Characteristics of Saccades and Vergence in Two Kinds of Sequential Looking Tasks

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Characteristics of Saccades and Vergence in Two Kinds of Sequential Looking Tasks Vision Research 40 (2000) 2083–2090 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Rapid communication Characteristics of saccades and vergence in two kinds of sequential looking tasks Ilya V. Malinov a,*, Julie Epelboim b, Andrew N. Herst a, Robert M. Steinman a a Department of Psychology, Uni6ersity of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742-4411, USA b Center for Study of Language and Information, Stanford Uni6ersity, Stanford, CA 94305-4415, USA Received 23 August 1999; received in revised form 29 December 1999 Abstract We determined how saccades were used in the experiments described in Epelboim, Steinman, Kowler, Edwards, Pizlo, Erkelens and Collewijn (1995) [Vision Research, 35, 3401–3422], where unrestrained subjects looked at or tapped nearby targets. We report: (i) the size of binocular saccades; (ii) how well saccade size matched in the two eyes; and (iii) saccadic vergence. A representative sample (3375 saccades) was measured: 83% were B15°, 53% were B5°. Only two were ‘microsaccades’. Saccade sizes were very similar in the two eyes. These results imply that subjects prefer avoiding large saccades. They can do this simply by re-orienting the head appropriately. Subjects under-verged by 25–35% and preformed well. None experienced diplopia. © 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Saccade; Vergence; Saccade symmetry; Vergence distance; Vergence angle 1. Introduction the extent to which head movements were used to shift gaze, eliminating a need to make large saccades. Also, Saccades, the voluntary, rapid eye movements used note that ‘‘microsaccades were omitted since the noise to shift gaze, have a wide dynamic range. They can be inherent in the EOG technique made it difficult to as small as 5% and as large as 100° with peak velocities measure saccades of one degree or less’’ (p. 468). ranging from 3 to 600 deg/s. Even the smallest saccades Interest in the control of gaze during natural tasks (B12%), called ‘microsaccades’, are under voluntary has increased recently, but the contribution of saccades control (Haddad & Steinman, 1973; Steinman, Haddad, to gaze-shifts has not been treated quantitatively in this Skavenski & Wyman, 1973). How are saccades, which work. For example, Land, Mennie and Rusted (1998) can vary by two log units with respect to both size and used a head-mounted video camera to observe the speed, used in everyday life? The first, and only, at- approximate direction of gaze as subjects made a cup of tempt to answer this question known to the authors tea, and when they constructed a peanut butter sand- was made by Bahill, Adler and Stark (1975), who wich (Hayhoe & Land, 1999). Both reports emphasized reported that ‘‘most naturally occurring human sac- the temporal relationship between where one looked cades have magnitudes of 15° or less’’ (p. 468). For and what one did. Steinman, Epelboim, Forofonova these authors ‘naturally occurring saccades’ referred to and Bogacz (1998) used the Maryland Revolving Field saccades human subjects made as they walked around Monitor (MRFM) to make accurate measurements of the campus of the University of California at Berkeley. binocular gaze control while a subject performed tasks Note that they measured saccades with respect to the drawn from the everyday life of human beings and their head, which means that there was no way of knowing ancestors, viz., use a stick to pick honey out of a comb, cut meat with a stone knife, fashion an obsidian arrow- * Corresponding author. head and assemble a Barbie doll from component parts. 0042-6989/00/$ - see front matter © 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0042-6989(00)00063-8 2084 I.V. Malino6 et al. / Vision Research 40 (2000) 2083–2090 This report emphasized the control of vergence and the duced a spatially homogeneous magnetic field through- accuracy of cyclopean gaze under natural conditions. out a large fraction (1m3) of the cube’s volume. The nature of saccades, as such, was not described. SKALAR-DELFT sensor-coils measured horizontal The present paper begins to fill this gap. It describes: and vertical eye-angles. Head roll-, pitch- and yaw-an- (i) the size of binocular saccades; (ii) how well saccades gles were measured with two orthogonal coils mounted size matched in the two eyes; and (iii) saccadic vergence on the head with velcro straps. (2) A sparker tracking under naturalistic conditions. subsystem measured 3-D translations of the head by detecting the arrival time of acoustic signals generated by a ‘sparker’ mounted on the head: Translation preci- 2. Methods sion=0.2 mm, accuracy 1 mm. (3) A worktable (91 cm wide×67 cm deep×72 cm high) provided a plat- 2.1. Database form for the targets. Its surface