Responses of Primary Visual Cortical Neurons to Binocular Disparity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Responses of Primary Visual Cortical Neurons to Binocular Disparity letters to nature resistance against pathogens and parasites in animals is scarce, and comes largely from studies on populations of farm or laboratory animals with highly modified genetic backgrounds (reviewed in Responses of primary visual ref. 1). Finally, because of the enormous amount known about Drosophila genetics, we suggest that the interactions between cortical neurons to binocular D. melanogaster and A. tabida may be a valuable model system for the study of the evolution of resistance and of the genetic basis of disparity without depth adaptation. M ......................................................................................................................... perception Methods Competitive ability of selected and control lines. Competitive ability was B. G. Cumming & A. J. Parker assessed by placing 15 second-instar larvae from the experimental lines (either University Laboratory of Physiology, Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PT, UK control or selected insects) with 15 second-instar larvae from the tester stock ......................................................................................................................... (sparkling poliert, an eye-colour mutant) in agar-lined Petri dishes with The identification of brain regions that are associated with the variable amounts of larval medium. Four food levels were used: 0.4, 0.2, 0.1 or conscious perception of visual stimuli is a major goal in 0.05 ml of larval medium (25 g live baker’s yeast per 100 ml water), which neuroscience1. Here we present a test of whether the signals on represent weak to strong competition regimes. We recorded the number of neurons in cortical area V1 correspond directly to our conscious experimental and tester flies that survived, the development time to pupation perception of binocular stereoscopic depth. Depth perception and to adult eclosion, and female size and fluctuating asymmetry (wing requires that image features on one retina are first matched length). We performed 15 replicates of each combination of line and food level. with appropriate features on the other retina. The mechanisms Data analysis. Survival data were analysed using both a robust competition that perform this matching can be examined by using random-dot index (I) and a more sophisticated analysis using generalized linear modelling stereograms2, in which the left and right eyes view randomly techniques (II). For analysis (I) we calculated the competition index15, positioned but binocularly correlated dots. We exploit the fact logðe=ðt þ 1ÞÞ, where e is the number of experimental and t is the number of that anticorrelated random-dot stereograms (in which dots in one tester flies that survived in each replicate. The means of the competition index eye are matched geometrically to dots of the opposite contrast in for the eight lines were calculated and differences between selected and control the other eye) do not give rise to the perception of depth3 because lines tested using the t-test with unequal variances. For analysis (II) the the matching process does not find a consistent solution. Anti- untransformed survival data for the experimental flies were analysed in a correlated random-dot stereograms contain binocular features generalized linear model with binomial error variances, but using quasi- that could excite neurons that have not solved the correspondence likelihood estimation to account for overdispersion17. The numbers of tester problem. We demonstrate that disparity-selective neurons in V1 flies surviving was used as a covariate and significance assessed by nesting lines signal the disparity of anticorrelated random-dot stereograms, within control or selection treatments (giving an F-statistic with one and six indicating that they do not unambiguously signal stereoscopic degrees of freedom). Examination of the distribution of residuals supported the depth. Hence single V1 neurons cannot account for the conscious choice of model. The results of the two statistical analyses were in broad perception of stereopsis, although combining the outputs of many agreement (values in the text are from analysis (II)), with P for (I) given first: V1 neurons could solve the matching problem. The accompanying 0.4 ml, P . 0:1, P . 0:1; 0.2 ml, P . 0:1, P . 