How Verb Subcategorization Frequencies Are Affected by Corpus Choice

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Verb Subcategorization Frequencies Are Affected by Corpus Choice How Verb Subcategorization Frequencies Are Affected By Corpus Choice Douglas Roland Daniel Jurafsky University of Colorado University of Colorado Department of Linguistics Dept. of Linguistics & Inst. of Cognitive Science Boulder, CO 80309-0295 Boulder, CO 80309-0295 [email protected] j urafsky @colorado.edu 1997), and in psychological theories of language Abstract processing (Clifton et al. 1984, Ferreira & The probabilistic relation between verbs and McClure 1997, Garnsey et al. 1997, Jur'afsky 1996, their arguments plays an important role in MacDonald 1994, Mitchell & Holmes 1985, modern statistical parsers and supertaggers, Tanenhaus et al. 1990, Trueswell et al. 1993). and in psychological theories of language processing. But these probabilities are These probabilities are computed in very different computed in very different ways by the two ways by the two sets of researchers. sets of researchers. Computational linguists Psychological studies use methods such as compute verb subcategorization probabilities sentence completion and sentence production for from large corpora while psycholinguists collecting verb argument structure probabilities. compute them from psychological studies In sentence completion, subjects are asked to (sentence production and completion tasks). complete a sentence fragment. Garnsey at al. Recent studies have found differences (1997) used a proper name followed by a verb, between corpus frequencies and such as "Debbie remembered " In psycholinguistic measures. We analyze sentence subjects are asked to write any sentence subcategorization frequencies from four containing a given verb. An example of this type different corpora: psychological sentence of study is Connine et al. (1984). production data (Connine et al. 1984), written text (Brown and WSJ), and telephone An alternative to these psychological methods is conversation data (Switchboard). We find to use corpus data. This can be done two different sources for the differences. automatically with unparsed corpora (Briscoe and Discourse influence is a result of how verb Carroll 1997, Manning 1993, Ushioda et al. 1993), use is affected by different discourse types from parsed corpora such as Marcus et al.'s (1993) such as narrative, connected discourse, and Treebank (Merlo 1994, Franlis 1994) or manually single sentence productions. Semantic as was done for COMLEX (Macleod and influence is a result of different corpora using Grishman 1994). The advantage of any of these different senses of verbs, which have different corpus methods is the much greater amount of subcategorization frequencies. We conclude data that can be used, and the much more natural that verb sense and discourse type play an contexts. This seems to make it preferable to important role in the frequencies observed in data generated in psychological studies. different experimental and corpus based sources of verb subcategorization frequencies. Recent studies (Merlo 1994, Gibson et al. 1996) have found differences between corpus 1 Introduction frequencies and experimental measures. This The probabilistic relation between verbs and their suggests that corpus-based frequencies and arguments plays an important role in modern experiment-based frequencies may not be statistical parsers and supertaggers (Charniak interchangeable. To clarify the nature of the 1995, Collins 1996/1997, Joshi and Srinivas 1994, differences between various corpora and to find Kim, Srinivas, and Trueswell 1997, Stolcke et al. the causes of these differences, we analyzed 1122 psychological sentence production data (Connine Clifton. The Connine data (CFJCF) consists of et al. 1984), written discourse (Brown and WSJ examples of 127 verbs, each classified as ltom Penn Treebank - Marcus et al. 1993), and belonging to one of 15 subcategorization frames. conversational data (Switchboard - Godfrey et al. We added a 16th category for direct quotations 1992). We found that the subcategorization (which appeared in the corpus data but not the frequencies in each of these sources are different. Connine data). Examples of these categories, We performed three experiments to (1) find the taken from the Brown Corpus, appear in figure 1 causes of general differences between corpora, (2) below. There are approximately 14,000 verb measure the size of these differences, and (3) find tokens in the CFJCF data set. verb specific differences. The rest of this paper describes our methodology and the two sources of For the BC, WSJ, and SWBD data, we counted subcategorization probability differences: subcategorizations using tgrep scripts based on the discourse influence and semantic influence. Penn Treebank. We automatically extracted and categorized all examples of the 127 verbs used in 2 Methodology the Comfine study. We used the same verb For the sentence production data, we used the subcategorization categories as the Comline study. numbers published in the original Connine et al. There were approximately 21,000 relevant verb paper as well as the original data, which we were tokens in the Brown Corpus, 25,000 relevant verb able to review thanks to the generosity of Charles 1 [O1 Barbara asked, as ~ey heard the front door close.