammal. Interestingly, a brain size for an animal of a particular crow has an EQ similar to that of a monkey, and a dolphin has a very large EQ indeed. body size. People who study crows would agree that they are intelligent, whereas people who study dolphins, although admitting that they are intelligent social creatures, are still uncertain about the reason for their need of such a large brain. Figure 1-17 (bottom) Rat lists the EQs for a number of species in the human lineage. Clearly, we modern humans are descended from a lineage of large-brained animals, but we have the Cat largest brain. Elephant Underlying the principle of proper mass is the idea that a larger brain is needed Common animals for increasingly complex behavior. Although it is not always easy to compare the com- Crow plexity of behavior in different animal species, there are some fairly obvious examples Fruit bat of more complex behaviors that correlate with greater brain size. These examples are seen as we progress up the chordate ladder from older to more recent groups of ani- Dolphin mals. For instance, among the older chordates, cyclostomes, such as the lamprey, move by making snakelike, side-to-side body movements, whereas the more recent Monkey fish have fins with which to move, in addition to using these lateral movements of the body. Fish, significantly, have relatively larger brains than those of cyclostomes. Fins Chimpanzee evolved into limbs in amphibians, and these limbs are used in a more complex way

Australopithecus than fins are to enable walking on land. Amphibians, significantly, have relatively Human larger brains than those of fish. Birds and mammals use their limbs for still more lineage Homo habilis complex movements, both for locomotion and for handling objects. Birds and mam- mals, significantly, have relatively larger brains than those of amphibians. Being a pri- Homo erectus mate is associated with many other behavioral innovations. Primates engage in com- plex social and sexual behavior, as well as complex feeding habits, care of their young, Homo sapiens defense of their territories, and, in regard to humans, the use of language. Primates, significantly, have relatively larger brains than other mammals do. 01234 567 Encephalization quotients Why is there this trend toward greater brain size and behavioral complexity in chordates? Why, in the course of evolutionary history, did animals not retain very simple nervous systems? A number of factors have created relatively constant oppor- Figure 1-17 tunities and pressures for animals to modify their behavior and thus their nervous The EQ (encephalization quotient) of systems. For example, because the first animals were relatively simple, they left a wide (top) some common animals and (bottom) variety of more complex behaviors available as potential means of gaining a survival members of the human lineage. advantage. Filling these behavioral niches was often favored through natural selection. In addition, various cataclysmic events have wiped out large numbers of species, changing the environment and offering opportunities for new evolutionary adapta- tions. For example, a comet that struck the earth about 60 million years ago probably led to the and opened up new opportunities for the rapid evolu- tion of mammals. There have also been many changes in the earth’s landmass and cli- mate that have challenged animals to evolve in order to adapt and survive. As a result of all these factors, behavioral and structural complexity has grown.

WHY THE HOMINID BRAIN ENLARGED Sea lamprey The evolution of modern humans—from the time when humanlike creatures first ap- peared until the time when humans like ourselves existed—took about 5 million years. As illustrated by the relative size differences of skulls presented in Figure 1-18, much of this evolution entailed a change in brain size, which was accompanied by changes in be- havior. There was a nearly threefold increase in brain size from apes (EQ 2.5) to mod- ern humans (EQ 7.0). What caused this substantial growth of the brain? Most likely, the p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 25 appearance of each new hominid species was associated with climate changes that produced new environments that isolated populations of existing hominids and pro- duced a rapid selection of traits that were adaptive for the new environment. The first of these climate changes was triggered about 8 million years ago. Before that time, most of Africa was a rich forest inhabited by monkeys and apes, as well as other animal species. Then a massive tectonic event (a deformation of the earth’s crust) produced the Great Rift Valley, which runs from south to north across the African continent. This reshaping of the African landmass left a wet jungle climate to the west and a much drier climate to the east. To the west, the apes continued unchanged in their former habitat, but, in the drier re- K. O’Farrell/Concepts gion to the east, apes had to evolve rapidly to adapt to the mixture of tree-covered and Figure 1-18 grassy regions that formed their new home. An upright posture is an efficient means of In the course of human evolution, the rapid locomotion across grass-covered areas. Such an upright posture may have relative size of the brain increased evolved in Australopithecus because these animals were forced to spend more time on threefold, as illustrated here by a the ground moving between clumps of trees. In addition, an upright posture may have comparison between Australopithecus helped to regulate body temperature by reducing the amount of the body’s surface di- afarensis (left), Homo erectus (center), rectly exposed to the sun. and a modern human. The part of the Just before the appearance of Homo habilis, the African climate changed again, Australopithecus skull shown in blue becoming drier, with increasing amounts of grassland and even fewer trees. Anthro- is missing. pologists speculate that a group of hominids that evolved into Homo habilis adapted From The Origin of Modern Humans (p. 165), by R. Lewin, 1998, New York: Scientific American to this new habitat by becoming scavengers on the dead of the large herds of grazing Library. animals that then roamed the open grasslands. The appearance of Homo erectus may have been associated with further change in climate that opened up land bridges into Europe and Asia. At the same time, the new hominids added hunting to their behav- ioral skills and were constantly upgrading the quality of their tools for killing, skin- ning, and butchering animals. Archeologists think there were a number of migrations AFRICA y of hominids from Africa into other parts of the world, with modern humans being e ll a V the last of these migrants. Each of the migrations may have been forced by changes in t f i