0:1; 0.1 ml, P ¼ 0:033, paper4 suggests an additional function for disparity signals from P ¼ 0:011; 0.05 ml, P ¼ 0:066, P ¼ 0:061. The data for size and development V1: they may be important for the rapid involuntary control of time were analysed using the appropriate general linear models equivalent to vergence eye movements (eye movements that bring the images on analysis (II). the two foveae into register). When the image of an object falls on different locations on the Received 25 March; accepted 8 July 1997. two retinae, this binocular disparity gives rise to a sensation of depth 1. Read, A. F. et al.inEcology of Infectious Diseases in Natural Populations (eds Grenfell, B. T. & Dobson, A. P.) 450–477 (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1995). (stereopsis). This process requires that an image feature on one 2. Partridge, L. & Fowler, K. Direct and correlated responses to selection on age at reproduction in retina be matched with an appropriate feature on the other retina, Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 46, 76–91 (1992). even though there are inevitably similar features nearby offering 3. Rose, M. R. Laboratory evolution of postponed senescence in Drosophila melanogaster. Evolution 38, 1004–1010 (1984). potential matches. This problem is particularly apparent in 4. Salt, G. The Cellular Defence Reactions of Insects (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1970). random-dot stereograms (RDS), where there are many identical 5. Gupta, A. P. in Comprehensive Insect Physiology, Biochemistry and Pharmocology, Vol. 3 (eds Kerkut, G. A. & Gilbert, L. I.) 401–451 (Pergamon, Oxford, 1985). dots in both images. Of course it is not necessary to consider false 6. Strand, M. R. & Pech, L. L. Immunological basis for compatibility in parasitoid-host relationships. matches in RDS on a dot-by-dot basis—spatial filtering of the Annu. Rev. Entomol. 40, 31–56 (1995). image before matching can substantially reduce the number of false 7. Godfray, H. C. J. Parasitoids, Behavioral and Evolutionary Ecology, (Princeton Univ. Press, NJ, 1994). 5 8. Carton, Y. & Bouletreau, M. Encapsulation ability of Drosophila melanogaster: a genetic analysis. Dev. matches . However, this filtering alone is insufficient to solve the Comp. Immunol. 9, 211–219 (1985). correspondence problem and further computation is required to 9. Carton, Y. & Nappi, A. J. The Drosophila immune reaction and the parasitoid capacity to evade it: genetic and coevolutionary aspects. Acta Oecol. 12, 89–104 (1991). eliminate false matches. Consideration of the overall pattern of 10. Henter, H. J. & Via, S. The potential for coevolution in a host-parasitoid system. 1. Genetic variation disparities can indicate which potential matches form a globally within an aphid population in susceptibility to a parasitic wasp. Evolution 49, 427–438 (1995). consistent pattern, and many computer algorithms achieve this5,6. 11. Hughes, K. & Sokolowski, M. B. Natural selection in the laboratory for a change in resistance by Drosophila melanogaster to the parasitoid wasp Asobara tabida. J. Insect Behav. 9, 477–491 (1996). Although single neurons in V1 have long been known to signal 12. Kraaijeveld, A. R. & van der Wel, N. N. Geographic variation in reproductive success of the parasitoid the disparity of a stimulus7,8, their role in stereo matching is unclear. Asobara tabida in larvae of several Drosophila species. Ecol. Entomol. 19, 221–229 (1994). 13. Kraaijeveld, A. R. & van Alphen, J. J. M. Geographical variation in encapsulation ability of Drosophila If they are directly responsible for the conscious perception of melanogaster larvae and evidence for parasitoid-specific components. Evol. Ecol. 9, 10–17 (1995). depth, they should respond only when matches within the receptive 14. Lewonton, R. C. The effects of population density and composition on viability in Drosophila field are registered as globally correct. If binocular neurons respond melanogaster. Evolution 9, 27–41 (1955). 15. Santos, M., Fowler, K. & Partridge, L. On the use of tester stocks to predict the competitive ability of equally well to false matches at appropriate disparities, then further genotypes. Heredity 69, 489–495 (1992). processing (presumably outside V1) is required to account for the 16. Atkinson, W. D. A field investigation of larval competition in domestic Drosophila. J. Anim. Ecol. 48, 91–102 (1979). psychophysical ability to discard false matches. 17. McCullagh, P. & Nelder, J. A. Generalized Linear Models 2nd edn (Chapman & Hall, London, 1989). Whether or not disparity-selective neurons respond only to correct matches is an open question. It has been demonstrated Acknowledgements. We thank J. van Alphen, G. Boskamp, A. Burt, D. Ebert, J. Ellers, M. Fellowes, A. James, A. Leroi, J. McCabe, A. Orr, L. Partridge, A. Read, M. Rees and J. Werren for help and advice. that some V1 complex cells display disparity selectivity to dynamic RDSs and it was concluded that these neurons signal ‘‘the correct Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to H.C.J.G. (e-mail: [email protected]). binocular matches among a multitude of false matches ... (global Nature © Macmillan Publishers Ltd 1997 280 NATURE | VOL 389 | 18 SEPTEMBER 1997 letters to nature Figure 1 Example stereograms. The pair formed by the central image and the left Figure 2 Responses of a model complex cell to RDS. The model neuron was image forms a C-RDS showing a central circular patch standing out in depth tuned to zero-disparity stimuli. Filled symbols show responses to C-RDS, open (shown for cross-eyed fusion). The pair formed by the central image and the right symbols show responses to A-RDS. Both axes have arbitrary units. The model image is an A-RDS, and appears rivalrous with no consistent depth. Note that the neuron had subunits that were matched in their spatial and temporal properties, right image is an exact copy of the left image in which the dot brightnesses have so the two tuning curves are exact mirror images.