- " 2 [PP] Guerrillas were racing [toward him]. ' '" 3 [hlf-S] : Hank thanked them and promised [to observe file rules]. 1 4 [inf-S]/PP/ Labor fights [to change its collar from blue io white]. 5 [wh-S] I know now [why the students insisted that] go to Hiroshima even when I to'ld them I didn't want to]. 6 [that-Sl l She promised ['that she wou'ld'soon take 'a few day's leave and visit the uncie she had never seen, on the island of Oyajima --which was not very far from Yokosuka]. 7 [verb-ing] But I couldn't help [thinking that Nadine and Wally were getting just what ~ey deserved]. 8 [perception Far off, in the dusk, he heard [voices singing, muffled but strong]. ' ..... complement.] 9 INP]' ° Tile t~tle immediately Withdrew into its private counci'lroom' to study [the phenome'non]. 10 [NP]tNP] "The mayor of ~e town taught [~em] [Englisl~ ]~UciFrench]. 11 INP][PP] They bought [rustled cattle] [from the outlaw]i kept him supplied wifil guns mad ammunition, harbored his men in their houses. 12 [NPl[inf-Sl ' Side had assumed before 'then that one day he wou'ld ask [her] [to marry him]. [NP][wh-S] I asked "[Wisman] [what would happen if' he broke out tile go codes and 'tried to start transmitting one]. 14 lNP][that-S] But, in departing, Lewis begged [Breasted] [that there be no liquor in ~e apartment at the Grosvenor on his return], and he took with him the first thirty galleys of Elmer Gantry. 15 lpassive] A col'd supper was ordered'and a bottle of port. 16 Quoi'es He writes ["confucius held that in times oi s~ess, one should take short'views - only up to lunchtime."] Figure 1 - examples of each subcategorization frame from Brown Corpus 1123 tokens in the Wall Street Journal Corpus, and 10,000 in Switchboard. Unlike the Connine data, Passive is generally used in English to emphasize where all verbs were equally represented, the the undergoer (to keep the topic in subject frequencies of each verb in the corpora varied. position) and/or to de-emphasize the identity of For each calculation where individual verb the agent (Thompson 1987). Both of these frequency could affect the outcome, we reasons are affected by the type of discourse. If normalized for frequency, and eliminated verbs there is no preceding discourse, then there is no with less than 50 examples. This left 77 out of pre-existing topic to keep in subject position. In 127 verbs in the Brown Corpus, 74 in the Wall addition, with no context Ibr the sentence, there is Street Journal, and only 30 verbs in Switchboard. less likely to be a reason to de-emphasize the This was not a problem with the Connine data agent of the sentence. where most verbs had approximately 100 tokens. 3.1.2 Zero Anaphora 3 Experiment 1 The increase in zero anaphora (not overtly The purpose of the first experiment is to analyze mentioning understood arguments) is caused by the general (non-verb-specific) differences two factors. Generally, as the amount of between argument structure frequencies in the surrounding context increases (going from single data sources. In order to do this, the data for each sentence to connected discourse) the need to verb in the corpus was normalized to remove the overtly express all of the arguments with a verb effects of verb frequency. The average decreases. frequency of each subcategorization flame was calculated for each corpus. The average Data Source % [0] subcat frame frequencies for each of the data sources were then CFJCF 7% compared. Wall Street Journal 8% Brown 13 % 3.1 Results Switchboard 18% We found that the three corpora consisting of connected discourse (BC, WSJ, SWBD) shared a Verbs that can describe actions (agree, disappear, common set of differences when compared to the escape, follow, leave, sing, wait) were typically CFJCF sentence production data. There were used with some form of argument in single three general categories of differences between the sentences, such as: corpora, and all can be related to discourse type. "I had a test that day, so I really wanted to escape These categories are: from school." (CFJCF data). (1) passive sentences Such verbs were more likely to be used without (2) zero anaphora any arguments in connected discourse as in: (3) quotations "She escaped , crawled through the usual mine fields, under barbed wire, was shot at, swam a 3.1.1 Passive Sentences river, and we finally picked her up in Linz." The CFJCF single sentence productions had the (Brown Corpus) smallest number of passive sentences. The In this case, the argument of "escaped", connected spoken discourse in Switchboard had ("imprisonment") was understood from the more passives, followed by the written discourse previous sentence. Verbs of propositional in the Wall Street Journal and the Brown Corpus.
