Available online at www.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Biological mechanisms for observational

1,2,3,4,5,6 3,4,5,6

Ioana Carcea and Robert C Froemke

Observational learning occurs when an animal capitalizes on Introduction

the experience of another to change its own in a given Observational learning is the capacity to acquire or opti-

context. This form of learning is an efficient strategy for mize behavior by witnessing a similar or relevant experi-

adapting to changes in environmental conditions, but little is ence in another animal. Observational learning, and the

known about the underlying neural mechanisms. There is an more general concept of social learning, was proposed and

abundance of literature supporting observational learning in demonstrated in humans by , in an effort

humans and other primates, and more recent studies have to bridge behaviorist and cognitive learning theories

begun documenting observational learning in other species [1,2,3]. Bandura’s most famous and controversial result

such as birds and rodents. The neural mechanisms for is the on the social acquisition of

observational learning depend on the species’ brain aggressive behavior, in which children observed an adult

organization and on the specific behavior being acquired. that verbally and physically ‘attacked’ an inflatable Bobo

However, as a general rule, it appears that social doll. After this episode, children manifested increased

impinges on neural circuits for direct learning, mimicking or aggressive behavior towards the doll, often imitating the

enhancing neuronal activity patterns that function during manner in which the adult behaved [2,3,4]. Thus Bandura

pavlovian, spatial or instrumental learning. Understanding the clearly enunciated that learning novel can rely

biological mechanisms for social learning could boost purely on observation or direct instruction, even in the

translational studies into behavioral interventions for a wide absence of classical [1,2,3,4]. Bandura’s

range of learning disorders. theory was particularly relevant to the acquisition of

language, which could not be satisfactorily explained

by previous behaviorist or cognitive theories [4,5].

Addresses Over the last few decades, there have been an increasing

1

Brain Health Institute, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey,

number of studies documenting evidence for observa-

Newark, NJ, 07103 USA

2 tional learning in non-human primates, birds, rodents,

Department of Pharmacology, Physiology and Neuroscience, New

Jersey Medical School, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, fishes and invertebrates [6,7,8,9]. The range of behaviors

Newark, NJ, 07103 USA that can be acquired by observational learning appears to

3

Skirball Institute for Biomolecular Medicine, Department of

be extensive, from fear associations to , tool

Neuroscience and Physiology, New York University School of Medicine,

use and prey catching behavior [9,10,11,12,13]. Observa-

New York, NY, 10016 USA

4 tional learning paradigms usually consist of a model (i.e. a

Neuroscience Institute, Department of Neuroscience and Physiology,

New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY, 10016 USA demonstrator or actor) that experiences an event or that

5

Department of Otolaryngology, Department of Neuroscience and executes a behavior, and an observer that voluntarily or

Physiology, New York University School of Medicine, New York, NY,

involuntarily witnesses the behavior of the model or the

10016 USA

6 consequences of the event experienced by the model.

Department of Neuroscience and Physiology, New York University

School of Medicine, New York, NY, 10016 USA During social learning, new behaviors can be acquired by

different mechanisms [14]. For certain behaviors, like the

Corresponding author:

social transmission of stress or maternal behavior in

Froemke, Robert C ([email protected])

rodents, the mere social presence of a conspecific repre-

sents a sufficient trigger (Figure 1a) [15 ,16,17]. In other

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185 cases, the demonstrator simply draws the of the

observer to a specific location (local enhancement) or a

This review comes from a themed issue on Neurobiology of learning

and plasticity specific object (object enhancement) that will be relevant

for producing the behavior (Figure 1b) [6,18]. of

Edited by Scott Waddell and Jesper Sjo¨ stro¨ m

actions is another variant of social learning, where the

For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial

observer copies a movement or a sequence of movements,

Available online 6th December 2018

usually with the purpose of obtaining a reward or of

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2018.11.008 averting . Imitation has been demonstrated

0959-4388/ã 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. in a number of species by the two-action paradigm where

the observer reproduces the movement of the demonstra-

tor to obtain a reward that could have been reached by at

least one other action (Figure 1c) [12,19,20,21,22].

