What Is the Relationship Between Language Production and Comprehension?
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Journal of Memory and Language 89 (2016) 1–7 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Memory and Language journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml Editorial Same, different, or closely related: What is the relationship between language production and comprehension? The historical tradition in psycholinguistics has largely became a hot issue through Lee’s (1950) discovery of the been to study either language production or comprehen- delayed auditory feedback (DAF) effect. When you hear your sion. Almost all of nineteenth century psycholinguistics, own speech delayed by some 150 ms, speech fluency dra- for instance, concerned the production of language, culmi- matically breaks down. Based on these observations, Lee nating in Wundt’s two-volume Die Sprache of 1900. This designed an engineering model of self-monitoring, which also held for research in language acquisition which, lar- required feedback to take place within the syllable being gely based on diary data, almost exclusively concerned spoken. the child’s production of speech until Eimas, Siqueland, Almost simultaneously, Broadbent (1952) demon- Jusczyk, and Vigorito (1971) introduced the experimental strated that participants were unable to understand a study of speech perception in infants. During the 1970s new question while answering a previous question. Atten- psycholinguistics became almost exclusively comprehen- tion can focus on one or the other task, but not on both sion research. Johnson-Laird opened his review of experi- simultaneously. This insight led to Broadbent’s famous fil- mental psycholinguistics in the 1974 Annual Review of ter model of selective attention. The issue kept returning in Psychology with the statement: ‘‘The fundamental problem psycholinguistics. If we cannot simultaneously focus atten- of psycholinguistics is simple to formulate: what happens tion on listening to speech and producing it, these pro- if we understand sentences?” Production research long cesses must involve access to the same processing remained an island of (quite sophisticated) speech error resources. Which resources are involved here? Kempen and hesitation analysis. But also when during the 1980s (2000; see also Kempen, Olsthoorn, & Sprenger, 2012) and 1990s the research agenda became more balanced argued that these are grammatical unification processes, between comprehension and production research, the which are shared in grammatical encoding and decoding. two fields developed largely independently. Historically Where these processes happen to be identical, concurrent only a small set of studies have explicitly addressed the listening and speaking is indeed possible, as demonstrated issue of relations between these two skills of the language in Marslen-Wilson’s (1973) studies of close shadowing. user. These studies are briefly surveyed below. Scholars have often observed that relations between Relations between production and comprehension were comprehension and production are affected by adaptation often invoked as causes of language change. Bredsdorff and learning. Levelt (2014) provides many examples. (1821,1886) mentioned mishearing, misunderstanding, or Wundt (1900, vol. I, p. 476), for instance, discusses the misrecollection of sounds as potential causes of sound adaptations in L2 or dialect perception to native articula- change in the speaker. Also indolence of the speaker would tion: ‘‘each [perceived] speech sound is a complex in which affect articulation and hence the sound patterns perceived one’s own articulatory experience takes part”. Conversely, by the listener, a view with which others, in particular it is an almost universal assumption in nineteenth century Wundt (1900), concurred. and in behaviorist twentieth century acquisition theory An important nineteenth century theory invoking the that the child exclusively learns by imitation. Perception interaction of production and perception of speech is leads production. Exceptions were noticed though. For Wernicke’s (1874) theory of self-monitoring in speech. Dur- instance, in the infant’s babbling spontaneous production ing speech the sensory speech center is actively involved in takes the lead, as authors such as Preyer (1882) and controlling whether the spoken words match stored sound Tracy and Stimpfl (1894,1909) argued. Wundt (1900) images. When the system breaks down, as in conduction agrees, but dismisses claims by Darwin (1877), Taine aphasia, paraphasias will occur. Self-monitoring in speech (1876), and Preyer (1882) that the child can also invent http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.03.002 0749-596X/Ó 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 2 Editorial / Journal of Memory and Language 89 (2016) 1–7 new words: ‘‘After all, children’s speech is a product of the Proposals for the architecture of the language user’s child’s environment, in which the child itself is, essentially, conceptual base emerged in the artificial intelligence only passively involved.” (Vol. I, p. 296). Behaviorist Kantor world, for instance in work by Schank (1972) and (1936) considered the listener as a ‘‘stimulus” to which the Winograd (1972). These proposals often included sketches speaker ‘‘adjusts”. The mechanism underlying this adjust- of parsers mapping input sentences onto conceptual struc- ment however remains in the dark. tures, for which Wood’s (1970) augmented transition net- The discussion of relations between comprehending work (ATN) was an attractive formalism because it and producing language took a new turn following the acknowledged the incrementality of sentence parsing and ‘‘cognitive revolution” (see Levelt, 1974/2008, Vol III for a because the conditions on transitions in the network can detailed review). The premise that language users are be syntactic, semantic, or conceptual. However, no speaker endowed with linguistic ‘‘competence”, a generative gram- models worth mentioning were proposed. The mostly tacit mar of their language, raised the question how that gram- assumption was that speaking and listening involved a mar was implemented in ‘‘performance”, in particular in shared conceptual base. Interactions between the two speaking and listening. The initial idea was that linguistic modes of language use were hardly considered. rules match one-to-one to parsing operations in the lis- That was quite different in the domain of phonetics tener and generative operations in the speaker. This where, since the 1950s, efforts emerged to incorporate ‘‘derivational theory of complexity” was at the basis of articulatory representations into phonetic models of Matthews’s (1962) analysis-by-synthesis model, where speech perception. As Liberman (1957) put it: ‘‘speech is the listener generates structures till a matching one is perceived by reference to articulation” (p. 122). This had found, and of Yngve’s (1961) ‘‘depth hypothesis”, where a already been suggested by Stetson (1951). Stevens (1960) sentence’s complexity for the speaker/listener depends proposed a schematic ‘‘analysis-by-synthesis” model of on the maximum number of phrase structure nodes that speech perception in which the ‘‘spectral representation” have to be put on hold when generating or parsing the sen- auditorily derived from the speech signal is converted into tence. This version of the derivational theory was soon an articulatory description, whose ‘‘active synthesis” yields replaced by the so-called ‘‘coding hypothesis”, first formu- a matching spectral representation. Liberman’s life’s work lated by Mehler (1963). When you listen to or memorize a was to develop and experimentally test his version of this sentence you recode its surface form as deep structure plus analysis-by-synthesis approach, the ‘‘motor theory of transformations (such as ‘‘passive” or ‘‘negative”). Repro- speech perception”, fully reviewed in Liberman (1996). Lis- ducing the sentence is going back from deep to surface teners map the acoustic signal onto the articulatory move- structure. These are ‘‘psychologically real” linguistic struc- ments that could have produced the signal and from there tures, identical for comprehension and production. The onto the intended gestural pattern. complexity of mapping surface structure onto deep struc- Speech perception affecting speech production has ture or inversely is a function of the number of transforma- been a recurrent theme in the history of psycholinguis- tions involved. A declarative sentence is easier to produce tics. Meringer and Mayer (1895) already defined a cate- or understand than a passive one, which is harder than a gory of ‘‘extraneous intrusions” in their analysis of slips passive negative one. Hence transformations are ‘‘psycho- of the tongue. A word just heard or seen could erro- logically real” in both comprehension and production. A neously intrude into the utterance a speaker was plan- flurry of experiments followed. ning, an observation time and again reconfirmed in However, this transformational version of derivational modern speech error research. Garrett (1980) called them complexity also had to be abandoned. Fodor and Garrett ‘‘environmental contaminants”. A similar phenomenon (1967) proposed to keep the coding hypothesis, i.e., the was observed by conversational analysts. Schenkein surface/deep structure mapping by the listener, but to for- (1980) discussed a wealth of cases in which a word or get about transformational complexity. The aim should phrase is repeated in immediately adjacent turns, such rather be to find out which cues in the surface structure as Does that sound wrong? – It sounds