Novel Estimation Methods for Unsupervised Discovery of Latent Structure in Natural Language Text

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Novel Estimation Methods for Unsupervised Discovery of Latent Structure in Natural Language Text Novel Estimation Methods for Unsupervised Discovery of Latent Structure in Natural Language Text by Noah Ashton Smith A dissertation submitted to The Johns Hopkins University in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Baltimore, Maryland October, 2006 c Noah Ashton Smith 2006 All rights reserved Abstract This thesis is about estimating probabilistic models to uncover useful hidden struc- ture in data; specifically, we address the problem of discovering syntactic structure in nat- ural language text. We present three new parameter estimation techniques that generalize the standard approach, maximum likelihood estimation, in different ways. Contrastive estimation maximizes the conditional probability of the observed data given a “neighbor- hood” of implicit negative examples. Skewed deterministic annealing locally maximizes likelihood using a cautious parameter search strategy that starts with an easier optimiza- tion problem than likelihood, and iteratively moves to harder problems, culminating in likelihood. Structural annealing is similar, but starts with a heavy bias toward simple syntactic structures and gradually relaxes the bias. Our estimation methods do not make use of annotated examples. We consider their performance in both an unsupervised model selection setting, where models trained under different initialization and regularization settings are compared by evaluating the training objective on a small set of unseen, unannotated development data, and supervised model selection, where the most accurate model on the development set (now with annotations) is selected. The latter is far superior, but surprisingly few annotated examples are required. The experimentation focuses on a single dependency grammar induction task, in depth. The aim is to give strong support for the usefulness of the new techniques in one scenario. It must be noted, however, that the task (as defined here and in prior work) is somewhat artificial, and improved performance on this particular task is not a direct contri- bution to the greater field of natural language processing. The real problem the task seeks to simulate—the induction of syntactic structure in natural language text—is certainly of interest to the community, but this thesis does not directly approach the problem of ex- ploiting induced syntax in applications. We also do not attempt any realistic simulation of ii human language learning, as our newspaper text data do not resemble the data encoun- tered by a child during language acquisition. Further, our iterative learning algorithms assume a fixed batch of data that can be repeatedly accessed, not a long stream of data observed over time in tandem with acquisition. (Of course, the cognitive criticisms apply to virtually all existing learning methods in natural language processing, not just the new ones presented here.) Nonetheless, the novel estimation methods presented are, we will argue, better suited to adaptation for real engineering tasks than the maximum likelihood baseline. Our new methods are shown to achieve significant improvements over maximum likelihood estimation and maximum a posteriori estimation, using the EM algorithm, for a state-of-the-art probabilistic model used in dependency grammar induction (Klein and Manning, 2004). The task is to induce dependency trees from part-of-speech tag sequences; we follow standard practice and train and test on sequences of ten tags or fewer. Our results are the best published to date for six languages, with supervised model selection: English (improvement from 41.6% directed attachment accuracy to 66.7%, a 43% relative error rate reduction), German (54.4 → 71.8%, a 38% error reduction), Bulgarian (45.6% → 58.3%, a 23% error reduction), Mandarin (50.0% → 58.0%, a 16% error reduction), Turkish (48.0% → 62.4%, a 28% error reduction, but only 2% error reduction from a left-branching baseline, which gives 61.8%), and Portuguese (42.5% → 71.8%, a 51% error reduction). We also demonstrate the success of contrastive estimation at learning to disambiguate part- of-speech tags (from unannotated English text): 78.0% to 88.7% tagging accuracy on a known-dictionary task (a 49% relative error rate reduction), and 66.5% to 78.4% on a more difficult task with less dictionary knowledge (a 35% error rate reduction). The experiments presented in this thesis give one