Nasal-Oral Contrast of Short and Long in : An Acoustic Study

Kofi Adu Manyah Kwame Nkrumah University of Science & Technology, Kumasi, Ghana Institute, University of Strasbourg, France

1. Introduction

The under study has nine oral vowels         and five nasal vowels:       The nasals are the contrasting counterparts of the two high front oral vowels, and the two high back oral vowels,    and the low . Nasal vowels are used for lexical distinctions and they contrast with the oral vowels after voiceless (  ,  ,  ,  as can be seen in examples 1 to 9). As shown in the examples that follow, nasal vowels rarely occur before or after the semi-vowels   and never after voiced consonants like and , in their distribution, unless the vowel is followed by a nasal :

(1)  ‘head’ vs.  ’scratch’

(2) ‘throw, buttocks’  ‘bake, roast’

(3) ‘bite’  ‘say’

(4) ‘take’  ‘half’

(5) ‘go out’  ’dirt’, ‘dirty’

(6) ‘sharpen’  ‘tooth’

(7) ‘hand’  ‘alcoholic drink’

(8) ‘on’  ‘be big’

(9)  ‘character’  ‘cry’

In both the oral and written contexts the Twi language also has two contrasting lengths in words. Quantity is used for lexical and grammatical distinctions where the oral or nasal vowel is either short or long. Quantity contrasts affect the entire vocalic system of the language. Differences in quantity distinguish the present progressive, the present habitual, the present perfect, the simple future and the

* Acknowledgments: This research was supported by grants from the “Perturbations & Réajustements: parole normale vs. parole pathologique” Program of the Maison Interuniversitaire des Sciences de l’Homme-Alsace (MISHA), 2008-2012 and the ANR-07-CORP-018-01-DOCVACIM, 2008-2011. I gratefully acknowledge the work of two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and criticisms that contributed to the improvement of this paper.

© 2011 Kofi Adu Manyah. Selected Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, ed. Eyamba G. Bokamba et al., 95-104. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 96 immediate future from the past forms of the verb. Lexical-grammatical distinctions (verb-noun, verb- adjective, verb-adverb, noun-adjective, noun-adverb and adjective-adverb) are also made through differences in . Some examples are given below:

(10)   ‘mouse’ vs.   ‘village’

(11)  ‘like, desire, same’ vs.  ‘liked, desired’

(12) ‘thicken’ vs. ‘thickened, plenty’

(13)  ‘go’ vs.   ‘went, red’.

The primary aim of this study is to undertake acoustic analyses and to compare the production of oral and nasal vowels and present the acoustic results for the oral and nasal in both the short and long categories. First, durational analyses of oral and nasal short/long vowels are made. Second, formant structure investigations are undertaken for the two phonological classes. Building in part on previous research, this investigation differs from previous work in one important respect. Nasal study or nasal-oral comparison that takes into account both short and long vowels, i.e. phonemic quantity contrasts, has received very little or no attention in the literature. In their acoustic and/or aerodynamic studies (Clairet 2008, Delvaux et al. 2008, d’Imperio et al. 2008), and in their acoustic, articulatory or perception contributions (Maeda 1993, Delvaux et al. 2002, Montagu 2002, Montagu 2004, Delvaux 2006, Beddor 2007, Menard et al. 2007, Amelot et al. 2008, Delvaux 2009), none of these sources refer to phonemic quantity contrasts. In other words, unlike most descriptive or oral-nasal comparative methods proposed in the literature, this study analyzes both short and long vowels of the two (oral-nasal) phonological classes. This is one of the main reasons for this study. Nasals in Twi have also remained under-studied or received very little attention in the literature, so this investigation is a contribution to research on oral-nasal comparison that has been extensively studied in a good number of . The current study, based on acoustic data not on articulatory data, will examine segmental durations and formants of the two phonological classes. The paper is organized as follows. In Section 2, I describe the method used for the durational and spectral analyses. The results, as well as the discussion, are found in Section 3. The conclusion is presented in Section 4.