R climate that altered the habitat to which the animals residing there were adapted. In t

a Dry e

any case, modern humans eventually replaced all other species of hominids every- r G where on earth. Wet At least three factors are thought to be related to the development of a larger brain as our species evolved. The first is the primate life style, which favored a more complex nervous system. The second is the development of a new way of cooling a larger brain mass. And the third is neoteny, a process by which maturation is delayed and an adult retains infant characteristics.

The Primate Life Style That the primate life style favors a larger brain can be illus- trated by examining how primates forage for food. Foraging is a very important activ- ity for all animals, but some foraging activities are quite simple, whereas others are more complex. Eating grass or vegetation is not an especially difficult task; if there is lots of vegetation, an animal need only munch. Vegetation eaters do not have espe- cially large brains. Among the apes, gorillas, which are mainly vegetation eaters, have relatively small brains. In contrast, apes that eat fruit, such as chimpanzees, have rela- tively large brains. The relation between fruit foraging and larger brain size can be seen in a study by Katharine Milton (1993), who examined the feeding behavior and p

26 CHAPTER 1

brain size of two South American (New World) monkeys that have the same body size—the spider monkey and the howler monkey. As is illustrated in Figure 1-19, the spider monkey obtains 72 percent of its nutrients from eating fruit and has a brain that is twice as large as that of the howler monkey, which obtains only 42 percent of its nutrients from fruit. What is so special about eating fruit that favors a larger brain? The answer is not that fruit contains a brain-, although fruit is a source of sugar, on which the brain depends for energy. The answer is that foraging for fruit is a difficult, com- plex activity. Unlike plentiful vegetation within easy reach on the ground, fruit grows on trees, and only on certain trees, in certain seasons. There are many kinds of fruit, some better for eating than others, and many different animals and insects compete for a fruit crop. Moreover, after a fruit crop has been eaten, it takes time for a new crop to grow. Each of these factors poses a challenge for an animal that eats mostly fruit. Good sensory skills, such as color vision, are needed to recognize ripe fruit in a tree, and good motor skills are required to reach and manipulate it. Good spatial skills are needed to navigate to trees that contain fruit. Good memory skills are required to remember where fruit trees are, when the fruit will be ripe, and in which trees the fruit has already been eaten. Fruit eaters have to be prepared to deal with competitors, including members of their own species, who also want the fruit. To keep track of ripening fruit, it also benefits a fruit eater to have friends who can help search. As a re- sult, successful fruit-eating animals tend to have complex social relations and a means of communicating with others of their species. In addition, it is very helpful for a fruit eater to have a parent who can teach fruit-finding skills, so it is useful to be both a good learner and a good teacher. Each of these abilities requires the evolution of new brain areas or more brain cells in existing brain regions. Added up, more brain cells produce a larger brain.

