National IQ and Economic Outcomes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

National IQ and Economic Outcomes Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2011) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid National IQ and economic outcomes Gerhard Meisenberg Department of Biochemistry, Ross University School of Medicine, Picard Estate, Portsmouth, Dominica article info abstract Article history: One of the most consequential parts of Richard Lynn’s work is the establishment of a comprehensive data Available online xxxx set of ‘‘national IQ’’ for nearly all countries in the world. The present contribution demonstrates the use of this database for the explanation of two economic outcomes: (1) economic growth and level of attained Keywords: wealth at the country level; and (2) income distribution in countries as measured by the Gini index. The IQ results show that high IQ is associated not only with high per-capita GDP and fast economic growth, but School achievement also with more equal income distribution. These outcomes are not mediated by educational exposure. Intelligence Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Education Human capital Economic growth Gini index 1. Introduction for the countries and 90.5 for the totality of individuals living in these countries. The discrepancy is caused by the higher average In today’s world we find enormous differences between coun- population size of high-IQ countries. tries in wealth, social and political structures, and many ‘‘cultural’’ Lynn and his coworkers showed that national IQ is closely traits. Similar differences are observed between ethnic, racial, reli- related with the results of international student assessments in gious and other groups, even if they live in the same country. mathematics, science, and other curricular subjects (Lynn & According to the ‘‘reductionist’’ approach, many of these differ- Meisenberg, 2010; Lynn, Meisenberg, Mikk, & Williams, 2007; ences result from differences in personality traits and cognitive Lynn & Mikk, 2009). The reported correlations of averaged school abilities between human groups. Richard Lynn has been the most achievement scores with IQ are as high as r = 0.919 (N = 67 coun- prominent protagonist of this approach in recent years (Lynn, tries, Lynn et al., 2007) and, with more recent data, r = 0.917 2008a). (N = 86 countries, Lynn & Meisenberg, 2010). These results suggest Lynn’s most outstanding contribution to this field is the compi- that IQ and school achievement are alternative indicators for the lation of a data base of ‘‘national IQ’’ for most countries of the average intelligence in a country. In economic terms, both are mea- world. Data for an initial set of 81 countries were published in sures of ‘‘human capital’’. School achievement results are currently 2001 and 2002 (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2001, 2002), followed by an ex- available for 111 countries. Eighty-seven countries have data for panded list of national IQs for 113 countries (Lynn & Vanhanen, both school achievement and IQ, and 160 countries have either 2006). The most recent update expands this set to a total of 136 IQ or school achievement or both. countries (Lynn, 2010). Based on the well established relationship between IQ and National IQs are based on studies with a wide variety of cogni- earnings at the individual level (reviewed in Strenze, 2007), Lynn tive tests, with different versions of the non-verbal Raven’s Pro- and Vanhanen (2002, 2006) proposed national IQ as a cause for gressive Matrices as the most widely used test (data from 95 differences in per-capita gross domestic product (GDP) and other countries). The computing of results from different cognitive tests country-level economic outcomes. The theory is that wealth- into a single score is justified by the high correlations between producing activities such as running a business, designing build- alternative cognitive tests. All cognitive tests are thought to mea- ings, treating diseases and innovating are done more effectively sure, to various extents, ‘‘general’’ cognitive ability or g (Jensen, by persons with higher general intelligence. 1998). Fifty-two of the 136 national IQs are based on a single study. The most important implication of this postulated causal path is For all other countries the national IQ is calculated from multiple that economic conditions in today’s less developed countries can studies, numbering up to 22 for Japan. National IQs range from be improved by increasing, within biological limits, the cognitive 60 (Malawi) to 108 (Hong Kong, Singapore). The average is 86.0 abilities of the population. Lynn has always been adamant in claiming that IQ is a cause rather than merely a consequence of prosperity, and that genetic race differences explain part but not E-mail address: [email protected] all of the international IQ differences (Lynn, 2006). 