2. Method

Two male adult native speakers with no speech or hearing impairment were chosen for this experiment. They produced Twi minimal pairs, of the two phonological classes, embedded in a carrier sentence ‘K  __ Kofi’ meaning ‘Say ___ Kofi’. The corpus was made up of oral-nasal contrasts for the short and long categories in a C1VC2 context where C1 is /p/, /t/, /k/, is the short and long oral or nasal vowel, and C2 is /k/ of the carrier phrase. Acoustic recordings were done with a Sennheisser E 845 S directional microphone connected to a Professional Solid State Recorder PMD660. The recordings were done at a self-selected speaking rate in an anechoic room at the laboratory of the Strasbourg Phonetics Institute (Institut de Phonétique de Strasbourg). The randomised list of utterances was produced at least 10 times by each speaker. First, by means of a PRAAT (Boersma & Weenink, 2006) sound editor, the acoustic analysis was performed. Measurements of duration were taken for the target vowel and the post-vocalic consonant /k/, thus obtaining 3 different durations: the target vowel, the post-vocalic consonant and the syllable V+ durations. Second, to control vowel quality for the 2 phonological categories, formant values (F1, F2, F3 and F4) were extracted at three equidistant points within each of the oral and nasal vowels: at 25%, 50% and 75% of the duration of the vowel. The data were then averaged over the ten repetitions of each (short/long) oral and nasal vowel. The extraction of formant values was done manually. Statistical analyses (ANOVAs) were carried out on all measures obtained from the speakers (p00.01). Here are some examples of monosyllabic words from the corpus: 97

(14) ‘bite’ vs.  ‘say’, ‘drive’

(15) ‘bit’ vs.  ‘said’, ‘drove’

(16) ‘throw’, ‘buttocks’ vs.  ‘roast’, ‘bake’

(17) ‘threw’ vs.   ‘roasted’, ‘baked’

(18)  ‘vomit’ (animals) vs.  ‘smoke’ (fish, meat)

(19)  ‘vomited’ vs.  ‘smoked’ (fish, meat)

3. Results and discussion 3.1. Durational data analyses

Vowel data comparison of absolute values in the oral and nasal groups reveals that the nasal vowels are systematically longer than the oral counterparts for both short and long vowels, for all the subjects. This is in agreement with previous findings on vowel durations (Jha 1985, Whalen & Beddor 1989, Duez 2006, Lovatto et al. 2007, Delattre & Mannot 2009). As depicted in figures 1 and 2, short  measures 88 ms and long  252 ms with very low standard deviations of 11 ms and 29 ms, whereas the nasal counterpart measures 113 ms and 279 ms (low standard deviations of 14 ms and 23 ms) for the short and long respectively for Speaker 1. The corresponding measures for the second speaker are 58 ms and 124 ms (standard deviations of 10 ms and 28 ms) for oral and 87 ms and 195 ms, with low standard deviations of 09 ms and 25 ms for nasal  .

450

400

350

300

250

200 duration (ms) 150

100

50

0       

oral and nasal vowels & post vocalic consonants

short standard deviation long standard deviation

Figure 1. This figure shows average oral vowel, nasal vowel and post-vocalic consonant durations and standard deviations for the first speaker (ms) 98

350

300

250

200

150 duration (ms)

100

50

0      

oral and nasal vowels & post vocalic consonants

short standard deviation long standard deviation 

Figure 2. This figure shows average oral vowel, nasal vowel and post-vocalic consonant durations and standard deviations for the second speaker (ms)