contained an 11×14 grid of 154 wells separated by 4.5 cm. Rods, inserted This paper reports the results of additional analyses into these wells, were topped with different colored of data collected with the Maryland Revolving Field LEDs that served as targets. Targets were turned on Monitor (MRFM) that led to three publications, viz., before the trial began, and remained stationary and Epelboim, Steinman, Kowler, Edwards, Pizlo, Erkelens visible throughout the trial in a well illuminated room. et al. (1995), Epelboim, Steinman, Kowler, Edwards, Pizlo, Erkelens et al. (1997) and Epelboim (1998). These 2.4. Calibrations publications dealt with the control of gaze (the line-of- sight in spatial coordinates). They described: (i) visual The phase-detection principle used in the MRFM search, gaze-shift accuracy and the function of gaze- allows absolute calibration of angles. Two calibrations shifts; (ii) gaze-shift dynamics; and (iii) gaze and reti- were performed at the start of each session with the nal-image-stability in two kinds of sequential looking head in a known position on a biteboard to determine tasks performed under naturalistic conditions. Tasks the zero-reference for each sensor coil because their were naturalistic in the sense that the head and torso position varies across sessions. Each sensor-coils’ orien- were free to move. tation, relative to the line-of-sight, was determined when the subject fixated each eye’s pupil (seen monocu- 2.2. Sampling procedure larly) in a mirror parallel to the back edge of the worktable. The head coils’ zero-references were deter- A representative sample of binocular eye/head move- mined when the XYZ head-position was indicated by ment trials described in Epelboim et al. (1995) was the sparker with the head on a biteboard. Two addi- analyzed. Trials were randomly-sampled within subjects tional calibrations were performed once: (i) sparkers of (four), tasks (two), number of targets (three) and repli- different heights, placed in 18 locations on the work- cations (ten). The two tasks were: (i) look at a sequence table, calibrated ‘sparker-space’ and (ii) the sighting- of 2, 4 or 6 targets (LOOK) or (ii) tap a similar centers of each subject’s eyes were measured sequence (TAP). The angular separation of targets was psychophysically with the head on a bite-board. random, varying between about 1.5° and 35° of visual angle. The distance from the subjects’ eyes to the 2.5. Saccade measurement and analysis targets varied from about 50 to 90 cm, depending on where the targets were and how much each seated Saccades were detected manually with a mouse on a subject moved. graphic display of the data. Why? We anticipated mea- suring saccade amplitudes ranging from 5% to 100°. We 2.3. Apparatus had saccade-detecting algorithms, but they could not cover the entire range efficiently. With parameters set The MRFM, which records binocular gaze accu- to detect microsaccades, fast drifts would be flagged. rately with the head and torso free, was described in Set for large saccades, microsaccades would be missed. Edwards, Pizlo, Erkelens, Collewijn, Epelboim, Kowler Once the range of saccade sizes and peak velocities is et al. (1994) and summarized in the papers just cited. large, visual inspection of all of the algorithm’s detec- Only specifications will be provided. The MRFM con- tions becomes necessary. We chose to sample represen- sists of three subsystems: (1) A revolving-magnetic-field tative trials, rather than use the algorithm on all monitor/sensor coil subsystem using phase-detection to records, and then be forced to separate wheat from measure angular eye and head positions (angle preci- chaff by visual inspection. sion=1%, linearity=0.01%). Data were acquired at 976 Saccades were detected by three experienced individ- Hz, successive sample-pairs averaged and stored at 488 uals, all using the same criteria for estimating the onset Hz. Cube-surface field coils (2.14 m on a side), pro- and offset of the saccades. These criteria were estab- I.V. Malino6 et al. / Vision Research 40 (2000) 2083–2090 2085 Fig. 1. Examples of saccades of various sizes as recorded with the MRFM. Top: A representative 3 s interval in an eye movement record. It shows the POSITION of the right eye on the horizontal meridian as a function of TIME. Four large saccades can be seen. The first, 0.2 s into the record was about 18°, the second, one second into the record, was about 6°, the third, 2 s into the record was about 32°, and the last, about 3 s into the record, was about 25°. The smaller saccade circled about 0.5 s into the record is shown magnified on the bottom left. It was about 31%. One of the two microsaccades found in our sample is shown on the bottom right, where the POSITION of one eye on the vertical meridian is plotted against TIME. The vertical component of this microsaccade was about 7%. It is clearly visible against the 91% bit-noise-level of the recording instrument.