of the most thorough explorations to date of unsupervised parameter estimation for models of discrete structures. Two sides of the problem are considered in depth: the choice of objective function to be optimized during training, and the method of optimizing it. We find that both are important in un- supervised learning. Our best results on most of the six languages involve both improved objectives and improved search. The methods presented in this thesis were originally presented in Smith and Eisner (2004, 2005a,b, 2006). The thesis gives a more thorough exposition, relating the methods to other work, presents more experimental results and error analysis, and directly compares the methods to each other. iii Thesis committee (∗readers, †advisor): ∗†Jason Eisner (Assistant Professor, Computer Science, Johns Hopkins University) Dale Schuurmans (Professor, Computing Science, University of Alberta) Paul Smolensky (Professor, Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University) ∗David Yarowsky (Professor, Computer Science, Johns Hopkins University) iv For K.R.T. v Acknowledgments I would have written a shorter letter, but I did not have the time. —attributed to Cicero (106–43 BCE), Blaise Pascal (1623–1662), Mark Twain (1835–1910), and T. S. Eliot (1888–1965) First, I would like to thank my advisor, Jason Eisner, for many helpful and insightful technical conversations during the course of my graduate work. He usually knew when to let me tread on my own and when to grab me by the ankles and drag me (kicking and screaming) to the True Path,1 and when to let me go play in the woods. Most importantly he taught me to do well by doing good. If I can emulate in my entire career half of the rhetorical flair, vision, or enthusiasm he’s displayed in the course of my Ph.D., then I’ll count it a success. Thanks also to Debbie and Talia, who on occasion dined sans Jason because of me. My committee, consisting of David Yarowsky, Paul Smolensky, Dale Schuurmans, and, of course, Jason Eisner, have been supportive beyond the call of duty. They have provided valuable insight and held this work to a high standard. (Any errors that re- main are of course my own.) Other current and former members of the CLSP faculty have kindly spent time and shared ideas with me: Bill Byrne, Bob Frank, Keith Hall, Fred Jelinek,2 Damianos Karakos, Sanjeev Khudanpur,2 and Zak Shafran. Other profes- sors have contributed to my grad experience by teaching great classes: Yair Amir, Scott Smith, Rao Kosaraju, and Jong-Shi Pang. I thank researchers from other places for their helpful comments and discussions of many kinds at various times over the years: Eric Brill, Eugene Charniak, Michael Collins, Joshua Goodman, Mark Johnson, Rebecca Hwa, Dan Klein, John Lafferty, Chris Manning, Miles Osborne, Dan Roth, and Giorgio Satta; also 1This statement is not meant to insinuate that Jason ever inappropriately touched my ankles. He is also not to be blamed for my liberal use of footnotes. 2Special thanks to these individuals, who voluntarily read drafts and gave feedback. Any remaining errors are of course my own. vi anonymous reviewers on my papers. Deep gratitude especially to Philip Resnik and Dan Melamed, who provided early encouragement, continued mentoring, sunlight, and water. My work during the academic years from 2001 through 2006 was supported gener- ously by the Fannie and John Hertz Foundation. While I can’t predict the future impact of the work in these pages, I am certain that it would have been significantly reduced without the rare opportunity for unfettered exploration that this fellowship has offered. I am es- pecially grateful for my annual progress meetings with Lowell Wood; special thanks also to John Holzrichter, Barbara Nichols, and Linda Kubiak. Research included in this disser- tation was also supported (during the summers of 2003, 2005, and 2006) by grant number IIS-0313193 from the National Science Foundation to Jason Eisner. My student colleagues at Hopkins have made the time fun and the atmosphere con- structively critical and always creative—a never-ending brainstorm. Their exceptional in- telligence need no mention. Thanks to: Radu “Hans” Florian, Silviu Cucerzan, Rich Wicen- towski, Jun Wu, Charles Schafer, Gideon Mann, Shankar Kumar, John Hale, Paola Virga, Ahmed Emami, Peng Xu, Jia Cui, Yonggang Deng, Veera Venkataramani, Woosung Kim, Yu David Liu, Paul Ruhlen (R.I.P.), Elliott Drabek,´ 3 Reza Curtmola, Sourin Das, Geetu Ambwani, David Smith,2 Roy Tromble,2 Arnab Ghoshal, Lambert Mathias, Erin Fitzger- ald, Ali Yazgan, Yi Su, Chal Hathaidharm, Sam Carliles, Markus Dreyer,2 Brock Pytlik, Trish Driscoll, Nguyen Bach, Eric Goldlust, John Blatz, Chris White, Nikesh Garera, Lisa Yung, and visitors David Martinez, Pavel Pecina,ˇ Filip Jurcicek,ˇ and Vaclav´ Novak.