PROPENSITY SCORE-MATCHING METHODS for NONEXPERIMENTAL CAUSAL STUDIES Rajeev H

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

PROPENSITY SCORE-MATCHING METHODS for NONEXPERIMENTAL CAUSAL STUDIES Rajeev H PROPENSITY SCORE-MATCHING METHODS FOR NONEXPERIMENTAL CAUSAL STUDIES Rajeev H. Dehejia and Sadek Wahba* Abstract—This paper considers causal inference and sample selection bias treatment impact.1 The first generation of matching methods in nonexperimental settings in which (i) few units in the nonexperimental paired observations based on either a single variable or comparison group are comparable to the treatment units, and (ii) selecting a subset of comparison units similar to the treatment units is difficult weighting several variables. (See, inter alia, Bassi (1984), because units must be compared across a high-dimensional set of pre- Cave and Bos (1995), Czajka et al. (1992), Cochran and treatment characteristics. We discuss the use of propensity score-matching Rubin (1973), Raynor (1983), Rosenbaum (1995), Rubin methods, and implement them using data from the National Supported Work experiment. Following LaLonde (1986), we pair the experimental (1973, 1979), Westat (1981), and studies cited by Barnow treated units with nonexperimental comparison units from the CPS and (1987).) PSID, and compare the estimates of the treatment effect obtained using The motivation for focusing on propensity score- our methods to the benchmark results from the experiment. For both matching methods is that, in many applications of interest, comparison groups, we show that the methods succeed in focusing attention on the small subset of the comparison units comparable to the the dimensionality of the observable characteristics is high. treated units and, hence, in alleviating the bias due to systematic differ- With a small number of characteristics (for example, two ences between the treated and comparison units. binary variables), matching is straightforward (one would group units in four cells). However, when there are many variables, it is difficult to determine along which dimen- I. Introduction sions to match units or which weighting scheme to adopt. n important problem of causal inference is how to Propensity score-matching methods, as we demonstrate, are Aestimate treatment effects in observational studies, especially useful under such circumstances because they situations (like an experiment) in which a group of units is provide a natural weighting scheme that yields unbiased exposed to a well-defined treatment, but (unlike an experi- estimates of the treatment impact. ment) no systematic methods of experimental design are The key contribution of this paper is to discuss and apply used to maintain a control group. It is well recognized that propensity score-matching methods, which are new to the the estimate of a causal effect obtained by comparing a economics literature. (Previous papers include Dehejia and treatment group with a nonexperimental comparison group Wahba (1999), Heckman et al. (1996, 1998), Heckman, could be biased because of problems such as self-selection Ichimura, and Todd (1997, 1998). See Friedlander, Green- or some systematic judgment by the researcher in selecting berg, and Robins (1997) for a review.) This paper differs units to be assigned to the treatment. This paper discusses from Dehejia and Wahba (1999) by focusing on matching methods in detail, and it complements the Heckman et al. the use of propensity score-matching methods to correct for papers by discussing a different array of matching estima- sample selection bias due to observable differences between tors in the context of a different data set. the treatment and comparison groups. An important feature of our method is that, after units are Matching involves pairing treatment and comparison matched, the unmatched comparison units are discarded and units that are similar in terms of their observable character- are not directly used in estimating the treatment impact. Our istics. When the relevant differences between any two units approach has two motivations. First, in some settings of are captured in the observable (pretreatment) covariates, interest, data on the outcome variable for the comparison which occurs when outcomes are independent of assign- group are costly to obtain. For example, in economics, some ment to treatment conditional on pretreatment covariates, data sets provide outcome information for only one year; if matching methods can yield an unbiased estimate of the the outcome of interest takes place in a later period, possibly thousands of comparison units have to be linked across data Received for publication February 12, 1998. Revision accepted for sets or resurveyed. In such settings, the ability to obtain the publication January 24, 2001. needed data for a subset of relevant comparison units, * Columbia University and Morgan Stanley, respectively. discarding the irrelevant potential comparison units, is ex- Previous versions of this paper were circulated under the title “An Oversampling Algorithm for Nonexperimental Causal Studies with In- tremely valuable. Second, even if information on the out- complete Matching and Missing Outcome Variables” (1995) and as come is available for all comparison units (as it is in our National Bureau of Economic Research working paper no. 6829. We thank