Figure 1-19 Spider monkey diet Katharine Milton examined the feeding behavior and brain size of two South A spider monkey,

American (New World) monkeys that A. Souders/Corbis P. with a brain size have the same body size. She found that Fruit of 107 g, obtains 72 percent of its the spider monkey obtains 72 percent of nutrients from fruit. its nutrients from eating fruit and that it has a brain that is twice the size of that of the howler monkey, which obtains Flowers only 42 percent of its nutrients from fruit. Leaves

Howler monkey diet

A howler monkey, K. Schafer/Corbis with a brain size of Fruit 50 g, obtains only 42 percent of its nutrients from fruit. Flowers

Leaves p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 27

We humans are fruit eaters and we are descended from fruit eaters, so we are de- Neoteny. A process in which maturation scended from animals with large brains. In our evolution, we also exploited and elabo- is delayed, so an adult retains infant char- rated fruit-eating skills to obtain other temporary and perishable food items as we acteristics; the idea derived from the ob- scavenged, hunted, and gathered. These new food-getting efforts required navigating servation that newly evolved species re- for long distances, and they required recognition of a variety of food sources. At the semble the young of their ancestors. same time, they required making tools for digging up food, killing animals, cutting skin, and breaking bones. These tasks also required a good deal of cooperative behav- ior. The elaboration of all of these skills necessitated new brain areas or more brain cells in existing brain regions. Added up, more brain cells produce an even larger brain.

The Radiator Hypothesis An event that may have given a special boost to greater brain size in our human ancestors was a new form of brain cooling. Dean Falk (1990), a neuropsychologist who studies brain evolution, came up with this idea from something that her car mechanic told her. He said that, to increase the size of a car’s engine, you have to also increase the size of the radiator that cools it. Consequently, Falk proposed the radiator hypothesis, the theory that a change in blood flow around the brain in- creases the rate at which the brain is cooled, thus allowing the brain to become larger. Why is brain cooling so important? The answer is the tremendous amount of work done by a human brain. Although your brain makes up less than 2 percent of your body, it uses 25 percent of your body’s oxygen and 70 percent of its glucose. As a result of all this metabolic activity, your brain generates a great deal of heat and is at risk of overheating under conditions of exercise or heat stress. This risk of overheating, Falk argues, places a limit on how big the brain can be. In animals with less efficient brain-cooling systems than we have, the size limit on the brain is even lower. This more stringent constraint has kept the brain of the chimpanzee at its current size. Falk speculates that a more efficient brain-cooling mechanism arose in the hom- inid line between Australopithecus and more humanlike species, such as Homo habilis. When examining hominid skeletons, Falk noticed that, unlike australopith skulls, Homo skulls contain holes through which blood vessels pass. These holes suggest that Homo species had a much more widely dispersed blood flow from the brain than did earlier hominids, and this more widely dispersed blood flow would have greatly en- hanced brain cooling. The increase in brain cooling, in turn, would have allowed the brain to become larger, which it apparently did in response to evolutionary pressures posed by the new environments exploited by these animals.

Neoteny One mechanism through which an increase in brain size could have taken place is through neoteny, in which a species’ rate of maturation slows down, so juve- nile stages of predecessors become the adult features of descendants. Because the head of an infant is large relative to body size, this process would have led to adults with larger skulls and larger brains. Many other features of human anatomy, besides a large brain-size-to-body-size ratio, also link us with the juve- nile stages of other primates. Figure 1-20 These features include a small An adult human more closely resembles face, a vaulted cranium, an un- a juvenile chimpanzee than an adult R. Stacks/Index Stock rotated big toe, an upright pos- C. A. Schmidecker/FPG chimp. The rounder head of the baby ture, and a primary distribution chimpanzee compared with the more of hair on the head, armpits, elongated head of the adult chimpanzee and pubic areas. Figure 1-20 leads to the hypothesis that we humans illustrates that the head shape may be neotenic descendants of our of a baby chimpanzee is more more apelike distant ancestors. p

28 CHAPTER 1

similar to an adult human head shape than it is to the head shape of an adult chim- panzee. We also retain behavioral features of primate infants, including play, explo- ration, and an intense interest in learning new things. Neoteny is a very common occurrence in the animal world. Domesticated dogs are thought to be neotenic wolves, and sheep are thought to be neotenic goats. Another aspect of neoteny in relation to human brain development is that a slow- ing down of human maturation would have allowed more time for brain cells to be produced (McKinney, 1998). Most brain cells in humans develop just before and after birth, so an extended prenatal and neonatal period would have prolonged the stage of life in which brain cells are developing. This, in turn, would have enabled the creation of increased numbers of brain cells.