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185 www.sciencedirect.com

Biological mechanisms for observational learning Carcea and Froemke 179

Figure 1

cortex, ACC), memory formation (e.g. amygdala, hippo-

campus), and movement control (e.g. mirror neurons in

(a) Emotional Contagion the premotor cortex). Several recent studies suggest that

social information might feed into circuits involved in

Observer Demonstrator more conventional direct learning or conditioning, and

therefore might employ partially overlapping functional

connectivity and/or synaptic plasticity mechanisms to

adjust behaviors in an effective manner. We will review

here some of the evidence supporting this hypothesis.

Imitation and emulation in humans and other primates

(b)

Local/Object Enhancement The highly evolved capacity for social learning in people

is thought to contribute to the development of traditions

and , and is at the core of several educational

theories and successful pedagogical approaches [27,28].

In particular one form of observational learning, emula-

tion, is highly developed and sophisticated in the human

delay

population. This is likely facilitated by the use of spoken

and written language, a fast mean to communicate

instructions [26]. However, even pre-lingual can

acquire behaviors by emulation and by imitation [29].

(c)

Imitation in a two-action task The drive for this is possibly the evolutionarily selected

advantage of bonding with the adult that can provide

Enclosure fence

protection [30]. Adult humans and non-human primates

Poke

Lift can also form social attachments by non-verbal imitation

[31,32], and this appears to be part of self-sustained

behavioral loop: social interactions are generally reward-

ing for primates (as well as other ), and could

play the role of a positive reinforcer in learning to imitate

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

[31,32,33]; imitation then boost the rewarding nature of

the specific interaction. Generally, in primates and other

Mechanisms of social learning.

mammals, social information appears to modulate neuro-

(a) Emotional contagion in rodents. The observer animal displays

nal activity in structures of the classical reward pathway

distress in response to a stimulus after detecting fear conditioning to

that stimulus in another animal. (ventral tegmental dopamine neurons, ventral striatum,

(b) Local enhancement in fish. Fishes can direct the trajectory of medial prefrontal cortex), a major circuit for direct learn-

others that observe their location during foraging.

ing [34,35,36,37]. Social information is also encoded in the

(c) Two-action device for testing imitation in primates. The device has

amygdala, a structure known to play an important role in

2 or more ways of allowing access to reward. The observer picks the

aversion [38]. This evidence points to the neural mecha-

option successfully used by a demonstrator animal.

nisms by which social information could substitute for

reward and punishment during observational learning.

Finally, social learning can also refer to emulation, where Many species of non-human primates use tools to obtain

the observer learns the goal of the behavior from the food in the wild. For example, chimpanzees manufacture

demonstrator but attains that goal by an action that is appropriate sticks and use them to fish termites out of

different from the one observed [23,24,25]. their mounds. This involves a complex sequence of

actions that would be difficult to achieve by an individual

The different mechanisms of social learning likely rely on through trial and error. It is more likely that chimpanzees

different cognitive processes: attention to the model, and other primates acquire the knowledge of tool use as

retention of the observed event or behavior, inferring juveniles, by observing their parents or other adults

the goal of the action, reproduction or implementation, perform the task, and then imitating the behavior [39].

motivation to reproduce the observed behavior, anticipa- This hypothesis has been tested by two-actions tasks

tion of consequences and response to feedback [2,3,14]. designed for laboratory or field conditions. A common

The detailed neural substrates for these complex pro- approach is using an ‘artificial fruit’, a device that has a

cesses seem to involve brain structures implicated in reward at the center and one or several ‘protective’ layers

perception of social stimuli (e.g. sensory and insular that need to be removed by the animal. These layers can

cortex), executive control (e.g. the anterior cingulate be removed by at least two different actions, only one of

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185

180 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

which is witnessed by the observing animal. Consistently, imitative behaviors, such as marmosets [47], or by iden-

following observation, the method of opening the artifi- tification of clear behavioral analogs in rodent models.