´ Com- ments on drafts of my papers and at my seminar talks were always insightful and helpful. David & Cynthia, Roy & Nicole, and Markus: thanks for the camaraderie and the white- board. Thanks generally to the GRO. In the CS Department, Linda Rorke, Nancy Scheeler, Kris Sisson, Conway Benishek, Erin Guest, and Jamie Lurz have made administrivia as painless as they could. In Barton Hall, Laura Graham and Sue Porterfield (the CLSP dream team) are unrivaled in com- petence and warmth. To Steve Rifkin, Steve DeBlasio, and Brett Anderson in CS, Jacob Laderman and Eiwe Lingefors in CLSP, and Hans, David, and Charles (encore une fois) in the NLP lab: thanks for the cycles. Many, many friends have given me fond memories of the extra-lab time during my Baltimore years. These include the Book Smattering book group: Rita Turner, Ben Kle- 3Special thanks to these individuals, who shared knowledge about languages of experimentation.
Recommended publications
  • Computer Vision Stochastic Grammars for Scene Parsing
    Computer Vision Stochastic Grammars for Scene Parsing Song-Chun Zhu Ying Nian Wu August 18, 2020 Contents 0.1 This Book in the Series . ix Part I Stochastic Grammars in Vision 1 Introduction 3 1.1 Vision as Joint Parsing of Objects, Scenes, and Events . .3 1.2 Unified Representation for Models, Algorithms, and Attributes . .6 1.2.1 Three Families of Probabilistic Models . .6 1.2.2 Dynamics of Three Inferential Channels . .7 1.2.3 Three Attributes associated with each Node . .7 1.3 Missing Themes in Popular Data-Driven Approaches . .8 1.3.1 Top-Down Inference in Space and Time . .8 1.3.2 Vision is to be a Continuous Computational Process . .9 1.3.3 Resolving Ambiguities and Preserving Distinct Solutions . 11 1.3.4 Vision is Driven by a Large Number of Tasks . 12 1.4 Scope of Book: Compositional Patterns in the Middle Entropy Regime . 13 1.4.1 Information Scaling and Three Entropy Regimes . 14 1.4.2 Organization of Book . 14 2 Overview of Stochastic Grammar 17 2.1 Grammar as a Universal Representation of Intelligence . 17 2.2 An Empiricist’s View of Grammars . 18 2.3 The Formalism of Grammars . 20 2.4 The Mathematical Structure of Grammars . 21 2.5 Stochastic Grammar . 23 2.6 Ambiguity and Overlapping Reusable Parts . 24 2.7 Stochastic Grammar with Context . 26 3 Spatial And-Or Graph 29 3.1 Three New Issues in Image Grammars in Contrast to Language . 29 3.2 Visual Vocabulary . 31 3.2.1 The Hierarchical Visual Vocabulary – the "Lego Land" .
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Human Activity
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Human Activity Understanding and Prediction with Stochastic Grammar A thesis submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Master of Science in Computer Science by Baoxiong Jia 2019 c Copyright by Baoxiong Jia 2019 ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS Human Activity Understanding and Prediction with Stochastic Grammar by Baoxiong Jia Master of Science in Computer Science University of California, Los Angeles, 2019 Professor Song-Chun Zhu, Chair Video understanding is a booming research problem in computer vision. With its innate fea- ture where spatial and temporal information entangles with each other, video understand- ing has been challenging mainly because of the difficulty for having a unified framework where these two aspects can be modeled jointly. Among the tasks in video understand- ing, human activity understanding and prediction serve as a good starting point where the spatial-temporal reasoning capability of learning modules can be tested. Most of the current approaches towards solving the human activity understanding and prediction problems use deep neural networks for spatial-temporal reasoning. However, this type of approach lacks the ability to reason beyond the local frames and conduct long-term temporal reasoning. On the other hand, stochastic grammar models are used to model observed sequences on a symbolic level with all history information considered, but they perform poorly on handling noisy input sequences. Given these insights and problems of current approaches, we propose the generalized Earley parser for bridging the gap between sequence inputs and symbolic grammars. By combining the advantages of these two types of methods, we show that the proposed model achieves a better performance on both human activity recognition and future prediction.
