<<

HABITAT LOCATION AND SELECTION BY THE SAYI: THE ROLE OF SENSORY CUES

by

Lorin E. West

A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of

The Charles E. Schmidt College of Science

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of

Master of Science

Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton, Florida August 2012

Copyright by Lorin E. West 2012

ii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First and foremost, I would like to thank my advisor Dr. Randy Brooks for his guidance and supervision throughout my graduate career and for his time spent sacrificing his boat, which made collecting samples in the field possible. Special thanks go to Dr. Brian Lapointe for taking me under his wing as if I were one of his own graduate students and for providing me with amazing research opportunities. I would also like to thank the rest of my committee members, Dr. Tamara Frank and Dr. Dennis

Hanisak, for agreeing to be on my committee despite their busy schedules and for their direction throughout the development of my thesis. Thanks to my labmates, Chelsea

Bennice and Ed Davis, for their help with sampling and making late hours in the lab much more tolerable.

iv

ABSTRACT

Author: Lorin E. West

Title: Habitat Location and Selection by the Sargassum Crab : The Role of Sensory Cues Institution: Florida Atlantic University

Thesis Advisor: Dr. W. Randy Brooks

Degree: Master of Science

Year: 2012

The Sargassum community consists of a unique and diverse assemblage of fauna

critical to pelagic food chains. Associated organisms presumably have adaptations to

assist in finding Sargassum. This study investigated cues used for habitat location and

selection by the Sargassum crab, Portunus sayi. Chemical detection trials were conducted

with a two-chamber choice apparatus with Sargassum spp. and as

source odors. Visual detection trials (devoid of chemical cues) and habitat selection trials

were conducted in which were given a choice between habitats. Results showed that P. sayi responded to chemical odors from Sargassum spp. Crabs visually located

habitats but did not visually distinguish between different habitats. In habitat selection

trials, crabs selected Sargassum spp. over artificial Sargassum and T. testudinum. These results suggest that crabs isolated from Sargassum likely use chemoreception from longer distances; within visual proximity of a potential patch, crabs use both chemical and visual information.

v

HABITAT LOCATION AND SELECTION BY THE SARGASSUM CRAB PORTUNUS SAYI: THE ROLE OF SENSORY CUES

List of Tables ...... viii

List of Figures ...... ix

Introduction ...... 1

Chemoreception in decapods ...... 4

Visual reception in decapods ...... 6

Objectives ...... 8

Methods...... 9

Collection and maintenance of specimens ...... 9

Chemoreception experiments ...... 10

Visual reception experiments ...... 12

Habitat selection experiments ...... 13

Statistical analysis ...... 14

Results ...... 15

Chemoreception experiments ...... 15

Visual reception experiments ...... 15

Habitat selection experiments ...... 16

Discussion ...... 18

Chemoreception experiments ...... 18

Visual reception experiments ...... 20

vi

Habitat selection experiments ...... 21

Conclusions ...... 23

Appendixes ...... 25

References ...... 39

vii

TABLES

Table 1. Fisher’s Exact and logistic regression results for chemoreception trials...... 25

Table 2. Binomial test results for visual reception trials ...... 26

Table 3. Fisher’s Exact and logistic regression results for visual reception trials ...... 27

Table 4. Fisher’s Exact and logistic regression results for visual reception controls ...... 28

Table 5. Fisher’s Exact and logistic regression results for habitat selection trials ...... 29

Table 6. Fisher’s Exact and logistic regression results for habitat selection controls ...... 30

viii

FIGURES

Figure 1. Chemoreception experimental apparatus ...... 31

Figure 2. Visual reception diagram ...... 32

Figure 3. Habitat selection diagram...... 33

Figure 4. Chemoreception graph ...... 34

Figure 5. Visual reception graph ...... 35

Figure 6. Visual reception control graph ...... 36

Figure 7. Habitat selection graph ...... 37

Figure 8. Habitat selection control graph ...... 38

ix

INTRODUCTION

Pelagic Sargassum is a brown alga characterized by numerous blades, a highly

branched thallus, and air bladders called pneumatocysts, which creates a unique floating

habitat common throughout the western North and the

(Parr, 1939). Most of these pelagic mats are comprised of two , Sargassum

fluitans Børgesen and Sargassum natans (Linnaeus) Gaillon (Parr, 1939). The

distribution of both species of Sargassum is highly variable in space and time, and influenced by currents, gyres (such as the North Atlantic Central Gyre), eddies, and winds (Wells and Rooker, 2004). These Sargassum communities, or “weedlines,” accumulate at the surface as a result of windrows from Langmuir circulation (Ryther,

1956) and support a diverse assemblage of invertebrates, , sea turtles, pelagic birds, and marine mammals (Butler et al. 1983; Casazza and Ross, 2008).

Habitat utilization begins with early colonizers such as bacteria, hydroids, and bryozoans that start ecological succession to provide the base of the Sargassum community food chain (Dooley 1972). There are approximately 100 species of sessile and motile invertebrates within this community, of which many are endemic (Butler et al.

1983). One of the major groups of invertebrates within Sargassum is decapod

(Butler et al., 1983). Specifically, the Sargassum crab, Portunus sayi, is one of the most frequently observed associates within Sargassum mats. Like many of these

1

endemic invertebrates, P. sayi has adapted to life within the Sargassum community and

displays coloration patterns mimicking the appearance of Sargassum stipes and blades for

camouflage (Dooley, 1972).

Sargassum and the array of invertebrates in this community serve as shelter and

prey for more than 100 fish species (Wells and Rooker, 2004; Casazza and Ross, 2008).

These species of fish range from obligatory, like the endemic Sargassum fish (Histrio histrio) and the Sargassum pipefish (Sygnathus pelagicus) with morphological adaptations for life in Sargassum, to facultative relationships (Wells and Rooker, 2004).

For pelagic fish species, many of which are commercially and recreationally important

(e.g., dolphinfish, tuna, ballyhoo, jacks, sailfish, swordfish, and marlin), Sargassum mats serve as a nursery for juvenile fish (Dooley, 1972; Costen-Clements, 1991; Wells and

Rooker, 2004; Rudershausen et al., 2010). As a result, Sargassum mats are designated as

Essential Fish Habitat (EFH) by the National Marine Fisheries Service (NMFS) (NOAA,

1996). Sargassum also provides habitat for four of the seven sea turtle species (Carr,

1987; Manzella and Williams, 1991) and 23 seabirds species (Haney, 1986).

The first documentation of Sargassum mats can be traced back to Columbus’ journal written in 1492 (Dunn and Kelly, 1989). Since then much research has been conducted on the fauna associated with Sargassum and habitat use by fish species (Parr,

1939; Weis, 1968; Dooley, 1972; Ryland, 1974; Butler et al., 1983; Stoner and Greening,

1984; Kingsford and Choat, 1985; Wells and Rooker, 2004; Casazza and Ross, 2008).

However, little work has been done on the ecological and biological interactions of inhabitants within Sargassum communities (Johnson and Atema, 1986; Brooks et al.,

2007; Jobe and Brooks, 2009). The numerous associated invertebrate and vertebrate

2

fauna of Sargassum mats offer potentially dynamic research discovery opportunities. The

Sargassum crab, P. sayi, is one of the more ubiquitous, macroscopic faunal inhabitants in

these mats and was chosen as the focus of this study.