Recommended publications
  • Motion Perception of Saccade-Induced Retinal Translation
    Motion perception of saccade-induced retinal translation Eric Castet*, Se´ bastien Jeanjean, and Guillaume S. Masson Institut de Neurosciences Physiologiques et Cognitives, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, 31 chemin Joseph Aiguier, 13402 Marseille Cedex 20, France Edited by Dale Purves, Duke University Medical Center, Durham, NC, and approved September 25, 2002 (received for review June 25, 2002) Active visual perception relies on the ability to interpret correctly to the occurrence of saccades (Fig. 1). Only two percepts were retinal motion signals induced either by moving objects viewed reported across trials; the grating appeared either as static or as with static eyes or by stationary objects viewed with moving eyes. moving against the saccade direction. Observers indicated their A motionless environment is not normally perceived as moving percept by pressing one of two buttons. In all experiments, during saccadic eye movements. It is commonly believed that this observers were encouraged to use a conservative criterion, that phenomenon involves central oculomotor signals that inhibit in- is, to respond ‘‘motion’’ only when the motion percept was trasaccadic visual motion processing. The keystone of this ex- conspicuous. To assess intrasaccadic perception of naı¨ve observ- traretinal theory relies on experimental reports showing that ers (who were not aware that the stimulus was always stationary physically stationary scenes displayed only during saccades, thus on the screen), we first run preliminary sessions in which the producing high retinal velocities, are never perceived as moving observers were not required to report any specific percept. At the but appear as static blurred images. We, however, provide evi- end of each of these preliminary sessions, observers were simply dence that stimuli optimized for high-speed motion detection elicit asked to describe the appearance of the stimuli presented across clear motion perception against saccade direction, thus making the trials.
    [Show full text]
  • Subliminal Afterimages Via Ocular Delayed Luminescence: Transsaccade Stability of the Visual Perception and Color Illusion
    ACTIVITAS NERVOSA SUPERIOR Activitas Nervosa Superior 2012, 54, No. 1-2 REVIEW ARTICLE SUBLIMINAL AFTERIMAGES VIA OCULAR DELAYED LUMINESCENCE: TRANSSACCADE STABILITY OF THE VISUAL PERCEPTION AND COLOR ILLUSION István Bókkon1,2 & Ram L.P. Vimal2 1Doctoral School of Pharmaceutical and Pharmacological Sciences, Semmelweis University, Budapest, Hungary 2Vision Research Institute, Lowell, MA, USA Abstract Here, we suggest the existence and possible roles of evanescent nonconscious afterimages in visual saccades and color illusions during normal vision. These suggested functions of subliminal afterimages are based on our previous papers (i) (Bókkon, Vimal et al. 2011, J. Photochem. Photobiol. B) related to visible light induced ocular delayed bioluminescence as a possible origin of negative afterimage and (ii) Wang, Bókkon et al. (Brain Res. 2011)’s experiments that proved the existence of spontaneous and visible light induced delayed ultraweak photon emission from in vitro freshly isolated rat’s whole eye, lens, vitreous humor and retina. We also argue about the existence of rich detailed, subliminal visual short-term memory across saccades in early retinotopic areas. We conclude that if we want to understand the complex visual processes, mere electrical processes are hardly enough for explanations; for that we have to consider the natural photobiophysical processes as elaborated in this article. Key words: Saccades Nonconscious afterimages Ocular delayed bioluminescence Color illusion 1. INTRODUCTION Previously, we presented a common photobiophysical basis for various visual related phenomena such as discrete retinal noise, retinal phosphenes, as well as negative afterimages. These new concepts have been supported by experiments (Wang, Bókkon et al., 2011). They performed the first experimental proof of spontaneous ultraweak biophoton emission and visible light induced delayed ultraweak photon emission from in vitro freshly isolated rat’s whole eye, lens, vitreous humor, and retina.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perception of Facial Emotional Expressions During Saccades
