Analysis of a Choice-Reaction Task Yields a New Interpretation of Libet's Experiments ⁎ Christoph S

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Analysis of a Choice-Reaction Task Yields a New Interpretation of Libet's Experiments ⁎ Christoph S Available online at www.sciencedirect.com International Journal of Psychophysiology 67 (2008) 151–157 www.elsevier.com/locate/ijpsycho Fast track report Analysis of a choice-reaction task yields a new interpretation of Libet's experiments ⁎ Christoph S. Herrmann a,b,e, , Michael Pauen b,d,f, Byoung-Kyong Min a, Niko A. Busch a, Jochem W. Rieger c a Otto-von-Guericke-University, Dept. for Biological Psychology, Magdeburg, Germany b Center for Behavioural Brain Sciences, Magdeburg, Germany c Otto-von-Guericke-University, Dept., for Neurology II, Magdeburg, Germany d Humboldt-University, Institute for Philosophy, Berlin, Germany e Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Science, Leipzig, Germany f Berlin School of Mind and Brain, Germany Received 23 March 2007; received in revised form 11 October 2007; accepted 15 October 2007 Available online 22 November 2007 Abstract Benjamin Libet has demonstrated that the readiness potential precedes the time at which participants consciously decide to perform an intentional motor act, and suggested that free will is an illusion. We performed an experiment where participants observed a stimulus on a computer monitor and were instructed to press one of two buttons, depending on the presented stimulus. We found neural activity preceding the motor response, similar to Libet's experiments. However, this activity was already present prior to stimulus presentation, and thus before participants could decide which button to press. Therefore, we argue that this activity does not specifically determine behaviour. Instead, it may reflect a general expectation. This interpretation would not interfere with the notion of free will. © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Bereitschaftsfeld; Bereitschaftspotential; EEG; MEG; Voluntary action 1. Introduction determined, since it is known that this activity precedes motor acts. Libet's results showed that RPs preceded W by 350 ms. The actual In several experiments, Benjamin Libet and his colleagues motor act followed W after about 200 ms (Libet, 1985). Libet investigated the sequence of events between the neural preparation concluded that W and, consequently, the motor act were initiated of a motor act, the conscious intention to act, and the actual motor unconsciously ‘by the brain’, before the participant was aware of act (Libet et al., 1982, 1983). In the critical conditions, the his or her wish to move. Accordingly, it was concluded that the participants were instructed to flex their hand whenever they participant's conscious will plays no causal role in the initiation of wished to do so, and to determine the time of the related conscious the motor act (Libet et al., 1982; Libet, 1985). This interpretation act of will (W) from a fast rotating dot on a clockface. At the same was provocative and elicited an ongoing debate (summarized in time, the emergence of the so called ‘Bereitschaftspotential’ Libet, 1993). The increasing interest in neurophysiological results (readiness potential, RP; Kornhuber and Deecke, 1965)was and their interpretations in philosophy and popular science and the normative conclusions from Libet's results drawn by neurophy- siologists (Libet, 1993) demonstrate that these results may have a strong impact on human self-understanding. Therefore, Libet's ⁎ Corresponding author. Otto-von-Guericke-University of Magdeburg, PO experiments and their premises should be examined carefully. box 4120 39016 Magdeburg, Germany. Tel.: +49 391 67 18477; fax: +49 391 67 12222. Aside from the questionable generalisation of a millisecond E-mail address: [email protected] time scale to long term behavioural planning, Libet's conclu- (C.S. Herrmann). sions critically depend on several assumptions. One of them is 0167-8760/$ - see front matter © 2007 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.10.013 152 C.S. Herrmann et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 67 (2008) 151–157 the idea that the RP determines the specific voluntary movement, movement. Accordingly, 40% of the W-times reported by and not just indicates a general preparation to move. However, as Trevena and Miller (2002) were later than the movements. Bridgeman (1985) pointed out, the participants' movements in Two conclusions can be drawn from these observations: First, Libet's experiments were requested by the experimenter. The W-judgements are problematic in time-critical tasks. Second, the participants were only ‘free’ to decide when to perform the specificity of the causal role of the RP (and of the LRP) with respect requested movement within a limited time range of several to W and the subsequent movement is still unclear. Although seconds, and only once per trial. This raises the question of the results of Haggard and Eimer (1999) and Trevena and Miller