Ordering and Reordering in the Auditory and Visual Modalities

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ordering and Reordering in the Auditory and Visual Modalities Memory & Cognition 1985, 13 (5), 435-441 Ordering and reordering in the auditory and visual modalities JANET METCALFE and DONALD SHARPE University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada This study investigates the hypothesis that the modality effect (i.e., the often-found recall advantage of the last few items presented in the auditory- rather than visual-input modality) is attributable to a directional auditory trace. Such a directional trace should help perfor­ mance of forward item-to-item recall, but should hurt performance if mental reordering is re­ quired. However, it was found in our Experiment 1 that mental item-to-item reordering of the materials did not affect the modality effect. In Experiment 2, strict control was exercised over the order of recall of chunks of items, as well as over the order of recall within chunks. It was found that the item-to-item reordering of the materials had little effect on the modality effect. However, the larger scale order of recall of the chunks had a large effect on the modality effect. If recall was in a forward order (i.e., first chunk, second chunk, third chunk), there was a dra­ matic superiority of the auditory materials on the last chunk. If the recall order was backward (third chunk, second chunk, first chunk), the modality effect was inconsequential. These results suggest that the modality effect is related to access to large memorial units rather than to item­ to-item associations. Many experiments investigating the role of input mo­ item of the list. This output-order performance might dality on human memory find that the last few items reflect differences in the directional format of the stored presented are better retained with auditory- rather than information. Third, Nilsson, Wright, and Murdock (1979) with visual-presentation modality (Craik, 1969; Crowder found that, when subjects were precued to recall in either & Morton, 1969; see Penney, 1975, for a review). The a modified forward (auditory-like) order or a backward present experiments investigate the possibility that direc­ (visual-like) order, the extent of the modality effect was tion of recall is important for this effect. As Drewnowski reduced, as compared to a control condition in which sub­ (1980) noted, auditory input may result in an obligatory jects were allowed to free recall. Although it is not obvi­ forward-directional trace. Such a trace could explain the ous from the directional auditory trace hypothesis why modality effect because it might facilitate forward recall the auditory-like recall order should help performance and perhaps have most impact on the recency items. when the input had been visual, the decrement in the mo­ A number of experimental findings are consistent with dality effect with backward recall fits nicely with the the idea of a directional auditory trace. First, the modal­ hypothesis. Fourth, whereas Madigan (1971) found the ity effect is usually studied with forward-serial recall or usual modality effect with forward-serial recall, the mo­ with free recall, both types of recall for which a forward dality effect disappeared with backward-serial recall. obligatory trace could improve performance. Second, Fifth, Experiment 3 by Nilsson (1979) showed that, when Nilsson, Wright, and Murdock (1975) found that, in free critical to-be-remembered words were presented succes­ recall, the order of output differs depending upon the in­ sively in a list, a modality effect was obtained, but when put modality. With auditory input, subjects tend to recall the critical words were randomly scattered throughout the the recency items first in a forward order, starting several list, no modality effect was found. These results are con­ items from the end of the list. With visual input, subjects sistent with a directional trace insofar as the cuing func­ tend to recall in a backward order, starting with the last tion of such a trace should be helpful only if the to-be­ remembered words are adjacent in memory, and may otherwise be harmful to performance. We wish to thank Elizabeth Bjork, Robert Bjork. and Eric Eich for Finally, Metcalfe, Glavanov, and Murdock (1981) have helpful discussions related to this article. Judith Goldberg provided ex­ shown that, although auditory superiority results when perimental assistance. We also wish to thank Emily Howard for her help in the presentation of some of the results from Experiment 1 to the An­ subjects are required to recall in a temporal order within nual Convention of the American Psychological Association in Ana­ a chunk of words, visual superiority results when sub­ heim, CA. 1983. This research was facilitated by a Natural Sciences jects are required to recall in a spatial order which, by and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) postdoctoral