Syntactic Persistence in Language Production

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Syntactic Persistence in Language Production COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY 18, 35.5-387 (1986) Syntactic Persistence in Language Production J. KATHRYN BOCK Michigun Stn~r University Activation processes appear to have an important impact on the mechanisms of language use, including those responsible for syntactic structure in speech. Some implications of this claim for theories of language performance were examined with a syntactic priming procedure. On each priming trial, subjects produced a priming sentence in one of several syntactic forms. They then viewed an unre- lated event in a picture and described it in one sentence. The probability of a particular syntactic form being used in the description increased when that form had occurred in the prime, under presentation conditions that minimized sub- jects’ attention to their speech, to the syntactic features of the priming sentences, and to connections between the priming sentences and the subsequent pictures. This syntactic repetition effect suggests that sentence formulation processes are somewhat inertial and subject to such probabilistic factors as the frequency or recency of use of particular structural forms. Two further experiments showed that this effect was not appreciably modified by variations in certain conceptual characteristics of sentences. and all three experiments found evidence that the effects of priming were specific to features of sentence form, independent of sen- tence content. The empirical isolability of structural features from conceptual characteristics of successive utterances is consistent with the assumption thal some syntactic processes are organized into a functionally independent sub- system. 0 1986 Academic Press. Inc. The fundamental feature of the native speaker’s knowledge of language is productivity. This implies a capacity, under ideal circumstances, to generate and understand an unlimited number of different grammatical sentences in one’s language. The primary constraints on this capacity have been ascribed to a set of performance factors that customarily in- cludes memory limitations, a tendency to make mistakes, and distractibi- Experiment 1 was supported by a grant from the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation to the LJni- versity of Pennsylvania, and Experiment 2 by a faculty research grant from the Department of Psychology at Cornell University. Many people have contributed to this work, and 1 thank them all. Barbara Finlay, Jennifer Freyd, David Irwin, Frank Keil, Anthony Kroch, Joseph Stemberger, and Rose Zacks commented on earlier versions of the manuscript. Dorrit Billman, Anthony Kroch, John McKinney, Ruth Ostrin, and Eleanor Saffran offered valuable suggestions and materials for the research. Gina Cherry, Douglas Files, Paul Pe- tersen, James Yeomans, and, singularly, Constance Stillinger assisted in carrying out the experiments. Finally, W. J. M. Levelr and two anonymous reviewers helped to clarify the issues addressed in the paper. Please send correspondence, including requests for reprints, to Kathryn Bock, Department of Psychology, Psychology Research Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824. 355 0010-0285186 $7.50 Copyriglu 0 1986 by Academic Press, Inc. All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 356 J. KATHRYN BOCK lity (Chomsky, 1965). This paper investigates a performance constraint of a type not directly implicated in this traditional list. The constraint is manifested in a tendency to repeatedly employ the same syntactic form across successive utterances, suggesting the existence of additional pro- cesses that are antagonistic to the productivity of syntax in actual lan- guage use. The processes in question are central to a number of current discus- sions of sentence production mechanisms (Bates & MacWhinney, 1982; Bock, 1982; Dell, 1985, in press; Harley, 1984; MacKay, 1982; Motley, Baars, & Camden, 1983; Stemberger, 1985). The common feature of these accounts is their incorporation of activation or strength constructs. In essence, the activation or strengthening of information implies a quasi-neurological energizing, excitation, or threshold reduction that per- sists over time, increasing the probability that the activated or strength- ened information will influence subsequent cognitive processes (An- derson, 1983; Posner, 1978). The intuitive evidence for activation processes in language production is most striking in everyday speech errors, which provide much of the data for current models of speech formulation. Consider mistakes such as “Get out of the Clark” (where “car” was intended, said while glancing at a store front with the name “Clark’s” printed on it; Harley, 1984), “I am a sheep in lamb’s clothing” (where “wolf’s clothing” was intended; Norman, 1981), and “If he says, ‘here’s looking at you, babe,’ take your foot out of the stirrups and wallop him in the chollops” (where “chops” was intended, Garrett, 1980). These errors suggest, respectively, the acti- vation by environmental events of information which then intruded into the utterance, the activation by one word in an utterance of a related word that displaced an intended item, and the persistence of activation of a phonological pattern from one word into a subsequent word. Although effects of activation processes in language production are most clearly documented in cases involving sounds and words, there are also subtle indications of their role in determining the syntactic features or utterances. Some of the arguments again come from speech errors. For example, mistakes such as “Do I have to put on my seatbelt on?” and “This is getting very difficult to