Recommended publications
  • The Morphological Basis for Binocular and ON/OFF Convergence in Tree Shrew Striate Cortex
    The Journal of Neuroscience, April 1992, 12(4): 1319-I 334 The Morphological Basis for Binocular and ON/OFF Convergence in Tree Shrew Striate Cortex E. Christopher Muly and David Fitzpatrick Department of Neurobiology, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina 27710 We used retrograde and anterograde transport methods and sponse),reflecting distinct inputs from corresponding popula- single-cell reconstructions to examine the projection from tions of retinal ganglion cells (Kuffler, 1953; Hubel and Wiesel, layer IV to supragranular layers in the tree shrew’s striate 1961). In contrast, single cortical neurons have both ON and cortex. We found that neurons in the ON and OFF subdivi- OFF responsesdue to the convergenceof information conveyed sions of layer IV (IVa and IVb, respectively) have overlapping by ON and OFF center LGN neurons(Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; terminal fields throughout layers II and Ill. Despite their over- Schiller, 1982; Tanaka, 1983; Sherk and Horton, 1984). lap, these projections are organized in a highly stratified, The generation of binocular, dual-sign (ON/OFF) responses mirror-symmetric fashion that respects the vertical position from monocular, single-sign(ON or OFF) responsesmust reflect of neurons within each sublayer. Neurons in the middle of a simple anatomical fact: the axons of four different classesof layer IV (lower IVa and upper IVb) project to layers Ills/b, II, LGN neurons (i.e., ipsilateral ON, ipsilateral OFF, contralateral and I; neurons located at the edges of layer IV (upper IVa ON, and contralateral OFF), or cortical neuronsthat convey the and lower IVb) project to the lower half of layer Illc; and information from theseLGN neurons, synapseon a singlepost- neurons in the middle of IVa and the middle of IVb project synaptic neuron.