the design of the experiment, is orthogonal to the tem­ fellowship, and by NSERC Grant A0505 to the first author. Requests poral order. The most obvious interpretation of these for reprints should be sent to J. Metcalfe, Department of Psychology. University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada. results is that there is an affinity between auditory presen­ V6T lW5. tation and temporal recall, and an affinity between visual 435 Copyright 1985 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 436 METCALFE AND SHARPE presentation and spatial recall. Although this interpreta­ be written in the rightmost blank. In the RO condition, subjects tion is intuitively compelling, an equally viable interpre­ were told that for each chunk, the one-syllable word should be writ­ tation of the results rests on the idea of a directional au­ ten in the leftmost blank, the two-syllable word in the center blank, ditory trace. It may be that, in order to perform the spatial and the three-syllable word in the right blank. Subjects read the words on the flashcards, either silently for the visual conditions, task, subjects had to mentally reorder the materials from or aloud for the auditory conditions. After list presentation, sub­ the temporal order. Ifthe auditory trace were intrinsically jects immediately began written recall. They were allowed a maxi­ forward directional (and the visual trace were not), this mum of 2 min to recall each list, but no subject needed that much reordering alone would be sufficient to produce the audi­ time. Subjects could go back at the end of list recall to fill in miss­ tory deficit in the spatial case. This argument states that ing items that they had not recalled in the initial pass through the list. Such items were rare, were marked as such, and are not in­ the spatial nature of the task in Metcalfe et al. 's (1981) cluded in any of the analyses that will be presented in this paper. study had little to do with the results. Rather, what may Each subject received 24 lists altogether, 6 in each of the four major have been critical was the fact that subjects had to reorder treatment combinations-auditory temporal, visual temporal, au­ the materials in one condition (resulting in poor auditory ditory reordered, and visual reordered. Subjects were tested in­ performance) and not in the other (resulting in a modal­ dividually. Materials. The materials for this experiment consisted of a set ity effect). of one-, two-, and three-syllable words chosen from Thorndike and Lorge's (1944) norms to be "A" words (i.e., with a frequency of EXPERIMENT 1 50 to 99 per million). Homophones, contractions, and proper nouns were omitted. An attempt was made to avoid very long and very Experiment 1 is similar to the experiments of Metcalfe short words. Design. The experiment was a 2 X 2 x 3 x 3 x 6 completely et al. (1981), except that, instead of using spatial recall, within-subjects, factorial design. The factors were modality (audi­ subjects had to reorder the words within each chunk based tory or visual), item-to-item order of recall [temporal order (TO) on a nonspatial marker. Each subject was presented with or reordered (RO) by syllables], chunk (first, second, or third), lists of words, one at a time, that were divided into chunks position within chunk (1, 2, or 3), and list (I to 6). For purposes of three words each. Within each chunk, there was a one­ of the analyses that follow, the results were collapsed across lists syllable, a two-syllable, and a three-syllable word. Sub­ to result in a number correct (based on six observations) for each of the other four factors in the experiment. jects were asked either to recall the words in temporal The experiment consisted of 24 separate lists of words that cor­ order (TO) within each chunk, or to reorder the words respond to the 24 trials in which each subject engaged. These lists so that recall was in the order one-, two-, and three­ were divided into four groups of 6 lists each. The four groups of syllable word (RO). The order in which the items would lists were counterbalanced across subjects so that each list was as­ have to be recalled in the RO condition was not predicta­ signed to each major treatment combination-auditory temporal, visual temporal, auditory reordered, visual reordered-an equal ble from the temporal order. Instead, if the traces were number of times. In addition, the order of the four major treatment organized in an obligatory forward order, as the direc­ combinations was counterbalanced so that each of the 24 possible tional auditory trace hypothesis proposes, this organiza­ permutations of the order was represented, across subjects, exactly tion would be expected to be disruptive in the RO condi­ twice (to account for the 48 subjects). The groups of lists, as given tion. The specific prediction we made about this above, were randomly assigned with respect to the order of condi­ tions.