In Review

Constant changes in the climate and physical features of the earth have eliminated cer- tain animal species and created new opportunities for other species to emerge. The large human brain evolved in response to a number of pressures and opportunities. They included changes in climate, the appearance of new food resources to exploit, a more widely dispersed blood flow from the brain that enabled better brain cooling, and the retention of certain juvenile physical and behavioral traits.

STUDYING BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR IN MODERN HUMANS So far, we have taken an evolutionary approach in exploring the human brain. But be- cause this approach is designed mainly for comparisons between species, special care must be taken in extending its principles to comparisons within species, especially comparisons within groups of modern humans. We will illustrate the difficulty of within-species comparisons by considering attempts to correlate human brain size with intelligence. Then we will turn to another aspect of studying the brain and be- havior in modern humans—the fact that, unlike the behavior of other animal species, so much of modern human behavior is culturally learned.

Human Brain-Size Comparisons In comparisons between animal species, larger brain size correlates with more com- plex behavior. Does the same relation hold true in comparisons between individual members of a single species? For instance, do people with the largest brains display the most complex and intelligent behavior? Stephen Jay Gould (1981), in his book ti- tled The Mismeasure of Man, reviewed numerous attempts to correlate human brain size and intelligence. He is critical of this research because its logic and methods leave conclusions completely muddled. For one thing, it is difficult to determine how to measure the size of a per- son’s brain. If a tape measure is simply placed around a person’s head, it is impos- sible to factor out the thickness of the skull. There is also no agreement about whether volume or weight is a better indicator of brain size. And, no matter which indicator we use, we must consider as well the relation between body size and brain size. For instance, the human brain varies in weight from about 1000 p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 29 grams to more than 2000 grams, but people also vary in body mass. To what ex- Species-typical behavior. A behavior tent should we factor in body mass in deciding if a particular brain is large or that is characteristic of all members of a small? And how should we measure the mass of the body, given that a person’s to- species. tal weight can fluctuate quite widely? There is also the matter of when in life brain size should be measured, because age and health affect the brain’s mass. If we wait until after death to measure a brain, the cause of death, the water content of the brain, and the time after death will all affect the results. And, even if the problems of measurement could be solved, there remains the question of what is causing what. Exposure to a complex environment can promote growth in exist- ing brain cells. So, if larger brains are found to correlate with higher intelligence, does the complex problem solving cause the greater brain mass or does the greater brain mass enable the more complex behavior? As if these matters were not perplexing enough, there is also the question of what is meant by intelligence. When we compare the behavior of different species, we are comparing species-typical behavior—in other words, behavior displayed by all members of a species. When we compare behavior within a species, however, we are usually comparing how well one individual member performs a certain task in relation to other members. This comparison is a completely different kind of mea- sure. In addition, individual performance on a task is influenced by many factors unrelated to inherent ability, such as interest level, training, motivation, and health. People vary enormously in their individual abilities, depending on the particular task. One person may have superior verbal skills but mediocre spatial abilities, whereas another person may be adept at solving spatial puzzles but struggle with written work, and still another excels at mathematical reasoning but is average in everything else. Which of these people should we consider the most intelligent? Should certain skills get greater weight as measures of intelligence? Clearly, it is diffi- cult to say. Given these questions, it is not surprising that brain size and intelligence within the human species do not seem particularly related to each other. People who virtu- ally everyone agrees are very intelligent have been found to have brains that vary in size from the low end to the high end of the range for our species. For instance, the brilliant physicist Albert Einstein had a brain of average size. Similarly, women have brains that weigh about 10 percent less than men’s brains (roughly equivalent to the average difference in female and male body size), but there is little evidence that the two sexes differ in average intelligence. The lack of correlation between brain size and intelligence within a single species is found not only in humans. For instance, Figure 1-21 plots the average brain size in Figure 1-21 100 In a comparison of intelligence rankings and brain size in breeds of dogs— ranked) (Lower- 80 animals that are all members of the same Bulldog Pekinese species—the correlation is r = 0.009, Old English which indicates no relationship (an r = 1 60 St. Bernard Dachshund sheepdog would show a perfect relationship). If your dog does not show well, do not be