0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.022 Please cite this article in press as: Meisenberg, G. National IQ and economic outcomes. Personality and Individual Differences (2011), doi:10.1016/ j.paid.2011.06.022 2 G. Meisenberg / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2011) xxx–xxx The Lynn/Vanhanen theory about the causal importance of primary, secondary and tertiary enrolment ratios in 2002, as re- country-level IQ differences for prosperity and other development ported in the 2005 Human Development Report of the United indicators has been attacked on theoretical grounds (e.g., Morse, Nations. 2008). However, the few empiric studies conducted so far were Corruption is calculated from the reverse of Transparency Inter- mainly supportive. The relationship between IQ and national national’s Corruption Perception Index for the years 1999–2005 wealth has been confirmed in some studies (Hunt & Wittmann, (http://www.transparency.org), combined with older data includ- 2008; Whetzel & McDaniel, 2006). Others found a relationship of ing the Business International corruption score from 1980–1983 IQ with economic growth (Jones & Schneider, 2006; Weede, (Treisman, 2000), and the ‘‘no corruption’’ domain of the Heritage 2004; Weede & Kämpf, 2002), although one study found no inde- Foundation’s Economic Freedom Index of 1995 (http://www.heri- pendent relationship between cognitive test results and economic tage.org/research/). growth, claiming that previously observed effects were due to the Economic Freedom is calculated from the unrotated first factors inclusion of the ‘‘Asian Tigers’’ (Chen & Luoh, 2010). of maximum-likelihood factor analyses of areas 2–5 of the Fraser There are reasons to expect that high national IQ reduces in- Institute’s Economic Freedom Index for the periods 1975–2005 come inequality in addition to raising the average income level. (Gwartney et al., 2009), and domains 1, 2, and 5–8 of the Heritage One reason is that through market forces, the skill premium is ex- Foundation Index for 1995–2005 (http://www.heritage.org/re- pected to be higher in low-IQ countries than in high-IQ countries. search/). This measure indexes the extent of business regulation In low-IQ countries many low-IQ people compete for unskilled and red tape. work, but few high-IQ people compete for cognitively demanding Big Government indexes the government’s share of GDP. It is cal- work in management, engineering and other professions. This re- culated from area 1 of the Fraser Institute’s Economic Freedom In- sults in high pay for individuals doing cognitively demanding work dex for the periods 1975–2005 (size of government), and domains relative to the pay of unskilled labourers. Another reason to expect 3 and 4 of the Heritage Foundation Index for 1995–2005 (fiscal less income inequality in high-IQ countries is the existence in these freedom and government spending). These measures are factorial- countries of institutions for collective bargaining and for the redis- ly and conceptually different from the other components of the tribution of wealth from the rich to the poor. Cognitive sophistica- Fraser Institute and Heritage Foundation indices for ‘‘economic tion is required to create and maintain such institutions. So far, an freedom’’. inverse relationship of national IQ with income inequality has been Gini index: The primary data source is the World Income reported as an incidental finding in only two empiric studies Inequality Database (WIID2a) of the United Nations University, (Meisenberg, 2007, 2008), with no follow-up and no contradictory available at www.wider.unu.edu/wiid/wiid.htm, as described in findings in the literature. Meisenberg (2007). Missing data points were extrapolated from Economic outcomes are expected to depend not only on intelli- the World Bank’s World Development Indicators of 2005, the gence, but also on geographic location, natural resources, political Human Development Report 2005 of the United Nations, and the and economic institutions, and history. Studies about the effects of CIA’s World Factbook of 2009. IQ on economic outcomes must take these additional factors into Racial diversity is defined by the racial diversity index described account. Failure to do so can lead to spurious results because many in Meisenberg (2007). Racial distances were quantified as genetic variables used by economists correlate highly with national IQ. distance according to Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (2003). The present paper extends the previous studies on the possible Freedom/Democracy is the average of political freedom defined causal effects of intelligence on economic growth and on income as the averaged scores of political rights + civil liberties from Free- inequality, using the most recent data. Based on the near-equiva- dom House at http://www.freedomhouse.org/research/freeworld, lence of IQ and school achievement (Lynn & Meisenberg, 2010), average 1975–2005; and democracy, defined as Vanhanen’s the IQ data are augmented by school achievement data. This per- democracy index (average 1975–2004), from the Finnish Social Sci- mits a far broader coverage of countries than in any previous study. ence Data Archive at http://www.fsd.uta.fi/english/data/catalogue/ FSD1289/.