    [Show full text]
  • Seeing in Three Dimensions: the Neurophysiology of Stereopsis Gregory C
    Review DeAngelis – Neurophysiology of stereopsis Seeing in three dimensions: the neurophysiology of stereopsis Gregory C. DeAngelis From the pair of 2-D images formed on the retinas, the brain is capable of synthesizing a rich 3-D representation of our visual surroundings. The horizontal separation of the two eyes gives rise to small positional differences, called binocular disparities, between corresponding features in the two retinal images. These disparities provide a powerful source of information about 3-D scene structure, and alone are sufficient for depth perception. How do visual cortical areas of the brain extract and process these small retinal disparities, and how is this information transformed into non-retinal coordinates useful for guiding action? Although neurons selective for binocular disparity have been found in several visual areas, the brain circuits that give rise to stereoscopic vision are not very well understood. I review recent electrophysiological studies that address four issues: the encoding of disparity at the first stages of binocular processing, the organization of disparity-selective neurons into topographic maps, the contributions of specific visual areas to different stereoscopic tasks, and the integration of binocular disparity and viewing-distance information to yield egocentric distance. Some of these studies combine traditional electrophysiology with psychophysical and computational approaches, and this convergence promises substantial future gains in our understanding of stereoscopic vision. We perceive our surroundings vividly in three dimen- lished the first reports of disparity-selective neurons in the sions, even though the image formed on the retina of each primary visual cortex (V1, or area 17) of anesthetized cats5,6.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision
    BINOCULAR VISION Rahul Bhola, MD Pediatric Ophthalmology Fellow The University of Iowa Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences posted Jan. 18, 2006, updated Jan. 23, 2006 Binocular vision is one of the hallmarks of the human race that has bestowed on it the supremacy in the hierarchy of the animal kingdom. It is an asset with normal alignment of the two eyes, but becomes a liability when the alignment is lost. Binocular Single Vision may be defined as the state of simultaneous vision, which is achieved by the coordinated use of both eyes, so that separate and slightly dissimilar images arising in each eye are appreciated as a single image by the process of fusion. Thus binocular vision implies fusion, the blending of sight from the two eyes to form a single percept. Binocular Single Vision can be: 1. Normal – Binocular Single vision can be classified as normal when it is bifoveal and there is no manifest deviation. 2. Anomalous - Binocular Single vision is anomalous when the images of the fixated object are projected from the fovea of one eye and an extrafoveal area of the other eye i.e. when the visual direction of the retinal elements has changed. A small manifest strabismus is therefore always present in anomalous Binocular Single vision. Normal Binocular Single vision requires: 1. Clear Visual Axis leading to a reasonably clear vision in both eyes 2. The ability of the retino-cortical elements to function in association with each other to promote the fusion of two slightly dissimilar images i.e. Sensory fusion. 3. The precise co-ordination of the two eyes for all direction of gazes, so that corresponding retino-cortical element are placed in a position to deal with two images i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Dichoptic Visual Masking Reveals That Early Binocular Neurons Exhibit Weak Interocular Suppression: Implications for Binocular Vision and Visual Awareness
    Dichoptic Visual Masking Reveals that Early Binocular Neurons Exhibit Weak Interocular Suppression: Implications for Binocular Vision and Visual Awareness Stephen L. Macknik* and Susana Martinez-Conde* Abstract & Visual masking effects are illusions in which a target is together fail to support models of masking that require rendered invisible by a mask, which can either overlap or not circuits found only in the cortex, but support our proposed overlap the target spatially and/or temporally. These illusions model that suggests that simple ubiquitous lateral inhibition provide a powerful tool to study visibility and consciousness, may itself be the fundamental mechanism that explains visual object grouping, brightness perception, and much more. As masking across multiple levels in the brain. We also show that such, the physiological mechanisms underlying the perception area V1 neurons are dichoptic in terms of excitation, but of masking are critically important to our understanding of monoptic in terms of inhibition. That is, responses within area visibility. Several models that require cortical circuits have been V1 binocular neurons reveal that excitation to monocular proposed previously to explain the mysterious spatial and targets is inhibited strongly only by masks presented to the timing effects associated with visual masking. Here we describe same eye, and not by masks presented to the opposite eye. single-unit physiological experiments from the awake monkey These results lead us to redefine the model for the first stage of that show that visual masking occurs in at least two separate binocular processing in the visual system, and may be crucial to and independent circuits, one that is binocular and one that is interpreting the effects of other similar binocular and dichoptic monocular (possibly even subcortical), without feedback from stimulation paradigms, such as the binocular rivalry family of higher-level visual brain areas.