Recommended publications
  • Common and Distinct Neural Substrates for Pragmatic, Semantic, and Syntactic Processing of Spoken Sentences: an Fmri Study
    Common and Distinct Neural Substrates for Pragmatic, Semantic, and Syntactic Processing of Spoken Sentences: An fMRI Study G.R.Kuperberg and P.K.McGuire Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/12/2/321/1758711/089892900562138.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Institute of Psychiatry, London E.T.Bullmore Institute of Psychiatry, London and the University of Cambridge M.J.Brammer, S.Rabe-Hesketh, I.C.Wright, D.J.Lythgoe, S.C.R.Williams, and A.S.David Institute of Psychiatry, London Abstract & Extracting meaning from speech requires the use of difference between conditions in activation of the left-super- pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic information. A central ior-temporal gyrus for the pragmatic experiment than the question is:Does the processing of these different types of semantic/syntactic experiments; (2) a greater difference be- linguistic information have common or distinct neuroanatomi- tween conditions in activation of the right-superior and middle- cal substrates? We addressed this issue using functional temporal gyrus in the semantic experiment than in the syntactic magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure neural activity experiment; and (3) no regions activated to a greater degree in when subjects listened to spoken normal sentences contrasted the syntactic experiment than in the semantic experiment. with sentences that had either (A) pragmatical, (B) semantic These data show that, while left- and right-superior-temporal (selection restriction), or (C) syntactic (subcategorical) viola- regions may be differentially involved in processing pragmatic tions sentences. All three contrasts revealed robust activation of and lexico-semantic information within sentences, the left- the left-inferior-temporal/fusiform gyrus.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-Occurrence Outline
    Introducing LexIt Building Distributional Profiles Ongoing Work Conclusions Outline Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-occurrence 1 Introducing LexIt A Computational Lexical Resource for Italian Verbs The Project Distributional Profiles 1 2 Gabriella Lapesa ,AlessandroLenci 2 Building Distributional Profiles Pre-processing 1University of Osnabr¨uck, Institute of Cognitive Science Subcategorization Frames 2 University of Pisa, Department of Linguistics Lexical sets Selectional preferences Explorations in Syntactic Government and Subcategorisation nd University of Cambridge, 2 September 2011 3 Ongoing Work 4 Conclusions Gabriella Lapesa, Alessandro Lenci Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-occurrence 2/ 38 Introducing LexIt Introducing LexIt Building Distributional Profiles The Project Building Distributional Profiles The Project Ongoing Work Distributional Profiles Ongoing Work Distributional Profiles Conclusions Conclusions Computational approaches to argument structure LexIt: a computational lexical resource for Italian The automatic acquisition of lexical information from corpora is a longstanding research avenue in computational linguistics LexIt is a computational framework for the automatic acquisition subcategorization frames (Korhonen 2002, Schulte im Walde and exploration of corpus-based distributional profiles of Italian 2009, etc.) verbs, nouns and adjectives selectional preferences (Resnik 1993, Light & Greiff 2002, Erk et al. 2010, etc.) LexIt is publicly available through a web interface: verb classes (Merlo & Stevenson 2001, Schulte im Walde 2006, http://sesia.humnet.unipi.it/lexit/ Kipper-Schuler et al. 2008, etc.) Corpus-based information has been used to build lexical LexIt is the first large-scale resource of such type for Italian, resources aiming at characterizing the valence properties of predicates cf. VALEX for English (Korohnen et al. 2006), LexSchem for fully on distributional ground French (Messiant et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Acquiring Verb Subcategorization from Spanish Corpora