Faunal diversity (including crab presence) is typically high, while the spatial

presence of Sargassum is temporally and spatially variable. For example, seasonal variation in Sargassum abundance exists (Stoner, 1983) and physical factors influence

Sargassum distribution (Wells and Rooker, 2004). Lapointe (1986, 1995) showed that primary productivity of Sargassum is much greater in neritic than oceanic waters, due to

increased nitrogen and phosphorus availability. This variation in the abundance and

presence of Sargassum mats raises the question on how recruitment of organisms occurs.

Both juveniles and those individuals separated from mats by biotic events [e.g., feeding by large, pelagic species like dolphinfish (Dooley, 1972; Costen-Clements, 1991; Wells and Rooker, 2004; Rudershausen et al., 2010)], and abiotic events (e.g., wind and waves that break up patch sizes, or when mats are driven onto beaches) must locate a new mat.

What cues might P. sayi and other motile inhabitants use for such relocation? Visual cues, in the daytime, would be available at certain distances. However, chemical cues would potentially be available at all times, and distance would be less of a problem than using visual cues exclusively. Molecules released by Sargassum spp. could potentially be

detected in the water column (Hanson, 1977; Arnold and Targett, 2000; Wong and

Cheung, 2001; Turner and Rooker, 2006; van Ginneken et al., 2011).

The seagrass, Thalassia testudinum, has been observed floating inshore and

washed up on shore intermixed within patches of Sargassum. When Sargassum washes

on shore, the associated fauna is highly susceptible to desiccation and predation and will

3

likely abandon the current mats. Although P. sayi has morphological adaptations for

living within the Sargassum community, it is feasible that these crabs may be generalists

and select more than one habitat (such as T. testudinum) if given a choice. Two other

Sargassum associates, the shrimps Leander tenuicornis and Latreutes fucorum, are known to inhabit T. testudinum in benthic, coastal regions (Bauer, 1985;

Leber, 1985). Thus, this seagrass serves as an alternative habitat for some of the

Sargassum decapod inhabitants.

Chemoreception in decapods

Chemical cues are a specific type of sensory cue involving the detection of molecules. Within the marine environment, molecules released by organisms disperse and can induce a behavioral response in organisms that perceive the signals (Zimmer and

Butman, 2000). Decapod crustaceans can detect numerous waterborne compounds, including amino acids, amines, ammonium, hydrocarbons, nucleotides, peptides, proteins, sugars, and fatty acids (Rittschof, 1992; Markowska et al., 2008). Chemical signaling processes play an important role in marine ecological interactions. For example, sensory perception of chemical cues in decapods influence mate and symbiont detection, habitat identification and selection, metamorphosis, predator avoidance, prey location, and conspecific communication (Gleeson, 1980; Brooks, 1991; Zimmer and Butman,

2000; Krimsky and Epifanio, 2008; Anderson and Epifanio, 2010).

Chemical signaling used by decapods is important for mating behavior and mate selection (Dunham, 1978). Gleeson (1980) showed that when pubescent blue crab females release hormones, males display specific courtship behavior. Aquiloni et al.

4

(2009) found that male crayfish rely solely on olfactory cues to locate female conspecifics, while females use chemical and visual stimuli to locate and select male mates. Brooks (1991) showed that chemical cues can also be used for symbiont detection, in which the hermit crab, Dardanus venosus, chemically detected its symbiotic sea anemone, Calliactis tricolor. Brooks and Rittschof (1995) also showed that the porcellanid crab, Porcellana sayana, was chemically attracted to its symbiont, C. tricolor.

Habitat identification is a primary cue for metamorphosis of decapod larvae and important for decapod distribution. Forward et al. (2003) found that megalopae of the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, respond to chemical cues from aquatic vegetation by orienting toward these areas. Krimsky and Epifanio (2008) also found that there was a significant effect on stone crab metamorphosis times when crabs were exposed to cues associated with S. fluitans, indicating that juvenile stone crabs use Sargassum mats as a possible nursery habitat. In addition, Lecchini et al. (2010) showed that xanthid crabs selected sand over macroalgae using chemical cues.

Conspecific location is also an important chemical cue for decapod larva metamorphosis. A study with the Asian shore crab, Hemigrapsus sanguineus, showed that there was a higher percentage of megalopae undergoing metamorphosis when adult crabs were present, indicating that megalopae can chemically detect conspecific adults

(Anderson and Epifanio, 2010). Another study by Gebauer et al. (1998) showed that chemical detection of adult conspecific odors by megalopae of the Atlantic burrowing crab, Chasmagnathus granulate, reduced the time until metamorphosis.

5

Chemical detection of predators by decapods can also affect decapod distribution

and abundance. Crowl and Covich (1994) showed that the distribution of freshwater

shrimp within a stream is determined by the presence of chemical cues from a larger

predatory shrimp. Forward et al. (2003) found that the blue crab reverses its orientation

when chemical cues from predators are detected. Another study by Brooks (1991)

showed that the hermit crabs, Dardanus venosus and Pagurus pollicaris, strongly avoided chemical cues from a predatory octopus.

Chemical cues can alter the outcome of foraging by decapods responding to prey odors. Foraging behavior of the blue crab, C. sapidus, is affected by chemical cues from clam and oyster mantle fluid and chemicals from wounded prey (Finelli et al. 2000).

When these cues are detected, C. sapidus begins to search and eventually move in the general direction of the chemical source.

Visual reception in decapods

Arthropods, including decapod crustaceans, are unique in that they possess moveable, stalked compound eyes, permitting broader fields of view and an increased binocular spread (Cronin, 1986). Marine crustaceans that inhabit different light environments throughout their life-history require sensory changes and almost always metamorphose from a planktonic larval stage into either a benthic or nektonic adult stage

(Cronin and Jinks, 2001). Even when the light environment of the larval and adult stage of crustaceans is the same, the visual system of the adults is still more complex than that of the larvae (Cronin and Jinks, 2001).

6

The primary functions of decapod larval vision include: orientation in the water

column, vertical migration, and avoidance of predators (Forward, 1976, 1977; Forward et

al., 1984; Cronin, 1986; Cronin and Jinks, 2001). Forward et al. (1984) showed that

vertical migration of the estuarine crab, Rhithropanopeus harrisii, results from a

photoresponse to light levels during the day. Other studies by Forward (1976, 1977) showed that larval crabs use vision to avoid predation by photonegative swimming when there was a decrease in light intensity. The behavior to this light intensity decrease has been termed the “shadow response” and occurs at dusk or when a predator passes by

(Forward, 1976, 1977).

In addition to the functions of decapod larval vision, adult decapods use vision for navigation and orientation, prey recognition and capture, habitat selection, mate selection,

and communication (Cronin, 1986; Vannini, 1987; Diaz et al., 1995; Cronin and Jinks,

2001; Detto, 2007). The pebble crab, Eriphia smithi, uses visual cues to relocate holes

within a cliff after foraging, as blinded crabs took spiral paths and eventually stopped

walking until submerged by the high tide (Vannini, 1987). Habitat selection based on

vision is demonstrated by the hermit crab, Clibanarius vittatus, which visually

discriminates between shells of different species for its home (Diaz et al., 1995). Detto

(2007) documented the use of vision for mate selection in the female fiddler crab, Uca

mjoebergi, in which the females use color vision to detect claw coloration for recognition

of male conspecifics. Similarly, male fiddler crabs and male semaphore crabs exhibit a

claw wave as a means of visual communication with conspecifics (Pope, 2000; Kitaura

and Wada, 2006, respectively). Visual cues for predator avoidance have been observed

7

for the blue crab, C. sapidus, which engages in rapid movements in the offshore direction when presented with an image of a predator (Woodbury, 1986).