Intelligence 40 distressed, because there are a lot of problems in determining dog intelligence. 20 Intelligence rankings are from The Miniature Standard Labrador Intelligence of Dogs, by S. Coren, 1994, schnauzer poodle retriever Toronto: The Free Press. Size measures are ranked) (Higher- 0 from “Brain Weight–Body Weight Scaling in 40 60 80 100 120 140 Dogs and Cats,” by R. T. Bronson, 1979, Brain weight (in grams) Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 16, 227–236. p

30 CHAPTER 1

different breeds of dogs against each breed’s level of intelligence as ranked by dog ex- perts. The brain sizes, which range from less than 50 grams to nearly 130 grams, were not adjusted for the breeds’ body sizes, because such adjustments are not made in studies of humans. As you can see, there is no relation at all between the overall size of a breed’s brain and that breed’s intelligence ranking. There must be differences of some kind in the brains of individual persons because people differ in behavior and talents. At present, though, we are not yet sure of what structural or functional measures are related to behavioral traits. However, it is very unlikely that gross brain size will provide an accurate or useful measure. Researchers who study this question believe that measures of the relative size and function of particular brain regions will be more helpful. Important, too, will probably be differences in how the various parts of the brain are organized and interrelated.

Culture The most remarkable thing about the brains of modern humans is that they have al- lowed us to develop an extraordinarily rich culture. Human culture consists of the complex learned behaviors characteristic of a group of people who pass those behav- iors on to one another and from generation to generation. Biologist G. P. Murdock compiled the following list, in alphabetical order, of major categories of behavior that are part of human culture:

Age-grading, athletic sports, bodily adornment, calendar [use], cleanli- ness training, community organization, cooking, cooperative labor, cos- mology, courtship, dancing, decorative art, divination, division of labor, dream interpretation, education, eschatology, ethics, ethnobotany, eti- quette, faith healing, family feasting, fire making, folklore, food taboos, funeral rites, games, gestures, gift giving, government, greetings, hair styles, hospitality, housing, hygiene, incest taboos, inheritance rules, jok- ing, kin groups, kinship nomenclature, language, law, luck, superstitions, magic, marriage, mealtimes, medicine, obstetrics, penal sanctions, per- sonal names, population policy, postnatal care, pregnancy usages, prop- erty rights, propitiation of supernatural beings, puberty customs, reli- gious ritual, residence rules, sexual restrictions, soul concepts, status differentiation, surgery, toolmaking, trade, visiting, weaving, and weather control. (Murdock, 1945)

Not all the items in this list are unique to humans. Many other animal species display elements of some of these behaviors. For example, many other animals display age grading (any age-related behavior or status), courtship behavior, and rudimentary el- ements of language. To the extent that the behaviors of other animals are similar to those of humans, it is likely that humans inherited both the behaviors and the related brain circuitry. Despite such behavioral similarities across species, humans clearly have pro- gressed much further in the development of culture than other animals have. For humans, every category of activity on Murdock’s list requires extensive learning from other members of the species, and exactly how each behavior is performed Culture. Behaviors that are learned and can differ widely from one group of people to another. A human brain must func- passed on from one generation to the next tion adequately to acquire these complex cultural skills. When its functioning is in- through teaching and learning. adequate, a person may be unable to learn even basic elements of culture. “Learning p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 31