Recommended publications
  • Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism
    J. Intell. 2014, 2, 122-155; doi:10.3390/jintelligence2040122 OPEN ACCESS Journal of Intelligence ISSN 2079-3200 www.mdpi.com/journal/jintelligence Article Future Efforts in Flynn Effect Research: Balancing Reductionism with Holism Michael A. Mingroni Newark, Delaware, USA; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-302-753-3533 External Editor: Joseph L. Rodgers Received: 13 February 2014: in revised form 2 October 2014 / Accepted: 2 October 2014 / Published: 15 October 2014 Abstract: After nearly thirty years of concerted effort by many investigators, the cause or causes of the secular gains in IQ test scores, known as the Flynn effect, remain elusive. In this target article, I offer six suggestions as to how we might proceed in our efforts to solve this intractable mystery. The suggestions are as follows: (1) compare parents to children; (2) consider other traits and conditions; (3) compare siblings; (4) conduct more and better intervention programs; (5) use subtest profile data in context; and (6) quantify the potential contribution of heterosis. This last section contains new simulations of the process of heterosis, which provide a plausible scenario whereby rapid secular changes in multiple genetically influenced traits are possible. If there is any theme to the present paper, it is that future study designs should be simpler and more highly focused, coordinating multiple studies on single populations. Keywords: Flynn effect; intelligence; secular trend; heterosis 1. Introduction It has been nearly thirty years since James Flynn brought widespread attention to the occurrence of rapid gains in IQ test scores [1,2]. However, in honestly assessing the situation today one would have to conclude that we are not much closer to identifying the cause than we were three decades ago.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of General Intelligence ⇑ Satoshi Kanazawa
    Personality and Individual Differences 53 (2012) 90–93 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid The evolution of general intelligence ⇑ Satoshi Kanazawa Department of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, United Kingdom article info abstract Article history: Among Richard Lynn’s numerous significant contributions to science is his cold winters theory of the evo- Available online 12 June 2011 lution of general intelligence. The cold winters of Eurasia presented novel adaptive problems for our ancestors to solve, such as obtaining food by hunting large animals and keeping warm by building cloth- Keywords: ing, shelter and fire, and they functioned as strong selection pressures for higher intelligence. Empirical Richard Lynn analyses support both Lynn’s cold winters theory and my evolutionary novelty theory of the evolution of Cold winters theory general intelligence. Mean annual temperature and the degree of evolutionary novelty in the environ- J. Philippe Rushton ment independently predict the average intelligence of the population. Both theories can also account Evolutionary novelty theory for the observed race difference in intelligence. Race differences in intelligence Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. How did human intelligence evolve? Why did humans attain ecological niches and confront novel adaptive problems, the size such high levels of general intelligence? And why are there notable of their brain relative to their body (encephalization quo- differences in average intelligence in different populations and tient = EQ), and thus intelligence, increase in the course of evolu- races in different geographical locations? tion. The average living mammals are defined to have EQ of 1.0.
    [Show full text]
  • The Bell Curve of Intelligence, Economic Growth and Technological Achievement: How Robust Is the Cross-Country Evidence?