Recommended publications
  • Understanding the Role of the Modality Principle in Multimedia Learning Environments Amy Marie Oberfoell Iowa State University
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Digital Repository @ Iowa State University Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Graduate Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 2015 Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments Amy Marie Oberfoell Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd Part of the Curriculum and Instruction Commons, and the Instructional Media Design Commons Recommended Citation Oberfoell, Amy Marie, "Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments" (2015). Graduate Theses and Dissertations. 14602. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/14602 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments by Amy Marie Oberfoell A thesis submitted to the graduate faculty in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Major: Education (Curriculum and Technology Instruction) Program of Study Committee Ana-Paula Correia, Major Professor Denise Schmidt-Crawford Volker Hegelheimer Iowa State University Ames, Iowa 2015 Copyright © Amy Marie Oberfoell,
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract the EFFECTS of PRESENTATION MODALITY ON
    Abstract THE EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION MODALITY ON LEARNING AND MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN WITH SPECIFIC LEARNING DISABILITIES IN THE AREA OF LANGUAGE By Marijo F. O’Connell This study investigated the learning and memory patterns in low-income, African- American children ages 9-13 with specific learning disabilities in the area of language (SLD/SLI) as compared to typically developing peers. All subjects (n=34) were administered the California Verbal Learning Test for Children (CVLT-C) and the experimental tasks. The experimental tasks measured supralist free recall under three presentation modalities; auditory, visual and simultaneous presentation of auditory and visual. Results indicate SLD/SLI children show significantly slower rates of learning than controls. Additionally, the visual presentation modality, with or without auditory information, demonstrates significantly better learning rates than the auditory modality alone. The results support the hypotheses stating that SLD/SLI children learn verbal information at a slower rate than typical children and the superiority of the visual presentation modality for verbal learning and memory. Effect of Presentation Modality on Learning and Memory Performance in Children with Specific Learning Disabilities in the Area of Language A Thesis Submitted to Miami University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology by Marijo F. O’Connell Miami University Oxford, Ohio 2005 Advisor: ______________________________
    [Show full text]
  • The Split-Attention Principle in Multimedia Learning
    CHAPTER 8 The Split-Attention Principle in Multimedia Learning Paul Ayres John Sweller University of New South Wales Abstract Definition of Split-Attention The split-attention principle states that Instructional split-attention occurs when when designing instruction, including mul­ learners are required to split their atten­ timedia instruction, it is important to tion between and mentally integrate sev­ avoid formats that require learners to split eral sources of physically or temporally dis­ their attention between, and mentally in­ tegrate, multiple sources of information. parate information, where each source of Instead, materials should be formatted so information is essential for understanding that disparate sources of information are the material. Cognitive load is increased by physically and temporally integrated thus the need to mentally integrate the multi­ obviating the need for learners to en­ ple sources of information. This increase in gage in mental integration. By eliminat­ extraneous cognitive load (see chapter 2) is ing the need to mentally integrate multiple likely to have a negative impact on learn­ sources of information, extraneous work­ ing compared to conditions where the infor­ ing memory load is reduced, freeing re­ mation has been restructured to eliminate sources for learning. This chapter provides the need to split attention. Restructuring the theoretical rationale, based on cog­ occurs by physically or temporally inte­ nitive load theory, for the split-attention principle, describes the major experiments grating disparate sources of information to that establish the validity of the princi­ eliminate the need for mental integration. ple, and indicates the instructional de­ The split-attention effect occurs when learn­ sign implications when dealing with multi­ ers studying integrated information outper­ media materials.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding the Role of the Modality Principle in Multimedia Learning Environments Amy Marie Oberfoell Iowa State University
    Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Graduate Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 2015 Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments Amy Marie Oberfoell Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd Part of the Curriculum and Instruction Commons, and the Instructional Media Design Commons Recommended Citation Oberfoell, Amy Marie, "Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments" (2015). Graduate Theses and Dissertations. 14602. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/14602 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Understanding the role of the modality principle in multimedia learning environments by Amy Marie Oberfoell A thesis submitted to the graduate faculty in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE Major: Education (Curriculum and Technology Instruction) Program of Study Committee Ana-Paula Correia, Major Professor Denise Schmidt-Crawford Volker Hegelheimer Iowa State University Ames, Iowa 2015 Copyright © Amy Marie Oberfoell, 2015. All rights reserved ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES………………………………………………………………………………iv LIST OF TABLES………………………………………………………………………………...v
