2.32 Implicit Learning P

2.32 Implicit Learning P

2.32 Implicit Learning P. Perruchet, University of Bourgogne, Dijon, France ª 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 2.32.1 Introduction 598 2.32.2 Rules, Instance-Based Processing, or Sensitivity to Statistical Regularities? 598 2.32.2.1 Learning Rules 598 2.32.2.2 The Instance-Based or Episodic Account 599 2.32.2.3 The Sensitivity to Statistical Regularities 600 2.32.2.4 Rules versus Statistics: A Crucial Test 601 2.32.2.5 The Phenomenon of Transfer: The Data 601 2.32.2.6 The Phenomenon of Transfer: The Interpretations 602 2.32.2.6.1 Rules? 602 2.32.2.6.2 Explicit inferences during the test? 602 2.32.2.6.3 Disentangling rules and abstraction 603 2.32.2.7 A Provisional Conclusion 604 2.32.3 Learning about Statistical Regularities 605 2.32.3.1 What Is Learnable? 605 2.32.3.1.1 Frequency, transitional probability, contingency 605 2.32.3.1.2 Adjacent and nonadjacent dependencies 605 2.32.3.1.3 Processing multiple cues concurrently 606 2.32.3.1.4 Does learning depend on materials? 606 2.32.3.1.5 About the learners 607 2.32.3.2 Statistical Computations and Chunk Formation 607 2.32.3.2.1 Computing statistics? 607 2.32.3.2.2 The formation of chunks 608 2.32.3.2.3 Are statistical computations a necessary prerequisite? 608 2.32.4 How Implicit Is ‘Implicit Learning’? 609 2.32.4.1 Implicitness during the Training Phase 609 2.32.4.1.1 Incidental and intentional learning 609 2.32.4.1.2 Is attention necessary? 609 2.32.4.2 Implicitness during the Test Phase 610 2.32.4.2.1 The lack of conscious knowledge about the study material 610 2.32.4.2.2 The Shanks and St. John information criterion 610 2.32.4.2.3 The Shanks and St. John sensitivity criterion 610 2.32.4.2.4 The problems of forgetting 611 2.32.4.2.5 The problem of the reliability of measures 611 2.32.4.2.6 An intractable issue? 611 2.32.4.2.7 The subjective measures 612 2.32.4.2.8 The lack of control 612 2.32.4.2.9 The lack of intentional exploitation of acquired knowledge 613 2.32.4.3 Processing Fluency and Conscious Experience 613 2.32.4.4 Summary and Discussion 614 2.32.5 Implicit Learning in Real-World Settings 615 2.32.5.1 Exploiting the Properties of Real-World Situations 615 2.32.5.2 Exploiting our Knowledge about Implicit Learning 615 2.32.6 Discussion: About Nativism and Empiricism 616 References 617 597 598 Implicit Learning 2.32.1 Introduction years ago (Reber, 1967). The implications of the results issued from IL research for the nativist/ All of us have learned much without parental super- empiricist debate will be addressed in the final dis- vision and outside of any form of planned academic cussion, after having examined what is learned in this instruction, and more generally without any inten- context, how ‘implicit’ is implicit learning, and the tional attempts to acquire information about the relations of laboratory research with real-world surrounding world. Countless examples could be situations of learning. found in domains as diverse as first-language acquisi- tion, category elaboration, sensitivity to musical structure, acquisition of knowledge about the physi- 2.32.2 Rules, Instance-Based cal world, and various social skills. All of these Processing, or Sensitivity to Statistical domains have several features in common. In partic- Regularities? ular, they are commonly described as governed by 2.32.2.1 Learning Rules complex abstract rules by scientists, whether they would be linguists, musicologists, physicists, or A large part of the literature on IL exploits the sociologists. Also, learning in those situations mainly artificial grammar learning paradigm, initially pro- proceeds through the learner’s exposure to a struc- posed by Reber (1967). Participants first study a set of tured environment, without negative evidence (i.e., letter strings generated from a finite-state grammar without direct information about what would contra- that defines legal letters and permissible transitions dict the rules underlying the domain). between them (Figure 1). Typical instructions do Despite the pervasiveness of these forms of learn- not mention the existence of a grammar and are ing in real-world settings, it is worth stressing that framed so as to discourage participants from engaging they have been virtually ignored by experimental in explicit, intentional analysis of the material. psychology for decades. At the beginning of the cog- Participants are then subsequently informed about the rule-governed nature of the strings and asked to nitive era, the study of learning was essentially categorize new grammatical and nongrammatical let- devoted to classical and operant conditioning on the ter strings. Participants are typically able to perform one hand, and to the formation of concepts or prob- this task with better-than-chance accuracy, while lem solving processes on the other. The above remaining unable to articulate the rules used to gen- phenomena seem hardly reducible to simple condi- erate the material. This empirical outcome has been tioning effects in regards to their complexity, and unambiguously confirmed by a vast number of sub- research on concept learning and problem solving sequent experimental studies involving