    [Show full text]
  • Development of Binocular Vision in the Kitten's Striate Cortex
    The Journal of Neuroscience, December 1992, fZ(12): 4721-4736 Development of Binocular Vision in the Kitten’s Striate Cortex Ralph D. Freeman and lzumi Ohzawa Group in Neurobiology, School of Optometry, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720 Studies of the development and plasticity of the visual path- factors may be applicable. In any case, although major re- way are well documented, but a basic question remains open: finements occur in stereoscopic vision postnatally, the what is the physiological status of the system prior to ex- physiological connections that underlie this function are tensive visual experience? Somewhat conflicting answers operational at a very early postnatal age. have been put forward, and in a major area, binocular vision, reports have ranged from severe immaturity to well-devel- The development and plasticity of visual function has been oped maturity. This is an important question to resolve since studied extensively by useof behavioral, physiological, anatom- binocular cells in the visual cortex are thought to be the ical, and neurochemical approaches.This work has engendered neural substrate for stereoscopic depth perception. a considerable literature, which is reviewed in several articles We have addressed this question by recording from single (see,e.g., Fregnecand Imbert, 1984; Mitchell and Timney, 1984). cells in the striate cortex of kittens at postnatal ages 2, 3, In order to understand the limits and rules of plasticity, one and 4 weeks and from adults for comparison. Gratings with requires firm knowledge of normal visual functions at various sinusoidal luminance distribution are presented to left, right, stagesof development. By far the most studied animal in this or both eyes.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Process in Human Vision BRIAN TIMNEY, LAURIE M
    On the evidence for a 'pure' binocular process in human vision BRIAN TIMNEY, LAURIE M. WILCOX and ROBERT ST. JOHN* Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada N6A 5C2 Received13 October 1987;accepted 28 February 1989 Abstract-Wolfe (1986,Psychol. Rev. 93, 269-282)proposed a model of human binocular vision based on the assumption of two functionallydistinct classesof binocular neuron. These neurons may be regarded as logicalAND and OR gates.In the present paper we assessthe evidencerelevant to this assumption.We find that whileboth types of binocular neuron have been describedin the cortex of cat and monkey,there is no indicationthat they form functionallyseparate populations.Critical analysis of the psychophysicalevidence for AND and OR channelsin human vision suggeststhat much of the data presentedin favor of an AND channelis subject to alternativeinterpretations. We concludethat the availabledata are not consistentwith the existenceof separate channels as proposed by Wolfe. INTRODUCTION In a recent paper, Wolfe (1986) proposed a model of human binocular vision that attempted to integrate the mechanisms of stereopsis and binocular rivalry. His model depends critically upon two functionally distinct classes of binocular neuron. The neurons of one class may be described as logical AND gates. They can be activated only through simultaneous stimulation of both eyes and are assumed to play a major role in stereopsis. Neurons in the second class may be considered as OR gates. They can be stimulated through either eye and are assumed to play a major role in binocular rivalry. The OR neurons are considered to be secondary to the AND cells in the mediation of stereopsis.
    [Show full text]
  • THE PHYSIOLOGY of STEREOPSIS B. G. Cumming G. C. Deangelis
    P1: FUI January 16, 2001 9:19 Annual Reviews AR121-08 Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001. 24:203–38 Copyright c 2001 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved THE PHYSIOLOGY OF STEREOPSIS B. G. Cumming University Laboratory of Physiology, Oxford, OX1 3PT,1 United Kingdom; e-mail: [email protected] G. C. DeAngelis Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, Missouri 63110-1093; e-mail: [email protected] Key Words binocular vision, disparity, striate cortex, extrastriate cortex, depth perception ■ Abstract Binocular disparity provides the visual system with information con- cerning the three-dimensional layout of the environment. Recent physiological studies in the primary visual cortex provide a successful account of the mechanisms by which single neurons are able to signal disparity. This work also reveals that additional pro- cessing is required to make explicit the types of signal required for depth perception (such as the ability to match features correctly between the two monocular images). Some of these signals, such as those encoding relative disparity, are found in ex- trastriate cortex. Several other lines of evidence also suggest that the link between perception and neuronal activity is stronger in extrastriate cortex (especially MT) than in the primary visual cortex. INTRODUCTION A central problem faced by the visual system is providing information about a three-dimensional environment from two-dimensional retinal images. In many animals, one of the most precise sources of information arises from the fact that the two eyes have different vantage points. This means that the images on the two retinae are not identical (see Figure 1).