    [Show full text]
  • Modality Effects and the Structure of Short-Term Verbal Memory
    Memory & Cognition 1989, 17 (4), 398-422 Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory CATHERINE G. PENNEY Memorial University of Newfoundland, St. John's, Newfoundland, Canada The effects of auditory and visual presentation upon short-term retention of verbal stimuli are reviewed, and a model of the structure of short-term memory is presented. The main assumption ofthe model is that verbal information presented to the auditory and visual modalities is processed in separate streams that have different properties and capabilities. Auditory items are automat­ ically encoded in both the A (acoustic) code, which, in the absence of subsequent input, can be maintained for some time without deliberate allocation of attention, and a P (phonological) code. Visual items are retained in both the P code and a visual code. Within the auditory stream, suc­ cessive items are strongly associated; in contrast, in the visual modality, it is simultaneously presented items that are strongly associated. These assumptions about the structure of short­ term verbal memory are shown to account for many ofthe observed effects ofpresentation modality. Penney (1975) reviewed the literature on the effects of the distraction did not require a long period of vocaliza­ presentation modality on short-term retention of verbal tion. Overt vocalization of a visually presented list by the material and concluded that there were separate stores for subject produced much the same effect as did auditory auditorily and visually presented information. To empha­ presentation on the recency part of the serial position size the idea that active processing underlies retention in curve, but subject vocalization tended to reduce recall in short-term memory, and to discourage the conceptuali­ the nonrecency part of the serial position curve.
    [Show full text]
  • An Examination of the Effects of Mathematics Anxiety
    AN EXAMINATION OF THE EFFECTS OF MATHEMATICS ANXIETY, MODALITY, AND LEARNER-CONTROL ON TEACHER CANDIDATES IN MULTIMEDIA LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS by Elena Elizabeth Ward A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Education in conformity with the requirements for the degree Master of Education Queen’s University Kingston, Ontario, Canada September, 2008 Copyright © Elena Elizabeth Ward, 2008 ISBN: 978-0-494-42743-9 Abstract This study examined mathematics anxiety among elementary teacher candidates, and to what extent it interacted with the modality principle under various degrees of learner-control. The experiment involved a sample of 186 elementary teacher candidates learning from eight versions of a computer program on division with fractions. The eight versions varied in modality of presentation (diagrams with narration, or diagrams with written text), control of pacing (pacing was controlled by either the learner or the system), and control of sequence (sequence was controlled by either the learner or the system). A pre-test, post-test, demographic questionnaire, subjective measure of mental effort, and the Abbreviated Math Anxiety Survey were also administered. This study revealed that mathematics anxiety was significantly positively correlated with mental effort, and significantly negatively correlated with engagement, pre-test and post-test scores. Additionally, a modality x pacing interaction was observed for both high prior knowledge and low mathematics-anxious students. Under system-pacing, the modality effect was observed, and these students achieved higher far transfer scores when learning from the diagrams and narration modality condition. However, under learner-pacing, the modality effect reversed, and high prior knowledge and low mathematics-anxious students performed better on far transfer scores when learning from the diagrams and written text modality condition.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: MODALITY EFFECTS in FREE RECALL 1
    Running head: MODALITY EFFECTS IN FREE RECALL 1 Modality effects in free recall: A retrieved-context account Pazdera, J. K. and Kahana, M. J. University of Pennsylvania Author Note Support for this work was provided by the National Institutes of Health (MH55687). The authors would like thank Rivka Cohen for assistance in designing and programming our simulations, Tung Phan for helping to design our intrusion analysis for Experiment 1, as well as Bob Greene, Alice Healy, and Geoff Ward for providing insightful feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael J. Kahana. E-mail: [email protected] MODALITY EFFECTS IN FREE RECALL 2 Abstract The modality effect refers to the robust finding that memory performance differs for items presented aurally, as compared with visually. Whereas auditory presentation leads to stronger recency performance in immediate recall, visual presentation often produces better primacy performance. To investigate these differences, we conducted two large-scale immediate free recall experiments via Amazon Mechanical Turk. In both experiments, participants studied visual and auditory word lists of varying lengths and rates of presentation. We observed a recency advantage and primacy disadvantage during auditory trials, relative to visual trials, reflecting common modality and inverse modality effects in free recall. Participants were more likely to initiate recall from recency items on auditory trials than on visual trials, though the modality effect persisted regardless of recall start location. Meanwhile, an analysis of intrusion errors revealed that participants were more likely to erroneously recall words from one list prior on visual trials than they were on auditory trials.