whether Libet in fact investigated decisions, given that there (2002) raise doubts whether the RP really determines W and were no alternatives to choose between. The decisive point is the movement, they provide no conclusive evidence with respect that, if the instructions determined what the participants were to to this problem. A critical test for the specificity of the causal role of do, it is less clear whether the RP really determines what the the RP that is not affected by problems concerning W-judgements, participants will do. Maybe it leaves room for different actions, would be to elicit an RP at a time when it is still uncertain which including, say, a movement of the other hand. This offers motor act has to be performed. If different movements can be participants the ability to do otherwise, which is often regarded performed after RP onset, this would undermine the assumption as one of the most important components of free action. that the RP determines a specific movement. This problem is also discussed by Haggard and Eimer We used a choice-reaction paradigm in which participants were (1999). In one of their experiments, participants could choose instructed to press a button with either their right or left hand after between a movement of the left or the right hand. The authors they had perceived the appropriate visual stimulus. Participants in recorded the lateralized RP (LRP; Eimer, 1998) in addition to our experiment did not make a free choice, because we were only the RP. While the RP is symmetric and represents the early part interested in the role of one constituent of voluntary acts, namely of the motor activity preceding movements, the LRP represents the RP. The advantage of a choice-reaction paradigm is that the a later phase and is lateralised to the hemisphere contralateral to presentation of the stimulus provides a clear terminus ante, with the side of movement. Haggard and Eimer (1999) analysed respect to the participant's knowledge of the response that has to covariations between the time of W and the onset times of the be performed, thus evading all the difficulties with W-judgements. RP/LRP. It turned out that the onset time of the RP did not covary with the W-time, while the onset time of the LRP did. 2. Materials and methods Based on the assumption that causally linked processes should covary over time, the authors argued that only the LRP, but not Sixteen subjects participated in the study. Six of them had to the RP, may be the unconscious cause of the conscious act of be rejected due to excessive artefacts or inappropriate will. However, there are several reasons why Haggard & behavioural performance. Data of ten participants with a Eimer's results are inconclusive as far as the causal role of the mean age of 21.7 years (ranging from 18 to 25 years, 6 female) RP is concerned. First, a causal connection between the RP and were analyzed. Participants gave written informed consent prior W cannot be ruled out completely, because the authors admit to the start of the experiment. All participants had normal or that they could not determine a clear baseline for the RP within corrected to normal vision and had no recorded history of the epoch they investigated. Note that, according to their own neurological or psychiatric disorders. The study protocol standards, such a covariation would provide evidence for a conformed with the guidelines of the ethics committee of the causal connection between the RP and W. Second, the causal Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Science connection between LRP and W is questionable. Two of the (Leipzig, Germany) and the Declaration of Helsinki. eight participants in the early W condition (participants 5 and 8 The present investigation is a reanalysis of the data reported in Table 2 in Haggard and Eimer, 1999) reported W-times in Herrmann and Mecklinger (2000). The stimulus material before LRP onset. So, although there is still a statistically consisted of two Kanizsa figures, i.e. a Kanizsa square and a significant covariance between the LRP and W, this observation Kanizsa triangle, and two non-Kanizsa figures, i.e. a non- raises doubts concerning a causal interpretation of this covari- Kanizsa square and a non-Kanizsa triangle (cf. Herrmann and ation. In addition, the experimental setup does not provide Mecklinger, 2000, for details). Stimuli were presented for conclusive evidence concerning the time of the decision be- 700 ms with randomized interstimulus-intervals ranging from tween the two options. It may well be that participants made 1000 to 1500 ms. Figures were displayed in black along with a their choice between left and right-hand movements shortly black central fixation cross on white background. Stimuli after completing the previous trial and long before W. In this subtended a visual angle of 4.28°, including inducer disks. We case, the W-judgement would reflect only the moment when the used a liquid crystal display (LCD) monitor whithout raster predetermined action is triggered. Moreover, W-judgements scan.