cial fruit is the same as the one used by the demonstrator

conspecific [12,19,40]. Some primates can acquire such

imitative behavior from a human actor, showing that Social transmission of song production and

information can be transmitted not only within species tool use in birds

but also across species [40]. In birds, vocal imitation is the best documented form of

social learning [48,49]. Juvenile songbirds such as zebra

An important step towards understanding the physiologi- finches sequentially acquire their father’s song by listen-

cal substrate for action imitation was the discovery of ing and then reproducing it, progressing from an unstruc-

mirror neurons in the premotor area, inferior frontal gyrus tured vocal output to a precisely structured mature song.

and inferior parietal lobule of macaque monkeys by Songbirds seem to have an internal drive for copying the

Giacomo Rizzolatti et al. [41]. These neurons fired song of their father (or another tutor), and they can do this

robustly when the monkey made a certain hand move- when the song is played back in isolation or produced by a

ment and also when it observed the human researcher robot [50 ].

produce the same hand movement. Similar neurons were

later found to mirror mouth and facial movements [42]. It Much has been discovered about the neural and synaptic

appears that mirror neurons integrate sensory and motor mechanisms for vocal learning in zebra finch. The HVC

information, therefore it is believed that they could play forebrain nucleus is important for song imitation, and acts

an important role in understanding the action of another as an interface between the sensory information received

animal, and perhaps in the implicit learning of an action in from higher order auditory areas and the motor commands

order to reproduce it later [43]. Both of these roles would generated internally [50 ]. A recent study by Vallentin

be important steps in , however there is et al. showed that inhibitory synaptic inputs to HVC

no clear evidence to date that mirror neurons might be projection neurons are involved in vocal imitation

implicated in observational learning or in direct motor [50 ]. Intracellular recordings in awake birds showed

learning [30]. that auditory cues from the tutor song evoked robust

and temporally precise firing in about half of the HVC

Since this discovery, the existence of mirror-like neurons projection neurons in juvenile but not adult birds

has been reported in birds and humans [44]. In humans, (Figure 2a). Vallentin et al. used two-photon guided in

functional imaging studies described a mirror system, vivo voltage-clamp recordings to reveal sensory-evoked

localized in similar brain areas as originally described excitatory events in projection neurons and then show

in the macaque [45]. The existence of mirror systems that in adult birds, the played-back song recruits robust

for facial gestures in particular, generated much specula- local inhibition in HVC that suppresses spiking of motor

tion on their possible role in the social transmission of neurons. Played-back tutor song drives inhibitory cells

emotional states. Humans and other primates that are spiking in both adult and juvenile birds, as revealed by

capable of complex facial expressions are also experts in directly recording interneuron activity (Figure 2b). This

quickly detecting and recognizing the emotional state of produced temporally-precise inhibitory currents in HVC

another individual [9]. Often, this results in emotional projection neurons for the parts of the song that had

contagion, where the observer assimilates the emotion already been learned but not for the rest of the song that

perceived in another [9,16]. The neural substrate seems had yet to be correctly imitated (Figure 2c). Thus, activity

to involve the amygdala, a structure that is important for of inhibitory neurons was driven by social auditory cues to

processing certain emotions such as fear or avoidance induce structured firing of HVC projection neurons, and

[9,46]. For example, if humans either observe another appeared to orchestrate the development of social learn-

person experiencing negative consequences or are simply ing for song production.

told about negative consequences, similar levels of amyg-

dala activation occur as would be expected from directly Birds can also learn other behaviors by observation.

experiencing the negative outcomes themselves [9]. If Chicks, for example, can learn the association between

mirror neurons were important for emotional recognition an object and an aversive taste after witnessing a single

and contagion, they would be expected to be functionally event in which a demonstrator bird pecks a bead covered

connected to the amygdalar complex or to subcortical in an aversive substance [51]. Budgerigars and other

structures including brainstem nuclei, the hypothalamus birds can learn to use tools in order to acquire food by

and the dopaminergic ventral tegmental area, important observing other birds and even humans using that tool

in encoding or rewards. There is much to be [23,52,53]. Zebra finches can acquire nest building

learned about the development and plasticity of mirror behavior by watching streamlined videos of conspecifics

neuron circuitry, an endeavor that could be helped in the building their nest [54]. The neural mechanisms for

future by new developments in using optogenetic and these other forms of observational learning are less well

transgenic approaches in primates that show robust understood.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185 www.sciencedirect.com