Learning Disabilities Focus on Disorders During the first third of their lives, children expend much In more recent times, Norman Geshwind and Albert energy in learning a society’s culture, and acquiring the lan- Galaburda proposed a way in which such an impairment guage skills of their culture is a challenge that we expect might come about. These researchers were struck by the them to meet. Yet some people have difficulties in mastering finding that dyslexia is far more common in boys than in language-related tasks. These difficulties and others that af- girls. Perhaps, they reasoned, excessive amounts of the fect performance in school are classified under the “um- hormone testosterone, which produces male physical char- brella” of learning disabilities. acteristics early in development, might also produce ab- One of the most common learning disabilities is dyslexia normal development in language areas of the brain. Pursu- (from the Latin dys, meaning “poor,” and lexia, meaning “read- ing this hypothesis, they examined the brains of a small ing”), an impairment in learning to read. Not surprisingly, chil- sample of dyslexic people who had died. They found ab- dren with dyslexia have difficulty learning to write as well as to normal collections of neurons, or “warts,” in the language read. In 1895, James Hinshelwood, an eye surgeon, examined areas of the left hemisphere. This relation between struc- some schoolchildren who were having reading problems, but tural abnormalities in the brain and learning difficulties is he could find nothing wrong with their vision. Hinshelwood further evidence that an intact brain is necessary for nor- was the first person to suggest that these children had an im- mal human functioning. pairment in brain areas associated with the use of language.

Disabilities,” above, describes how incapacitating it can be to have a brain that has difficulty in learning to read. Because of vast differences in cultural achievements, the behavior of modern humans is completely unlike that of Homo sapiens 100,000 years ago. Granted, simple toolmaking and tool use predate modern humans, but art, such as carvings and paintings, dates back only some 30,000 years, well after the appearance of Homo sapiens. Agriculture appears still more recently in human history, about 10,000 to 15,000 years ago. And reading and writing, the foundations of our mod- ern literate and technical societies, were invented only about 7000 years ago. St. Ambrose, who lived in the fourth century, is reported to be the first person who could read silently. So silent reading is an even more recent behavior than writing. Most forms of mathematics, another basis of modern technology, were invented still more recently than reading and writing were. Although most researchers think that the origin of verbal language coincided with the beginning of modern hu- mans, it may have developed much later. If we consider that most of our culture was invented long after our brains evolved into their present form, most of our verbal language skills also could have been learned long after Homo sapiens first evolved. It is likely that, when you have completed your college or university cur- riculum, your vocabulary will be larger and your language will be richer than those of your parents and grandparents. A remarkable feature of the modern human brain is that it can do so many things for which it was not seemingly designed. The brains of early Homo sapiens certainly did not develop to help program computers or travel to distant planets. And yet the brains of modern humans are capable of both these complex tasks and more. Apparently, the p

32 CHAPTER 1

things that the human brain did evolve to do contained all the elements necessary for the invention and use of far more sophisticated skills. The human brain is apparently a highly flexible organ that allows the great variety of knowledge and achievements that are part of modern culture. The acquisition of a complex culture was a gradual, step-by-step process, with one achievement leading to another. The development of language, for instance, must have started when our early ancestors began to use concepts to represent the things important to them in their world. In her book titled The Chimpanzees of Gombe, primatologist Jane Goodall described the process by which such concepts might have developed in chimpanzees. She used the development of the concept of “fig” as an example, explaining how a chimp might progress from knowing a fig only as a tangible, here-and-now entity to having a special vocal call that repre- sents this concept symbolically. Goodall writes:

We can trace a pathway along which representations of ...a fig become progressively more distant from the fig itself. The value of a fig to a chim- panzee lies in eating it. It is important that he quickly learn to recognize as fig the fruit above his head in a tree (which he has already learned to know through taste). He also needs to learn that a certain characteristic odor is representative of fig, even though the fig is out of sight. Food calls made by other chimpanzees in the place where he remembers the fig tree to be located may also conjure up a concept of fig. Given the chim- panzees’ proven learning ability, there does not seem to be any great cog- nitive leap from these achievements to understanding that some quite new and different stimulus (a symbol) can also be representative of fig. Although chimpanzee calls are, for the most part, dictated by emotions, cognitive abilities are sometimes required to interpret them. And the in- terpretations themselves may be precursors of symbolic thought. (Goodall, 1986, pp. 588–589)

Presumably, in our own distant ancestors, the repeated acquisition of con- cepts, as well as the education of children in those concepts, gradually led to the acquisition of language and other aspects of a complex culture. The study of the human brain, then, is not just the study of the structure of an organ that evolved thousands of years ago. It is also the study of how that organ acquires sophisticated cultural skills—that is, of how the human brain functions in today’s world.