Objectives

In this laboratory study, the following questions related to habitat location and selection by the Sargassum crab, P sayi, were addressed:

1. Does P. sayi respond to chemical cues from S. fluitans, S. natans, and T.

testudinum?

2. Does P. sayi respond to visual cues from S. fluitans, S. natans, and T.

testudinum?

3. Does P. sayi exhibit habitat selection differences between S. fluitans, S.

natans, artificial Sargassum, and T. testudinum?

8

METHODS

Collection and maintenance of specimens

All specimens were collected via boat 4-12 km offshore from the Boca Raton

Inlet in southeast Florida from April 2011 through September 2011. S. natans, S. fluitans,

T. testudinum, and P. sayi were collected from the surface of Sargassum mats with a fine

mesh dip net. All specimens were quickly sorted on the boat and specimens not used for

experiments were released back into the Atlantic Ocean. All Sargassum, Sargassum

crabs, and T. testudinum required for experimentation were transported in StyrofoamTM containers with portable aerators to the laboratory in the Biological Sciences building at

Florida Atlantic University.

In the laboratory, Sargassum species and T. testudinum were separated and placed in aquaria. Sargassum crabs were placed in individual aquaria and acclimated for 24 h prior to experimental trials. All aquaria were set up with seawater obtained from Gumbo

Limbo Nature Center in Boca Raton Florida, where saltwater is pumped directly from the

Atlantic Ocean. Salinity within all aquaria remained between 32-35 ppt and specimens were exposed to a 12L:12D photoperiod. All crabs were fed brine shrimp daily.

Crab size was determined by measuring the carapace width. However, juvenile and adult categories were not assigned (due to lack of literature on the life history of P. sayi). Preliminary observations showed that crabs collected varied in size from 2-41 mm.

Crabs tested were > 5 mm, as smaller crabs were too difficult to handle without causing

9

potential injury. Individual crab sex was distinguished by morphological differences in

males vs. females (Feldmann, 1998) and equal numbers of male and female crabs were

used for all experiments.

Chemoreception experiments

Chemoreception experiments were conducted in a test apparatus adapted from

Wass and Colgan (1992) and consisted of four tanks with gravity fed seawater flow

(Figure 1). The two top “stimulus” tanks (41x20x25 cm high) fed water through plastic

tubes (6 mm in diameter) into the lower “choice” tank (41x20x25 cm high) at a rate of 10

ml per minute (Reeves and Brooks, 2001). One “stimulus” tank contained seawater with

chemical cues, while the second “stimulus” tank contained only seawater. Both

“stimulus” tanks were out of view from P. sayi, to eliminate visual cues.

The “choice” tank contained two opaque partitions sealed to the bottom and the back side of the tank, to create three semi-enclosed compartments. Each side compartment had an 8-cm wide opening for the crabs to pass through from the center compartment. A second pair of outflow tubes (secured along the backside of the center compartment) allowed seawater to flow at a rate of 10 ml per minute into a fourth

“collection” tank placed below the “choice” tank. All positioning of tubing was measured

and placed at equal distances to ensure that flow patterns within each compartment were

identical. Two different colored dyes were used before experiments to color seawater

(one for each “stimulus” tank) to verify that the flow patterns were identical within both side compartments and that there was minimal mixing in the central compartment.

10

During each trial, tubing from the “stimulus” tank released seawater containing

one of the three chemical cue sources: 1) S. natans, 2) S. fluitans, and 3) T. testudinum,

into one side of the “choice” tank. The other “stimulus” tank contained and released

seawater devoid of chemical cues into the other side of the “choice” tank. The chemical

cue “stimulus” source was in the form of a clump weighing 30 + 0.1 g adapted from Jobe

and Brooks (2009). Clumps of Sargassum spp. and T. testudinum were used only once to establish odor water for the “stimulus” tank. Prior to the start of each trial the clump was randomly placed in one “stimulus” tank and after 20 minutes all tubing was unclamped.

After 60 seconds, one crab was placed in front of the outflow tubes in the center compartment with a dip-net. The observer recorded the following data: 1) activity and movements of the crab; 2) the compartment that the crab first entered; 3) elapsed time before the crab left the central compartment. Tests ended when the crab chose a side compartment by leaving the central compartment and crossing the partition (adapted from

Waas and Colgan, 1992).

Preliminary trials showed that the majority of crabs chose a side compartment within the first 5 minutes and remained there (n = 20). Therefore, trials lasted no longer than 30 min. At the end of each trial, all tanks and tubing were emptied and rinsed to remove potential lingering odors before the beginning of the next trial. Twenty replicates were conducted for each of the experimental variables and each individual crab was used only once for one treatment. Trials were also conducted to determine if P. sayi could chemically distinguish between both species of Sargassum simultaneously. Thus, instead of keeping one “stimulus” tank devoid of chemical cues, S. fluitans was in one “stimulus” tank while S. natans was in the other one. Controls in which both “stimulus” tanks

11

contained seawater were conducted to determine how crabs responded in the absence of

chemical cues. The sizes of crabs ranged from 7.0-36.7 mm and weighed between 0.087-

4.711 g.

Visual reception experiments

Experiments were conducted to determine if visual cues are used by P. sayi for

habitat location and selection. Each crab was placed individually in aquaria with a choice

between two habitats, each of which was placed inside a clear plastic container (9x16x11

cm high) (Figure 2). Each container was placed at opposite ends within aquaria

(41x20x25 cm high). The specific visual reception treatments conducted were as follows:

1) S. fluitans vs. S. natans; 2) S. fluitans vs. artificial Sargassum (a plastic mimic custom

made by Bio Models Co. used to display the visual features of Sargassum); 3) S. natans

vs. artificial Sargassum; 4) S. fluitans vs. T. testudinum; 5) S. natans vs. T. testudinum.

Control tests were also conducted to determine the response of crabs in the presence of

only one habitat (e.g., one clear container was empty), which included the following

trials: 1) S. fluitans vs. control; 2) S. natans vs. control; 3) artificial Sargassum vs.

control; 4) T. testudinum vs. control. Preliminary trials showed that crabs had no zone

preferences within aquaria when both containers were empty (n = 20). Each clump of

Sargassum (live and artificial) and T. testudinum was 9x16x7 cm within its respective

container and weighed 30 + 0.1 g. P. sayi was placed in the center of the tank after the

two habitat patches were arranged within the plastic containers. To avoid interference of

chemical and tactile cues, the opening of the plastic containers remained above the water

level in the aquarium.

12

For recording purposes, the aquaria were visually divided into three zones by

drawing lines on the upper rim of the aquarium not visible to the crab (adapted from Jobe and Brooks, 2009). The visual selection of the crab was recorded based on the zone in which the crab was located after a 2 h trial. Preliminary trials showed that crabs might switch back and forth between habitats for about 30 min before choosing and remaining in a habitat zone (n = 20). Therefore, the crab’s location within the aquaria was monitored and recorded initially and every 30 min. Crab sizes ranged from 6.0-39.0 mm and weights ranged from 0.085-4.711 g.