    Munich Personal RePEc Archive The Bell Curve of Intelligence, Economic Growth and Technological Achievement: How Robust is the Cross-Country Evidence? Burhan, Nik Ahmad Sufian and Che Razak, Razli and Rosli, Muhamad Ridhwan and Selamat, Muhamad Rosli Faculty of Entrepreneurship and Business, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Federation of Malay Students Union / Gabungan Pelajar Melayu Semenanjung, Kuala Lumpur, Faculty of Arts and Humanities, International University of Malaya-Wales, Kuala Lumpur, Faculty of Modern Languages and Communication, Universiti Putra Malaysia January 2017 Online at https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/77469/ MPRA Paper No. 77469, posted 13 Mar 2017 14:25 UTC The Bell Curve of Intelligence, Economic Growth and Technological Achievement: How Robust is the Cross-Country Evidence? Nik Ahmad Sufian Burhan a,b,*, Razli Che Razak c, Muhamad Ridhwan Rosli b,d, Muhamad Rosli Selamat e a Department of Human Sciences, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Kota Bharu, Malaysia b Federation of Malay Students Union / Gabungan Pelajar Melayu Semenanjung, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia c Faculty of Entrepreneurship and Business, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Kota Bharu, Malaysia d Faculty of Arts and Humanities, International University of Malaya-Wales, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia e Faculty of Modern Languages and Communication, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Malaysia *Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Intelligence quotient (IQ) scores are normally distributed within a nation’s population. In a cross-country regression, Burhan et al. (2014, Intelligence, 46, 1–8) had statistically proven that intellectual class represented by the 95th percentile IQ had contributed most to economic growth. Those with average ability (50th percentile IQ) contributed second most, followed by the non-intellectual class (5th percentile IQ).
    [Show full text]
  • National IQ and National Productivity: the Hive Mind Across Asia
    National IQ and National Productivity: The Hive Mind Across Asia Garett Jones Center for Study of Public Choice Department of Economics George Mason University January 2011 [Published in Asian Development Review, June 2011] Abstract A recent line of research demonstrates that cognitive skills—IQ scores, math skills, and the like— have only a modest influence on individual wages, but are strongly correlated with national outcomes. Is this largely due to human capital spillovers? This paper argues that the answer is yes. It presents four different channels through which intelligence may matter more for nations than for individuals: 1. Intelligence is associated with patience and hence higher savings rates; 2. Intelligence causes cooperation; 3. Higher group intelligence opens the door to using fragile, high- value production technologies, and 4. Intelligence is associated with supporting market-oriented policies. Abundant evidence from across the ADB region demonstrating that environmental improvements can raise cognitive skills is reviewed. Garett Jones is BB&T Professor for the Study of Capitalism at the Mercatus Center, and Associate Professor of Economics at the Center for Study of Public Choice at George Mason University. I. INTRODUCTION Within Asia, average intelligence quotient (IQ) scores differ dramatically across countries, from only around 80 points in South Asia to nearly 110 points in East Asia. This span is large: within a country, one standard deviation is defined as 15 IQ points. This paper argues that this is no mere epiphenomenon. Building upon conventional results in psychology and economics, it will be argued that intelligence matters far more for national productivity than it does for individual productivity and that group intelligence—a Hive Mind—is more important than individual intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • The European Bell Curve
    Kocovic P., Kocovic Z., Kocovic V.,Manic V. THE EUROPEAN BELL CURVE... Originalni naučni rad UDC 378(4/9) THE EUROPEAN BELL CURVE: IQ, EDUCATIONS AND INEQUALITY Petar Kocovic1, Zaklina Kocovic2, Vladimir Kocovic3, Predrag Manic4 1University ,,Union – Nikola Tesla“, School for Strategic and Operational Management, Belgrade, Serbia 2Mong, Belgrade, Serbia 3University of Belgrade, School for Mechanical Engineers, Serbia 4School for Business Studies and Law, Belgrade, Serbia Summary: In famous book “The Bell Curve” from Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray [1] was showed that in the United States there is a socioeconomic hierarchy of race and intelligence. They showed that whites are at the top of this hierarchy with the highest average IQ (103) and the highest socioeconomic status and earnings. Hispanics come next with an average IQ of 89 and intermediate socioeconomic status and earnings. Blacks come last with the lowest average IQ of 85 and the low- est socioeconomic status and earnings. They argued that the racial socioeconomic hierarchy is largely determined by differences in intelligence. IQ was consequence of education and will to be successful. Inequality is also link between educations and will to be successful. This paper present link between tree parameters: will, education and GDP. Keywords: Albania, Austria, Balkans, Bulgaria, Croatia, Education, Flynn effect, GDP per capita, Greece, IQ, IQ of nations, Montenegro, Serbia, Slovenia, Turkey 1. INTRODUCTION The first attempt to measure the intelligence of man dating back to the late 19th cen- tury. Alfred Binet and Theodore Simon had several attempts of making an intelligence test - a success arrived in 1905. From this time, lots of methods are changed - lot of different tests for the assessment of intelligence and emotion were produced.