Biological mechanisms for observational learning Carcea and Froemke 181

Figure 2

(a) Juvenile (73 days old) Adult (155 days old) Tutor song Tutor song

Premotor 250 ms neuron 10 mV

7 7 Trial 1 1

(b) Tutor song playback: HVC interneuron Juvenile 1 (60 dph) B Adult (154 dph)

40 80 Trial Trial

1 1 10 Hz 5 Hz

100 ms (c) Tutor song playback: premotor neuron (synaptic inhibition) Juvenile 1 (60 dph, 43.6% similarity) F Juvenile 2 (67 dph, 78.8% similarity)

1 nA 1 nA

0.2 nA Average Average 200 ms (31 responses) (49 responses)

0 pA 0 pA

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Vocal learning in song birds.

(a) Juvenile birds have an underdeveloped song, and display tutor song-evoked spiking responses in HVC projection neurons. Adult birds have a

mature song, and their HVC projection neurons are silent during played back song.

(b) Inhibitory interneurons in HVC have song-evoked spiking activity.

(c) Inhibitory currents in HVC projection neurons are temporally precise only for the parts of the song that have been learned.

Social transmission of threat avoidance in [9,55 ,56 ]. To learn this behavior, the animals have to

rodents recognize fear responses in conspecifics. In mice and rats,

In laboratory rodents, the best documented form of this recognition is probably done via multiple modalities:

observational learning is the ability to associate a context visual (the freezing or escaping behavior of the demon-

or a stimulus with fear, after being exposed to a conspe- strator), auditory (distress calls produced in response to

cific that experiences classical fear conditioning pain), and/or olfactory (pheromones that can be

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185

182 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

Figure 3

(a) ACC neuron (Paired, EO) cue

20 hab.

15

obs. cond. 10 20 5 Estimated rate change trial Firing rate (Hz) Firing rate (Hz) observational conditioning 0 0 -50 5 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 Time (s) Trial number

BLA neuron cue (Paired, EO) 30 habituation observational conditioning hab.

20 obs. cond. 15 10

Firing rate (Hz) Firing rate (Hz) Estimated rate change trial

0 0

-5 0 5 0 510152025303540 45 Time (s) Trial number (b) Obs. conditioning Test day 1620 16 10

Freezing (s) Freezing (s) 0 0 BL Cue BL Cue BL Cue BL Cue Freezing (s) Freezing (s) (cue - baseline) (cue - baseline)

0 0 eYFP NpHReYFP NpHR

Current Opinion in Neurobiology

Social transmission of threat avoidance in rodents.

(a) During observational learning, ACC plasticity precedes plasticity in BLA neurons.

(b) Activity of ACC!BLA projection neurons is necessary for observational learning of fear responses.

discharged during painful and dangerous conditions) distress calls (in the 22 kHz band) seem to be capable of

[15 ,57 ]. Sterley et al. found that a mouse experiencing transmitting information about the emotional state of a

foot shocks secrets an anogenital pheromone that can be conspecific that received foot shocks [57 ]. It is therefore

investigated by conspecifics [15 ]. Exposure to this likely that witnessing the stress of a conspecific represents

pheromone is sufficient to induce in the receiver mouse an aversive stimulus in rodents, similar to the uncondi-

increased levels of circulating cortisol, and metaplasticity tioned stimulus during classical fear learning. The speed

of excitatory synapses on corticotropin-releasing hormone with which the observer rodent acquires the novel fear

neurons in the hypothalamus. Similarly, monogamous association depends on how much access it has to these

prairie voles can detect the emotional state of the partner fear-related behavioral outcomes, and in some cases on

after the later experienced fear conditioning to a tone the relationship with the demonstrator [55 ,58 ].