cut this” have been interpreted as a reflection of the simultaneous activation of two different syntactic struc- tures capable of expressing the same semantic intention (Stemberger, 1982, in press). Possible influences of activation processes on syntax are also sug- gested by a pattern that has been observed in normal language use. This involves the repetition of syntactic forms across successive utterances. There have been several discussions of this and related phenomena SYNTAX IN LANGUAGE PRODUCTION 357 (Kempen, 1977; Lashley, 1951; Selz, 1922, cited by Levelt & Kelter, 1982; Schenkein, 1980), but two recent studies will serve to illustrate it. Weiner and Labov (1983) have shown that in sociolinguistic interviews, one of the factors that is significantly associated with the occurrence of a passive utterance is the presence of another passive somewhere in the previous five sentences. Levelt and Kelter (1982) also found repetition across speakers in question-answer sequences. For example, they called several hundred merchants in the Netherlands and asked them the Dutch equivalents of the questions (a) At what time does your shop close? or (b) What time does your shop close. 7. The responses to these questions varied in a very regular way: When the question contained a preposi- tional phrase, as in (a), the answer tended to be in the form of preposi- tional phrase, such as At five o’clock; when the question contained a simple noun phrase, as in (b), simple noun phrase responses such as Five o’clock increased in frequency.’ Observations such as these, which involve the structural features of utterances, raise a critical issue about the nature of the activated infor- mation. In the case of lexical and phonological intrusions, exchanges, anticipations, and related errors, it seems natural to ascribe them to the activation of mental representations of the intruding elements. Indeed, they can be elegantly explained and their relative incidence predicted by speech production models that include such activation and representa- tional assumptions (Dell, 1985, in press; Stemberger, 1985 ). In the case of syntactic intrusions or perseverations, however, it is not clear that the effects can be accounted for in a comparable way. The major problems center on differences between words and sen- tences that are related to the abstractness and discreteness of their repre- sentations. In the case of lexical knowledge, it is customary to assume that the mental representations encode such features of words as the sounds that comprise them and the order in which the sounds occur. In addition, because lexical and phonological knowledge can be represented as relatively discrete inventories of items that repeatedly enter into be- havior in much the same form, it is possible to link their mental represen- tations within networks that define similarity relationships among ele- ments in terms of shared features. Activation can then be viewed as spreading from the representation of one element in the network to the representation of another element, causing it to influence subsequent be- havior. 1 Levelt and Kelter interpreted the repetition effect they observed in terms of a tendency to reuse particular words, which in their studies were always prepositions. However, struc- tural repetition is also involved in the effect, since the repetition of the preposition required the repetition of prepositional phrase structure. 358 J. KATHRYN BOCK Neither of these assumptions holds in any clear way for the representa- tions of sentences. First, the type-to-token relationships are more com- plex than for sounds and words. In particular, it makes little sense to assume that there are permanently stored representations of sentence types that directly encode such information as the order of their compo- nent words, whether in terms of the words themselves or their form-class categories (e.g., an encoding such as “definite article, adjective, noun, past tense singular
Recommended publications
  • What Is the Relationship Between Language Production and Comprehension?
    Journal of Memory and Language 89 (2016) 1–7 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Editorial Same, different, or closely related: What is the relationship between language production and comprehension? The historical tradition in psycholinguistics has largely became a hot issue through Lee’s (1950) discovery of the been to study either language production or comprehen- delayed auditory feedback (DAF) effect. When you hear your sion. Almost all of nineteenth century psycholinguistics, own speech delayed by some 150 ms, speech fluency dra- for instance, concerned the production of language, culmi- matically breaks down. Based on these observations, Lee nating in Wundt’s two-volume Die Sprache of 1900. This designed an engineering model of self-monitoring, which also held for research in language acquisition which, lar- required feedback to take place within the syllable being gely based on diary data, almost exclusively concerned spoken. the child’s production of speech until Eimas, Siqueland, Almost simultaneously, Broadbent (1952) demon- Jusczyk, and Vigorito (1971) introduced the experimental strated that participants were unable to understand a study of speech perception in infants. During the 1970s new question while answering a previous question. Atten- psycholinguistics became almost exclusively comprehen- tion can focus on one or the other task, but not on both sion research. Johnson-Laird opened his review of experi- simultaneously. This insight led to Broadbent’s famous fil- mental psycholinguistics in the 1974 Annual Review of ter model of selective attention. The issue kept returning in Psychology with the statement: ‘‘The fundamental problem psycholinguistics.