    [Show full text]
  • Modality Effects in the Coding Reproduction of Rhythms
    Memory & Cognition 1989. 17 (4), 373-383 Modality effects in the coding and reproduction of rhythms ARTHUR M. GLENBERG, STUART MANN, LISA ALTMAN, TIM FORMAN, and SEAN PROCISE University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin The temporal coding assumption is that time of presentation is coded more accurately for audi­ tory events than for visual events. This assumption has been used to explain the modality effect, in which recall of recent auditory events is superior to recall of recent visual events. We tested the temporal coding assumption by examining the coding and reproduction of quintessentially temporal stimuli-rhythms. The rhythms were produced by sequences of short and long auditory stimuli or short and long visual stimuli; in either case, the task was to reproduce the temporal sequence. The results from four experiments demonstrated reproduction of auditory rhythms superior to that of visual rhythms. We conclude that speech-based explanations of modality ef­ fects cannot accommodate these findings, whereas the findings are consistent with explanations based on the temporal coding assumption. In classic information processing models of memory Glavanov, and Murdock (1981) presented stimuli in dif­ (e.g., Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968), the input modality of ferent spatial locations. Auditory stimuli were recalled bet­ information plays little role after the information is coded ter than were visual stimuli when recall was by temporal in short-term store. In fact, early work on the modality position, but this difference was eliminated when recall effect (e.g., Crowder & Morton, 1969) seemed to con­ was by spatial location. Glenberg and Swanson (1986) firm this limited role: Auditory information is recalled manipulated the duration of the intervals separating suc­ better than is visual information, but only at the end of cessive to-be-remembered items on a list.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Auditory Modality Superiority Effect in Serial Recall: Separating Input and Output Factors
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 2004 by the American Psychological Association Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.639 2004, Vol. 30, No. 3, 639–644 On the Auditory Modality Superiority Effect in Serial Recall: Separating Input and Output Factors Nelson Cowan and J. Scott Saults Gordon D. A. Brown University of Missouri—Columbia University of Warwick The modality effect in immediate recall refers to superior recall of the last few items within lists presented in spoken as opposed to printed form. The locus of this well-known effect has been unclear. N. Cowan, J. S. Saults, E. M. Elliott, and M. Moreno (2002) introduced a new method to distinguish between the effects of input serial position, output serial position, and the number of items yet to be recalled and found that large modality effects occurred only in conditions in which delay and interference at output (from items already recalled) was high. The authors replicated that finding, even when the response period included output interference acoustically similar to the spoken stimuli to be recalled. However, the authors found that output delay and interference act only by lowering the level of performance to a more sensitive range. The modality effect thus originates during encoding of the list to be recalled, not during output. In research on human cognition, the overwhelming majority of nine-item digit lists were presented and recall began at Serial experiments have involved the presentation of stimuli to the eyes, Position 1, 4, or 7. On some trials, the participant was to stop after the ears, or both.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Presentation Format on Memory for Order
    Effects of Presentation Format on Memory for Order Margaret J. Peterson ([email protected]) Erik M. Altmann ([email protected]) Human Factors & Applied Cognition George Mason University Fairfax, VA 22030 Abstract ual presentation, especially for the last items in a list. This pronounced recency effect for auditory modality is similar to Memory for order is important in everyday settings, for the effect seen in item memory. Similarly, Neath (1997) example in eyewitness testimony about who started a con- showed that set-size, presentation modality, and word con- flict. Although current theories claim that memory for or- creteness all have similar effects on order memory as they do der is different from memory for the to-be-remembered items themselves, many of the same manipulations that af- on item recognition and recall. fect