    Acquiring Verb Subcategorization from Spanish Corpora Grzegorz ChrupaÃla [email protected] Universitat de Barcelona Department of General Linguistics PhD Program “Cognitive Science and Language” Supervised by Dr. Irene Castell´onMasalles September 2003 Contents 1 Introduction 5 2 Verb Subcategorization in Linguistic Theory 7 2.1 Introduction . 7 2.2 Government-Binding and related approaches . 7 2.3 Categorial Grammar . 9 2.4 Lexical-Functional Grammar . 11 2.5 Generalized Phrase-Structure Grammar . 12 2.6 Head-Driven Phrase-Structure Grammar . 14 2.7 Discussion . 17 3 Diathesis Alternations 19 3.1 Introduction . 19 3.2 Diathesis . 19 3.2.1 Alternations . 20 3.3 Diathesis alternations in Spanish . 21 3.3.1 Change of focus . 22 3.3.2 Underspecification . 26 3.3.3 Resultative construction . 27 3.3.4 Middle construction . 27 3.3.5 Conclusions . 29 4 Verb classification 30 4.1 Introduction . 30 4.2 Semantic decomposition . 30 4.3 Levin classes . 32 4.3.1 Beth Levin’s classification . 32 4.3.2 Intersective Levin Classes . 33 4.4 Spanish: verbs of change and verbs of path . 35 4.4.1 Verbs of change . 35 4.4.2 Verbs of path . 37 4.4.3 Discussion . 39 4.5 Lexicographical databases . 40 1 4.5.1 WordNet . 40 4.5.2 VerbNet . 41 4.5.3 FrameNet . 42 5 Subcategorization Acquisition 44 5.1 Evaluation measures . 45 5.1.1 Precision, recall and the F-measure . 45 5.1.2 Types and tokens . 46 5.2 SF acquisition systems . 47 5.2.1 Raw text .
    [Show full text]
  • Features from Aspects Via the Minimalist Program to Combinatory Categorial Grammar
    Á. Gallego & D. Ott (eds.). 2015. 50 Years Later: Reflections on Chomsky’s Aspects. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL. Available for purchase at http://mitwpl.mit.edu © The Authors FEATURES FROM ASPECTS VIA THE MINIMALIST PROGRAM TO COMBINATORY CATEGORIAL GRAMMAR NEIL SMITH ANNABEL CORMACK University College London 1 Background One major contribution of Aspects (Chomsky 1965)1 was to initiate the development of a theory of syntactic features. There is no use of features, either syntactic or morphophonemic, in Chomsky’s earliest work The Morphophonemics of Modern Hebrew (1951/79); they do not appear in his monumental The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory (1955/75); nor in Syntactic Structures (1957) (except for category labels); nor, except for phonological distinctive features, in Current Issues in Linguistic Theory (1964). In Aspects features play a crucial role. Chomsky suggests (p.214) that “We might, then, take a lexical entry to be simply a set of features, some syntactic, some phonological, some semantic”. But even here the use of features was somewhat haphazard, apparently unconstrained, and a terminological mess. In this contribution we want to trace – in broad outline – the development of a theory of morphosyntactic features and propose a fully Merge-based and parsimonious version of such a theory. Our work is intended as a contribution to Chomsky’s Minimalist Program, pursuing the same ends on the basis of some different background assumptions. These assumptions and the specific analyses derived from them have been developed using an austere version of Combinatorial Categorial Grammar, but are of wider applicability. All current theories use phonological features and ‘semantic’ features which we will largely ignore except to point out that the term ‘semantic’ is used in radically different ways to cover truth-theoretic, conceptual or encyclopaedic distinctions.