    behavioral sciences Article Visual Perception of Facial Emotional Expressions during Saccades Vladimir A. Barabanschikov and Ivan Y. Zherdev * Institute of Experimental Psychology, Moscow State University of Psychology and Education, 29 Sretenka street, Moscow 127051, Russia; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +7-999-829-52-79 Received: 28 October 2019; Accepted: 23 November 2019; Published: 27 November 2019 Abstract: The regularities of visual perception of both complex and ecologically valid objects during extremely short photo expositions are studied. Images of a person experiencing basic emotions were displayed for as low as 14 ms amidst a saccade spanning 10 degrees of visual angle. The observers had a main task to recognize the emotion depicted, and a secondary task to point at the perceived location of the photo on the screen. It is shown that probability of correct recognition of emotion is above chance (0.62), and that it depends on its type. False localizations of stimuli and their compression in the direction of the saccade were also observed. According to the acquired data, complex environmentally valid objects are perceived differently during saccades in comparison to isolated dots, lines or gratings. The rhythmic structure of oculomotor activity (fixation–saccade–fixation) does not violate the continuity of the visual processing. The perceptual genesis of facial expressions does not take place only during gaze fixation, but also during peak speed of rapid eye movements both at the center and in closest proximity of the visual acuity area. Keywords: transsaccadic processing; facial expression; emotional perception; saccadic suppression; gaze contingency; visual recognition 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Saccade Generation for a Space-Variant Artificial Retina
    Saccade Generation for a Space-Variant Artificial Retina Sumitha Balasuriya and Paul Siebert Department of Computing Science, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, Scotland {sumitha,psiebert}@dcs.gla.ac.uk Abstract Biological and artificial systems that use a space-variant strategy to extract visual information from a scene using a retina face the problem of targeting their sensor so that the central high acuity foveal region inspects salient regions in the scene. At the same time the coarse peripheral region of the retina must extract visual information over a wide field of view to find new interesting locations for future detailed examination with the fovea. This paper describes the saccadic exploration of an image using an artificial retina with a space- variant pseudo-random receptive field tessellation. A space-variant vision hierarchy extracts visual information and accumulates space-variant saliency data to determine the location for the next saccadic fixation. 1. Introduction This paper reports an investigation into a vision architecture that supports machine sensors which resemble the space-variant sampling characteristics found in human retinae. These artificial retinae have a very high acuity in their central or foveal region and have increasingly reduced acuity towards the retina’s periphery. Such a retina will therefore have a wide field of view but only a limited high resolution centre. In a human retina only a tiny fraction of the field of view is sampled with the fovea. Ballistic eye movements called saccades are used to target different scene locations such that we perceive a seamless integrated whole and are rarely consciously aware that our visual system is based on a space-variant sensor.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Erg/Vep and Eye Movement Recordings in Children with Ocular Motor Apraxia
    THE ROLE OF ERG/VEP AND EYE MOVEMENT RECORDINGS IN CHILDREN WITH OCULAR MOTOR APRAXIA FATIMA S. SHAWKAT, CHRISTOPHER M. HARRIS, DAVID S. I. TAYLOR and ANTHONY KRISS London SUMMARY several reports of OMA or saccade failure occurring Ocular motor apraxia (OMA) is characterised by an congenitally, with no other clinical entity?-5 How­ intermittent inability to initiate voluntary sacca des, and ever, it can also occur as part of a wider neurological a failure to produce optokinetic and vestibular quick disorder: for example with structural brain abnorm­ phases. Some patients have no other abnormalities alities, such as agenesis of the corpus callosum6 and (idiopathic OMA), whereas in others it appears vermis hypoplasia;7 with neurodegenerative condi­ associated with a variety of neurological conditions tions;8 and with acquired neurological disease such as which may affect the sensory visual pathway. Electro­ posterior fossa tumours,9 ataxia telangiectasia,lO retinograms (ERGs), flash and pattern visual evoked fronto-parietal lesions,l1.l2 occipital cortex lesions,13 potentials (VEPs) and eye movements were assessed in cerebellar and brains tern neoplasm14 and olivoponto­ 53 children with OMA (age range 17 days to 14 years) cerebellar degeneration. 