Recommended publications
  • Human Cortical Oscillations: a Neuromagnetic View Through the Skull
    R EVIEW R. Hari and R. Salmelin – Human cortical rhythms VA -IN S I N V Human cortical oscillations: a O E N I neuromagnetic view through the skull M G AG I N Riitta Hari and Riitta Salmelin The mammalian cerebral cortex generates a variety of rhythmic oscillations, detectable directly from the cortex or the scalp. Recent non-invasive recordings from intact humans, by means of neuromagnetometers with large sensor arrays, have shown that several regions of the healthy human cortex have their own intrinsic rhythms, typically 8–40 Hz in frequency, with modality- and frequency-specific reactivity. The conventional hypotheses about the functional significance of brain rhythms extend from epiphenomena to perceptual binding and object segmentation. Recent data indicate that some cortical rhythms can be related to periodic activity of peripheral sensor and effector organs. Trends Neurosci. (1997) 20, 44–49 EURONES IN THE HUMAN BRAIN, especially in signals can be identified easily. By contrast, MEG (and Nthalamic nuclei and in the cerebral cortex, ex- EEG) sensors pick up signals from extensive brain hibit intrinsic oscillations1–3, which probably form the regions, which might be even several centimetres basis for macroscopic rhythms, detectable with electro- away from the sensor. Therefore the sites of active encephalography (EEG) and magnetoencephalography neuronal populations have to be deduced from the (MEG). Analysis of cortical rhythms forms an essential measured signal distribution. Although this ‘inverse part of clinical EEG evaluation, which relies on corre- problem’ does not have a unique solution in the gen- lations between the signal phenomenology and brain eral case6,9, modelling the generators of MEG signals as disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • Brain Activity Relating to the Contingent Negative Variation: an Fmri Investigation
    www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 21 (2004) 1232–1241 Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation Y. Nagai,a,* H.D. Critchley,b E. Featherstone,b P.B.C. Fenwick,c M.R. Trimble,a and R.J. Dolanb a Institute of Neurology, Department of Clinical and Experimental Epilepsy, London WC1N 3BG, UK b Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Institute of Neurology, UCL, London WC1N 3BG, UK c Institute of Psychiatry, KCL, London SE5 8AF GB, UK Received 6 May 2003; revised 30 October 2003; accepted 31 October 2003 The contingent negative variation (CNV) is a long-latency electro- and S2) at the vertex, has been termed the ‘‘expectancy wave’’ encephalography (EEG) surface negative potential with cognitive and (Walter et al., 1964). A more general model of the CNVencapsulates motor components, observed during response anticipation. CNV is an a concept of cortical arousal related to anticipatory attention, index of cortical arousal during orienting and attention, yet its preparation, motivation, and information processing (Tecce, 1972). functional neuroanatomical basis is poorly understood. We used Neurophysiological studies indicate that cortical surface-nega- functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with simultaneous EEG and recording of galvanic skin response (GSR) to investigate tive potentials, such as the CNV, result from depolarization of CNV-related central neural activity and its relationship to peripheral apical dendrites of cortical pyramidal cells by thalamic afferents autonomic arousal. In a group analysis, blood oxygenation level and reflect excitation over an extended cortical area (Birbaumer et dependent (BOLD) activity during the period of CNV generation was al., 1990).
    [Show full text]
  • Electroencephalographic Correlates of Temporal Bayesian Belief Updating and Surprise
    Electroencephalographic Correlates of Temporal Bayesian Belief Updating and Surprise a,b,* a b,c d c,e Antonino Visalli , Mariagrazia Capizzi , Ettore Ambrosini , Bruno Kopp , Antonino Vallesi a Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova, 35128 Padova, Italy b Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy c Department of Neuroscience & Padova Neuroscience Center, University of Padova, 35131 Padova, Italy d Department of Neurology, Hannover Medical School, 30625 Hannover, Germany e Brain Imaging and Neural Dynamics Research Group, IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, 30126 Venice, Italy *Address for correspondence: Antonino Visalli Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova Via Giustiniani 5, 35128 Padova, Italy Phone number: (+39) 049 8214450 Email: [email protected] 1 Abstract The brain predicts the timing of forthcoming events to optimize responses to them. Temporal predictions have been formalized in terms of the hazard function, which integrates prior beliefs on the likely timing of stimulus occurrence with information conveyed by the passage of time. However, how the human brain updates prior temporal beliefs is still elusive. Here we investigated electroencephalographic (EEG) signatures associated with Bayes-optimal updating of temporal beliefs. Given that updating usually occurs in response to surprising events, we sought to disentangle EEG correlates of updating from those associated with surprise. Twenty-siX participants performed a temporal foreperiod task, which comprised a subset of surprising events not eliciting updating. EEG data were analyzed through a regression-based massive approach in the electrode and source space. Distinct late positive, centro-parietally distributed, event-related potentials (ERPs) were associated with surprise and belief updating in the electrode space.
