A comparison of object permanence progression during sitting development in with typical development and infants with motor delay Mihee An, PT, PhD1, Regina Harbourne, PT, PhD1, Jaclynn Stankus, M.S.Ed1, Lin-Ya Hsu, PT, PhD2, Emily C. Marcinowski, PhD3, Stacey C. Dusing, PT, PhD3, and START- Consortium 1Duquesne University, Pittsburgh, PA, 2University of Washington, Seattle, WA, 3Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond, VA

BACKGROUND METHODS

▪ Object Permanence is the ability to understand that Participants Object Permanence Scale (OPS) Score Behavior objects continue to exist even when they cannot be ▪ Infants were recruited as part of two larger ▪ Consists of 7 tasks extracted from 0 Child does not look at object or follow object observed. studies (START-Play and CHoR) at the onset developmental studies on object Child looks at object in one location, then shifts gaze to new 1 ▪ Object permanence is fundamental to representing of sitting emergence permanence.5-7 location to find object when object is moved objects and an important cognitive construct that Child re-orients body part other than head to gaze at moved object ▪ Sitting emergence: infants were able to sit ▪ Developed to measure object 2 develops during early life. propped on their arms for at least 3 seconds permanence from minimal to advanced when object shifted in space Child re-orients body posture to follow moved out of view (e.g., ▪ The construct of object permanence links to motor but unable to get in and out of sitting skills, in which the child has an 3 looking over edge of tray in high chair when toy dropped) experience such as sitting and self-mobility.1-2 ▪ 43 infants with motor delay (Mean age opportunity to earn a score (0-10). Looks inside of wide container and attempts to retrieve toy ▪ Adequate postural control in sitting allows infants 10.3months) ▪ During the test, infants sit on the floor 4 dropped inside to process visual information and use their hands ▪ Inclusion criteria: > 1SD below mean for or sit in a supported chair depending Pulls cloth off interesting toy after watching cloth being placed and freely to manipulate objects, which facilities 5 corrected age on motor domain of the on their ability to maintain a sitting toy partially visible .3-4 Bayley Scales of and position. 6 Pulls cloth off toy after watching toy being slid under cloth ▪ Infants with limited motor experience due to motor Development, 7-16 months of age Pulls cloth off interesting toy after watching cloth being placed and delays may be delayed in developing the object ▪ Exclusion criteria: Blindness, diagnosis of Procedure 7 toy completely covered, with identical cloth nearby permanence construct. progressive disorder ▪ The OPS was administered and Purpose ▪ 29 typically developing infants (Mean age 5.3 videotaped 4 times over 6 months, at 8 Finds a toy hidden under one of two cups Find a toy hidden under one of two cups when the cups are ▪ To compare the development of object permanence months) baseline, 1.5-month, 3-month and 6- 9 reversed after the toy is hidden between infants with and without motor delay, by month visits. ▪ Inclusion criteria: no history of delay, Double visual displacement used as a toy is hidden under one cup, ▪ Videos were scored independently by 10 examining change on a scale of object permanence or significant health removed and hidden a second time under the second cup over 6 months. conditions, < 7 months of age blinded assessors

RESULTS CONCLUSIONS

▪ Results of a mixed analysis of variance Infants with motor delay Infants with typical development ▪ Infants with motor delay demonstrated delayed development of object permanence, as measured by the (Figure 1) 10 object permanence scale (OPS). ▪ Main effect for time (F(3, 210) = 20.48, 9 ▪ Although infants with motor delay were approximately 5 months older than typically developing p < 0.001), infants, their performance on the OPS was similar to typically developing infants at the onset of sitting. ▪ No between group difference (F(1, 70) = 8 ▪ Progression of object permanence continued over 6 months for both groups, but the slope of 0.43, p = 0.52) 7 progression in infants with motor delay tended to be less. ▪ No interaction between group and time 6 ▪ Interventionists targeting early motor skills should track the progression of cognitive skills such as the (F(3, 210) = 2.53, p = 0.06). object permanence construct, which may be closely related to change in . ▪ The mean score on the OPS increased over 5 REFERENCES time in both groups, but did not differ 4 1. Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion. Developmental , 46(1), 129-138. between groups. 3 2. Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1(2), 149-219. ▪ But, mean age at baseline was 3. Harbourne, R. T., Ryalls, B., & Stergiou, N. (2014). Sitting and looking: A comparison of stability and visual exploration in infants with typical development and infants with 2 motor delay. Physical & Occupational Therapy in , 34(2), 197-212. significantly older in infants with motor 4. Surkar, S. M., Edelbrock, C., Stergiou, N., Berger, S., & Harbourne, R. (2015). Sitting postural control affects the development of focused attention in children with . Pediatric Physical Therapy, 27(1), 16-22. delay than typically developing infants (p 1 5. Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1978). Infancy: Its place in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. < 0.001), indicating delayed development baseline 1.5months 3months 6months 6. Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: University of Illinois Press 7. Lowe, J. R., Duncan, A. F., Bann, C. M., Fuller, J., Hintz, S. R., Das, A., ... & Watterberg, K. L. (2013). Early as a racially and ethnically neutral measure of of object permanence by age, but Figure 1. Change in object permanence scores over 6 months outcome in extremely preterm children at 18–22 months. Early human development, 89(12), 1055-1061. coinciding with sitting emergence. Bars represent standard errors of the mean ❖ This research was funded by the IES grant (NCT02593825) awarded to the START-Play Consortium, the CHoR grant (647408) awarded to SCD & ECM. ❖ For any questions about this research, please contact Mihee An: [email protected]