    [Show full text]
  • Modelling Binocular Neurons in the Primary Visual Cortex
    Mo delling Bino cular Neurons in the Primary Visual Cortex David J Fleet David J Heeger Hermann Wagner Intro duction Neurons sensitive to bino cular disparityhave b een found in the visual cortex of many mammals and in the visual wulst of the owl and are thought to play a signicant role in stereopsis Barlow et al Nikara et al Hub el and Wiesel Clarke et al Pettigrew and Konishi Poggio and Fischer Fischer and Kruger Ferster Poggio and Talb ot OhzawaandFreeman a Ohzawa and Freeman b LeVayandVoigt Ohzawa et al DeAngelis et al Wagner and Frost A number of physiologists have suggested that disparity might b e enco ded by a shift of receptiveeld p osition Hub el and Wiesel Pettigrew et al Pettigrew Maske et al Poggio et al Wagner and Frost According to this p ositionshift mo del disparity selective cells combine the outputs of similarly shap ed mono cular receptive elds from dierent retinal p ositions in the left and righteyes More recently Ohzawa et al and DeAngelis et al have suggested that disparity sensitivity might instead b e a result of intero cular phase shifts In this phaseshift mo del the centers of the left and righteye receptive elds coincide but the arrangement of receptive eld subregions is dierent This chapter presents a formal description and analysis of a bino cular energy mo del of disparity selectivity According to this mo del disparity selectivity results from a combination of p ositionshifts andor phaseshifts Our theoretical analysis suggests how one might p erform an exp eriment
    [Show full text]
  • Theory for the Development of Neuron Selectivity: Orientation Specificity and Binocular Interaction in Visual Cortex1
    0270~6474/82/0201-0032$02.00/O The Journal of Neuroscience Copyright 0 Society for Neuroscience Vol. 2, No. 1, pp. 32-48 Printed in U.S.A. January 1982 THEORY FOR THE DEVELOPMENT OF NEURON SELECTIVITY: ORIENTATION SPECIFICITY AND BINOCULAR INTERACTION IN VISUAL CORTEX1 ELIE L. BIENENSTOCK,2 LEON N COOPER,3 AND PAUL W. MUNRO Center for Neural Science, Department of Physics, and Division of Applied Mathematics, Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island 02912 Received June 5, 1981; Revised August 27, 1981; Accepted September 1, 1981 Abstract The development of stimulus selectivity in the primary sensory cortex of higher vertebrates is considered in a general mathematical framework. A synaptic evolution scheme of a new kind is proposed in which incoming patterns rather than converging afferents compete. The change in the efficacy of a given synapse depends not only on instantaneous pre- and postsynaptic activities but also on a slowly varying time-averaged value of the postsynaptic activity. Assuming an appropriate nonlinear form for this dependence, development of selectivity is obtained under quite general conditions on the sensory environment. One does not require nonlinearity of the neuron’s integrative power nor does one need to assume any particular form for intracortical circuitry. This is first illustrated in simple cases, e.g., when the environment consists of only two different stimuli presented alternately in a random manner. The following formal statement then holds: the state of the system converges with probability 1 to points of maximum selectivity in the state space. We next consider the problem of early development of orientation selectivity and binocular interaction in primary visual cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptation to Binocular Anticorrelation Results in Increased Neural Excitability
    Adaptation to Binocular Anticorrelation Results in Increased Neural Excitability Reuben Rideaux, Elizabeth Michael, and Andrew E. Welchman Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/32/1/100/1861419/jocn_a_01471.pdf by guest on 05 May 2021 Abstract ■ Throughout the brain, information from individual sources Receptive fields of disparity-tuned simple cells in macaque V1. converges onto higher order neurons. For example, information Neuron, 38,103–114, 2003; Cumming, B. G., & Parker, A. J. from the two eyes first converges in binocular neurons in area V1. Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular dispar- Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/jocn/article-pdf/32/1/100/1931154/jocn_a_01471.pdf by guest on 26 September 2021 Some neurons are tuned to similarities between sources of infor- ity without depth perception. Nature, 389, 280–283, 1997], their mation, which makes intuitive sense in a system striving to match function has remained opaque. To determine how neural mech- multiple sensory signals to a single external cause—that is, estab- anisms tuned to dissimilarities support perception, here we use lish causal inference. However, there are also neurons that are electroencephalography to measure human observers’ steady- tuned to dissimilar information. In particular, some binocular neu- state visually evoked potentials in response to change in depth rons respond maximally to a dark feature in one eye and a light after prolonged viewing of anticorrelated and correlated feature in the other. Despite compelling neurophysiological and random-dot stereograms (RDS). We find that adaptation to an- behavioral evidence supporting the existence of these neurons ticorrelated RDS results in larger steady-state visually evoked [Katyal, S., Vergeer, M., He, S., He, B., & Engel, S.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Suppression in the Macaque Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Reveals Early Competitive Interactions Between the Eyes
    Research Article: New Research | Sensory and Motor Systems Binocular suppression in the macaque lateral geniculate nucleus reveals early competitive interactions between the eyes https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0364-20.2020 Cite as: eNeuro 2021; 10.1523/ENEURO.0364-20.2020 Received: 20 August 2020 Revised: 6 November 2020 Accepted: 28 November 2020 This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been through the composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style or formatting and will contain links to any extended data. Alerts: Sign up at www.eneuro.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fully formatted version of this article is published. Copyright © 2021 Dougherty et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed. 1 Binocular suppression in the macaque lateral geniculate nucleus 2 reveals early competitive interactions between the eyes 3 4 Abbreviated title: Binocular modulation in primate LGN 5 6 7 Kacie Dougherty1,2, Brock M. Carlson1, Michele A. Cox1,3, Jacob A. Westerberg1, Wolf Zinke1, Michael 8 C. Schmid4, Paul R. Martin5, Alexander Maier1 9 10 1Department of Psychology, College of Arts and Science, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for 11 Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37235, USA 12 2Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 09540, USA 13 3Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14620, USA 14 4Faculty of Science and Medicine, University of Fribourg, Chemin du Musée 5 CH-1700 Fribourg, 15 Switzerland 16 5Save Sight Institute and Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for Integrative Brain 17 Function, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales 2006, Australia.
    [Show full text]
  • Innervation Patterns of Single Physiologically Identified
    Proc. Nail. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 87, pp. 449-453, January 1990 Neurobiology Innervation patterns of single physiologically identified geniculocortical axons in the striate cortex of the tree shrew (geniculocortical afferents/visual cortex/on- and off-center pathways/binocular convergence) DAVID FITZPATRICK* AND DENiS RACZKOWSKI Department of Neurobiology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27710 Communicated by Irving T. Diamond, October 10, 1989 ABSTRACT We examined the termination patterns of MATERIALS AND METHODS single geniculocortical axons in the striate cortex of the tree shrew by using intracellular recording and horseradish per- We obtained the data for this report from adult tree shrews oxidase staining methods. Axons were classified by whether raised in our breeding colony. At the outset ofan experiment, they responded to light onset (ON center) or light offset (OFF we anesthetized the tree shrew with ketamine and xylazine, center) and whether they were driven by the ipsi- or contra- cannulated a vein for infusion of a dextrose/saline solution lateral eye. Afferents with ON-center responses end in the containing sodium thiopental (delivered at a rate of 3 upper part oflayer IV (IVa) whereas afferents with OFF-center mgkg-1hr-1 for the remainder of the experiment), per- responses end in the lower part of layer IV (IVb). Within each formed a tracheotomy, and placed a catheter in the bladder. tier, axons driven by the ipsilateral and contralateral eye The animal was placed in a stereotaxic apparatus, paralyzed overlap. These results suggest that binocular convergence with pancuronium bromide to prevent eye movements and occurs within layer IV without mixing the information from the ventilated with a respirator to maintain expired CO2 between ON- and OFF-center pathways and we consider the significance 2 and 3%.
    [Show full text]