many does not provide a priori a better account, primarily variants of the situation. due to the fact that the hypothesis testing strategies Reber’s (1967) original proposal was that partici- essential in these research domains do not seem pants have internalized the constraints embodied by applicable in situations where negative evidence is lacking. This empirical and conceptual vacuum opened the door to the upsurge of the nativist per- spective, which characterized the cognitive approach T from its outset. V Out This chapter presents a stream of research that is primarily aimed at exploring the forms of learning M T illustrated in the examples above through laboratory In X Out situations involving arbitrary materials (for over- R views, see Berry and Dienes, 1993; Berry, 1997; V M Cleeremans et al., 1998; French and Cleeremans, Out 2002; Jimenez, 2003; Perruchet and Pacton, 2006; X Reber, 1993; Seger, 1994; Shanks, 2005; Stadler and R Frensch, 1998). This field of research evolved essen- Figure 1 tially from the end of the 1980s, although its roots are The artificial grammar used by Reber and Allen (1978), Dulany et al. (1984), and Perruchet and Pacteau in the pioneering studies of Arthur Reber, who (1990) among others. For example, MTTV and VXVRXVT are coined the term ‘implicit learning’ (IL) about 40 grammatical, whereas MXVT is not grammatical. Implicit Learning 599 the generation of rules during training. Rule abstrac- invariant digit. The authors inferred that participants tion is assumed to occur during the study phase, had learned the critical rule unconsciously. when participants are exposed to a sample of letter These results, and most of the others in the early strings generated from the grammar. During the test IL literature, have been shown to be empirically phase, participants are assumed to use the acquired robust in subsequent studies. However, two other knowledge, stored in an abstract format, to judge the interpretations have been proposed. Their common grammaticality of new items. Other illustrations of intuition is that people do not abstract the rules of the this reasoning can be found in many subsequent domain, but instead learn about the product of the studies. Let us consider those by Lewicki et al. rules. (1988) and McGeorge and Burton (1990). In the Lewicki et al. (1988) paradigm, participants were asked to perform a four-choice reaction-time 2.32.2.2 The Instance-Based or Episodic task, with the targets appearing in one of four quad- Account rants on a computer screen. They were simply asked to track the targets on the numeric keypad of the The first historical alternative to the abstractionist computer as fast as possible. The sequence looked position in the field of artificial grammar learning is like a long and continuous series of randomly located the so-called instance-based or memory-based model targets. However, this sequence was organized on the proposed by Brooks (Brooks, 1978; Vokey and basis of subtle, nonsalient rules. Indeed, unbeknown Brooks, 1992). In Brooks’ model, subjects who are to participants, the sequence was divided into a suc- shown grammatical strings during the study phase cession of ‘logical’ blocks of five trials each. In each store the strings in memory, without any form of block, the first two target locations were random, condensation or summary representation. During while the last three were determined by rules. The the test phase, they judge for grammaticality of test strings as a function of their similarity to specific participants were unable to verbalize the nature of stored strings. The instance-based model works the manipulation and, in particular, they had no because, if no special care is taken to generate the explicit knowledge of the subdivision into logical material, grammatical test items tend to look globally blocks of five trials, which was a precondition that more similar to study items than ungrammatical test had to be satisfied if they were to grasp the other items. rules. However, performance on the final trials of Vokey and Brooks (1992) made independent the each block, the locations of which were predictable usually confounded factors of specific similarity and from the rules, improved at a faster rate and was grammaticality, in order to assess the size of the effect better overall than performance on the first, random, of each factor on grammaticality judgments.

View Full Text

Details

  • File Type
    pdf
  • Upload Time
    -
  • Content Languages
    English
  • Upload User
    Anonymous/Not logged-in
  • File Pages
    25 Page
  • File Size
    -

Download

Channel Download Status
Express Download Enable

Copyright

We respect the copyrights and intellectual property rights of all users. All uploaded documents are either original works of the uploader or authorized works of the rightful owners.

  • Not to be reproduced or distributed without explicit permission.
  • Not used for commercial purposes outside of approved use cases.
  • Not used to infringe on the rights of the original creators.
  • If you believe any content infringes your copyright, please contact us immediately.

Support

For help with questions, suggestions, or problems, please contact us