Relative values show that in the VC domain phonologically short vowels are followed by phonetically long consonants. As shown in Adu Manyah (2003), differences in consonant duration between short and long oral vowels are statistically significant (p>0.001). With regards to phonologically long oral vowels, three cases can be observed: the post-vocalic consonant is, either slightly longer than, equally long or slightly shorter than the vowel. Thus, there seems to be no compensatory relation between the long vowel and the consonant in the VC domain. However, this is not the case with the nasal vowels since there is a compensatory relation between phonologically long vowels and post-vocalic consonants: phonologically long vowels are systematically followed by phonetically short consonants in the VC domain. The durational figures for Speaker 1 are: 279 ms and 223 ms, 293 ms and 231 ms for  and respectively. The corresponding durational data for the second speaker are 195 ms and 148 ms for  , 214 ms and 191 ms for   and 198 ms and 143 ms for   (see figures 1 and 2 again). In the VC domain, phonologically short vowels are followed by phonetically long consonants in both the oral and nasal categories. Post-vocalic consonant duration differences seem to reinforce vowel quantity contrasts in the language under study. For the short nasal vowels    we have the following ratios: 0.31, 0.38, and 0.37 for Speaker 1. The data for the second speaker are 0.33, 0.45, and 0.38. The corresponding data for the short oral vowels  are 0.22, 0.27, 0.23 for the first speaker and 0.18, 0.27 and 0.26 for the second speaker. We can notice, by comparing the ratios, that the nasal category of vowels registers higher ratios than the oral category for both speakers. Differences in consonant duration between long and short vowels have also been attested for in the dialects of Modern Swedish (Schaeffling & Wretling. 2003). The authors further posit that there is a complementarity pattern whereby the long vowel+consonant (VGC) sequences have the tendency of possessing almost the same duration as the short vowel+consonant (VCG) sequences. According to their findings the VCG types are in general slightly shorter in total duration than the VGC sequences. In the present study, the phenomenon seems to be observed for all oral and nasal vowels produced by the first Twi speaker (67% of all cases), where the VCG sequences are in general slightly shorter than the VGC counterparts. However, in the case of the second speaker, the complementarity pattern seems to apply 99 partially, without reference to the vowel duration. Indeed, the tendency is verified in only 39% of the cases in both the oral and nasal contexts. Comparing the data of only the nasal vowels, it can be observed that this trend applies to the two front vowels    and the   but not to the 2 back vowels   and   for speaker 1. The short vowel  + consonant sequence has an average duration of 473 ms as compared to 502 ms for the long vowel   + consonant type. The average is 395 ms and 526 ms for the pair short  + consonant and long   + consonant, and 502 ms compared to 517 ms for short  + consonant and long  + consonant. Concerning the two nasal back vowels  and  , the average values are 524 ms and 543 ms respectively for the category of short vowel + consonant and 520 ms and 524 ms for the category of long vowel + consonant. For the second speaker, the short  + consonant (238 ms) vs. long   + consonant (308 ms), short  + consonant (378 ms) vs long  + consonant (405 ms) and  + consonant (316 ms) vs  + consonant (341 ms) series, conform to the model. Concerning the other two nasal vowels, it is rather short  and  + consonants (350 ms and 372 ms) that are longer than   and  + consonants (343 ms and 333 ms). (See figures 1 and 2 again). Thus, the observation on the complementarity pattern is in accordance with results obtained for Bolognese where it is shown that the phenomenon is only partially applicable (Hajek 1994). Two remarks can be made to conclude this section. First, the findings of this study on phonological durational contrasts and the contribution of the post-vocalic consonant to distinguish the two phonological classes have already been documented on studies in Thai (Mixdorff et al. 2002) and Bolognese (Hajek 1994). Second, in the group of long vowels, figures 1 and 2 clearly show that post- vocalic consonant durational values are close to vowel durations. The difference between the two average values is not statistically significant (p=ns). Even though the two speakers have different absolute values for vowel and consonant durations, which can be explained by individual speaking rates (the second speaker has a relatively faster speaking rate than the first speaker as illustrated in figures 1 and 2), we observe, thanks to relative data analysis, that it is basically the same strategy that is adopted by the two speakers to preserve phonological contrasts and distinguish between the two phonological sets. A comparison of the oral and nasal classes shows that the two speakers maintain the proportion of the vowel relatively stable within the VC syllable domain: a difference of around 10 % separates both phonologically short and long categories. Such results concerning oral vowels have been previously reported for unrelated languages like Swedish and Wolof (Sock et al. 1996).