15.16 The inability to gener­ to determine their efficacy in helping to distinguish ate saccades often leads to the development of between idiopathic and non-idiopathic cases. Seven patients (13.2%) had idiopathic OMA and the remain­ compensatory behaviour to shift direction of gaze; ing 46 (86.8%) had other associated clinical conditions. this includes headthrusting, blinking and tilted head All patients had episodes of absent quick phases ('lock posture, which enables the use of vertical eye up') during optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) and/or movements that are usually unaffected.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Eye Movements and Blinks on Afterimage Appearance and Duration
    Journal of Vision (2015) 15(3):20, 1–15 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/15/3/20 1 The effect of eye movements and blinks on afterimage appearance and duration School of Psychology, Cardiff University, # Georgina Powell Cardiff, Wales, UK $ School of Psychology, Cardiff University, # Petroc Sumner Cardiff, Wales, UK $ Lyon Neuroscience Research Center, # Aline Bompas Centre Hospitalier Vinatier, Bron, Cedex, France $ The question of whether eye movements influence argued that afterimage signals are inherently ambigu- afterimage perception has been asked since the 18th ous, and this could explain why their visibility is century, and yet there is surprisingly little consensus on influenced by cues, such as surrounding luminance how robust these effects are and why they occur. The edges, more than are real stimuli (Powell, Bompas, & number of historical theories aiming to explain the Sumner, 2012). Helmholtz (1962) identified another cue effects are more numerous than clear experimental that is important to take into account when conducting demonstrations of such effects. We provide a clearer afterimage experiments: characterization of when eye movements and blinks do or do not affect afterimages with the aim to distinguish For obtaining really beautiful positive after- between historical theories and integrate them with a images, the following additional rules should be modern understanding of perception. We found neither saccades nor pursuit reduced strong afterimage observed. Both before and after they are devel- duration, and blinks actually increased afterimage oped, any movement of the eye or any sudden duration when tested in the light. However, for weak movement of the body must be carefully avoided, afterimages, we found saccades reduced duration, and because under such circumstances they invariably blinks and pursuit eye movements did not.
    [Show full text]
  • How the Brain Moves Your Eyes About
    C. Casanova and M. Ptito (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 134 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved CHAPTER 9 Look and see: how the brain moves your eyes about Peter H. Schiller Ł and Edward J. Tehovnik Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA Abstract: Two major cortical streams are involved in the generation of visually guided saccadic eye movements: the anterior and the posterior. The anterior stream from the frontal and medial eye fields has direct access to brainstem oculomotor centers. The posterior stream from the occipital cortices reaches brainstem oculomotor centers through the superior colliculus. The parietal cortex interconnects with both streams. Our findings suggest that the posterior stream plays an unique role in the execution of rapid, short latency eye movements called ‘express saccades’. Both the anterior and posterior streams play a role in the selection of targets to which saccades are to be generated, but do so in different ways. Areas V1, V2 and LIP contribute to decisions involved in where to look as well as where not to look. In addition, area LIP is involved in decisions about how long to maintain fixation prior to the execution of a saccade. Area V4 does not appear to be directly involved in eye-movement generation. In the anterior stream, the frontal eye fields, and to a lesser extent the medial eye fields, are involved in the correct execution of saccades subsequent to decisions made about where to look and where not to look. Introduction studied quite a number of brain areas that include V1, V2, V4, the lateral intraparietal sulcus (LIP), the Our eyes are on the move most of the time during frontal eye fields (FEF), the medial eye fields (MEF), our waking hours.