[58 ]. When the stressed vole freezes in response to the

conditioned stimulus, its partner also freezes and experi- The multiple processes implicated in observational learn-

ences a surge in cortisol levels. In rats, the ultrasonic ing require complex interactions between key brain

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185 www.sciencedirect.com

Biological mechanisms for observational learning Carcea and Froemke 183

structures. In particular, in the case of observational fear studies should investigate if grid cells and other neurons

learning in rodents, interactions between limbic struc- importantforspatialnavigationarealso capable ofencoding

tures seem to play an important role [55 ,56 ,58 ]. In the trajectories of others, in both rodent models and humans.

observer mouse, ACC activity is necessary for expressing

freezing behavior after witnessing a conspecific that Conclusions

undergoes contextual or cued fear learning, and for The ability to learn from the experience of a conspecific

retaining the memory of the aversive stimulus 24 hours likely provides selective advantage during evolution, and

later [55 ,56 ]. Immediate/early gene expression, a therefore should be present in many species. Indeed, in

marker for neuronal activity, was also demonstrated in addition to the mammalian examples discussed here,

the ACC in voles during exposure to a stressed partner there is evidence for social learning in invertebrates,

[58 ]. This indicates that the ACC might be an important whether these are highly social (eusocial insects like bees

brain structure responsible for processing social stimuli. and ants) or minimally social (asocial ) [63,64].

Information from ACC appears to be transmitted to the

basolateral amygdala (BLA), a brain structure known to In most species studied, the neural substrate for social

encode fear learning during [46]. learning seems to at least partially overlap with neural

During observational learning, the activity in ACC and circuits for learning from direct experience. Social informa-

BLA is synchronized in the theta range [55 ]. Signifi- tion could feed into these circuits at several different ‘entry

cantly, ACC neurons fire in response to both the cue and points’. For example, during fear learning, social informa-

the shock delivered to the demonstrator mouse [56 ]. tion appears to be processed in the cortex, particularly the

Following conditioning by proxy, BLA neurons also start ACC and the insula, and from there transmitted to the

responding to the paired cue (Figure 3a). This BLA amygdala where it substitutes for the representation of the

plasticity during observational learning depends on direct unconditioned stimulus or of a secondary reinforcer. In the

projections received from ACC. During observational case of action learning such as vocal and tool use learning,

learning, BLA-projecting ACC neurons have increased social information seems to control the activity of motor

firing compared to non-BLA projecting neurons, and their neurons or of action-planning neurons. In the case of spatial

suppression decreases cue-evoked responses in the BLA navigation, social inputs can modulate the activity of place

during observational learning. When ACC to BLA pro- cells in the hippocampus. Future studies are required to

jection neurons are silenced, observational learning in determine how much neural circuit mechanisms for direct

impaired (Figure 3b). However, if the behavior has and observational learning resemble each other and how

already been acquired by observation, inhibiting the much they differ, and to what extent the overlap might

cingulate neurons projecting to the amygdala does not explain efficacy of observational learning. Finally, under-

impair the production of learned behavior. These experi- standing the mechanisms for observational learning could

ments indicate an important role for functional connec- eventually refine social behavioral interventions that are

tivity between the cingulate cortex and the amygdala currently used only minimally and empirically.

during observational learning of threat avoidance in

rodents. It remains to be determined if ACC plays and Conflict of interest statement

important role in other forms of socially transmitted Nothing declared. behavior in rodents.

Acknowledgements

Social-guided spatial navigation in rodents Funding: This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health

[grant number HD088411 and MH106744]; a Pew Scholarship, a McKnight

Spatial navigation and spatial learning use place cells in the

Scholarship, and a Howard Hughes Medical Institute Faculty Scholarship.

hippocampal CA1 region, and grid cells in the enthorinal

cortex in rodents and in humans [59,60]. Place cells develop

References and recommended reading

in response to allocentric and vestibular cues, and fire to

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,

indicate where the animal is located in space. During many have been highlighted as:

types of observational learning, it is essential that the

of special interest

observer correctly locates the position of the demonstrator of outstanding interest

in space. How is this achieved? Two recent papers identi-

1. Bandura A: . General Learning Corporation;

fied social place cells in rats and bats, in the same part of the

1971.