    [Show full text]
  • Using an Annotated Corpus As a Stochastic Grammar
    Using an Annotated Corpus as a Stochastic Grammar Rens Bod Department of Computational Linguistics University of Amsterdam Spuistraat 134 NL-1012 VB Amsterdam [email protected] Abstract plausible parse(s) of an input string from the implausible ones. In stochastic language processing, it In Data Oriented Parsing (DOP), an annotated is assumed that the most plausible parse of an input corpus is used as a stochastic grammar. An string is its most probable parse. Most instantiations input string is parsed by combining subtrees of this idea estimate the probability of a parse by from the corpus. As a consequence, one parse assigning application probabilities to context free tree can usually be generated by several rewrite roles (Jelinek, 1990), or by assigning derivations that involve different subtrces. This combination probabilities to elementary structures leads to a statistics where the probability of a (Resnik, 1992; Schabes, 1992). parse is equal to the sum of the probabilities of all its derivations. In (Scha, 1990) an informal There is some agreement now that context free rewrite introduction to DOP is given, while (Bed, rules are not adequate for estimating the probability of 1992a) provides a formalization of the theory. a parse, since they cannot capture syntactie/lexical In this paper we compare DOP with other context, and hence cannot describe how the probability stochastic grammars in the context of Formal of syntactic structures or lexical items depends on that Language Theory. It it proved that it is not context. In stochastic tree-adjoining grammar possible to create for every DOP-model a (Schabes, 1992), this lack of context-sensitivity is strongly equivalent stochastic CFG which also overcome by assigning probabilities to larger assigns the same probabilities to the parses.
    [Show full text]
  • GRAMMAR IS GRAMMAR and USAGE IS USAGE Frederick J
    GRAMMAR IS GRAMMAR AND USAGE IS USAGE Frederick J. Newmeyer Department of Linguistics University of Washington Seattle, WA 98195-4340 [email protected] January 2003 1. Usage-based models of grammar The late 1960s was an exciting time to enter field of linguistics for someone like me, whose interests tended to syntax and semantics.1 My first year as a graduate student witnessed the birth of the approach called ‘generative semantics’. Generative semantics promised to totally revolutionize the field of transformational generative grammar, which itself was barely a decade old at the time. With each passing year, generative semanticists declared that some seemingly well-established boundary was nothing but an illusion. It all began in 1967 when George Lakoff and Haj Ross challenged the existence of the level of Deep Structure, and with it the boundary between syntax and semantics (Lakoff and Ross 1967/1976). The following year Jim McCawley argued that syntactic and lexical structures were formal objects of the same sort (McCawley 1968). Then in quick succession, the dividing line between semantics and pragmatics (Lakoff 1970/1972), grammaticality and ungrammaticality (Lakoff 1973), category membership and non-membership (Ross 1973a; b), and, finally, grammar and usage (Lakoff 1974) were all cast into doubt. At the same time, many sociolinguists were proposing models in which statistical facts about the speech community were incorporated into grammatical rules (Labov 1969; 1972). But by the late 1970s, generative semantics, for reasons I have discussed elsewhere (Newmeyer 1986), had all but disappeared. Most syntacticians had re- embraced the boundaries whose demise had been heralded only a few years earlier.