3.2. Spectral data analyses

As illustrated in figures 3 and 4, the comparison of formant values of the oral and nasal vowels shows that F2 of the nasal vowels are lower than the oral counterparts, particularly for the high front short vowels  and (1651 Hz for the nasals and 2117 Hz for the oral counterparts for  and 2061 Hz and 2066 Hz for ). The corresponding F2 values for the long vowels  are 1626 Hz for the nasal and 2176 Hz for the oral counterpart. The F2 values for  are 1966 Hz and 2120 Hz for the nasal and oral categories respectively. The results in the present study seem similar to the F2 results obtained for French nasal vowels (Delvaux et al. 2002). However, this does not seem to apply to the low vowel  and the two high back vowels  and   In fact, for the high   it is rather F2 of the oral vowel which is lower than the nasal counterpart: oral (1236 Hz) and nasal (1519 Hz) for the short vowels, and oral (1079 Hz) and nasal (1432 Hz) for the long vowels (see figures 3 and 4 again). The comparison also reveals that, in the oral and nasal categories, the formants F1, F2, F3 and F4 of the long and short vowels do not exhibit any significant differences (p=ns). 100

5000 4500 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 frequency (Hz) 1000 500 0 

oral and nasal short vowels F1 oral F2 oral F3 oral F4 oral F1 nasal F2 nasal F3 nasal F4 nasal

Figure 3. This figure shows formant values for short oral and nasal vowels  for Speaker 2

5000 4500 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 frequency (Hz) 1000 500 0 

oral and nasal long vowels F1 oral F2 oral F3 oral F4 oral F1 nasal F2 nasal F3 nasal F4 nasal

Figure 4. This figure shows formant values for long oral and nasal vowels GG GG Gfor Speaker 2 101

On one hand, a comparison of F1 and F2 in the oral and nasal categories shows that the front vowels are characterized by wider or distant F1 and F2 whereas the back vowels are characterized by narrower or close F1 and F2. This is consistent with previous investigations on nasals where it is shown that for a back vowel F2 is low and close to F1, whereas for a front vowel F2 is high and far from F1 (Stevens. 1985, Menard et al. 2007). On the other hand, an F2 and F3 comparison indicates a closer characteristic for front vowels and wider or distant characteristic for back vowels. In other words, F2 is close to F3 for front vowels and but distant for the back vowels and . The same characteristics about vowels are found in an F3 and F4 comparison (see figures 3 and 4 again). In order to take a closer look at the three extreme vowels of the vowel triangle   the settings of the formant parameters were fixed for the sound editor, oral and nasal curves were plotted on the same graph allowing comparison of analytical data. Comparing formant values for oral and nasal vowels, we observe the following for the long vowels: for the high back vowel   there are very small differences in the formant structure of the oral-nasal contrast which are not statistically significant (p=ns). For the low vowel  the relatively weak difference in the formant structure is located at the first formant: F2, F3, F4 are almost identical (figure 5). It seems from the foregoing that quality differences between oral and nasal vowels operate more within the high front vowel  than the high back vowel   and the low vowel  Data also suggest that, for the high front vowel, the quality differences operate at the second and third formants rather than the first formant, unlike the low open vowel For the low vowel, the relative difference in quality is found at the first formant as illustrated in figure 5 where, on one hand, nasalization causes F1 to lower. On the other hand, nasalization seems to cause F2 to raise in the case of the high back vowel but not in that of the high front vowel  as depicted in figures 3 and 4.