    [Show full text]
  • Investigating the Association Between Parity and the Maternal Neural Response to Infant Cues
    Social Neuroscience ISSN: 1747-0919 (Print) 1747-0927 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/psns20 Investigating the association between parity and the maternal neural response to infant cues Angela N. Maupin, Helena J. V. Rutherford, Nicole Landi, Marc N. Potenza & Linda C. Mayes To cite this article: Angela N. Maupin, Helena J. V. Rutherford, Nicole Landi, Marc N. Potenza & Linda C. Mayes (2018): Investigating the association between parity and the maternal neural response to infant cues, Social Neuroscience, DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1422276 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1422276 Accepted author version posted online: 02 Jan 2018. Published online: 08 Jan 2018. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 32 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=psns20 SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2018 https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2017.1422276 ARTICLE Investigating the association between parity and the maternal neural response to infant cues Angela N. Maupina, Helena J. V. Rutherforda, Nicole Landia,b,c, Marc N. Potenzaa,d and Linda C. Mayesa aYale Child Study Center, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA; bHaskins Laboratories, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA; cDepartment of Psychology, University of Connecticut, New Haven, CT, USA; dDepartments of Psychiatry and Neurobiology and CASAColumbia, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Understanding the maternal neural response to infant affective cues has important implications for Received 8 June 2017 parent-child relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • ERP Peaks Review 1 LINKING BRAINWAVES to the BRAIN
    ERP Peaks Review 1 LINKING BRAINWAVES TO THE BRAIN: AN ERP PRIMER Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Psychological and Brain Sciences University of Louisville Louisville, Kentucky Short title: ERPs Peak Review. Key Words: ERP, peak, latency, brain activity source, electrophysiology. Please address all correspondence to: Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Ph.D. Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences 317 Life Sciences, University of Louisville Louisville, KY 40292-0001. [email protected] ERP Peaks Review 2 Linking Brainwaves To The Brain: An ERP Primer Alexandra Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Abstract This paper reviews literature on the characteristics and possible interpretations of the event- related potential (ERP) peaks commonly identified in research. The description of each peak includes typical latencies, cortical distributions, and possible brain sources of observed activity as well as the evoking paradigms and underlying psychological processes. The review is intended to serve as a tutorial for general readers interested in neuropsychological research and a references source for researchers using ERP techniques. ERP Peaks Review 3 Linking Brainwaves To The Brain: An ERP Primer Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Over the latter portion of the past century recordings of brain electrical activity such as the continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) and the stimulus-relevant event-related potentials (ERPs) became frequent tools of choice for investigating the brain’s role in the cognitive processing in different populations. These electrophysiological recording techniques are generally non-invasive, relatively inexpensive, and do not require participants to provide a motor or verbal response.
    [Show full text]
  • Rapid P300 Brain-Computer Interface Communication with a Head-Mounted Display
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 05 June 2015 doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00207 Rapid P300 brain-computer interface communication with a head-mounted display Ivo Käthner 1*, Andrea Kübler 1 and Sebastian Halder 1, 2 1 Institute of Psychology, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany, 2 Department of Rehabilitation for Brain Functions, Research Institute of National Rehabilitation Center for Persons with Disabilities, Tokorozawa, Japan Visual ERP (P300) based brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) allow for fast and reliable spelling and are intended as a muscle-independent communication channel for people with severe paralysis. However, they require the presentation of visual stimuli in the field of view of the user. A head-mounted display could allow convenient presentation of visual stimuli in situations, where mounting a conventional monitor might be difficult or not feasible (e.g., at a patient’s bedside). To explore if similar accuracies can be achieved with a virtual reality (VR) headset compared to a conventional flat screen monitor, we conducted an experiment with 18 healthy participants. We also evaluated it with a person in the locked-in state (LIS) to verify that usage of the headset is possible for Edited by: Emanuel Donchin, a severely paralyzed person. Healthy participants performed online spelling with three University of South Florida, USA different display methods. In one condition a 5 × 5 letter matrix was presented on a Reviewed by: conventional 22 inch TFT monitor. Two configurations of the VR headset were tested. In Fabien Lotte, the first (glasses A), the same 5×5 matrix filled the field of view of the user. In the second INRIA (National Institute for Computer Science and Control), France (glasses B), single letters of the matrix filled the field of view of the user.