    [Show full text]
  • Application of Stochastic Grammars to Understanding Action
    Application of sto chastic grammars to understanding action by Yuri A Ivanov MS Computer Science State AcademyofAir and Space Instrumentation St Petersburg Russia February Submitted to the Program in Media Arts and Sciences Scho ol of Architecture and Planning in partial fulllmentofthe requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE IN MEDIA ARTS AND SCIENCES at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology February c Massachusetts Institute of Technology All Rights Reserved Signature of Author Program in Media Arts and Sciences January Certied by Aaron F Bobick LG Electronics Inc Career Development AssistantProfessor of Computational Vision Program in Media Arts and Sciences Thesis Sup ervisor Accepted by Stephen A Benton Chairp erson Departmental Committee on Graduate Students Program in Media Arts and Sciences Application of sto chastic grammars to understanding action by Yuri A Ivanov Submitted to the Program in Media Arts and Sciences Scho ol of Architecture and Planning on January in partial fulllmentofthe requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Media Arts and Sciences Abstract This thesis addresses the problem of using probabilistic formal languages to describ e and understand actions with explicit structure The thesis explores several mechanisms of pars ing the uncertain input string aided byastochastic contextfree grammar This metho d originating in sp eechrecognition lets us combine a statistical recognition approach with a syntactical one in a unied syntacticsemantic framework for action recognition The basic
    [Show full text]
  • W. G. M., a Stochastic Model of Language Change Through Social
    A STOCHASTIC MODEL OF LANGUAGE CHANGE THROUGH SOCIAL STRUCTURE AND PREDICTION-DRIVEN INSTABILITY W. GARRETT MITCHENER ABSTRACT. We develop a new stochastic model of language learning and change that incorporates variable speech and age structure. Children detect correlations between age and speech, predict how the language is changing, and speak according to that prediction. Although the popu- lation tends to be dominated by one idealized grammar or another, the learning process amplifies chance correlation between age and speech, generating prediction-driven instability and spontaneous language change. The model is formulated as a Markov chain, and a stochastic differential equation. It exhibits the abrupt monotonic shifts among equilibria char- acteristic of language changes. Fundamental results are proved. 1. THE PARADOX OF LANGUAGE CHANGE One of the primary tools in the field of linguistics is the idealized gram- mar, that is, a formalism that identifies correct utterances (1, 2). Much of the research on how children acquire their native language focuses on how they might choose an idealized grammar from many innate possibilities on the basis of example sentences from the surrounding society (3–5). From the perspective of idealized grammar, language change is paradoxical: Children acquire their native language accurately and communicate with adults from preceding generations, yet over time, the language can change drastically. Some changes may be attributed to an external event, such as political up- heaval, but not every instance of language change seems to have an external cause. Despite their variability, languages do maintain considerable short- term stability, consistently accepting and rejecting large classes of sentences Key words and phrases.
    [Show full text]
  • The Plasticity of Grammar
    The Plasticity of Grammar JOAN BRESNAN Stanford University & CSLI, The Center for the Study of Language and Information The plasticity of grammar A simplifying assumption of linguistic theory: knowledge of language is characterized by a static, categorical system of grammar. The plasticity of grammar A simplifying assumption of linguistic theory: knowledge of language is characterized by a static, categorical system of grammar. —A fruitful idealization, but it ultimately underestimates human language capacities. The plasticity of grammar A simplifying assumption of linguistic theory: knowledge of language is characterized by a static, categorical system of grammar. —A fruitful idealization, but it ultimately underestimates human language capacities. Language users can match the probabilities of linguistic features of the environment and they have powerful predictive capabilities that enable them to anticipate the variable linguistic choices of others. The plasticity of grammar A simplifying assumption of linguistic theory: knowledge of language is