    [Show full text]
  • In Italy, North–South Differences in IQ Predict Differences in Income, Education, Infant Mortality, Stature, and Literacy
    Intelligence 38 (2010) 93–100 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence In Italy, north–south differences in IQ predict differences in income, education, infant mortality, stature, and literacy Richard Lynn University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, United Kingdom article info abstract Article history: Regional differences in IQ are presented for 12 regions of Italy showing that IQs are highest in Received 13 January 2009 the north and lowest in the south. Regional IQs obtained in 2006 are highly correlated with Received in revised form 27 July 2009 average incomes at r=0.937, and with stature, infant mortality, literacy and education. The Accepted 27 July 2009 lower IQ in southern Italy may be attributable to genetic admixture with populations from the Available online 19 August 2009 Near East and North Africa. © 2009 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: IQ Income Infant mortality Stature Education Italy 1. Introduction into the twenty first century (Tabellini, 2009). Numerous theories have been advanced to explain what has become Regional differences in per capita income are exception- known as “Italian economic dualism”. An Italian economist ally large in Italy. The north is as prosperous as central and has written that “there is a huge literature dealing with the northern Europe, but the south is much poorer. The American so-called “questione meridionale”—the social, cultural and sociologist Robert Putnam (1993, p. 4) has written that “To economic backwardness of southern Italy” (Felici, 2007, p. 1). travel from the north to the south in the 1970s was to return Another Italian economist has written that “works dedicated centuries into the past… many lived in one- and two-room to the southern question would fill an entire library but many hovels; farmers still threshed grain by hand… transportation of the economists' questions as to the size and causes of was provided by donkeys that shared their rocky shelters, Italian economic dualism remain unanswered” (Toniolo, alongside a few scrawny chickens and cats”.
    [Show full text]
  • Temperature and Evolutionary Novelty As Forces Behind the Evolution of General Intelligence ☆
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Intelligence 36 (2008) 99–108 Temperature and evolutionary novelty as forces behind the evolution of general intelligence ☆ Satoshi Kanazawa Interdisciplinary Institute of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, United Kingdom Received 16 March 2007; received in revised form 6 April 2007; accepted 7 April 2007 Available online 31 May 2007 Abstract How did human intelligence evolve to be so high? Lynn [Lynn, R. (1991). The evolution of race differences in intelligence. Mankind Quarterly, 32, 99–173] and Rushton [Rushton, J.P. (1995). Race, evolution, and behavior: A life history perspective. New Brunswick: Transaction] suggest that the main forces behind the evolution of human intelligence were the cold climate and harsh winters, which selected out individuals of lower intelligence. In contrast, Kanazawa [Kanazawa, S. (2004). General intelligence as a domain-specific adaptation. Psychological Review, 111, 512–523] contends that it is the evolutionary novelty of the environment which increased general intelligence. Multiple regression analyses support both theories. Annual mean temperature and evolutionary novelty (measured by latitude, longitude, and distance from the ancestral environment) simultaneously have independent effects on average intelligence of populations. Temperature and evolutionary novelty together explain half to two- thirds of variance in national IQ. © 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Evolutionary psychology; Population differences in general intelligence; IQ and the Wealth of Nations; IQ and Global Inequality 1. Introduction climate. Cognitive demands placed by the need to survive harsh winters in cold climate select for higher intelligence, How did human intelligence evolve? Why did and thus general intelligence is expected to evolve and humans attain such high levels of general intelligence? become higher in colder climates.
    [Show full text]
  • On the I.Q. of Nations; Smart, Smarter and Smartest?