    [Show full text]
  • Hirsh-Pasek Vita 2-21
    Date: February, 2021 Kathryn A. Hirsh-Pasek, Ph.D. The Debra and Stanley Lefkowitz Distinguished Faculty Fellow Senior Fellow Brookings Institution Academic Address Department of Psychology Temple University Philadelphia, PA 19122 (215) 204-5243 [email protected] twitter: KathyandRo1 Education University of Pennsylvania, Ph.D., 1981 (Human Development/Psycholinguistics) University of Pittsburgh, B.S., 1975 (Psychology/Music) Manchester College at Oxford University, Non-degree, 1973-74 (Psychology/Music) Honors and Awards Placemaking Winner, the Real Play City Challenge, Playful Learning Landscapes, November 2020 AERA Fellow, April 2020 SIMMS/Mann Whole Child Award, October 2019 Fellow of the Cognitive Science Society, December 2018 IDEO Award for Innovation in Early Childhood Education, June 2018 Outstanding Public Communication for Education Research Award, AERA, 2018 Living Now Book Awards, Bronze Medal for Becoming Brilliant, 2017 Society for Research in Child Development Distinguished Scientific Contributions to Child Development Award, 2017 President, International Congress on Infant Studies, 2016-2018 APS James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award - “a lifetime of outstanding contributions to applied psychological research” – 2015 Distinguished Scientific Lecturer 2015- Annual award given by the Science Directorate program of the American Psychological Association, for three research scientists to speak at regional psychological association meetings. President, International Congress on Infant Studies – 2014-2016 NCECDTL Research to Practice Consortium Member 2016-present Academy of Education, Arts and Sciences Bammy Award Top 5 Finalist “Best Education Professor,” 2013 Bronfenbrenner Award for Lifetime Contribution to Developmental Psychology in the service of science and society. August 2011 Featured in Chronicle of Higher Education (February 20, 2011): The Case for play. Featured in the New York Times (January 5, 2011) Effort to Restore Children’s Play Gains Momentum (article on play and the Ultimate Block Party; most emailed article of the day).
    [Show full text]
  • The Pervasive Role of Pragmatics in Early Language
    The pervasive role of pragmatics in early language Manuel Bohn Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Leipzig Research Center for Early Child Development, Leipzig University https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6006-1348 Michael C. Frank Department of Psychology, Stanford University https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7551-4378 Short Title: Pragmatics in early language ​ Author’s Note: We thank Erica Yoon and Bahar Köymen for comments on an earlier version of ​ this paper. MB received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Sklodowska-Curie grant agreement No 749229. MCF received funding from the Jacobs Foundation Research Fellowship program. Please address correspondence to [email protected]. 1 Abstract Language is a fundamentally social endeavor. Pragmatics is the study of how speakers and listeners use social reasoning to go beyond the literal meanings of words to interpret language in context. In this review, we take a pragmatic perspective on language development and argue for developmental continuity between early non-verbal communication, language learning, and linguistic pragmatics. We link phenomena from these different literatures by relating them to a computational framework (the rational speech act framework), which conceptualizes communication as fundamentally inferential and grounded in social cognition. The model specifies how different information sources (linguistic utterances, social cues, common ground) are combined when making pragmatic inferences. We present evidence in favor of this inferential view and review how pragmatic reasoning supports children’s learning, comprehension, and use of language. Keywords: Language development; Social cognition; Pragmatics; Computational modeling; ​ Communication 2 Introduction From a toddler pointing at a toy and saying “dat!” all the way to a lukewarm letter of recommendation, human beings use language to communicate an infinite range of meanings both flexibly and efficiently.