In Review

Care must be taken in extending principles learned in studying the evolution of the brain and behavior. What is true for comparisons across different species may not be true for comparisons within a single species. For instance, although a larger brain correlates with more complex behavior when comparing different species, brain size and intelligence are not particularly related when looking at individual persons within the species of modern humans. We humans are also distinguished in the animal kingdom by the amount of our behavior that is culturally learned. We have progressed much further in the development of culture than other species have. p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 33 SUMMARY 1. What is the brain and what is behavior? Behavior can be defined as any kind of movement in a living organism. As such, a behavior has both a cause and a function. The flexibility and complexity of behavior vary greatly in different species, with human behavior being highly flexible and complex. Located inside the skull, the brain is the organ that exerts control over behavior. The brain seems to need ongoing sensory and motor activity to maintain its intelligent activity. 2. How is the nervous system structured? The nervous system is composed of the cen- tral nervous system, which includes the brain and the spinal cord, and the periph- eral nervous system, through which the brain and spinal cord communicate with sensory receptors and with muscles. The brain and spinal cord communicate not only with the skeletal muscles that enable movement of the body, but also with the body’s many internal organs. The nerve pathways taking part in regulating internal bodily functions, including emotional responses, are collectively called the autonomic nervous system. 3. How have people through history viewed the relation between the brain and behav- ior? Aristotle believed that the brain has no role in behavior, but rather that be- havior is the product of an intangible entity called the psyche or mind. Descartes modified this theory, proposing that only rational behavior is produced by the mind, whereas other behaviors are produced mechanically by the brain. Finally, Darwin’s proposal that all living things are descended from a common ancestor led to the conclusion that the source of all behavior is the brain. 4. How did brain cells and the nervous system evolve? Brain cells and the nervous system evolved in animals over millions of years. The evolutionary stages through which the brain evolved can be traced though groups of living animals. The nervous system evolved in the animal kingdom and the brain and spinal cord evolved in the chordate phylum. Mammals are a class of chordates characterized by especially large brains. 5. What species were the early ancestors of modern humans? One of our early hom- inid ancestors was probably Australopithecus or a primate very much like it, who lived in Africa several million years ago. From an australopith species, more humanlike species likely evolved. Among these species are Homo habilis and Homo erectus. Modern humans, Homo sapiens, did not appear in Asia and North Africa until about 200,000 to 100,000 years ago. 6. How did the human brain evolve? The human brain evolved through the sequence of hominid species that are the ancestors of modern humans. Since Australopithe- cus, the brain has increased in size almost threefold. This evolution was stimulated by the natural selection of more complex behavior patterns. It was also made possible by changes in blood circulation that enabled a larger brain to be adequately cooled. 7. What are some important considerations in studying the brain and behavior of modern humans? It is important to realize that principles learned in studying the evolution of the brain and behavior may not apply to the brain and behavior within a single species, such as Homo sapiens. As animals evolved, a larger brain was associated with more complex behavior, yet, within our species, the most able and intelligent people do not necessarily have the largest brains. In the study of modern humans it is also important to recognize the great extent to which our behavior is not inherent in our nervous systems, but rather is culturally learned. p

34 CHAPTER 1

neuroscience Interactive KEY TERMS

bilateral symmetry, p. 17 hominid, p. 21 principle of proper mass, cladogram, p. 16 materialism, p. 12 p. 23 common ancestor, p. 14 mentalism, p. 8 segmentation, p. 17 common descent, p. 12 mind, p. 8 species-typical behavior, culture, p. 30 mind–body problem, p. 29 dualism, p. 9 p. 9 taxonomy, p. 15 encephalization quotient natural selection, p. 12 (EQ), p. 24 neoteny, p. 27

