Acoustic Evidence for Right-Edge Prominence in Nafsana)

Acoustic Evidence for Right-Edge Prominence in Nafsana)

...................................ARTICLE Acoustic evidence for right-edge prominence in Nafsana) Rosey Billington,b) Janet Fletcher,c) Nick Thieberger,d) and Ben Volchok ARC Centre of Excellence for the Dynamics of Language, School of Languages and Linguistics, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria 3052, Australia ABSTRACT: Oceanic languages are often described as preferring primary stress on penultimate syllables, but detailed surveys show that many different types of prominence patterns have been reported across and within Oceanic language families. In some cases, these interact with segmental and phonotactic factors, such as syllable weight. The range of Oceanic prominence patterns is exemplified across Vanuatu, a linguistically diverse archipelago with over 130 languages. However, both impressionistic and instrumentally-based descriptions of prosodic patterns and their correlates are limited for languages of this region. This paper investigates prominence in Nafsan, an Oceanic language of Vanuatu for which previous observations of prominence differ. Acoustic and durational results for disyl- labic and trisyllabic Nafsan words show a clear pattern of higher fundamental frequency values in final syllables, regardless of vowel length, pointing towards a preference for prominence at the right edge of words. Short vowels also show centralisation in penultimate syllables, providing supporting evidence for right-edge prominence and informing the understanding of vowel deletion processes in Nafsan. VC 2020 Acoustical Society of America. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000995 (Received 3 May 2019; revised 15 September 2019; accepted 16 October 2019; published online 30 April 2020) [Editor: Benjamin V. Tucker] Pages: 2829–2844 I. INTRODUCTION empirically. The present study is part of a wider project using instrumental phonetic approaches to investigate vari- This paper investigates prominence in Nafsan (South ous aspects of Nafsan phonology. Here, we examine promi- Efate), a Southern Oceanic language spoken by an estimated nence patterns observable at the lexical level in Nafsan, 6000 people in three villages (Erakor, Eratap, and Pango) on focusing on whether there is evidence that syllables are the island of Efate in Vanuatu. The phonology of Nafsan has been discussed in comparative and descriptive work (Clark, more prominent when they occur at the right edge. We iden- 1985; Lynch, 2000c; Thieberger, 2006), but for some tify the acoustic cues associated with prominence patterns, aspects of the segmental and prosodic system there have and discuss how the findings relating to prominence inform been different views and some challenges to identifying other aspects of the Nafsan sound system. clear patterns. In particular, until very recently there have been open questions relating to whether there is a vowel A. Prominence in Oceanic languages length distinction in the language, why some vowels For Oceanic languages, and Austronesian languages undergo deletion, and what patterns of word- and phrase- more generally, comprehensive descriptions of prosodic level prosodic prominence are used. There are also indica- patterns (based on auditory impressions or instrumental tions that there are crucial interactions between vowel investigations) are scarce (Himmelmann and Kaufman, distinctions, vowel deletion, and prominence patterns, and 2020). At the lexical level, Oceanic languages are often that these therefore need to be considered together in order described as preferring primary stress on penultimate sylla- to adequately describe the sound system of Nafsan. For example, it has been proposed that the major factors bles (Lynch et al., 2002; Ross, 1998). However, crosslin- influencing vowel deletion in Nafsan are phonemic vowel guistic examinations suggest that this tendency may not be length and proximity to an accented syllable, based on as widespread as previously thought, with a range of pat- impressions that final syllables have accentual prominence terns observed across Oceanic languages (Lynch, 2000b), (Billington et al., 2020), but these impressions differ from including among the more than 130 Southern Oceanic 1 some previous observations and have not been verified languages of Vanuatu. While some languages of Vanuatu are described as having regular penultimate stress, such as Erromangan (Crowley, 1998, p. 17), many are described as a)Portions of this work were presented at the 17th Australasian International having a weight-sensitive system whereby stress is penulti- Speech Science and Technology Conference, Sydney, Australia, mate unless the final syllable contains a coda consonant, as December 2018. in Tamambo (Riehl and Jauncey, 2005, p. 258), and/or a b)Electronic mail: [email protected], ORCID: 0000-0003-2708-5027. c)ORCID: 0000-0001-7223-4533. long vowel or diphthong, as in Naman (Crowley, 2006,p. d)ORCID: 0000-0001-8797-1018. 