    [Show full text]
  • Glossary for Syntax in Three Dimensions (2015) © Carola Trips
    Glossary for Syntax in three Dimensions (2015) © Carola Trips A - B - C - D - E - F - G - H - I - J - K - L - M - N - O - P - Q - R - S - T - U - V - W - X - Y – Z A abstract case Abstract Case (or "Deep Case") is an abstract notion of case distinct from the morphologically marked case (cf. "case") used in Case theory (subtheories of Generative Grammar). Abstract Case is present even in languages which lack morphological case on noun phrases; it is usually assumed to be congruent with morphological case when such features are present. For example in German dative case is marked morphologically as in Ich helfe ihm ('I help him') whereas no such case marking is found in the English sentence (but Case is still there). academy In previous centuries the need for an academy dealing with linguistic matters was discussed and debated in England (on the model of the French académie française). Due to the increase in the production of grammars in the 17th and 18th centuries and the rising number of grammarians, calls for an academy to codify the English language by publishing an authoritative grammar and dictionary dwindled. acceptance Step in the process of standardization: a selected variety of a particular language must gain acceptance by a group of speakers/users who are capable of influencing other speakers. actants In valency grammar, a functional unit determined by the valency of the verb. Actants are required by the valence of the verb and are the equivalents of arguments (in generative grammar). © Carola Trips 1 active voice Term used in grammatical analysis of voice, referring to a sentence, clause or verb form where from a semantic point of view the grammatical subject is typically the actor in relation to the verb.
    [Show full text]
  • Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 4 Issue 2 Article 11 1997 Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings Edward Kako University of Pennsylvania Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Kako, Edward (1997) "Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 4 : Iss. 2 , Article 11. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings Edward Kako 1. Introduction Do subcategorization frames have meanings independent of the verbs which appear in them? Advocates of the Projectionist position have answered “no” to this question, arguing that subcat frames serve only to satisfy the structural demands imposed upon sentences by the semantics of verbs (e.g., Chomsky, 1981; Pinker, 1989; Levin, 1993). Proponents of the Constructionist position, by contrast, have answered “yes” to this question, arguing that whether a verb and frame can peaceably cohabit in a sentence depends upon whether the two are “compatible” (e.g., Goldberg,
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Sense and Verb Subcategorization Probabilities Douglas Roland Daniel Jurafsky University of Colorado University of Colorado Department of Linguistics Dept
    To appear in Suzanne Stevenson and Paola Merlo’s volume of papers from the 1998 CUNY Sentence Processing Conference (Benjamins) Verb Sense and Verb Subcategorization Probabilities Douglas Roland Daniel Jurafsky University of Colorado University of Colorado Department of Linguistics Dept. of Linguistics, Computer Boulder, CO 80309-0295 Science, and Inst. of Cognitive Science [email protected] Boulder, CO 80309-0295 [email protected] 1 Introduction The probabilistic relation between verbs and their arguments plays an important role in psychological theories of human language processing. For example, Ford, Bresnan and Kaplan (1982) proposed that verbs like position have two lexical forms: a more preferred form that subcategorizes for three arguments (SUBJ, OBJ, PCOMP) and a less preferred form that subcategorizes for two arguments (SUBJ, OBJ). Many recent psychological experiments suggest that humans use these kinds of verb-argument preferences as an essential part of the process of sentence interpretation. (Clifton et al. 1984, Ferreira & McClure 1997, Garnsey et al. 1997, MacDonald 1994, Mitchell & Holmes 1985, Boland et al. 1990, Trueswell et al. 1993). It is not completely understood how these preferences are realized, but one possible model proposes that each lexical entry for a verb expresses a conditional probability for each potential subcategorization frame (Jurafsky 1996, Narayanan and Jurafsky 1998). Unfortunately, different methods of calculating verb subcategorization probabilities yield different results. Recent studies (Merlo 1994, Gibson et al. 1996, Roland & Jurafsky 1997) have found differences between syntactic and subcategorization frequencies computed from corpora and those computed from psychological experiments. Merlo (1994) showed that the subcategorization frequencies derived from corpus data were different from the subcategorization data derived from a variety of psychological protocols.