    [Show full text]
  • Readiness Potentials Driven by Non-Motoric Processes ⇑ Prescott Alexander A,1, , Alexander Schlegel A,1, Walter Sinnott-Armstrong B, Adina L
    Consciousness and Cognition 39 (2016) 38–47 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Consciousness and Cognition journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/concog Readiness potentials driven by non-motoric processes ⇑ Prescott Alexander a,1, , Alexander Schlegel a,1, Walter Sinnott-Armstrong b, Adina L. Roskies c, Thalia Wheatley a, Peter Ulric Tse a a Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, HB 6207 Moore Hall, Hanover, NH 03755, USA b Philosophy Department and Kenan Institute for Ethics, Duke University, Box 90432, Durham, NC 27708, USA c Department of Philosophy, Dartmouth College, HB 6035 Thornton Hall, Hanover, NH 03755, USA article info abstract Article history: An increase in brain activity known as the ‘‘readiness potential” (RP) can be seen over cen- Received 14 June 2015 tral scalp locations in the seconds leading up to a volitionally timed movement. This activ- Revised 9 November 2015 ity precedes awareness of the ensuing movement by as much as two seconds and has been Accepted 24 November 2015 hypothesized to reflect preconscious planning and/or preparation of the movement. Using Available online 9 December 2015 a novel experimental design, we teased apart the relative contribution of motor-related and non-motor-related processes to the RP. The results of our experiment reveal that Keywords: robust RPs occured in the absence of movement and that motor-related processes did Readiness potential not significantly modulate the RP. This suggests that the RP measured here is unlikely to Volition Libet reflect preconscious motor planning or preparation of an ensuing movement, and instead Consciousness may reflect decision-related or anticipatory processes that are non-motoric in nature.
    [Show full text]
  • Magnetoencephalographic Representation of the Sensorimotor
    1649 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.74.12.1649 on 24 November 2003. Downloaded from PAPER Magnetoencephalographic representation of the sensorimotor hand area in cases of intracerebral tumour M Oishi, M Fukuda, S Kameyama, T Kawaguchi, H Masuda, R Tanaka ............................................................................................................................... J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003;74:1649–1654 Objective: To assess the clinical value of magnetoencephalography (MEG) in localising the primary hand motor area and evaluating cortical distortion of the sensorimotor cortices in patients with intracerebral tumour. Methods: 10 normal volunteers (controls) and 14 patients with an intracerebral tumour located around the central region were studied. Somatosensory evoked magnetic fields (SEFs) following median nerve stimulation, and movement related cerebral magnetic fields (MRCFs) following index finger extension, were measured in all subjects and analysed by the equivalent current dipole (ECD) method to ascertain the See end of article for authors’ affiliations neuronal sources of the primary sensory and motor components (N20m and MF, respectively). These ECD ....................... locations were defined as the primary hand sensory and motor areas and the positional relations between these two functional areas in controls and patients were investigated. Correspondence to: Dr M Oishi, Department of Results: The standard range of ECD locations of MF to N20m was determined in controls. In 11 of the 14 Neurosurgery, National patients, MRCFs could identify the primary motor hand area. ECD locations of MF were significantly closer Nishi-Niigata Central to the N20m in the medial-lateral direction in patients than in controls. In patients with a tumour located Hospital, 1-14-1 Masago, Niigata 950-2085, Japan; below the sensorimotor hand area, relative ECD locations of MF to N20m moved anteriorly over the [email protected] standard range determined in the control subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • Decoding Upper-Limb Kinematics from Electrocorticography
    Decoding Upper-Limb Kinematics from Electrocorticography Ewan Scott Nurse ORCID: 0000-0001-8981-0074 Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (with coursework component) December 2017 Department of Biomedical Engineering Melbourne School of Engineering The University of Melbourne Abstract Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) are technologies for assisting individuals with motor impairments. Activity from the brain is recorded and then processed by a computer to control assistive devices. The prominent method for recording neural activity uses microelectrodes that penetrate the cortex to record from localized populations of neurons. This causes a severe inflammatory response, making this method unsuitable after approximately 1-2 years. Electrocorticog- raphy (ECoG), a method of recording potentials from the cortical surface, is a prudent alternative that shows promise as the basis of a clinically viable BCI. This thesis investigates aspects of ECoG relevant to the translation of BCI devices: signal longevity, motor information encoding, and decoding intended movement. Data was assessed from a first-in-human ECoG device trial to quantify changes in ECoG over multiple years. The mean power, calculated daily, was steady for all patients. It was demonstrated that the device could consistently record ECoG signal statistically distinct from noise up to approximately 100 Hz for the duration of the study. Therefore, long-term implanted ECoG can be expected to record movement-related high-gamma signals from humans for many years without deterioration of signal. ECoG was recorded from patients undertaking a two-dimensional center- out task. This data was used to generate encoder-decoder directional tuning models to describe and predict arm movement direction from ECoG.