4500 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000

frequency (Hz) frequency 1500 1000 500 0 iau oral and nasal long vowels

F1 oral F2 oral F3 oral F4 oral F1 nasal F2 nasal F3 nasal F4 nasal

Figure 5. Oral and nasal long vowels: Comparison of formant values for the three extreme vowels of the vowel triangle   for the second speaker 102

Assuming also that vowel dispersion refers to how dispersed the vowels are in phonological space, the scatter plots (F1 on the ordinate, the vertical axis and F2 on the abscissa, the horizontal axis) prepared to represent data, clearly illustrate that the oral vowels   are less widely dispersed in phonological space than the nasal counterparts. The phonological space is defined by the maximum and minimum values of F1 and F2. This is found to be reduced for the oral vowels. In this dispersion, each vowel’s distance from the average of all vowel formants is not taken into account (see figures 6 and 7).

F2

2500 2300 2100 1900 1700 1500 1300 1100 900 700 500 100

200

300

400

500 F1 600

700

800

900

i a u

Figure 6. Dispersion of long nasal vowels     for Speaker 2 (Hz)

F2 2500 2300 2100 1900 1700 1500 1300 1100 900 700 500 100

200

300

400

500 F1

600

700

800

900

 

Figure 7. Dispersion of long oral vowels   for Speaker 2 (Hz) 103

4. Conclusion

This study has shown on the basis of acoustic data, that nasal vowels in Twi are longer than their oral counterparts in both the short and long contexts. Thus the current study supports the durational analyses of orals and nasals reported in previous studies (Jha 1985, Whalen & Beddor 1989, Duez 2006, Lovatto et al. 2007, Delattre & Mannot 2009). Whereas phonologically long nasal vowels are systematically followed by phonetically short consonants in the VC domain, phonologically long oral vowels and post-vocalic consonants show no coherent compensatory behavior. Phonologically short vowels are followed by phonetically long consonants in the oral and nasal contexts. Acoustic evidence also shows that the formant values are generally lower for the nasal vowels, particularly the formant F2 in this study, than the oral vowels. This confirms the tendency which is characteristic of nasal vowels. However, the findings of this study suggest that this rule does not apply to the high back vowel   in that F2 values are rather lower for the oral vowels than the nasal vowels in both the short and long categories. I have also shown that even though the two phonological classes (oral and nasal vowels) exhibit slight qualitative differences which are not statistically significant, the front vowel  shows a relatively significant difference at the second and third formants compared to   and , suggesting that the degree of nasality, and for that matter nasal-oral contrast, depends on the vowel type. Acoustic investigations further reveal that the nasal vowels are more widely dispersed than the oral vowels in phonological space.