hippocampus where the classical place cells are located

2. Bandura A, Ross D, Ross SA: Transmission of aggression

[61 ,62 ]. These cells fired to indicate the spatial position of

through imitation of aggressive models. J Abnorm Psychol

a demonstrator conspecific. Remarkably, up to 13–15% of 1961, 63:575-582.

all place cells were also social place cells; conversely, about

3. Bandura A, Ross D, Ross SA: Vicarious reinforcement and

half of all social place cells could also be characterized as imitative learning. J Abnorm Psychol 1963, 67:527-534.

classical place cells. These findings indicate that there is 4. Skinner BF: Verbal Behavior. Copley Publishing Group; 1947.

partial overlap in neural circuitry encoding the position of

5. Chomsky N: A review of B. F. Skinner’s verbal behavior.

self andthe position ofa behaviorallyrelevantother. Future Language 1959, 3526-58.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185

184 Neurobiology of learning and plasticity

6. Thorpe WH: Learning and in Animals. Harvard Univ Press; 27. Montessori M: The Human Tendencies and Montessori Education.

1963. American Montessori Society; 1966.

7. Olsson A, Ebert JP, Banaji MrR, Phelps EA: The role of social 28. Kozulin A: Vygotsky in Context. MIT Press; 1986.

groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science 2005,

´

309:785-787. 29. Kupa´ n K, Kira´ ly I, Kupa´ n K, Kreko´ K, Miklo´ si A, Topa´ l J: Interacting

effect of two social factors on 18-month-old infants’ imitative

8. Zajonc RB: Social facilitation. Science 1965, 149:269-274. behavior: Communicative cues and demonstrator presence. J

Exp Child Psychol 2017, 161:186-194.

9. Olsson A, Phelps EA: Social learning of fear. Nat Neurosci 2007,

10:1095-1102. 30. Simpson EA, Murray L, Paukner A, Ferrari PF: The

system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from

10. Curio E: Cultural transmission of enemy recognition. Soc Learn: birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philos Trans

Psychol. Biol Persp 1988. R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2014:369.

11. Kavaliers M, Choleris E, Colwell DD: Learning from others to 31. Paukner A, Suomi SJ, Visalberghi E, Ferrari PF: PF.Capuchin

cope with biting flies: social learning of fear-induced monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them.

conditioned analgesia and active avoidance. Behav Neurosci Science 2009, 325:880-883.

2001, 115:661-674.

32. Chartrand TL, Bargh JA: The chameleon effect: the perception-

behavior link and social interaction

12. Whiten A, Horner V, de Waal FB: Conformity to cultural norms of . J Pers Soc Psychol 1999,

76

tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 2005, 437:737-740. :893-910.

33. Trezza V, Campolongo P, Vanderschuren LJ: Evaluating the

13. John ER, Chesler P, Bartlett F, Victor I: Observational learning in

rewarding nature of social interactions in laboratory animals.

cats. Science 1968, 59:1489-1491.

Dev Cogn Neurosci 2011.

14. Galef BG, Laland KN: Social learning in animals: empirical

34. Noritake A, Ninomiya T, Isoda M: Social reward monitoring and

studies and theoretical models. Biosci 2005, 55:489-499.

valuation in the macaque brain. Nat Neurosci 2018, 10:1452-

15. Sterley TL, Baimoukhametova D, Fu¨ zesi T, Zurek AA, Daviu N, 1462.

Rasiah NP, Rosenegger D, Bains JS: Social transmission and

35. Klein JT, Platt ML: Social information signaling by neurons in

buffering of synaptic changes after stress. Nat Neurosci 2018,

21:393-403. primate striatum. Curr Biol 2013, 23:691-696.