    [Show full text]
  • Convexity Bias and Perspective Cues in the Reverse-Perspective Illusion
    Short Report i-Perception Convexity Bias and January-February 2016: 1–7 ! The Author(s) 2016 DOI: 10.1177/2041669516631698 Perspective Cues in the ipe.sagepub.com Reverse-Perspective Illusion Joshua J. Dobias Department of Psychology and Counseling, Marywood University, Scranton, PA, USA Thomas V. Papathomas Department of Biomedical Engineering and Laboratory of Vision Research, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, USA Vanja M. Vlajnic Department of Statistics, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA Abstract The present experiment was designed to examine the roles of painted linear perspective cues, and the convexity bias that are known to influence human observers’ perception of three-dimensional (3D) objects and scenes. Reverse-perspective stimuli were used to elicit a depth-inversion illusion, in which far points on the stimulus appear to be closer than near points and vice versa, with a 2 (Type of stimulus) Â 2 (Fixation mark position) design. To study perspective, two types of stimuli were used: a version with painted linear perspective cues and a version with blank (unpainted) surfaces. To examine the role of convexity, two locations were used for the fixation mark: either in a locally convex or a locally concave part of each stimulus (painted and unpainted versions). Results indicated that the reverse-perspective illusion was stronger when the stimulus contained strong perspective cues and when observers fixated a locally concave region within the scene. Keywords Three-dimensional shape, reverse perspective, convexity bias, linear perspective, fixation location, visual context Introduction When viewing reverse-perspective stimuli (Wade & Hughes, 1999), painted linear perspective cues can compete with bottom-up monocular (motion parallax, shading, lens accommodation) and binocular (disparity, vergence angle) depth cues, thus creating a Corresponding author: Joshua J.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception of Intra-Saccadic Motion
    Chapter 10 Perception of Intra-saccadic Motion Eric Castet Abstract A typical saccadic eye movement lasts about 40 ms. During this short period of time, the image of the stationary world around us rapidly moves on the retina with a complex accelerating and decelerating profile. The reason why this 40 ms retinal motion flow does not elicit motion perception in everyday life is an issue that has received considerable interest. The present chapter first presents a brief history of the main ideas and experiments bearing on this issue since the seventies. Some key experimental paradigms and results in psychophysics are then described in detail. Finally, some suggestions for future investigations, both psychophysical and physiological, are made. A major goal of the chapter is to pinpoint some fundamental confusions that are often encountered in the literature. It is hoped that understanding these confusions will help identify more clearly the theoretical points – among which the role of temporal masking – on which scien- tists strongly disagree. 10.1 Introduction The stationary world around us does not appear to move during saccadic eye move- ments. Early authors already wondered why we are not aware of the activity elicited during the short saccadic period (about 40 ms) in which the image of the world does move on the retina (Dodge 1900, 1905; Holt 1903). This intra-saccadic issue should not be confused with another one usually referred to as the trans-saccadic fusion issue (Deubel et al. 2002). In the latter case, the problem is to understand why the E. Castet (*) Dynamics of Visual Perception and Action, Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives de la Méditerranée, CNRS and Université de la Méditerranée, 31 Chemin Joseph Aiguier, 13402, Marseille, France e-mail: [email protected] U.J.