characterized by a static, categorical system of grammar. —A fruitful idealization, but it ultimately underestimates human language capacities. Language users can match the probabilities of linguistic features of the environment and they have powerful predictive capabilities that enable them to anticipate the variable linguistic choices of others. Therefore, a strongly contrasting hypothesis: grammar itself is inherently variable and stochastic in nature, rather than categorical and algebraic. Implicit knowledge of variability Low-level continuous phonetic variation is sensitive to high-level construction probabilities in speech production (Gahl & Garnsey 2004, Tily et al. 2009, Kuperman & Bresnan 2012) Construction probability is not merely a summary statistic for determinative individual factors affecting pronunciation, but has a direct effect in itself (Kuperman & Bresnan 2012) Illustrations from Victor Kuperman & Joan Bresnan (2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Calibrating Generative Models: the Probabilistic Chomsky-Schutzenberger¨ Hierarchy∗
    Calibrating Generative Models: The Probabilistic Chomsky-Schutzenberger¨ Hierarchy∗ Thomas F. Icard Stanford University [email protected] December 14, 2019 1 Introduction and Motivation Probabilistic computation enjoys a central place in psychological modeling and theory, ranging in application from perception (Knill and Pouget, 2004; Orban´ et al., 2016), at- tention (Vul et al., 2009), and decision making (Busemeyer and Diederich, 2002; Wilson et al., 2014) to the analysis of memory (Ratcliff, 1978; Abbott et al., 2015), categorization (Nosofsky and Palmeri, 1997; Sanborn et al., 2010), causal inference (Denison et al., 2013), and various aspects of language processing (Chater and Manning, 2006). Indeed, much of cognition and behavior is usefully modeled as stochastic, whether this is ultimately due to indeterminacy at the neural level or at some higher level (see, e.g., Glimcher 2005). Ran- dom behavior may even enhance an agent’s cognitive efficiency (see, e.g., Icard 2019). A probabilistic process can be described in two ways. On one hand we can specify a stochastic procedure, which may only implicitly define a distribution on possible out- puts of the process. Familiar examples include well known classes of generative mod- els such as finite Markov processes and probabilistic automata (Miller, 1952; Paz, 1971), Boltzmann machines (Hinton and Sejnowski, 1983), Bayesian networks (Pearl, 1988), topic models (Griffiths et al., 2007), and probabilistic programming languages (Tenenbaum et al., 2011; Goodman and Tenenbaum, 2016). On the other hand we can specify an analytical ex- pression explicitly describing the distribution over outputs. Both styles of description are commonly employed, and it is natural to ask about the relationship between them.
    [Show full text]
  • Stochastic Definite Clause Grammars
    Stochastic Definite Clause Grammars Christian Theil Have Research group PLIS: Programming, Logic and Intelligent Systems Department of Communication, Business and Information Technologies Roskilde University, P.O.Box 260, DK-4000 Roskilde, Denmark [email protected] Abstract where corpus annotations dictate the derivations, can This paper introduces Stochastic Definite Clause be done by counting expansions used in the annota- Grammars, a stochastic variant of the well- tions. Estimation with incomplete data can be ac- known Definite Clause Grammars. The grammar complished using the Expectation-Maximization (EM) formalism supports parameter learning from an- algorithm [8]. notated or unannotated corpora and provides a In stochastic unification grammars, the choice of mechanism for parse selection by means of sta- rules to expand is stochastic and the values assigned tistical inference. Unlike probabilistic context- free grammars, it is a context-sensitive gram- to unification variables are determined implicitly by mar formalism and it has the ability to model rule selection. This means that in some derivations, cross-serial dependencies in natural language. instances of the same logic variable may get different SDCG also provides some syntax extensions values and unification will fail as result. which makes it possible to write more compact Some of the first attempts to define stochastic uni- grammars and makes it straight-forward to add fication grammars did not address the issue of how lexicalization schemes to a grammar. they should be trained. Brew [2] and Eisele [9] tries to address this problem using EM, but their methods have problems handling cases where variables fails to 1 Introduction and background unify.