    [Show full text]
  • Valency Dictionary of Czech Verbs: Complex Tectogrammatical Annotation
    Valency Dictionary of Czech Verbs: Complex Tectogrammatical Annotation Marketa´ Stranˇak´ ova-Lopatk´ ova,´ Zdenekˇ Zabokrtskˇ y´ Center for Computational Linguistics Faculty of Mathematics and Physics, Charles University Malostranske´ nam.´ 25, CZ-11800 Prague, Czech Republic stranak,zabokrtsky ¡ @ckl.mff cuni.cz http://ckl.mff.cuni.cz Abstract A lexicon containing a certain kind of syntactic information about verbs is one of the crucial prerequisities for most tasks in Natural Language Processing. The goal of the project described in the paper is to create a human- and machine-readable lexicon capturing in detail valency behavior of hundreds most frequent Czech verbs. Manual annotation effort consumed at this project limits the speed of its growth on the one hand, but guarantees significantly higher data consistency than that of automatically acquired lexicons. In this paper, we outline the theoretical background on which the lexicon is based, and describe the annotation schema (lexicon data structure, annotation tools, etc.). Selected quantitative characteristics of the lexicon are presented as well. 1. Introduction The noun phrase jeho bratra [his brother] preceded by The verb is traditionally considered to be the center of no preposition can be Gen.sg or Acc.sg. The verb ptat´ the sentence, and thus the description of syntactic behav- se [to ask] allows only the former possibility. ior of verbs is a substantial task for linguists. A syntactic ¢ Syntactic analysis (considering a dependency ori- lexicon of verbs with the subcategorization information is ented formalism, syntactic analysis can be informally obviously crucial also for many tasks in Natural Language expressed as ‘determining which word depends on Processing (NLP) domain.
    [Show full text]
  • Dependency Grammar
    Formal & Computational Aspects of Dependency Grammar { Argument structure in DG { Geert-Jan M. Kruijff Computational linguistics University of the Saarland Saarbrucken¨ Germany [email protected] h i Contents First Last Prev Next J Goals Today we will have a look at two levels of argument structure, namely sub- • categorization and valency. At each level, we look at argumenthood from different viewpoints: • { Role of dependency: heads and dependents, types of relations, the meaning of relations. { Different dimensions of 'argumenthood': obligatoriness versus optionality, core versus free modification. { Instantiations of these notions in various dependency- and phrase structure- grammar frameworks. We argue that subcategorization and valency describe distinct levels of struc- • ture in a grammar. { We examplify why these levels are not isomorphic. { We develop a typological theory of abstract case to relate them. Contents First Last Prev Next J Contents Goals .................................................... 2 Subcategorization and valency .............................. 5 Subcategorization and valency are not isomorphic ....... 6 Consequences of non-isomorphy ....................... 8 Remarks on subcategorization in DG ........................ 9 Between subcategorization and surface word order ...... 12 Surface word order in DG ............................ 13 Separating dependency and linearization ............... 15 Resulting notions of subcategorization in DG ........... 17 Valency .................................................. 18
    [Show full text]
  • MITA Working Papers in Psycholinguistics, Volume 2. INSTITUTION Keio Univ., Tokyo (Japan)
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 358 719 FL 021 283 AUTHOR Otsu, Yukio, Ed. TITLE MITA Working Papers in Psycholinguistics, Volume 2. INSTITUTION Keio Univ., Tokyo (Japan). Inst. of Cultural and Linguistic Studies. PUB DATE 89 NOTE 102p.; Volume 1 is unavailable. For selected individual papers, see ED 331 302, ED 323 759, and FL 021 286-290. Publication partly supported bya grant from the Japanese Ministry of Education and Culture and the INS Corporation. AVAILABLE FROM Institute of Cultural & Linguistics Studies, Keio University, 2-15-45 Mita, Minato-ku, Tokyo 108 Japan. PUB TYPE Collected Works General (020) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC05 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS College Students; *English (Second Language); English for Academic Purposes; Error Patterns; Foreign Countries; Higher Education; High