    [Show full text]
  • Saccade Target Selection in Frontal Eye Field of Macaque. I. Visual and Premovement Activation
    The Journal of Neuroscience, October 1995, 75(10): 6906-6916 Saccade Target Selection in Frontal Eye Field of Macaque. I. Visual and Premovement Activation Jeffrey D. Schall, Doug P. Hanes, Kirk G. Thompson, and Dana J. King Department of Psychology, Wilson Hall, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee 37240 We investigated how the brain selects the targets for eye Essen, 1991; Merigan and Maunsell, 1993). Selecting the target movements, a process in which the outcome of visual pro- for an eye movement entails combining or comparing signals cessing is converted into guided action. Macaque monkeys from the different cortical areas. One structure mediating vis- were trained to make a saccade to fixate a salient target uomotor transformations is the frontal eye fields (FEF), in the presented either alone or with multiple distracters during rostra1bank of the arcuate sulcus. Anatomical studiesshow that visual search. Neural activity was recorded in the frontal FEF projects efferents to oculomotor structures including the eye field, a cortical area at the interface of visual process- caudate nucleus, the deep layers of the superior colliculus and ing and eye movement production. Neurons discharging brainstem pre-oculomotor nuclei (Fries, 1984; Schnyder et al., after stimulus presentation and before saccade initiation 1985; Huerta et al., 1986; Segravesand Goldberg, 1987; Stanton were analyzed. The initial visual response of frontal eye et al., 1988a,b; Shook et al., 1990, 1991; Parthasarathy et al., field neurons was modulated by the presence of multiple 1992; Segraves, 1992), and FEF receives afferents from most stimuli and by whether a saccade was going to be pro- prestriate visual cortical areas(Schall et al., 1995).
    [Show full text]
  • Pseudogaze in Afterimages
    Journal of Vision (2016) 16(5):6, 1–10 1 Where are you looking? Pseudogaze in afterimages Caltech Brain Imaging Center, Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, # Daw-An Wu Pasadena, CA, USA $ Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, Universite´ # Patrick Cavanagh Paris Descartes, Paris, France $ How do we know where we are looking? A frequent gaze seems exceptionally well defined and we seldom assumption is that the subjective experience of our feel that we don’t know where we are looking within a direction of gaze is assigned to the location in the world visual scene. The conceptual equivalence of mental and that falls on our fovea. However, we find that observers physical gaze is ingrained to the point that the two can shift their subjective direction of gaze among concepts share almost all their terminology, even in different nonfoveal points in an afterimage. Observers scientific and technical usage. Terms such as ‘‘gaze,’’ were asked to look directly at different corners of a ‘‘look at,’’ ‘‘fixate,’’ etc. do not distinguish between the diamond-shaped afterimage. When the requested corner subjective sense of visual targeting and the physical was 3.58 in the periphery, the observer often reported pointing of the eyes at something. that the image moved away in the direction of the Whereas a large body of work exists regarding the attempted gaze shift. However, when the corner was at perceived direction of visual targets in egocentric space 1.758 eccentricity, most reported successfully fixating at the point.
    [Show full text]
  • Horizontal Saccadic Eye Movements to Visual
    Engineer al ing ic & d e M m e d o i i c Journal of Biomedical Engineering and B a f l Zhai and Enderle, J Biomed Eng Med Devic 2016, 1: 1 o D l e a v n i r c u e DOI: 10.4172/2475-7586.1000109 s o J Medical Devices ISSN: 2475-7586 Research Article Open Access Horizontal Saccadic Eye Movements to Visual and Auditory-Visual Double-Step Stimuli: Saccade Characteristics and Neural Input Estimations Xiu Zhai and John D Enderle* Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269, USA *Corresponding author: John D Enderle, Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Connecticut, 260 Glenbrook Road, Storrs, Connecticut 06269-3247, USA, Tel: +18604865521; E-mail: [email protected] Rec date: Apr 20, 2016; Acc date: Apr 25, 2016; Pub date: Apr 29, 2016 Copyright: © 2016 Zhai X, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Abstract Goal-oriented human saccades were recorded under double-step paradigm. The stimuli consisted of either visual or auditory-visual bi-sensory targets. Eye movement data were analyzed based on a 3rd-order linear horizontal saccadic eye movement model, where the inputs to the muscle were agonist and antagonist active-state tensions that were described by pulse-slide-step waveforms with a post inhibitory rebound burst (PIRB) based on a time- optimal controller. Parameter estimations were calculated using the system identification technique for saccade parameters and neural inputs.
    [Show full text]