Habitat selection experiments

Experiments were conducted for habitat selection by P. sayi. Each crab was

placed in aquaria individually with the option of choosing one of the two habitats

provided. Habitat selection treatments conducted were as follows: 1) S. fluitans vs. S.

natans; 2) S. fluitans vs. artificial Sargassum; 3) S. natans vs. artificial Sargassum; 4) S.

fluitans vs. T. testudinum; 5) S. natans vs. T. testudinum. The two patches of habitat were

placed in aquaria (41x20x25 cm high) floating at opposite ends. Patches of Sargassum

(live and artificial) and T. testudinum were spread out to be approximately 11x16x6 cm

and weighed 30 + 0.1 g. Despite morphological differences between the more robust

Sargassum spp. and the flat blades of T. testudinum (and using the same weight), similar

habitat size was achieved by arranging the blades in various three dimensional directions

as they would be found naturally, instead of flat on top of one another. Similar to the

visual cues trials, aquaria were visually divided into three zones by drawing lines on the

top of the aquarium rim (Figure 3). Once the two habitat patches were arranged, each trial

13

began with the placement of one crab in the center of zone 2 in the aquarium. Habitat

selection was recorded based on the zone in which the crab was located after the 2-hr trial. The crab’s location within the aquarium was monitored and recorded initially and every 30 min.

Control trials were conducted to determine the response of crabs when only one habitat was available (e.g., one side of the aquaria was empty) and included the following trials: 1) S. fluitans vs. control; 2) S. natans vs. control; 3) artificial Sargassum vs.

control; 4) T. testudinum vs. control. Preliminary trials were conducted in which all zones

were empty to show that there were no zone preferences within aquaria (n = 20). The

sizes of crabs used for habitat selection trials ranged from 6.0-33.0 mm and weights ranged from 0.021-4.027 g.

Statistical analysis

Each crab was used only once for one trial in one treatment and a total of 20 replicates were conducted for each treatment for all experiments. Results from all experiments were analyzed using the binomial (Z) test of significance by comparing the number of crabs in each compartment/zone to the expected probability of 50% (Brooks and Rittschof, 1995; Reeves and Brooks, 2001; Jobe and Brooks, 2009). Fisher’s Exact test was used to determine any differences in selections between males and females. To determine effects of size and weight on selections made by P. sayi, a logistic regression was used.

14

RESULTS

Chemoreception experiments

P. sayi showed a significant ability to respond to chemical odors in two of the three chemoreception trials (Figure 4). P. sayi chose S. fluitans odors more than the control odors (17 out of 20 or 85%; Z = 3.131; P = 0.002) and S. natans odors more than

the control (16 out of 20 or 80%; Z = 2.683; P = 0.007). The difference between T.

testudinum odors and the control selection was not significant; however, numerically the control was chosen more by P. sayi than the T. testudinum odor (14 out of 20 or 70%; Z =

1.789; P = 0.07). P. sayi had no significant preference for S. fluitans vs. S. natans odors

(13 vs. 7; Z = 1.342; P = 0.18). The results from the control trials also showed no

significant difference between the right and left choice chambers (11 vs. 9; Z = 0.447; P

= 0.65). There was no significant difference in selection between male and female crabs

for all chemoreception trials (Table 1; Fisher’s Exact test, P > 0.05). Size and weight of

P. sayi had no significant effect on any chemical odor selections (Table 1; Logistic

regression, P > 0.05 for both).

Visual reception experiments

P. sayi did not significantly choose between different habitats for any of the five

visual reception treatments (P > 0.05, Table 2, Figure 5). Although the resulting trials

were not significant, the initial habitat selections between S. natans and T. testudinum by

15

P. sayi were significantly different. P. sayi initially chose S. natans significantly more

than T. testudinum (15 out of 20 or 75%; Z = 2.236; P = 0.025) and although not significant, S. fluitans was initially chosen more than T. testudinum (14 out of 20 or 70%)

when the trials first began. There was no significant difference between male and female

choices for visual selection trials (Table 3; Fisher’s Exact test, P > 0.05). The size and

weights of P. sayi had no significant effect on choices for visual selection trials (Table 3;

Logistic regression, P > 0.05).

Control trials showed that P. sayi significantly selected containers with habitats

over empty containers for all four treatments (Figure 6). Specifically, P. sayi chose S.

fluitans more than the control ( 15 out of 20 or 75%; Z = 2.236; P = 0.025), S. natans more than the control (16 out of 20 or 80%; Z = 2.683; P = 0.007), T. testudinum more than the control (16 out of 20 or 80%; Z = 2.683; P = 0.007), and artificial Sargassum

more than the control (17 out of 20 or 85%; Z = 3.131; P = 0.002). There were no

significant differences in choices between male and female crabs or between size and

weight for the visual location trials (Table 4; Fisher’s Exact test, P > 0.05; Logistic

regression, P > 0.05 for both size and weight).

Habitat selection experiments

P. sayi had significant habitat selections in four of the five habitat pairings (Figure

7). P. sayi chose S. fluitans more than artificial Sargassum (18 out of 20 or 90%; Z =

3.578; P < 0.001) and S. natans more than artificial Sargassum (16 out of 20 or 80%; Z =

2.683; P = 0.007). Differences in responses between both species of Sargassum and T.

testudinum were also significant. Specifically, P. sayi chose S. fluitans more than T.

16

testudinum (18 out of 20 or 90%; Z = 3.578; P < 0.001) and S. natans more than T.

testudinum (20 out of 20 or 100%; Z = 4.472; P < 0.001). There was no significant

selection differences between S. fluitans and S. natans by P. sayi, although S. fluitans was

chosen more than S. natans (14 out of 20 or 70%; Z = 1.789; P = 0.07). Comparisons between male and female crabs showed no significant difference in habitat selection

(Table 5; Fisher’s Exact test, P > 0.05). Crab sizes and weights also had no significant

effect on habitat preferences (Table 5; Logistic regression, P > 0.05 for both).

Control trials showed that P. sayi significantly selected zones with habitats over

empty zones for all four treatments (Figure 8). Specifically, P. sayi chose S. fluitans more

than the control (17 out of 20 or 85%; Z = 3.131; P = 0.002), S. natans more than the

control (19 out of 20 or 95%; Z = 3.859; P < 0.001), artificial Sargassum more than the

control (16 out of 20 or 80%; Z = 2.683; P = 0.007), and T. testudinum more than the

control (18 out of 20 or 90%; Z = 3.578; P < 0.001). There were no significant

differences in choices between male and female crabs or between size and weight of crabs for the habitat selection control trials (Table 6; Fisher’s Exact test, P > 0.05;

Logistic regression, P > 0.05 for both size and weight).