In this study the authors show that an anogenital pheromone released by

36. Do¨ len G, Darvishzadeh A, Huang KW, Malenka RC: Social reward

stressed mice is investigated by conspecific, and can induce behavioral,

requires coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin

hormonal and electrophysiological markers for stress in the receiver

and serotonin. Nature 2013, 501:179-184.

mouse. Furthermore, the study demonstrates a ‘collective’ propagation

of stress in mice. 37. Hung LW, Neuner S, Polepalli JS, Beier KT, Wright M, Walsh JJ,

Lewis EM, Luo L, Deisseroth K, Do¨ len G, Malenka RC: Gating of

16. Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD:

social reward by oxytocin in the ventral tegmental area.

Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory

Science 2017, 357:1406-1411.

components of pain. Science 2004, 303:1157-1162.

38. Mosher CP, Zimmerman PE, Gothard KM: Neurons in the

17. Okabe S, Tsuneoka Y, Takahashi A, Ooyama R, Watarai A, monkey amygdala detect eye contact during naturalistic

Maeda S, Honda Y, Nagasawa M, Mogi K, Nishimori K, Kuroda M, social interactions. Curr Biol 2014, 24:2459-2464.

Koide T, Kikusui T: Pup exposure facilitates retrieving behavior

via the oxytocin neural system in female mice. 39. Tomassello M: Cultural Transmission in The Tool Use and

Psychoendocrinology 2017, 79:20-30. Communicatory Signaling of Chimpanzees. Parker, S. T. & Gibson;

1990:274-311.

18. Webster MM, Laland KN: The learning mechanism underlying

public information use in ninespine sticklebacks (Pungitius 40. Isbaine F, Demolliens M, Belmalih A, Brovelli A, Boussaoud D:

pungitius). J Comp Psychol 2013, 127:154-165. Learning by observation in the macaque monkey under high

experimental constraints. Behav Brain Res. 2015, 289:141-148.

19. van de Waal E, Claidie` re N, Whiten A: Wild vervet monkeys copy

41. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L: Premotor cortex and

alternative methods for opening an artificial fruit. Anim Cogn

the recognition of motor actions. Cogn Brain Res 1996, 3:131- 2015, 18:617-627.

141.

20. Vale GL, Davis SJ, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Whiten AB, Galef G,

42. Ferrari PF, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G: Fogassi L Mirror neurons

Laland KN: Acquisition of a socially learned tool use sequence

responding to the observation of ingestive and

in chimpanzees: Implications for cumulative culture. Evol Hum

communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor

Behav 2017, 38:635-644.

cortex. Eur J Neurosci 2003, 17:1703-1714.

21. Lamon N, Neumann C, Gruber T, Zuberbu¨ hler K: Kin-based

43. Ferrari PF, Visalberghi E, Paukner A, Fogassi L, Ruggiero A,

cultural transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. Sci Adv

Suomi SJ: Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. Plos Biol

2017, 3.

2006, 4.

22. Gunhold T, Whiten A, Bugnyar T: Video demonstrations

44. Mukamel R, Ekstrom AD, Kaplan J, Iacoboni M: Fried I Single-

seed alternative problem-solving techniques in wild. Biol Lett

neuron responses in humans during execution and

2014, 10.

observation of actions. Curr Biol. 2010, 27:750-756.

23. Auersperg AM, von Bayern AM, Weber S, Szabadvari A,

45. Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC,

Bugnyar T, Kacelnik A: Social transmission of tool use and tool

Rizzolatti G: Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science

manufacture in Goffin cockatoos (Cacatua goffini). Proc Biol

1999, 24:2526-2528.

Sci 2014, 281:1793.

46. LeDoux J: The amygdala. Curr Biol 2007, 17:868-874.

24. Hopper LM, Lambeth SP, Schapiro SJ, Whiten A: Observational

learning in chimpanzees and children studied through’ ghost’ 47. Miller CT, Freiwald WA, Leopold DA, Mitchell JF, Silva AC, Wang X:

conditions. Proc Biol Sci 2008, 7:835-840. Marmosets: a neuroscientific model of human social behavior.

Neuron 2016, 20:219-233.

25. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M: Evidence for emulation in

chimpanzees in social settings using the floating peanut task. 48. Mooney R: Auditory-vocal mirroring in songbirds. Philos Trans

PloS One 2010, 12. 2014, 369:1644.