    1 On the I.Q. of Nations; Smart, Smarter and Smartest? William McConochie, Ph.D. TestMaster, Inc. 71 E.15th. Ave. Eugene, Or. 97041 541-686-9934 [email protected]. 3/11/09 Do nations vary in average citizen intelligence? According to some researchers they do. For example, in their book I.Q. and the Wealth of Nations Lynn and Vanhanen asserted in 2002 that nations do vary significantly in average intelligence (Lynn & Vanhanan, 2002). This work was followed with several additional publications: I.Q. and Global Inequality in 2006 (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2006), Race differences in Intelligence (Lynn & Vanhanen, 2006), and a comparison of academic achievement and estimated national I.Q.s in a journal article in 2009 (Lynn & Mikk, 2009). In a detailed review of Race Differences in Intelligence Jason Malloy provides an overview of the extensive research over many decades by these and other researchers (Malloy, 2006). Researchers have debated for years the relationships between race, nations, I.Q., latitude, climate and prosperity. For example, these debates have revolved around correlations between I.Q. and productivity of nations and latitude. Higher I.Q.s are associated with higher per capita income of nations. Differences in Education and nutrition have been offered as explanations for differences in mean I.Q.s and productivity levels of nations. Breast-feeding, higher latitude and larger head size are associated with higher I.Q.s. I.Q.s for some groups tend to increase over time, the "Flynn Effect". I.Q. and the Wealth of Nations has been criticized soundly (cf. Wikipedia, I.Q.
    [Show full text]
  • Temperature and Evolutionary Novelty As Forces Behind the Evolution of General Intelligence ☆
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Intelligence 36 (2008) 99–108 Temperature and evolutionary novelty as forces behind the evolution of general intelligence ☆ Satoshi Kanazawa Interdisciplinary Institute of Management, London School of Economics and Political Science, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, United Kingdom Received 16 March 2007; received in revised form 6 April 2007; accepted 7 April 2007 Available online 31 May 2007 Abstract How did human intelligence evolve to be so high? Lynn [Lynn, R. (1991). The evolution of race differences in intelligence. Mankind Quarterly, 32, 99–173] and Rushton [Rushton, J.P. (1995). Race, evolution, and behavior: A life history perspective. New Brunswick: Transaction] suggest that the main forces behind the evolution of human intelligence were the cold climate and harsh winters, which selected out individuals of lower intelligence. In contrast, Kanazawa [Kanazawa, S. (2004). General intelligence as a domain-specific adaptation. Psychological Review, 111, 512–523] contends that it is the evolutionary novelty of the environment which increased general intelligence. Multiple regression analyses support both theories. Annual mean temperature and evolutionary novelty (measured by latitude, longitude, and distance from the ancestral environment) simultaneously have independent effects on average intelligence of populations. Temperature and evolutionary novelty together explain half to two- thirds of variance in national IQ. © 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Evolutionary psychology; Population differences in general intelligence; IQ and the Wealth of Nations; IQ and Global Inequality 1. Introduction climate. Cognitive demands placed by the need to survive harsh winters in cold climate select for higher intelligence, How did human intelligence evolve? Why did and thus general intelligence is expected to evolve and humans attain such high levels of general intelligence? become higher in colder climates.