17

DISCUSSION

Chemoreception experiments

Results of this study show that the Sargassum crab, Portunus sayi, could use

chemical cues alone (i.e., in the absence of visual and direct tactile cues) to distinguish

and select habitats composed of Sargassum spp. These results are consistent with other

studies with decapods. For example, Krimsky and Epifanio (2008) reported that

megalopae of the Florida stone crab, Menippe mercenaria, respond to chemical cues

associated with S. fluitans and that these cues induce megalopae metamorphosis. This

study did not address the specific molecule types involved in this system, but Sargassum

spp. release potentially bioactive molecules such as amino acids and fatty acids,

phlorotannins, and photosynthate (Hanson, 1977; Arnold and Targett, 2000; Wong and

Cheung, 2001; Turner and Rooker, 2006; van Ginneken et al., 2011). Specifically,

Hanson (1977) showed Sargassum releases up to 55% of its photosynthate and Arnold

and Targett (2000) showed Sargassum has high metabolic rates in which 100% turnover

of phlorotannins occurs in 17 days. Although phlorotannins are chemical defense

compounds found in brown algae, there is little evidence that they consistently function

as deterrents in herbivores and have been shown to promote metamorphosis in ascidians

(Tsukamoto et al., 1994). Additionally, some decapods are known to chemically detect

numerous waterborne, organic compounds, including amino acids (Rittschof, 1992;

Markowska et al., 2008). Clearly, the Sargassum crab is detecting molecules exuded from

18

Sargassum. Additional studies should investigate the specific nature of these bioactive

compounds.

The results also showed that although the crab could chemically detect both

species of alga, there was no significant preference for either species based on

chemoreception selection studies. These two species of Sargassum used in this study are

commonly found intermingled in clumps; thus there may be little need for P. sayi to

distinguish between the two species. It is also possible that the chemical composition of

S. fluitans and S. natans are too similar for the Sargassum crab to distinguish differences.

Turner and Rooker (2006) found that the fatty acid composition of S. fluitans and S. natans are nearly identical.

In trials involving chemical odors from T. testudinum, there was no significant

selection by P. sayi for this seagrass even though T. testudinum contains compounds such

as amino acids and proteins (Burkholder et al., 1959) that decapods are capable of

detecting. As crabs were not attracted or repelled by odors associated with this seagrass,

it is unlikely that P. sayi uses cues from T. testudinum as a potential indicator of an

approaching shoreline. However, this does not rule out the possibility of crabs using T.

testudinum seagrass beds as a habitat when Sargassum is unavailable. Similarly, Krimsky

and Epifanio (2008) demonstrated that stone crab megalopae do not respond to chemical

cues associated with T. testudinum, even though this crab is commonly found in seagrass

habitats.

While the identity and concentrations of chemical odors influencing P. sayi

behavior in this study were not identified, the results clearly demonstrate that molecules

were detected. Other factors that might influence crab behavior include additional

19

chemical cue sources within the Sargassum mats, such as cues from conspecifics and

heterospecifics, and other sensory cues such as visual and tactile cues. Interestingly,

chemical cues in the absence of visual cues were not used by the Sargassum shrimps, L.

tenuicornis and L. fucorum, to locate Sargassum mats (Jobe and Brooks, 2009). Clearly,

habitat location by fauna inhabiting these algal mats involves multiple strategies beyond

chemoreception exclusively. Although molecules are available for detection, crabs may

be unable to locate a chemical source if the direct flow of odors from currents is

unavailable. Future studies on these additional cues and flow patterns would help clarify

the role of chemical-based responses by P. sayi in establishing and maintaining

association with Sargassum mats.

Visual reception experiments

Results from these trials demonstrate that P. sayi can use vision alone (i.e., in the absence of chemical and direct tactile cues) to locate a habitat, but did not visually

distinguish between S. fluitans, S. natans, and artificial Sargassum. In contrast, Jobe and

Brooks (2009) showed that Sargassum shrimps, L. fucorum and L. tenuicornis, can

visually distinguish between S. fluitans and S. natans, and these selection patterns are due

to morphological differences. Specifically, the more slender L. fucorum is better

concealed within the thinner blades of S. natans, while the larger L. tenuicornis is better

concealed within the thicker blades of S. fluitans. P. sayi may make distinguishing

between these two algal species a lower priority than simply finding a habitat, particularly if P. sayi can easily be camouflaged within either species of pelagic

Sargassum.

20

In trials involving Sargassum paired with T. testudinum, there were no resulting

significant selection differences. However, S. natans was initially chosen significantly

more than T. testudinum by P. sayi, but later in trials crabs moved numerous times

between the two choices before eventually selecting one. Crabs presented with visual

cues from their normal habitat but in the absence of chemical and tactile cues were likely

confused and therefore decided to continue exploring. Sargassum has a yellow-brown

coloration and more robust structure compared to the flat green blades of T. testudinum.

Therefore, habitat coloration and structure may have played a role in the initial habitat

selections of P. sayi. Color discrimination has been shown in several crab species

including the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus (Bursey, 1984; Baldwin and Johnsen, 2012),

portunid crabs of the genus Carcinus (von Buddenbrock and Friedrich, 1933; Horridge,

1967), and in fiddler crabs of the genus Uca (Hyatt, 1975; Detto, 2007). In particular,

Baldwin and Johnson (2012) demonstrated that blue crab males, C. sapidus, could

distinguish between red and orange colors using both chromatic and achromatic cues, in

which photographs of female crabs with red and orange claws were used. Recognition of

structural visual cues and shapes has also been demonstrated in several crab species

(Vannini, 1987; Diaz et al., 1994; Diaz et al., 1995; Chiussi, 2003).

Habitat selection experiments

P. sayi was given direct access (i.e., chemical, visual, and tactile cues were all

available) to habitat patches in these pairwise choice trials. Specifically, Sargassum spp.

were chosen significantly more than artificial Sargassum and T. testudinum. These results

differ from the visual reception trials where crabs did not distinguish between live and

21

artificial Sargassum and T. testudinum. Again, in the current trials chemical, visual, and

tactile cues were all available, suggesting that the crabs use multiple sensory cues in

habitat location. However, based on the results of the visual selection trials where distinguishing specific species was rather obscure, chemical cues are likely involved in precision decision making by the crabs. A similar study involving the use of stimulus cues used by juvenile blue crabs, C. sapidus, showed that they orientate toward the direction of the stimulus and that a hierarchy in responses to odors over visual cues exists

(Diaz et al., 2003). In this study, tactile cues were not separated out of the habitat

selection trials and previous studies have shown the importance of tactile cues used by

decapods for homing, predator avoidance, and prey location and therefore may play a role in habitat selection by P. sayi. (Wehner, 1992; Crowl and Covich, 1994; Dittel et al.,

1996). Habitat architecture (Hacker and Steneck, 1990) might also influence habitat selection by these crabs as Sargassum spp. have more fronds that could better conceal them from potential predators compared to the more open spaces between the T.

testudinum blades. However, this influence may be minor based on the results from the

chemical cues experiments in which crabs responded to Sargassum odors but not to T.

testudinum.

The results from the habitat selection trials also showed that there was no

preference by P. sayi between Sargassum species when all cues were available. This underscores the point made previously that P. sayi may have little need to select one species of pelagic Sargassum over another as the two species are commonly found

intermingled. Similarly, Jobe and Brooks (2009) found that two species of Sargassum

22

shrimp showed no selection preference for either of the aforementioned Sargassum species.

During the control trials for the habitat selection experiments, crabs significantly selected habitats over the control (e.g. no habitat). These results show the importance of simply finding a habitat. Although crabs did not significantly respond to odors from T. testudinum in the chemoreception trials, crabs significantly selected T. testudinum habitats when alternative habitats were unavailable. This shows clearly that P. sayi may inhabit seagrass beds when Sargassum is unavailable.