References  Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2002. Introduction à la phonétique et à la phonologie africaines. Les sons de tous les jours: le cas akan (TWI). [Introduction to African Phonetics and Phonology. Everyday Sounds: The Case of Akan (TWI)]. Paris: l’Harmattan. Adu Manyah, Kofi, and Sock, Rudolph. 2002. La quantité vocalique en twi. Quelques considérations phonologiques et analyses acoustiques préliminaires. [Vowel Quantity in Twi. Phonological Considerations and Preliminary Acoustic Analyses]. XXIVèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole (JEP) de l’AFCP. Nancy. 41- 44. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2003a. Vowel quantity contrasts in Twi, 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS), Barcelona. 3185-3188. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2003b. Durée et qualité des contrastes vocaliques en twi. [Duration and Quality of Vowel Contrasts in Twi]. Travaux de l’Institut de Phonétique de Strasbourg 33. 1-23. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2005. Quantité et qualité vocaliques en twi: le cas des voyelles nasales. [On Quantity and Quality of Twi Nasal Vowels] XVèmes Rencontres Linguistiques en Pays Rhénan. SCOLIA 20. 9-30. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2006a. Relation Between Tone and Vowel Quality in Twi. The 2nd International Conference on Tonal Aspects of Languages (TAL 2006). La Rochelle. 37-40. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2006b. A propos du trait ATR des voyelles nasales du twi. [ATR Features of Twi Nasal Vowels] XXVIèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole (JEP) de l’AFCP. Dinard. 171-174. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2008. On Quantity and Quality of Oral and Nasal Vowels in Twi. 8th International Seminar on Speech Production, Strasbourg. 89-92. Adu Manyah, Kofi. 2010. Parlons twi. Langue et culture. [Let’s Speak Twi. Language and Culture] Paris: l’Harmattan. Amelot, Angélique; Basset, Patricia; Maeda, Shinji; Honda, Kiyoshi; and Crevier-Buchman, Lise. 2008. Etude simultanée des mouvements du voile du palais et de l’ouverture du port vélopharyngé. [A Simultaneous Study of the Movements of the Soft Palate and Velopharyngeal Port Opening]. XXVIIèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole (JEP) de l’AFCP. Avignon. 65-68. Beddor, Patrice Speeter. 2007. Nasals and Nasalization: The Relation between Segmental and Coarticulatory Timing. 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS) Saarbrücken. 249-254. Boersma, Paul and Weenink, David. 2006. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 4.4.34) http://www.praat.org/ Clairet, Sandrine. 2008. Une étude aérodynamique de la nasalité vocalique en français méridional. [An Aerodynamic Study of Southern French Nasal Vowels]. XXVIIèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole (JEP) de l’AFCP. Avignon. 297-300. 104