    [Show full text]
  • Stochastic Attribute-Value Grammars
    Stochastic Attribute-Value Grammars Steven P. Abney* AT&T Laboratories Probabilistic analogues of regular and context-free grammars are well known in computational linguistics, and currently the subject of intensive research. To date, however, no satisfactory probabilistic analogue of attribute-value grammars has been proposed: previous attempts have failed to define an adequate parameter-estimation algorithm. In the present paper, I define stochastic attribute-value grammars and give an algorithm for computing the maximum-likelihood estimate of their parameters. The estimation algorithm is adapted from Della Pietra, Della Pietra, and Lafferty (1995). To estimate model parameters, it is necessary to compute the expectations of certain functions under random fields. In the appli- cation discussed by Della Pietra, Della Pietra, and Lafferty (representing English orthographic constraints), Gibbs sampling can be used to estimate the needed expectations. The fact that attribute-value grammars generate constrained languages makes Gibbs sampling inapplicable, but I show that sampling can be done using the more general Metropolis-Hastings algorithm. 1. Introduction Stochastic versions of regular grammars and context-free grammars have received a great deal of attention in computational linguistics for the last several years, and ba- sic techniques of stochastic parsing and parameter estimation have been known for decades. However, regular and context-free grammars are widely deemed linguisti- cally inadequate; standard grammars in computational linguistics are attribute-value (AV) grammars of some variety. Before the advent of statistical methods, regular and context-free grammars were considered too inexpressive for serious consideration, and even now the reliance on stochastic versions of the less-expressive grammars is often seen as an expedient necessitated by the lack of an adequate stochastic version of attribute-value grammars.
    [Show full text]
  • Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice
    Unsupervised Language Acquisition: Theory and Practice Alexander Simon Clark Submitted for the degree of D.Phil. University of Sussex September, 2001 Declaration I hereby declare that this thesis has not been submitted, either in the same or different form, to this or any other university for a degree. Signature: Acknowledgements First, I would like to thank Bill Keller, for his supervision over the past three years. I would like to thank my colleagues at ISSCO for making me welcome and especially Susan Armstrong for giving me the job in the first place, and various people for helpful comments and discussions including Chris Manning, Dan Klein, Eric Gaussier, Nicola Cancedda, Franck Thollard, Andrei Popescu-Beilis, Menno van Zaanen and numerous other people including Sonia Halimi for check- ing my Arabic. I would also like to thank Gerald Gazdar and Geoffrey Sampson for their helpful comments as part of my thesis committee. I would also like to thank all of the people that have worked on the various software packages and operating systems that I have used, such as LaTeX, Gnu/Linux, and the gcc compiler, as well as the people who have worked on the preparation of the various corpora I have used: in particular I am grateful to John McCarthy and Ramin Nakisa for allowing me to use their painstakingly collected data sets for Arabic. Finally I would like to thank my wife, Dr Clare Hornsby, for her unfailing support and encouragement (in spite of her complete lack of understanding of the subject) and my children, Lily and Francis, for putting up with my absences both physical and mental, and showing me how language acquisition is really done – over-regularisation of past tense verbs included.
    [Show full text]
  • A Stochastic Grammar of Images
    A Stochastic Grammar of Images The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Zhu, Song Chun, and David Bryant Mumford. 2006. A stochastic grammar of images. Foundations and Trends in Computer Graphics and Vision 2(4): 259-362. Published Version doi:10.1561/0600000018 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:3637153 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Foundations and TrendsR in Computer Graphics and Vision Vol. 2, No. 4 (2006) 259–362 c 2007 S.-C. Zhu and D. Mumford DOI: 10.1561/0600000018 A Stochastic Grammar of Images Song-Chun Zhu1,∗ and David Mumford2 1 University of California, Los Angeles, USA, [email protected] 2 Brown University, USA, David [email protected] Abstract This exploratory paper quests for a stochastic and context sensitive grammar of images. The grammar should achieve the following four objectives and thus serves as a unified framework of representation, learning, and recognition for a large number of object categories. (i) The grammar represents both the hierarchical decompositions from scenes, to objects, parts, primitives and pixels by terminal and non-terminal nodes and the contexts for spatial and functional relations by horizon- tal links between the nodes. It formulates each object category as the set of all possible valid configurations produced by the grammar.
    [Show full text]