Conclusions

Overall, the ability of marine in pelagic waters to locate a suitable habitat represents a significant undertaking and challenge. Presumably, the methods to do so include a variety of sensory cues. This study examined the sensory modalities used by the

Sargassum crab P. sayi in locating and selecting Sargassum habitats. Based on the results of this study, the following model is proposed for habitat location and selection for adult crabs that have been isolated from their Sargassum algal patches.

Given that isolation from patches can occur at anytime, including night, chemical, waterborne cues that potentially disperse from algae could signal to the crabs (even in low light conditions) the general presence of an algal mat composed at least partially of

Sargassum spp. While T. testudinum may also be present, data from this experiment show no significant chemical response by the crabs to this aquatic plant. Chemoreception could serve as a longer distance means of initiating search behavior for specific patches compared to visual cues. Once in the visual proximity of a potential patch, the isolated

23

and still highly vulnerable crab will likely move quickly towards a habitat, continuing to

use chemical cues but now supplemented with visual cues. In the absence of a chemical source, crabs could still potentially locate a habitat using visual cues Once actual contact has been made with the new habitat patch, all three sensory cue modalities (including direct tactile information) can be used to confirm its likely preference of a patch composed of either species (or both) of Sargassum. Although this study was limited to adult crabs, it would be interesting to investigate habitat location and selection by larval stages of this crab, and other fauna associated with pelagic Sargassum mats, too.

24

APPENDIXES

Table 1. Chemoreception results from Fisher’s Exact tests (FET) for male and female crabs, logistic regression (LR Size) for the effects of size on odor selection of Portunus sayi, and logistic regression (LR Weight) for the effects of weight on odor selection of P. sayi, where P = probability.

Chemoreception Experiments FET LR (Size) LR (Weight) S. fluitans vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.242 P = 0.192 S. natans vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.527 P = 0.368 T. testudinum vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.624 P = 0.825 S. fluitans vs. S. natans P = 0.303 P = 0.459 P = 0.792 Control: left vs. right P = 1.0 P = 0.873 P = 0.458

25

Table 2. Visual reception results from Binomial Tests (Z) of Portunus sayi for different habitat pairings with the corresponding probabilities (P). Numbers in parentheses are equal to the number of crabs out of 20 that chose each habitat.

Visual Reception Experiments Z P(Z) S. fluitans (13) vs. S. natans (7) Z = 1.342 P = 0.180 S. fluitans (12) vs. artificial Sargassum (8) Z = 0.894 P = 0.371 S. natans (11) vs. artificial Sargassum (9) Z = 0.447 P = 0.655 S. fluitans (12) vs. T. testudinum (8) Z = 0.894 P = 0.371 S. natans (13) vs. T. testudinum (7) Z = 1.342 P = 0.180

26

Table 3. Visual reception results from Fisher’s Exact tests (FET) for male and female crabs, logistic regression (LR Size) for the effects of size on visual selection of Portunus sayi, and logistic regression (LR Weight) for the effects of weight on visual selection of P. sayi, where P = probability.

Visual Reception Experiments FET (Sex) LR (Size) LR (Weight) S. fluitans vs. S. natans P = 1.0 P = 0.789 P = 0.675 S. fluitans vs. artificial Sargassum P = 0.650 P = 0.730 P = 0.493 S. natans vs. artificial Sargassum P = 1.0 P = 0.962 P = 0.569 S. fluitans vs. T. testudinum P = 0.650 P = 0.670 P = 0.997 S. natans vs. T. testudinum P = 1.0 P = 0.475 P = 0.442

27

Table 4. Visual reception control results from Fisher’s Exact tests (FET) for male and female crabs, logistic regression (LR Size) for the effects of size on visual reception of Portunus sayi, and logistic regression (LR Weight) for the effects of weight on visual reception of P. sayi, where P = probability.

Visual Reception Controls FET LR (Size) LR (Weight) S. fluitans vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.3501 P = 0.5063 S. natans vs. control P = 0.5820 P = 0.8782 P = 0.7011 Artificial Sargassum vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.5563 P = 0.4548 T. testudinum vs. control P = 0.5820 P = 0.4959 P = 0.4326

28

Table 5. Habitat selection results from Fisher’s Exact tests (FET) for male and female crabs, logistic regression (LR Size) for the effects of size on habitat selection of Portunus sayi, and logistic regression (LR Weight) for the effects of weight on habitat selection of P. sayi, where P = probability.

Habitat Selection Experiments FET (Sex) LR (Size) LR (Weight) S. fluitans vs. S. natans P = 0.629 P = 0.325 P = 0.213 S. fluitans vs. artificial Sargassum P = 0.474 P = 0.585 P = 0.865 S. natans vs. artificial Sargassum P = 0.582 P = 0.615 P = 0.926 S. fluitans vs. T. testudinum P = 0.474 P = 0.227 P = 0.189 S. natans vs. T. testudinum P = 1.0 P = 1.0 P = 1.0

29

Table 6. Habitat selection control results from Fisher’s Exact tests (FET) for male and female crabs, logistic regression (LR Size) for the effects of size on habitat selection of Portunus sayi, and logistic regression (LR Weight) for the effects of weight on habitat selection of P. sayi, where P = probability.

Habitat Selection Controls FET (Sex) LR (Size) LR (Weight) S. fluitans vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.132 P = 0.109 S. natans vs. control P = 1.0 P = 0.251 P = 0.215 Artificial Sargassum vs. control P = 0.582 P = 0.973 P = 0.792 T. testudinum vs. control P = 0.474 P = 0.375 P = 0.114

30

Figure 1. Four tank experimental apparatus used for chemoreception trials. Opaque partitions in the choice tank create three semi-enclosed compartments. Arrows indicate water flow and “X” marks the spot where Portunus sayi was placed at the beginning of an experiment.

31

Figure 2. Visual reception diagram showing the visually divided zones. Plastic containers remained secured above the water level to prevent interference of chemical cues from the habitats. Portunus sayi was placed in the center of zone 2 at the beginning of each experiment.

32

Figure 3. Habitat selection diagram showing the visually divided zones. Portunus sayi was placed in zone 2 at the beginning of the experiment.

33

Figure 4. Chemoreception by Portunus sayi for all four treatments (black = Sargassum fluitans, grey = Sargassum natans, gridded = control, backward hatched = Thalassia testudinum). A sample size of 20 was used for each treatment. A significant odor selection was shown (★) for two of the four chemical odor treatments (P = 0.002, P = 0.007, P = .07, P = 0.18, respectively).

34

Figure 5. Visual reception trials selected by Portunus sayi for all five habitat pairings (black = Sargassum fluitans, grey = Sargassum natans, forward hatched = artificial Sargassum, backward hatched = Thalassia testudinum). A sample size of 20 was used for each habitat pairing. There was no significant visual selection within the five habitat pairings (P > 0.05 for all).

35

Figure 6. Visual reception control trials selected by Portunus sayi for all four pairings (black = Sargassum fluitans, grey = Sargassum natans, backward hatched = Thalassia testudinum, forward hatched = artificial Sargassum). A sample size of 20 was used for each treatment. A significant visual selection was shown (★) for all four visual reception controls (P = 0.025 , P = 0.007, P = 0.007, P = 0.002, respectively).