    [Show full text]
  • Simultanagnosia
    Brain (1991), 114, 1523-1545 SIMULTANAGNOSIA TO SEE BUT NOT TWO SEE by H. BRANCH COS LETT and ELEANOR SAFFRAN Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/brain/article/114/4/1523/387170 by guest on 29 September 2021 (From the Department of Neurology, Temple University School of Medicine, Philadelphia, PA, USA) SUMMARY Simultanagnosia is a disorder of visual perception characterized by the inability to interpret complex visual arrays despite preserved recognition of single objects. We report a series of investigations on a simultanagnosic patient which attempt to establish the nature of this visual processing disturbance. The patient performed normally on a feature detection task but was impaired on a test of attention-requiring visual search in which she was asked to distinguish between stimuli containing different numbers of targets. She was not impaired on a visual-spatial orienting task. She identified single briefly presented words and objects as rapidly and reliably as controls suggesting that access to stored structural descriptions was not impaired. With brief, simultaneous presentation of 2 words or drawings, she identified both stimuli significantly more frequently when the stimuli were semantically related than when they were unrelated. On the basis of these and other data, we suggest that the patient's simultanagnosia is attributable to an impairment in the process by which activated structural descriptions are linked to information coding the location of the object. INTRODUCTION Models of cognitive processes that provide an explanatory framework for normative data should also be able to account for phenomena that arise from impairments in these processes. In this paper we describe a perceptual disturbance that presents a challenge for current theories of visual processing.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 15 Second Language Acquisition
    Chapter 15 Second Language Acquisition Dorothy Chun and Jan Frodesen Key Terms • Automaticity • Communicative competence • Critical period hypothesis • Interaction hypothesis • Interlanguage • Nativist theories of SLA • Sociocultural theories of SLA Chapter Preview As we have seen in earlier chapters, language is a complex array of many different components, ranging from individual sounds to whether a particular utterance or sentence is appropriate in a given situation or culture. In this chapter we will look at the issues involved in learning or acquiring a second language as an adolescent or adult learner. The main question with regard to second language acquisition (SLA) is: Why do people acquire a first language with little conscious effort while it is so difficult to master all of the aspects of a second language and speak it like a native speaker? This chapter will first discuss the main linguistic issues with regard to how second languages are acquired (e.g., phonology, morphology, syntax, lexicon/vocabulary, pragmatics). It will then describe some of the influences from the field of psychology on the study of second language acquisition and will examine the cognitive processes that differ between first-language (L1) and second-language (L2) learning. Thirdly, the chapter will consider how social and affective issues of L2 learning have come to the forefront in the last decade of the 20th century and into the 21st century. Finally, interspersed throughout the chapter are discussions of the 1 relationship between current knowledge about how second languages are acquired and the practice of language teaching, including some of the current issues in language teaching, especially those arising from increased globalization.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Origins of Language
    Cambridge University Press 978-1-108-49945-3 — The Study of Language George Yule Excerpt More Information 1 The Origins of Language The first person to set foot on the continent of Australia was a woman named Warramurrungunji. She emerged from the sea onto an island off northern Australia, and then headed inland, creating children and putting each one in a specific place. As she moved across the landscape, Warramurrungunji told each child, “I am putting you here. This is the language you should talk! This is your language!” Erard (2016) This origin story from the Iwaidja people of Australia, illustrated in the painting above, offers an explanation of not only where language came from, but also why there are so many different languages. Among the English-speaking people, there have been multiple attempts to provide a comparable explanation, but not much proof to support any of them. Instead of a belief in a single mythical earth mother, we have a variety of possible beliefs, all fairly speculative. We simply don’t have a definitive answer to the question of how language originated. We do know that the ability to produce sound and simple vocal patterning (a hum versus a grunt, for example) appears to be in an ancient part of the brain that we share with all vertebrates, including fish, frogs, birds and other mammals. But that isn’t human language. We suspect that some type of spoken language must have developed between 100,000 and 50,000 years ago, well before written language (about 5,000 years ago).