39), in which case stress is final. Some languages are J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 147 (4), April 2020 0001-4966/2020/147(4)/2829/16/$30.00 VC 2020 Acoustical Society of America 2829 https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000995 reported to have a reliably final stress pattern, such as Vur€es speakers of Germanic languages (Cutler, 2005; van Heuven, (Malau, 2016, p. 38), while others may only have final stress 2019). The particular cue or combination of cues associated if a word-final vowel has been deleted, as in Lelepa with lexical stress is language-specific, and may depend to (Lacrampe, 2014). Very occasionally, antepenultimate stress some extent on other language-specific phonological proper- is suggested, as in Nguna (Schutz,€ 1969, p. 11), and in other ties. Some comparisons across languages suggest that cues cases stress may be lexically contrastive, as in Paamese to stress may be more robust and consistent in languages for (Crowley, 1982, p. 19). Some grammatical descriptions note which stress patterns are lexically contrastive, and thereby prominence patterns which suggest phrasal accent rather not predictable, compared to languages with fixed lexical than lexical stress, for example in Nahavaq (Dimock, 2009, stress patterns, for example, as noted by Dogil and Williams pp. 46–55), Daakaka (von Prince, 2015, pp. 34–40), and (1999) with reference to data for Lithuanian, Polish, Mwotlap (Franc¸ois, 2001, p. 81). Given the different German, and Spanish and by Vogel et al. (2016) with refer- hypotheses regarding the status of stress in these languages, ence to data for Hungarian, Turkish, Greek, and Spanish. It we use the more general term prominence in this paper. has also been argued that for languages in which a supraseg- There is increasing interest in developing instrumentally- mental property is used contrastively, for example, where based descriptions of word-, phrase-, and utterance-level pro- duration signals phonemic vowel length, this suprasegmen- sodic patterns in Oceanic languages, as can be seen in recent tal property will not be used as a primary perceptual cue to research on Central Pacific languages of Polynesia such as lexical stress (Berinstein, 1979). This idea has been inter- Samoan (Calhoun, 2015, 2017; Yu and Stabler, 2017; Zuraw preted as a prediction relating to patterns of production as et al., 2014), Maori (Mixdorff et al., 2018; Thompson et al., well as perception. Although an overview of reported corre- 2011), Tongan (Garellek and White, 2015; Kuo and Vicenik, lates of lexical stress in descriptions of 140 languages finds 2012), and Niuean (Clemens, 2014, 2019). There are also limited crosslinguistic evidence for such a relationship some prosodic analyses of languages from other Oceanic (Lunden et al., 2017), there are individual languages for groups, such as the Northwest Solomonic language Torau, of which phonetic data suggests this may be relevant, for Papua New Guinea (Jepson, 2014), the Southeast Solomonic example Hungarian, which has a vowel length contrast and language Gela (Simard and Wegener, 2017), from the for which duration is not a correlate of lexical stress (Vogel Solomon Islands, and the Southern Oceanic language Drehu, et al., 2016). Other types of language-specific restrictions on of New Caledonia (Torres et al., 2018). For the languages of the ways suprasegmental cues can be combined have also Vanuatu, there are some phonetically-based explorations of been found, for example, in the co-occurrence restrictions prosodic patterns, such as for Nahavaq (Dimock, 2009,pp. between tones and vowels in Maastricht Limburgish 46–55), Daakaka (von Prince, 2015, pp. 34–40), and Mavea (Gussenhoven, 2012). Phonotactic factors may also be rele- (Guerin, 2019), but for most languages, prosodic descriptions vant to both the assignment and realisation of lexical stress; are limited to impressionistic comments relating to word stress syllable weight plays a role in determining stress location in or lexical prominence, with little information regarding how many languages around the world (Goedemans, 2010), and these might be realised. Existing descriptions also have lim- it has been argued that in such languages, syllables with ited scope for examining how prominence phenomena interact long vowels or codas are phonetically well-suited to bearing with other aspects of a language’s phonology. This is of par- lexical stress due to not just the additional segmental mate- ticular interest in the Vanuatu context, given that beyond the rial in a syllable rhyme but the distinctiveness of the overall variety of attested prominence patterns, the languages of energy profile of these syllables (Gordon,

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    17 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us