36

Figure 7. Habitat selection by Portunus sayi for all five habitat pairings (black = Sargassum fluitans, grey = Sargassum natans, forward hatched = artificial Sargassum, backward hatched = Thalassia testudinum). A sample size of 20 was used for each habitat pairing. A significant habitat selection was shown (★) for four of the five habitat pairings (P = 0.07, P < 0.001, P = 0.007, P < 0.001, P < 0.001, respectively). For the treatment pairing of S. natans and T. testudinum (on far right), P. sayi chose S. natans for each of the 20 trials.

37

Figure 8. Habitat selection control trials selected by Portunus sayi for all four habitat pairings (black = Sargassum fluitans, grey = Sargassum natans, forward hatched = artificial Sargassum, backward hatched = Thalassia testudinum). A sample size of 20 was used for each habitat. A significant habitat selection was shown (★) for all four habitat controls (P = 0.002, P < 0.001, P = 0.007, P < 0.001, respectively).

38

REFERENCES

Anderson, J.A. and C.E. Epifanio. 2010. Response of the Asian shore crab Hemigrapsus

sanguineus to metamorphic cues under natural field conditions. J. Exp. Mar. Biol.

Ecol. 384: 87-90.

Arnold, T.M and N.M.Targett. 2000. Evidence for metabolic turnover of polyphenolics in

tropical brown algae. J. Chem. Ecol. 26: 1393-1410.

Aquiloni, L., A. Massolo, F. Gherardi. 2009. Sex identification in female crayfish is

bimodal. Naturwissenschaften 96: 103-110.

Baldwin, J. and S. Johnsen. 2012. The male blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, uses both

chromatic and achromatic cues during mate choice. J. Exp. Biol. 215: 1184-1191.

Bauer, R. T. 1985. Diel and seasonal variation in species composition and abundance of

caridean shrimps (crustacea, decopoda) from seagrass meadows on the north coast

of Puerto Rico). Bull. Mar. Sci. 36: 150-162.

Brooks, W.R. 1991. Chemical recognition by hermit crabs and their symbiotic sea

anemones and a predatory octopus. Hydrobiologia 216/217: 291-295.

Brooks, W.R. and D. Rittschof. 1995. Chemical detection and host selection by the

symbiotic crab Porcellana sayana. Invertebr. Biol. 114: 180-185.

Brooks, W.R., K.A. Hutchinson, M.G. Tolbert. 2007. Pelagic Sargassum mediates

predation among symbiotic and shrimps. Gulf Mex. Sci. 2: 144-132.

Burkholder, P.R., L.M. Burkholder, J.A. Rivero. 1959. Some chemical constituents of

39

turtle grass, Thalassia testudinum. Bull. Torrey Bot. Club. 86: 88-93.

Bursey, C.R. 1984. Color recognition by the blue crab Callinectes sapidus Rathbun

(, Brachyura). Crustaceana 47: 278-284.

Butler, J.N., B.F. Morris, J. Cadwaller, A.W. Stoner. 1983. Studies of Sargassum and

the Sargassum community. Bermuda Biol. Sta. Spec. Publ. 22: 1-85.

Carr, A. 1987. New perspectives on the pelagic stage of sea turtle development. Conserv.

Biol. 1:103-121.

Casazza, T.L. and S.W. Ross. 2008. Fishes associated with pelagic Sargassum and open

water lacking Sargassum in the Gulf Stream off North Carolina. Fish. Bull. 106:

348-363.

Chiussi, R. 2003. Orientation and shape discrimination in juveniles and adults of the

mangrove crab Aratus pisonii (H. Milne Edwards, 1837): effect of predator and

chemical cues. Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol. 36: 41-50.

Costen-Clements, L., L.R. Settle, D.E. Hoss, F.A. Cross. 1991. Utilization of the

Sargassum habitat by marine invertebrates and vertebrates- a review. NOAA

Technical Memorandum NMFS-SEFSC-296, 32p.

Cronin, T.W. 1986. Optical design and evolutionary adaptation in crustacean compound eyes. J. Crust. Biol. 6: 1-23.

Cronin, T.W., R.N. Jinks. 2001. Ontogeny of vision in Marine Crustaceans. Amer. Zool.

41:1098-1107.

Crowl, T.A., A.P. Covich. 1994. Responses of a freah-water shrimp to chemical and

tactile stimuli from a large decapods predator. J. N. Am. Benthol. Soc. 13: 291-

298.

40

Detto, T. 2007. The fiddler crab Uca mjoebergi uses colour vision in mate choice. Proc.

R. Soc. B 274: 2785-2790.

Diaz, H., R.B. Forward, Jr., B. Orihuela, D. Rittschof. 1994. Chemically stimulated

orientation and shape discrimination by the Hermit Crab Clibanarius vittatus

(Bosc). J. Crust. Biol. 14: 20-26.

Diaz, H., B. Orihuela, D. Rittschof, R.B. Forward, Jr. 1995. Visual Orientation to

Gastropod shells by chemically stimulated hermit crabs, Clibanarius vittatus. J.

Crust. Biol. 15: 70-78.

Diaz, H., B. Orihuela, R.B. Forward, D. Rittschof. 2003. Orientation of juvenile blue

crabs, Callinectes sapidus Rathbun, to currents, chemicals, and visual cues. J.

Crust. Biol. 23: 15-22.

Dittel, A., C.E. Epifanio, C. Natunewicz. 1996. Predation on mud crab megalopae,

Panopeus herbstii H. Milne Edwards: effects of habitat complexity, predator

species and postlarval densities. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 198: 191-202.

Dooley, J. K. 1972. Fishes associated with the pelagic Sargassum complex, with a

discussion of the Sargassum community. Contrib. Mar. Sci. 16: 1-32.

Dunham, P.J., 1978. Sex pheromones in Crustacea. Biol. Rev. 53: 555-583.

Dunn, O. and J.E. Kelley, Ed. 1989. The diario of Christopher Columbus’s first voyage to

America 1492-1493. Norman and London: University of Oklahoma Press.

Feldmann, R. M. 1998. Parasitic castration of the Crab, Tumidocarcinus giganteus

Glaessner, from the Miocene of New Zealand: Coevolution within the Crustacea.

J. Paleontology 72: 493-498.

Finelli, C.M., N.D. Pentcheff, R.K. Zimmer, D.S. Wethey. 2000. Physical constraints on

41

ecological processes: a field test of odor-mediated foraging. Ecol. 81: 784-797.

Forward, R.B., Jr., 1976. A shadow response in a larval crustacean. Biol. Bull. 151: 126-

140.

Forward, R.B. Jr., 1977. Occurrence of a shadow response among brachyuran larvae.

Mar. Biol. 39: 331-341.

Forward, R.B., Jr., T.W. Cronin, D.E. Stearns. 1984. Control of diel vertical migration:

photoresponses of a larval crustacean. Limnol. Oceanogr. 29: 146-154.

Forward, R.B., Jr., R.A. Tankersley, K.A. Smith and J.M. Welch. 2003. Effects of

chemical cues on orientation of blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, megalopae in

flow: Implications for location of nursery areas. Mar. Biol. 142: 747-756.

Gebauer, P., I. Walter, K. Anger. 1998. Effects of substratum and conspecific adults

on the metamorphosis of Chasmagnathus granulata (Dana) (Decapoda:

Grapsidae) megalopae. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 223: 185-198.

Gleeson, R.A. 1980. Pheromone communication in the reproductive-behaviour of the

blue-crab, Callinectes-sapidus. Mar. Behav. Physiol. 7: 119-134.