    [Show full text]
  • Phd Handbook in Linguistics
    The PhD program in Linguistics Table of Contents The PhD program in Linguistics 2 Graduate work in the Department of Linguistics 2 Graduate Courses in Linguistics 3 Graduate Introductory courses (400-level) 3 Graduate Core courses (400-level) 4 Advanced Graduate Courses (500-level) 4 Linguistics Requirements for PhD students 5 Planning the PhD degree program of study 5 Pre-Qualifying 5 Qualifying for Advancement to Candidacy for PhD students 5 Timing 6 Teaching Assistantships 6 Sample Schedules for a Linguistics PhD 7 Sample Linguistics and Brain and Cognitive Sciences schedule 7 Sample Linguistics and Computer Science (LIN/CS) schedule. 8 Course Descriptions 9 Linguistics 9 Relevant Brain and Cognitive Science Graduate Courses 14 Relevant Computer Science Graduate Courses 16 1 The PhD program in Linguistics The PhD program in Linguistics The Department of Linguistics at the University of Rochester offers a fully-funded five year PhD program in Linguistics, focusing on cross-disciplinary training and collaboration. Students in this program have a primary affiliation in Linguistics, with secondary affiliation in an allied department. At Rochester, cross-disciplinary, collaborative work is the norm. The Linguistics faculty are grounded in the traditional fields of formal linguistics, employing empirical methodologies to examine data and topics in experimental syntax, semantics, pragmatics, phonetics, laboratory phonology, and morphology in collaboration with faculty and students in allied fields. Our work incorporates contemporary issues and practices in these areas. Our principal allied fields are Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Computer Science, but we also have strong connections in related departments, such as Biomedical Engineering and departments at the Eastman School of Music.
    [Show full text]
  • 21 Chapter I Language Production 1.1 Introduction in This Chapter It Will Be
    Chapter I Language production 1.1 Introduction In this chapter it will be seen that both models of L1 production as well as accounts of L2 production can shed light on what happens with L2 performance. In particular, this chapter will aim to provide an answer to the following four questions: i) How is language produced? ii) How does L2 production differ from L1 production? iii) How do attention and memory mediate L2 production and development? iv) How can engaging in production potentially lead to second language development? In order to understand how L2 production works we first need to understand how L1 operates, and so L1 production will be considered in the first part of this chapter. Levelt’s (1989) L1 production model will be used as the main reference. Special emphasis will be placed on the areas of message generation, retrieval of lexical items, message formulation, and self-monitoring, with only a more limited consideration of articulation and speech comprehension. In the second part of the chapter, from a number of accounts of L2 production, those aspects that distinguish 21 it from L1 production will be pointed out, and lexical access, grammatical encoding, and self-monitoring will receive special attention. After that, the underlying constructs of attention and memory will be discussed. Regarding attention, the ideas of limited resources, selection, and capacity will be outlined, and the aspects most directly related to production will be underlined. The architecture and processes of memory will be presented, and the distinction between working memory (WM) and long-term memory (LTM) will be established.
    [Show full text]
  • Production Methods in Language Acquisition Research Sonja
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of Essex Research Repository Production methods in language acquisition research Sonja Eisenbeiss 1. Introduction Collecting and analyzing samples of spoken speech from learners has played a central role in acquisition research since its beginnings. Initially, such speech samples were mostly collected in naturalistic settings, where researchers recorded learners in spontaneous interactions with family members, friends or researchers. Many of these naturalistic or spontaneous speech samples are now freely available, typically in written form, i.e. as transcripts; and they continue to be used by (psycho)linguists with a wide range of research interests. Since the 1950s, acquisition researchers have supplemented naturalistic speech samples with production experiments. In these experiments, researchers systematically manipulate one or more variables and measure whether any changes with respect to these variables affect speakers‟ behavior. Other researchers have developed semi-structured elicitation techniques: they keep the communicative situation as natural as possible, but use interviewing techniques, videos or games to encourage the production of rich and comparable speech samples. In the following, I will discuss the rationale of naturalistic speech sampling, production experiments and semi-structured elicitation and show for which phenomena and learner types they are typically used. I will also give a brief overview of data collection and analysis procedures and discuss the advantages and disadvantages of different production methods. 2. Naturalistic studies 2.1 Rationale In naturalistic studies, researchers only interfere by audio/video-recording learners and other participants – sometimes without them even knowing that they are being recorded.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurolinguistics
    Neurolinguistics 1 What is neurolinguistics? Neurolinguistics studies the relation of language and communication to different aspects of brain function, i.e. it tries to explore how the brain understands and produces language and communication. This involves attempting to combine theory from neurology/neurophysiology (how the brain is structured and how it functions) with linguistic theory (how language is structured and how it functions). • "human language or communication (speech, hearing, reading, writing, or non-verbal modalities) related to any aspect of the brain or brain function" (Brain and Language: "Description") • The common problem area of relating aspects of language or communication to brain function in this dynamic formulation, is stated as a common question by Luria in "Basic problems in neurolinguistics": • "what are the real processes of formation of verbal communication and its comprehension, and what are the components of these processes and the conditions under which they take place" • (Luria, 1976, p.3) Interdisciplinary enterprise • linguistics,neuroanatomy, neurology, neurophysiology, philosophy, psychology, psychiatry, speech pathology and computer science, neurobiology, anthropology, chemistry, cognitive science and artificial intelligence. • Thus, the humanities, as well as medical, natural and social sciences, as well as technology are represented. Different views on the relation between brain and language • Localism tries to find locations or centers in the brain for different language functions. Associationism places language functions in the connections between different areas of the brain, making it possible to associate, for example, perceptions of different senses with words and/or “concepts”. • Dynamic localization of function assumes that functional systems of localized sub-functions perform language functions. Such systems are dynamic, so that they can be reorganized during language development or after a brain damage.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 August 2018 Vita Name Randi C. Martin Address Department Of
    1 August 2018 Vita Name Randi C. Martin Address Department of Psychology, MS-657 Rice University P.O. Box 1892 Houston, Texas 77251 e-mail: [email protected] Phone 713-348-3417 Personal Citizenship - U.S.A. Education B.A. University of Oregon (general social science, mathematics) 1971 M. A. Portland State University (psychology) 1975 M.S. Johns Hopkins University (psychology) 1977 Ph.D. Johns Hopkins University (psychology) 1979 Other training: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): An Introductory Course, Medical College of Wisconsin, Oct. 2000. Professional Societies Member: Academy of Aphasia, American Psychological Association, American Psychological Society, Psychonomic Society, Sigma Xi, American Association for the Advancement of Science, Society for Cognitive Neuroscience, Society of Experimental Psychologists, Women in Cognitive Science Offices: Elected Member-at-large of the Executive Committee of Division 3 (Experimental Psychology) of APA. APA/BSA liaison to the AAAS Psychology Section Steering Committee, American Association for the Advancement of Science, 2018-2020. Member-at-large, Psychology Section Steering Committee, American Association for the Advancement of Science, 2015-2018. Immediate Past Chair of the Governing Board, Academy of Aphasia, 2015-2017. Chair of the Governing Board, Academy of Aphasia, 2012-2014. Governing Board, Academy of Aphasia, 2009-2011. Executive Committee, Council of Graduate Departments of Psychology (COGDOP), 2002-2005. American Psychological Association: Member, Publications and Communications Board (2000- 2005). Chair: 2002. Women in Cognitive Science – co-organizer of meetings in association with the Psychonomic Society, 2002-2006. Member of Advisory Panel, 2006-present. Psychonomic Society - Secretary/Treasurer (1993-1995), Governing Board (1997-2002), Chair of Membership Committee (1999) Federation of Behavioral, Psychological and Cognitive Sciences - Secretary/Treasurer (1997-1999) Honors Invited address, Annual Meeting of the Association for Psychological Science, May 2018.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetic Encoding in Utterance Production: a Review of Open Issues from 1989 to 2018
    Language, Cognition and Neuroscience ISSN: 2327-3798 (Print) 2327-3801 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/plcp21 Phonetic encoding in utterance production: a review of open issues from 1989 to 2018 Marina Laganaro To cite this article: Marina Laganaro (2019) Phonetic encoding in utterance production: a review of open issues from 1989 to 2018, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34:9, 1193-1201, DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2019.1599128 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1599128 Published online: 01 Apr 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 349 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 5 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=plcp21 LANGUAGE, COGNITION AND NEUROSCIENCE 2019, VOL. 34, NO. 9, 1193–1201 https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1599128 REVIEW ARTICLE Phonetic encoding in utterance production: a review of open issues from 1989 to 2018 Marina Laganaro Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science, University of Geneva ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Phonetic encoding refers to the mapping of an abstract linguistic code of the utterance into motor Received 15 October 2018 programmes which guide speech articulation. The encoding of speech gestures involves complex Accepted 14 March 2019 cognitive-motor planning, which has received limited attention in the psycholinguistic literature on KEYWORDS language production relative to linguistic encoding processes. Here we will review some issues on Phonetic encoding; phonetic encoding and the related empirical results by integrating evidence from psycholinguistic, phonological; syllable; motor phonetic, neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies from the last 30 years.
    [Show full text]