Age Factors in Heritage Language Acquisition: Notes on Montrul 2008, Chapter 1

Age Factors in Heritage Language Acquisition: Notes on Montrul 2008, Chapter 1

Age factors in Heritage Language Acquisition: notes on Montrul 2008, Chapter 1 Sophia A. Malamud February 5, 2011 1 Compare and contrast cases To set up her point, that age matters not just in SLA, but also in HLA, Montrul gives several cases of language-learning: age of ultimate proficiency L1 L2 exposure L1 L2 Kevin, 25 English Spanish & about 18 native basic French Caroline, 35 Spanish English 13 (school); native advanced 24 (immerse) Francesco, 45 Italian Spanish 30 (immerse) native (-) native-like Kristi, 38 Chinese English 2 (adopted) nill (?) native Elena, 24 Russian & French(L1) ≈6 (interrup) advanced(?) native French Carlos, 29 Spanish English 9 (immerse) native(?) native(?) Alicia, 24 & Spanish English 4 (immerse) advanced(??) native Beatriz, 22 &2(immerse) Preliminary conclusion: age matters for L1 loss in early bilingualism. Since we’re looking for different outcomes in SLA and HLA, we need a definition of the ”target” attainment. Some established facts: • Normally developing monolingual children acquire basic grammar of L1 by 3-4 years: – phonology – morphosyntax – semantics – aspects of pragmatics and sociolinguistc conventions • L1 acquisition is, generally, uniform: learners converge on the grammar of others in the speech community (example: *What did you ask who Patricia gave?) • L1 attainment in uninterrupted monolingual situations is native (complete). 1 • In the course of L1 acquisition, the attainment is incomplete - speakers make errors and lack some aspects of knowledge. This goes away. Montrul argues (uncontroversially) that, with linguistic foundations of L1 competence in place by age 3-4, acquisition continues: • literacy schooling raises metalinguistic awareness • vocabulary expands • more complex and varied structures are learned • multiple social situations, genres, and registers are mastered • diglossic competence Why this is important: We need to understand what parts of language-learning hinge on what social and age variables: so, incomplete acquisition is perfect to tell us that. 2 Critical Period Hypothesis NATURE: The innateness view: language-learning is an innately pre-wired, separate module in the brain, based on innate structural principles that are ”set” or triggered by the environment. NURTURE: The emergentist view: language-learning is part of general cognitive abilities, based on largely from experience. The Critical Period hypothesis can be illustrated by this diagram (taken from Hyltenstam and Abrahamsson 2003): A refinement - Critical Periods hypothesis: different ages of onset/offset of sensitivity for dif- ferent components of language. Montrul reviews evidence for a critical/sensitive period for (first) language acquisition from delay of language acquisition in cases of deprivation (Genie vs. Isabel), and in the deaf, as well as from creole formation. 2 2.1 Evidence from deaf learners of sign language Newport’s (1990) study of congenitally deaf children of hearing parents: • Three groups: native = birth-3yrs, early learners = 4-6yrs, late learners = after 12 yrs • All had minimum of 30 years of daily exposure to ASL (age at time of study 35-70yrs) • All did well with word order • Clear age effect for morphology: native were accurate, early learners good but some errors, while late learners’ were quite bad. 2.2 Creole genesis Pidgin: make-shift communication system, with unstable lexicon, simple syntax (no embedding, lots of ellipsis of arguments and verbs), no or almost no inflectional morphology. • Children whose only linguistic input is pidgin - don’t get grammatical input during the critical period. • Children create a more complex grammar: turn lexical/content items into grammatical/function ones. • Why are they doing it? – nature - they’re innately prewired to add structure vs. – nurture - they are exposed in some degree to the languages from which the pidgin is composed, so they could be borrowing from those A good test case: emergence of sign languages, where nothing other than the impoverished, pidgin-like input is present. Montrul briefly reviews the case of the Nicaraguan Sign Language (ISN), but see also Al-Sayyid Bedouin Sign Language (ABSL). For references and additional information about ISN, see the Morgan and Kegl 2006 article on LATTE, Ann Senghas’s publications, or 60 minutes on ISN. • In Nicaragua pre-1970s, deaf people remained isolated, each with their own home sign sys- tem (even less language-like than pidgins) • After the revolution, a school for the deaf began a speech community: the children formed a pidgin (Montrul claims spoken Spanish as a superstrate, but it’s not clear how that influence would have been exherted, since the children had no spoken Spanish) • Subsequent cohorts of deaf children developed a creole (the Nicaraguan Sign Language) with full linguistic complexity: – Children younger than 7yrs of age were able to extend the system beyond the impov- erished input pidgin – Children between 7 and 16 years were able to learn the existing system, but did not extend it. • This serves as evidence for critical period, and perhaps innateness, since younger children seem particularly susceptable to developing grammars, even in the absence of grammatical stimulus! 3 3 Bilingual acquisition Two parameters of variation: (1) age of acquisition (early = pre-puberty vs. late) (2) order of acquisition (simultaneous vs. succesive/sequential) Simultaneous early bilingualism is also called ”bilingual L1 acquisition. Sequential bilingualism can be either early or late. The age boundaries in bilingual acquisition are set as follows: Age Label stages of L1 acquisition at onset of L2 0-2 Simultaneous, bilingual L1 before the linguistic foundations are in place 4-6 Early sequential, early child L2 spoken language fully developed but no formal schooling 7-10 Early sequential, late child L2 formal instruction in one or two languages 12+ Late sequential, adult L2 L1 fully acquired, only vocabulary size can increase Two other factors that vary: proficiency (level of attainment in each language), and balance (relative proficiency and use of the two languages). The single most important predictor of proficiency of L2 is age of acquisition. 4 Incomplete acquisition Definition: a mature linguistic state, the outcome of language acquisition that is not complete, or of attrition in childhood. Attrition will be considered, but it is not going to be a big factor, because the property must reach complete acquisition and remain stable for a while before it can begin to be lost through attrition (for Montrul). Age, and critical period hypothesis plays a crucial role in L1 loss/maintenance: late L2 learners are less likely to acquire L2 completely, but very likely to maintain full proficiency in L1; child L2 learners are more likely to acquire L2 completely, but very likely to lose ability in L1. Factors besides age that affect L1 maintenance/loss: motivation, language identity, education, peer pressure etc. (these play more of a role for children than for adults). 4.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    4 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us