There are many resources available for expanding your learning online: REVIEW QUESTIONS www.worthpublishers.com/kolb/ 1. Summarize the ideas of Aristotle, Descartes, and Darwin regarding the relation chapter1 between the brain and behavior. Try some self-tests to reinforce your mastery of the material in Chapter 1. 2. Trace your own lineage by using the taxonomic system described in this chapter. Look at some of the news updates 3. Can you recall the number of species of living organisms in each taxonomic reflecting current research on the brain. subgrouping? What do you think accounts for the apparent relation between You’ll also be able to link to other sites numbers of species and brain size? which will reinforce what you’ve 4. We suggested that brain size is one way of accounting for behavioral complexity learned. for interspecies comparisons but not for intraspecies comparisons. Why did we make this distinction? http://neurolab.jsc.nasa.gov/time 5. How does culture increase the difficulty of understanding human brain function? line.htm Review a timeline of important people in the study of the brain from René FOR FURTHER THOUGHT Descartes to Roger Sperry put together by the Spotlight on Neuroscience, Darwin’s principle of natural selection is based on there being large individual differences National Aeronautics and Space Admin- within species. There are large individual differences in the brain size of modern humans. istration (NASA). Under what conditions could a new human species with a still larger brain evolve?

http://serendip.brynmawr.edu/Mind/ Table.html RECOMMENDED READING Visit this site from R. H. Wozniak of Bryn Mawr College to learn more about Campbell, N. A. (1999). Biology, 2nd ed. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin Cummings. This the philosophical underpinnings of introductory biology textbook provides a comprehensive overview of the structure and dualism and materialism and to read a function of living organisms. detailed history of the origins of the Coren, S. (1994). The intelligence of dogs. Toronto: The Free Press. This very popular book mind–body question and the rise of includes a number of tests that are supposed to tell you how smart your dog is. The experimental psychology. book also provides comparisons of intelligence for different dog breeds as rated by dog trainers. Check your dog out against other breeds by using easy-to-perform tests. On your CD-ROM you’ll be able to quiz Remember, if your dog is not well trained, it might not do well on the tests. yourself on your comprehension of the Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of chapter. You’ll be able to begin learning Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872) If a dog growls at you, is the dog angry? about the anatomy of the brain in the Darwin thought so. Darwin’s only book on psychology is one in which he argues that module on the Central Nervous System. the expression of emotions is similar in animals, including humans, which suggests the This module is composed of a rotatable, inheritance of emotions from a common ancestor. This view is becoming popular today, three-dimensional brain as well as a but Darwin proposed it more than 100 years ago. number of sections of the brain that you Darwin, C. (1963). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of can move through with the click of a favored races in the struggle for life. New York: New American Library. (Original work mouse. In addition, the Research Methods published 1859) This book is the most important one ever written in biology. Darwin module contains various CT and MRI images of the brain, including a video extensively documents the evidence for his theory of natural selection. The book is an clip of a coronal MRI scan. enjoyable account of natural life, and one chapter, titled “Instincts,”describes behavior in both wild and domesticated animals. p

WHAT ARE THE ORIGINS OF BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR? 35

Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Goodall’s three-decade-long study of wild chimpanzees, begun in 1960, rates as one of the most scientifically important studies of animal behavior ever undertaken. Learn about chimpanzee family structure and chimpanzee behavior, and look at the beautiful photographs of chimpanzees engaged in various behaviors. Gould, S. J. (1981). The mismeasure of man. New York: Norton. Gould criticizes and repudiates extensive literature of the nineteenth and twentieth centuries that claims that differences in human intelligence and differences in the intelligence of the sexes are due to differ- ences in brain size. The appealing feature of this book is that Gould is highly critical of the methodology of the proponents of the brain-size hypothesis while also giving rea- sons derived from modern genetics to criticize their position. Lorenz, K. Z. (1981). The foundations of ethology. New York: Springer Verlag. Learn how to study animals and learn how they behave from one of the founders of ethology, the study of animal behavior. Martin, R. D. (1990). Primate origins and evolution: A phylogenetic reconstruction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Martin provides a detailed description of the origins and the . This book is an excellent primate reference. Weiner, J. (1995). The beak of the finch. New York: Vintage. This book is a marvelous study of evolution in action. Weiner documents how the populations of Galápagos finches are affected by changes in the availability of certain kinds of food. Careful measurements of the finches’ beaks demonstrate that certain beak sizes and shapes are useful when certain kinds of food are available; however, when the appropriate food becomes unavailable, populations of birds with differently shaped beaks become favored.