item memory also affect order memory. One manipula- If order memory is similar to item memory, one might tion that has shown effects in item memory, but that has expect it to differ across other manipulations that produce not been tested in order memory, is presentation format. differences in item memory. One such difference is between The hypothesis tested here is that order memory will be pictures and text. In recall and recognition tasks, pictorial better for actions described as pictures than for actions de- presentation of items produces higher accuracy than verbal scribed by text. The hypothesis is not supported by a main presentation (Baggett, 1979; Snodgrass, Wassner, Finkel- effect. However, presentation format does interact with se- stein, & Goldberg, 1974). Dual-coding theory provides one rial position, suggesting that format effects are analogous possible explanation for these results.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory
    JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 14, 575-589 (1975) Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory ALAN D. BADDELEY Medical Research Council, Applied Psychology Unit AND NElL THOMSON AND MARY BUCHANAN University of Stifling, Scotland A number of experiments explored the hypothesis that immediate memory span is not constant, but varies with the length of the words to be recalled. Results showed: (1) Memory span is inversely related to word length across a wide range of materials; (2) When number of syllables and number of phonemes are held constant, words of short temporal duration are better recalled than words of long duration; (3) Span could be predicted on the basis of the number of words which the subject can read in approximately 2 sec; (4) When articulation is suppressed by requiring the subject to articulate an irrelevant sound, the word length effect disappears with visual presentation, but remains when presentation is auditory. The results are interpreted in terms of a phonemically-based store of limited temporal capacity, which may function as an output buffer for speech production, and as a supplement to a more central working memory system. Miller (1956) has suggested that the items, where items are defined experimental capacity of short-term memory is constant units. Words represent one commonly ac- when measured in terms of number of cepted type of item, and in this case, the chunks, a chunk being a subjectively meaning- chunking hypothesis would predict that the ful unit. Because of the subjective definition capacity of short-term memory, as measured of a chunk, this hypothesis is essentially in words, should be constant regardless of the irrefutable unless an independent measure of size or duration of the words used.
    [Show full text]
  • The Modality Effect Tested in Children in a User-Paced Multimedia
    Original article doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2729.2009.00335.x The modality effect tested in children in a user-paced multimedia environmentjcal_335 132..142 M.J. Witteman*† & E. Segers* *Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands †Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Abstract The modality learning effect proposes that learning is enhanced when information is pre- sented in both the visual and the auditory domains (e.g. pictures and spoken information) compared with presenting information solely in the visual channel (e.g. pictures and written text). Most of the evidence for this effect comes from adults in a laboratory setting. Therefore, we tested the modality effect with 80 children in the highest grade of elementary school in a naturalistic setting. In a between-subjects design, the children either saw representational pictures with speech or representational pictures with text. Retention and transfer knowledge was tested at three moments: immediately after the intervention, one day after and after one week. The present study did not find any evidence for a modality effect in children when the lesson was learner-paced. Instead, we found a reversed modality effect directly after the intervention for retention. A reversed modality effect was also found for the transfer questions one day later. This effect was robust, even when controlling for individual differences. Keywords long term effects, individual differences, modality effect, multimedia learning, primary school, user-paced. adults, often on college students. It is not clear, however, Introduction whether these results can be generalized to children. The information age is in full swing, and digital media The goal of the present study is to test the modality influence more and more of our daily lives.
    [Show full text]