School Freshmen; High Schools; High School Students; *Language Processing; Linguistic Theory; *Second Language Learning; Semanti, ; Sentence Structure; Speech Acts; Verbs; Writing (Co-position) IDENTIFIERS Clauses; Connectives (Grammar); Japan; Reflexives; Universal Grammar ABSTRACT Seven original research papers are presented. The titles and authors are as follows: "Acquisition of the Argument-Structure of Verbs" (Mika Endo); "A Note on Semantic Selection" (Yoshio Endo); "The Governing Category Parameter in Second Language" (Makiko Hirakawa); "The Use of Connectives in English Academic Papers Written by Japanese Students" (Yasuko Kanno); "A Note on English As-Clauses" (Yoshihara Kumagai); "Structure-Dependence in Second Language Acquisition" (Kazuhiro Naoi); and
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Subcategorization Frequencies: American English Corpus Data, Methodological Studies, and Cross-Corpus Comparisons
    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (3), 432-443 Verb subcategorization frequencies: American English corpus data, methodological studies, and cross-corpus comparisons SUSANNE GAHL University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois DAN JURAFSKY Stanford University, Stanford, California and DOUGLAS ROLAND University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Verb subcategorization frequencies (verb biases) have been widely studied in psycholinguistics and play an important role in human sentence processing. Yet available resources on subcategorization fre- quencies suffer from limited coverage, limited ecological validity, and divergent coding criteria. Prior estimates of verb transitivity, for example, vary widely with corpus size, coverage, and coding criteria. This article provides norming data for 281 verbs of interest to psycholinguistic research, sampled from a corpus of American English, along with a detailed coding manual. We examine the effect on transi- tivity bias of various coding decisions and methods of computing verb biases. The frequency of linguistic structures, such as pho- duction (Gahl & Garnsey, in press). The experimental lit- nemes or words, has long been known to affect language erature on verb biases shows that, other things being equal, processing. Increasingly, research in sentence processing sentences that conform to a verb’s bias are easier to pro- has focused on the frequencies of more complex linguis- cess than sentences that violate a verb’s bias. Indeed, this tic structures.
    [Show full text]
  • Automatic Extraction of Subcategorization from Corpora
    Automatic Extraction of Subcategorization from Corpora Ted Briscoe John Carroll Computer Laboratory Cognitive and Computing Sciences University of Cambridge University of Sussex Pembroke Street, Cambridge CB2 3QG, UK Brighton BN1 9QH, UK ejb@cl, cam. ac. uk j otto. carroll@cogs, susx. ac. uk Abstract Several substantial machine-readable subcatego- rization dictionaries exist for English, either built We describe a novel technique and imple- largely automatically from machine-readable ver- mented system for constructing a subcate- sions of conventional learners' dictionaries, or manu- gorization dictionary from textual corpora. ally by (computational) linguists (e.g. the Alvey NL Each dictionary entry encodes the relative Tools (ANLT) dictionary, Boguraev et al. (1987); frequency of occurrence of a comprehen- the COMLEX Syntax dictionary, Grishman et al. sive set of subcategorization classes for En- (1994)). Unfortunately, neither approach can yield a glish. An initial experiment, on a sample genuinely accurate or comprehensive computational of 14 verbs which exhibit multiple comple- lexicon, because both rest ultimately on the manual mentation patterns, demonstrates that the efforts of lexicographers / linguists and are, there- technique achieves accuracy comparable to fore, prone to errors of omission and commission previous approaches, which are all limited which are hard or impossible to detect automatically to a highly restricted set of subcategoriza- (e.g. Boguraev & Briscoe, 1989; see also section 3.1 tion classes. We also demonstrate that a below for an example). Furthermore, manual encod- subcategorization dictionary built with the ing is labour intensive and, therefore, it is costly to system improves the accuracy of a parser extend it to neologisms, information not currently by an appreciable amount 1.
    [Show full text]