26. Whiten A, van de Waal E: Social learning, culture and the’ socio- 49. Konichi M: The role of auditory feedback in the control of

cultural brain’ of human and non-human primates. Neurosci vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. Z. Tierpsychol

Biobehav Rev 2017, 82:58-75. 1965, 22.

Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185 www.sciencedirect.com

Biological mechanisms for observational learning Carcea and Froemke 185

50. Vallentin D, Kosche G, Lipkind D, Long MA: Neural circuits. mediates approach and avoidance responses to social

Inhibition protects acquired song segments during vocal affective stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 2018, 21:404-414.

learning in zebra finches. Science 2016, 351:267-271. The authors show that rats can detect and recognize the distress state of

The authors use two-photon microscopy guided intracellular recordings a conspecific. The insular cortex might play an important role in the

in HVC of awake, singing birds to show that structure inhibition con- detection and the behavioral response to the perceived stress.

tributes to song learning in zebra finches. The recordings were done in

both juvenile birds that did not develop a mature song, and in adult birds 58. Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FB,

that are capable of correctly singing. Young LJ: Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in

rodents. Science 2016, 22:375-378.

51. Crowe SF, Hamalainen M: Comparability of a single-trial The authors show that monogamous voles can detect and recognize the

passive avoidance learning task in the young chick across stressed state of the partner. In response, they begin to allogroom, an

different laboratories. Neurobiol Lern Mem 2001, 75:140-148. activity that decreases stress levels in the partner. If allogrooming is

prevented, both partners exhibit high stress levels, indicating that the

52. Dawson BV, Foss BM: Observational learning in budgerigars.

emotional state was socially transmitted. The behavioral response to the

Anim Behav 1965, 13:470-474.

stress of the partner is mediated by oxytocin receptor neurons in ACC.

53. Zentall TR: Action imitation in birds. Learn Behav 2004, 32:15-23.

59. O’Keefe J: A computational theory of the hippocampal

cognitive map. Prog Brain Res 1990, 83:301-312.

54. Guillette LM, Healy SD: Social learning in nest-building birds

watching live-streaming video demonstrators.Integr Zool 2018, 13.

60. Doeller CF, Barry C: N. Burgess Evidence for grid cells in a

55. Jeon D, Kim S, Chetana M, Jo D, Ruley HE, Lin SY, Rabah D, human memory network. Nature 2010, 463:657-661.

Kinet JP, Shin HS: Observational fear learning involves

61. Teruko D, Taro T, Fujisawa S: Spatial representations of self and

affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat

other in the hippocampus. Science 2018, 359:213-218.

Neurosci 2010, 13:482-488.

The authors show the existence of social place cells in the rat CA1

This paper elegantly showed a role for the ACC in the observational

hippocampal region. These cells respond to the position of another rat

learning of contextual fear conditioning in mice.

on a T-maze.

56. Allsop SA, Wichmann R, Mills F, Burgos-Robles A, Chang CJ,

Felix-Ortiz AC, Vienne A, Beyeler A, Izadmehr EM et al.: 62. Omer DB, Maimon SR, Las L, Ulanovsky N: Social place-cells in

Corticoamygdala transfer of socially derived information the bat hippocampus. Science 2018, 359:218-224.

gates observational learning. Cell 2018, 173:1329-1342. The manuscript demonstrates the existence of social place cells in the bat

The authors found that projections from the ACC to the BLA support CA1 hippocampal region. These cells respond to the position of another

observational learning of cue fear conditioning in mice. They used optical bat during flight

tagged recordingin vivo to show that ACC neurons projecting to BLA

63. Chittka L, Leadbeater E: Social learning: public information in

preferentially represent the stimulus that has been conditioned by

observation. insects. Curr Biol. 2005, 15:869-871.

57. Rogers-Carter MM, Varela JA, Gribbons KB, Pierce AF, 64. Fiorito G, Scotto P: Observational learning in vulgaris.

McGoey MT, Ritchey M, Christianson JP: Insular cortex Science 1992, 24:545-547.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2019, 54:178–185