    [Show full text]
  • National IQ Means, Calibrated and Transformed from Educational Attainment, and Their Underlying Gene Frequencies
    Munich Personal RePEc Archive National IQ means, calibrated and transformed from educational attainment, and their underlying gene frequencies Weiss, Volkmar German Central Office for Genealogy 8 August 2008 Online at https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/13210/ MPRA Paper No. 13210, posted 07 Feb 2009 05:31 UTC Mankind Quarterly 49 (2008) 129-164 National IQ Means, Calibrated and Transformed from Educational Attainment, and Their Underlying Gene Frequencies Volkmar Weiss [email protected] Any general statement as to whether the secular trend of a society is eugenic or dysgenic depends upon a reliable calibration of the measurement of general intelligence. Richard Lynn set the mean IQ of the United Kingdom at 100 with a standard deviation of 15, and he calculated the mean IQs of other countries in relation to this “Greenwich IQ”. But because the UK test scores could be declining, the present paper recalibrates the mean IQ 100 to the average of seven countries having a historical mean IQ of 100. By comparing Lynn-Vanhanen-IQ with PISA scores and educational attainment of native and foreign born populations transformed into the IQ metric, we confirmed brain gain and brain drain in a number of nations during recent decades. Furthermore, the growth of gross domestic product per capita can be derived as a linear function of the percentage of people with an IQ above 105 and its underlying frequency of a hypothetical major gene of intelligence. Key Words: General intelligence; PISA; GDP; Dysgenics; Smart fraction theory. The Calibration of National IQ Means Most intelligence tests, such as those used by Lynn and Vanhanen (2002, 2006) in their compilations, have been constructed in Britain and the United States and have subsequently been administered to samples of the populations in other countries throughout the world.
    [Show full text]
  • IQ and National Productivity Garett Jones George Mason University
    IQ and National Productivity Garett Jones George Mason University Published in the New Palgrave Dictionary of Economics, 2011. [I would like to thank Alex Tabarrok, Tyler Cowen, and an anonymous reader for extremely helpful suggestions. All errors are my own.] Abstract: A recent line of research in economics and psychology hypothesizes that differences in national average intelligence, proxied by IQ tests, are important drivers of national economic outcomes. Cross-country regressions, while showing a robust IQ-growth relationship, cannot fully test this hypothesis. Thus, recent work explores the micro- foundations of the IQ-productivity relationship. The well-identified psychological relationship between IQ and patience implies higher savings rates and higher folk theorem- driven institutional quality in high average IQ countries. Experiments indicate that intelligence predicts greater pro-social behavior in public goods and prisoner’s dilemma games, supporting the hypothesis that high national average IQ causes higher institutional quality. High average IQ countries also have higher savings intensity by a variety of measures. Other possible IQ-productivity channels are discussed, as are possible environmental causes of differences in national average IQ. In recent years, some economists and psychologists have proposed that the average level of intelligence in a country—measured by conventional IQ tests—is an important independent driver of economic outcomes. As psychologists have known for decades, average IQ scores differ when given to large samples in different nations, and recent estimates indicate national average IQ correlates 0.7 with log GDP per capita (Figure 1). The macroeconomic question of interest is why IQ, which has a modest relationship with individual wages within a country, has such a strong relationship with average worker productivity across countries.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflections on Sixty-Eight Years of Research on Race and Intelligence
    Opinion Reflections on Sixty-Eight Years of Research on Race and Intelligence Richard Lynn Ulster Institute for Social Research, London NW26 9LQ, UK; [email protected] Received: 1 April 2019; Accepted: 16 April 2019; Published: 24 April 2019 1. Introduction I first encountered the question of race and intelligence sixty-eight years ago. This was in 1951 when I was a student reading psychology at Cambridge and attended Alice Heim’s lectures on intelligence. She told us that Blacks in the United States had a lower IQ than Whites and this was attributable to discrimination, which she subsequently asserted in her book (Heim, 1954) [1]. She also told us of the UNESCO (1951) [2] statement that “Available scientific knowledge provides no basis for believing that the groups of mankind differ in their innate capacity for intellectual and emotional development.” She did not tell us that this assertion was disputed by Sir Ronald Fisher (1951) [3], the Professor of Genetics at Cambridge, who wrote a dissent stating that evidence and everyday experience showed that human groups differ profoundly “in their innate capacity for intellectual and emotional development” and that “this problem is being obscured by entirely well-intentioned efforts to minimize the real differences that exist.” Nor did Alice Heim tell us that Henry Garrett, the Professor of Psychology at Columbia University, had argued that genetic factors are largely responsible for the lower IQ of Blacks than of Whites (Garrett, 1945) [4] so I and my fellow students at Cambridge were not well-informed about the issue of race differences in intelligence and its causes.
    [Show full text]