Delattre, Pierre, and Mannot, M. 2009. The Role of Duration in the Identification of French Nasal Vowels. IRAL, 6. 267-288. Delvaux, Véronique, Metens, Thierry, and Soquet, Alain. 2002. Propriétés acoustiques et articulatoires des voyelles nasales du français. [Acoustic and Articulatory Properties of French Nasal vowels]. XXIVèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole. Nancy. 348-352. Delvaux, Véronique, Metens, Thierry, and Soquet, Alain. 2002. French Nasal Vowels: acoustic and articulatory Properties. Proceedings of the 7th ICSLP, 53-56. Denver. Delvaux, Véronique. 2006. Production des voyelles nasales en français québécois. [Production of Nasal Vowels in Quebec French]. XXVIèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole. Dinard. 383-386. Delvaux, Véronique, Demolin, Didier, Harmegnies, Bernard, and Soquet, Alain. 2008. The aerodynamics of nazalization in French. Journal of Phonetics 36. 578-606. Delvaux, Véronique. 2009. Perception du contraste de nasalité vocalique en français. [Perception of French Nasal Vowel Contrasts]. Journal of Studies 19. 25-59. D’Imperio, Mariapaola, Raposo de Medeiros, Beatriz, and Espesser, Robert. 2008. La voyelle nasale en Portugais Brésilien et son appendice nasal: étude acoustique et aérodynamique. [The Nasal Vowel: An Acoustic and Aerodynamic Study]. XXVIIèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole (JEP) de l’AFCP. Avignon. 285-288. Duez, Danielle. 2006. Consonant and Vowel Duration in Parkinsonian French Speech. Speech Prosody. Dresden. Hajek, John. 1994. Phonological Length and Phonetic Duration in Bolognese: are they related? Proceedings of the Fifth Australian International Conference on Speech Science and Technology, 662-667. Perth. Jha, Sunil Kumar. 1985. The nasal vowels in Maithili: An Acoustic Study. Contributions to Nepalese Studies 13. 19-32. Lehiste, Isle. 1970. Suprasegmentals. M.I.T. Press. Cambridge. Lovatto, L., Amélot, Angélique, Crevier-Buchman, Lise, Basset, Patricia, and Vaissière, Jacqueline. 2007. A Fiberscopic Analysis of nasal vowels in Brazilian Portuguese. 16th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Saarbrucken. 549-552. Maeda, Shinji. 1993. Acoustics of Vowel Nasalization and Articulatory Shifts in French Nasal Vowels.) In Phonetics & Phonology. vol. 5: Nasals, Nasalization, and the Velum, ed. by M. K. Huffman and P. A. Krakaw, 147-167. Academic Press. Menard, Lucie, Schwartz, Jean-Luc, Boe, Louis-Jean, and Aubin, J. 2007. Articulatory-acoustic relationships during vocal tract growth for French vowels: Analysis of real data and simulations with an articulatory model. Journal of Phonetics 35. 1-19. Mixdorff, Hansjorg, Luksaneeyanawin, Sudaporn, Fujisaki, Hiroya, and Charnvivit, Patavee. 2002. Perception of Tone and Vowel Quantity in Thai. Proceedings of ICSLP, 753-756. Denver. Montagu, Julie. 2004. Les sons sous-jacents aux voyelles nasales en français parisien : indices perceptifs des changements. [Sounds Underlying Nasal Vowels in Parisian French: Perceptual cues of Changes] XXVèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole. Fez. Montagu, Julie. 2002. L’articulation labiale des voyelles nasales postérieures du français : comparaison entre locuteurs français et anglo-américains. [Labial Articulation of Back Nasal Vowels in French: A Comparison between French and Anglo-American Speakers]. XXIVèmes Journées d’Étude sur la Parole. Nancy. 253- 256. Padgett, Jaye. 1997. Perceptual Distance of Contrast: Vowel Height and Nasality. Phonology at Santa Cruz 5. 63- 78. Sock, Rudolph, Perrier, Pascal, and Löfqvist, Anders. 1996. Kinematic and Acoustic Correlates of Quantity in Swedish and Wolof: A Cross-Language Study, 1st ESCA Tutorial & Research Workshop on Speech Production Modeling: From Control Strategies to Acoustics & 4th Speech Production Seminar: Models & Data. Autrans. 81-84. Schaeffler, Felix, and Wretling, Par. 2003. Towards a Typology of Phonological Quantity in the Dialects of Modern Swedish. 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona. 2697-2700. Stevens, Kenneth N. 1985. Spectral prominences and phonetic distinctions in language. Speech Communication 4. 137-144. Uguzzoni, Arianna, Azzaro, Gabriele, and Schmid, Stephan. 2003. Short vs Long and/or Abruptly vs Smoothly cut Vowels. New Perspectives on a Debated Question. 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona. 2717-2720. Tronnier, Mechtild. 1995. A Method for Tracing Nasality, International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS). Stockholm. 452-455. Whalen, Doug H., and Beddor, Patrice Speeter. 1989. Connections between nasality and vowel duration and height. Elucidation of the Eastern Algonquian intrusive nasal. Language 65. 457-486. Selected Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics: African Languages and Linguistics Today edited by Eyamba G. Bokamba, Ryan K. Shosted, and Bezza Tesfaw Ayalew

Cascadilla Proceedings Project Somerville, MA 2011

Copyright information

Selected Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics: African Languages and Linguistics Today © 2011 Cascadilla Proceedings Project, Somerville, MA. All rights reserved ISBN 978-1-57473-446-1 library binding A copyright notice for each paper is located at the bottom of the first page of the paper. Reprints for course packs can be authorized by Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

Ordering information

Orders for the library binding edition are handled by Cascadilla Press. To place an order, go to www.lingref.com or contact:

Cascadilla Press, P.O. Box 440355, Somerville, MA 02144, USA phone: 1-617-776-2370, fax: 1-617-776-2271, [email protected]

Web access and citation information

This entire proceedings can also be viewed on the web at www.lingref.com. Each paper has a unique document # which can be added to citations to facilitate access. The document # should not replace the full citation.

This paper can be cited as:

Manyah, Kofi Adu. 2011. Nasal-Oral Contrast of Short and Long Vowels in Twi: An Acoustic Study. In Selected Proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics, ed. Eyamba G. Bokamba et al., 95-104. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. www.lingref.com, document #2568.