Hacker, S.D., R.S. Steneck. 1990. Habitat architecture and the abundance and body-size-

dependent habitat selection of a phytal amphipod. Ecol. 71: 2269-2285.

Haney, J.C. 1986. Seabird patchiness in tropical waters: the influence of Sargassum

“reefs”. The Auk 103:141-151.

Hanson, R.B. 1977. Pelagic Sargassum community metabolism: Carbon and nitrogen. J.

Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 29: 107-118.

Horridge, G.A. 1967. Perception of polarization plane, colour and movement in two

dimensions by the crab Carcinus. Zeitschr. Vergl. Physiol. 55: 207-224.

42

Hyatt, G.W. 1975. Physiological and behavioral evidence for color discrimination by

fiddler crabs (Brachyura, Ocypodidae, genus Uca) In: Vernberg FJ (ed)

Physiological ecology of estuarine organisms. University of South Carolina Press,

Columbia SC, pp 333-365.

Jobe, C.F. and W.R. Brooks. 2009. Habitat Selection and host location by symbiotic

shrimps associated with Sargassum communities: the role of chemical and visual

cues. Symbiosis. 49: 77-85.

Johnson, B.R. and J. Atema.1986. Chemical stimulants for a component of feeding

behavior in the common gulf-weed shrimp Leander tenuicornus (Say). Biol. Bull.

170: 1-10.

Kingsford, M.J. and J.H. Choat. 1985. The fauna associated with drift algae captured with

a plankton-mesh purse seine net. Limnol. Oceanogr. 30: 618-630.

Kitaura, J. and K. Wada. 2006. Evolution of waving display in brachyuran crabs of the

genus Ilyoplax. J. Crust. Biol. 26: 455-462.

Krimsky, L.S. and C.E. Epifanio. 2008. Multiple cues from multiple habitats: effect on

metamorphosis of the Florida stone crab, Menippe mercenaria. J. Exp. Mar. Biol.

Ecol. 358: 178-184.

Lapointe, B. E. 1986. Phosphorus-limited photosynthesis and growth of Sargassum

natans and Sargassum fluitans (Paheophyceae) in the western North Atlantic.

Deep Sea Res. 33: 391-399.

Lapointe, B. E. 1995. A comparison of nutrient-limited productivity in Sargassum natans

from neritic vs. oceanic waters of the western North Atlantic Ocean. Limnol

Oceanogr. 4: 625-633.

43

Leber, K. M. 1985. The influence of predatory decopods, refuge, and microhabit

selection on seagrass communities. Ecol. 66: 1951-1964.

Lecchini,D., S.C. Mills, C. Brie, R. Marin, and B. Banaigs. 2010. Ecological

determinants and sensory mechanisms in habitat selection of crustacean

postlarvae. Behav. Ecol. 21: 599-607.

Manzella, S. and J. Williams. 1991. Juvenile head-started Kemp's ridleys found in

floating grass mats. Mar. Turt. Newsl. 52:5-6.

Markowska, M., A. Kidawa, T. Janecki. 2008. Amino acids as food signals for two

Artic decapods, Hyas araneus and Eupagurus pubescens. Polish Polar Res. 29:

219-226.

NOAA. 1996. Magnuson-Stevens Fishery Conservation and Management Act, as

amended through Oct. 11, 1996. NOAA Tech. Mem. NMFS-F/SPO-23. 121 p.

Parr, A.D. 1939. Quantitative observations on the pelagic sargassum vegetation of the

western North Atlantic. Bull. Bingham Oceanogr. Coll. 6: 1-94.

Pope, D. S. 2000. Testing function of fiddler crab claw waving by manipulating social

context. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 47: 432-437.

Reeves, M.N. and W.R. Brooks. 2001. Host selection, chemical detection, and protection

of the symbiotic Pinnotherid crabs Dissodactylus crinitichelis and

Clypeasterophilus rugatus associated with echinoderms. Symbiosis 30: 239-256.

Rittschof, D. 1992. Chemosensation in the daily life of crabs. Amer. Zool. 32: 363-369.

Rudershausen, P.J., J.A. Buckel, J. Edwards, D.P. Gannon, C.M. Butler, T.W. Averett.

2010. Feeding ecology of blue marlins, dolphinfish, yellowfin tuna, and wahoos

from the North Atlantic Ocean and comparisions with other oceans. Trans. Am.

44

Fish. Soc. 139: 1335-1359.

Ryland, J.S. 1974. Observations on some epibionts of gulf-weed, Sargassum natans (L.)

Meyen. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 14: 17-25.

Ryther, J. H. 1956. The . Sci Am. 194: 98-104.

Turner, J.P. and J.R. Rooker. 2006. Fatty acid composition of flora and fauna associated

with Sargassum mats in the Gulf of Mexico. Mar. Biol. 149: 1025-1036.

Stoner, A.W. 1983. Pelagic sargassum: evidence for a major decrease in biomass. Deep

Sea Res. 30: 469-474.

Stoner, A.W. and H.S. Greening. 1984. Geographic variation in the macrofaunal

associates of pelagic Sargassum and some biogeographic implications. Mar.

Ecol. Prog. Ser. 20: 185-192.

Tsukamoto, S., H. Hirota, H. Kato, N. Fusetani. 1994. Phlorotannins and sulfoquinovsyl

diacylglycerols: Promoters of larval metamorphosis in ascidians, isolated from the

brown alga Sargassum thunbergii. Fisheries Sci. 60: 319-321.

van Ginneken, V.J., J.P. Helsper, W. De Visser, H. van Keulen, W.A. Brandenburg.

2011. Polyunsaturated fatty acids in various macroalgal species from north

Atlantic and tropical seas. Lipids Health Dis. 10:104.

Vannini, M. 1987. Notes on the ecology and behavior of the pebble crab Eriphia smithi

Mcleay (Decapoda Brachyura). Monitore Zool. Ital. (NS) 22: 383-410. von Buddenbrock, W., Friedrich, H. 1933. (Cited by K. Sakamoto, O. Hisatomi, F

Tokunaga, E. Eguchi.) Two opsins from the compound eye of the crab

Hemigrapsus sanguineus. J. Exp. Biol. 199: 441-450.

Waas, J.R. and P.W. Colgan. 1992. Chemical cues associated with visually elaborate

45

aggressive displays of three-spine stickelbacks. J. Chem. Ecol. 18: 2277-2284.

Wehner, R. 1992. . In, Homing, edited by F. Papi, Chapman & Hall,

London, pp. 45-144.

Weis, J.S. 1968. Fauna associated with pelagic Sargassum in the Gulf Stream. Am. Midl.

Nat. 80: 554-558.

Wells, R.J.D., and J.R. Rooker. 2004. Spatial and temporal patterns of habitat use by

fishes associated with sargassum mats in the northwestern Gulf of Mexico. Bull.

Mar. Sci. 74: 81-99.

Wong, K. and P.C. Cheung. 2001. Influence of drying treatment on three Sargassum

Species 2. Protein extractability, in vitro protein digestibility and amino acid

profile of protein concentrates. Journal of Appl. Phycol. 13: 43-50.

Woodbury, P. B. 1986. The geometry of predator avoidance by the blue crab,

Callinectess apidus. Anim. Behav. 34: 28-37.

Zimmer, R.K. and C.A. Butman. 2000. Chemical signaling processes in the marine

environment. Biol. Bull. 198: 167-187.

46