    [Show full text]
  • A Neural Precursor of the Decision to Withhold Action
    Veto and Vacillation: A Neural Precursor of the Decision to Withhold Action Erman Misirlisoy and Patrick Haggard Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/26/2/296/1780468/jocn_a_00479.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Abstract ■ The capacity to inhibit a planned action gives human behavior voluntary omission but not a rule-based omission. We also used its characteristic flexibility. How this mechanism operates and the regular temporal pattern of the action sequences to explore what factors influence a decision to act or not act remain relatively brain processes linked to omitting an action by time-locking EEG unexplored. We used EEG readiness potentials (RPs) to examine averagestotheinferredtimewhenanactionwouldhaveoc- preparatory activity before each action of an ongoing sequence, in curred had it not been omitted. When omissions were instructed which one action was occasionally omitted. We compared RPs be- by a rule, there was a negative-going trend in the EEG, recalling tween sequences in which omissions were instructed by a rule the rising ramp of an RP. No such component was found for (e.g., “omit every fourth action”) and sequences in which the voluntary omissions. The results are consistent with a model in participant themselves freely decided which action to omit. RP which spontaneously fluctuating activity in motor areas of the amplitude was reduced for actions that immediately preceded a brain could bias “free” decisions to act or not. ■ INTRODUCTION with anterior areas generating abstract aspects of a plan In everyday life, our initial impulses do not always produce and posterior motor areas executing them or generating optimal actions.
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory Load for Faces Modulates P300, N170, and N250r
    Working Memory Load for Faces Modulates P300, N170, and N250r Helen M. Morgan, Christoph Klein, Stephan G. Boehm, Kimron L. Shapiro, and David E. J. Linden Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/20/6/989/1759555/jocn.2008.20072.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & We used event-related potential (ERP) methodology to ex- was also found to modulate other ERPs. The amplitude of the amine neural activity associated with visual working memory N170 elicited by the target face decreased with load, and this (WM) for faces. There were two main goals. First, to extend N170 decrease leveled off at load 2, reflecting the behavioral WM previous findings of P300 load modulation to WM for faces. capacity of around two faces. In addition, the N250r, observed as Second, to examine whether N170 and N250r are also influ- an ERP difference for target faces that were present in the encod- enced by WM load. Between one and four unfamiliar faces were ing array relative to target faces that were absent, was also reduced simultaneously presented for memory encoding. After a 1-sec for higher WM loads. These findings extend previous work by delay, a target face appeared, and participants had to judge showing that P300 modulation by WM load also occurs for faces. whether this face was part of the previous face array. P300 Furthermore, we show, for the first time, that WM load affects the amplitude decreased as WM load increased, and this P300 sup- N250r and the early visual N170 component. This suggests that pression was observed at both encoding and retrieval.
    [Show full text]
  • Movement-Related Cortical Magnetic Fields Associated with Self-Paced Tongue Protrusion in Humans
    Title Movement-related cortical magnetic fields associated with self-paced tongue protrusion in humans Author(s) Maezawa, Hitoshi; Oguma, Hidetoshi; Hirai, Yoshiyuki; Hisadome, Kazunari; Shiraishi, Hideaki; Funahashi, Makoto Neuroscience Research, 117, 22-27 Citation https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2016.11.010 Issue Date 2017-04 Doc URL http://hdl.handle.net/2115/68828 © 2017. This manuscript version is made available under the CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 license Rights http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ Rights(URL) http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ Type article (author version) File Information Manuscript.pdf Instructions for use Hokkaido University Collection of Scholarly and Academic Papers : HUSCAP Movement-related cortical magnetic fields associated with self-paced tongue protrusion in humans Hitoshi Maezawaa,*, Hidetoshi Ogumab, Yoshiyuki Hiraia, Kazunari Hisadomea, Hideaki Shiraishic, Makoto Funahashia aDepartment of Oral Physiology, Graduate School of Dental Medicine, Hokkaido University, Kita-ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido 060-8586, Japan bSchool of Dental Medicine, Hokkaido University, Kita-ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido 060-8586, Japan cDepartment of Pediatrics, Graduate School of Medicine, Hokkaido University, Kita-ku, Sapporo 060-8638, Japan *Corresponding author: Hitoshi Maezawa, DDS, PhD Address: Department of Oral Physiology, Graduate School of Dental Medicine, Hokkaido University, Kita-ku, Sapporo, Hokkaido 060-8586, Japan TEL: 81-11-706-4229; FAX: 81-11-706-4229 E-mail: [email protected] Short title: Readiness
    [Show full text]