data), the process of searching for the best subset from the Robert Moffitt and two referees for detailed comments and suggestions that have improved the paper. We are grateful to Gary Chamberlain, Guido comparison group reveals the extent of overlap between the Imbens, and Donald Rubin for their support and encouragement, and treatment and comparison groups in terms of pretreatment greatly appreciate comments from Joshua Angrist, George Cave, and Jeff characteristics. Because methods that use the full set of Smith. Special thanks are due to Robert LaLonde for providing, and helping to reconstruct, the data from his 1986 study. Valuable comments were received from seminar participants at Harvard, MIT, and the Man- 1 More precisely, to estimate the treatment impact on the treated, the power Demonstration Research Corporation. Any remaining errors are the outcome in the untreated state must be independent of the treatment authors’ responsibility. assignment. The Review of Economics and Statistics, February 2002, 84(1): 151–161 © 2002 by the President and Fellows of Harvard College and the Massachusetts Institute of Technology Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/003465302317331982 by guest on 02 October 2021 152 THE REVIEW OF ECONOMICS AND STATISTICS comparison units extrapolate or smooth across the treatment where Ti ϭ 1(ϭ 0) if the ith unit was assigned to treatment and comparison groups, it is extremely useful to know how (control).3 The problem of unobservability is summarized many of the comparison units are in fact comparable and by the fact that we can estimate E(Yi1͉Ti ϭ 1), but not hence how much smoothing one’s estimator is expected to E(Yi0͉Ti ϭ 1). e perform. The difference, ␶ ϭ E(Yi1͉Ti ϭ 1) Ϫ E(Yi0͉Ti ϭ 0), The data we use, obtained from LaLonde (1986), are can be estimated, but it is potentially a biased estimator of from the National Supported Work (NSW) Demonstration, a ␶. Intuitively, if Yi0 for the treated and comparison units labor market experiment in which participants were ran- systematically differs, then in observing only Yi0 for the domized between treatment (on-the-job training lasting be- comparison group we do not correctly estimate Yi0 for the tween nine months and a year) and control groups. Follow- treated group. Such bias is of paramount concern in nonex- ing LaLonde, we use the experimental controls to obtain a perimental studies. The role of randomization is to prevent benchmark estimate for the treatment impact and then set this: them aside, wedding the treated units from the experiment to comparison units from the Population Survey of Income Yi1, Yi 0 ʈTi f E͑Yi0͉Ti ϭ 0͒ ϭ E͑Yi0͉Ti ϭ 1͒ ϭ E͑Yi͉Ti ϭ 0͒, Dynamics (PSID) and the Current Population Survey 2 (CPS). We compare estimates obtained using our nonex- where Yi ϭ TiYi1 ϩ (1 Ϫ Ti)Yi0 (the observed value of the perimental methods to the experimental benchmark. We outcome) and ʈ is the symbol for independence. The treated show that most of the nonexperimental comparison units are and control groups do not systematically differ from each not good matches for the treated group. We succeed in other, making the conditioning on Ti in the expectation selecting the comparison units that are most comparable to unnecessary (ignorable treatment assignment, in the termi- e 4 the treated units and in replicating the benchmark treatment nology of Rubin (1977)), and yielding ␶͉Tϭ1 ϭ␶. impact. The paper is organized as follows. In section II, we B. Exact Matching on Covariates discuss the theory behind our estimation strategy. In section III, we discuss propensity score-matching methods. In sec- To substitute for the absence of experimental control tion IV, we describe the NSW data, which we then use in units, we assume that data can be obtained for a set of section V to implement our matching procedures. Section potential comparison units, which are not necessarily drawn VI tests the matching assumption and examines the sensi- from the same population as the treated units but for whom tivity of our estimates to the specification of the propensity we observe the same set of pretreatment covariates, Xi. The score. Section VII concludes the paper. following proposition extends the framework of the previ- ous section to nonexperimental settings: II. Matching Methods Proposition 1 (Rubin, 1977). If for each unit we observe ʈ ͉ @ A. The Role of Randomization a vector of covariates Xi and Yi0 Ti Xi, i, then the popu- lation treatment effect for the treated, ␶͉Tϭ1, is identified: it A cause is viewed as a manipulation or treatment that is equal to the treatment effect conditional on covariates and brings about a change in the variable of interest, compared on assignment to treatment, ␶͉Tϭ1,X, averaged over the 5 to some baseline, called the control (Cox, 1992; Holland, distribution X͉Ti ϭ 1 . 1986). The basic problem in identifying a causal effect is that the variable of interest is observed under either the 3 In a nonexperimental setting, the treatment and comparison samples treatment or control regimes, but never both.
Recommended publications
  • Statistical Matching: a Paradigm for Assessing the Uncertainty in the Procedure
    Journal of Of®cial Statistics, Vol. 17, No. 3, 2001, pp. 407±422 Statistical Matching: A Paradigm for Assessing the Uncertainty in the Procedure Chris Moriarity1 and Fritz Scheuren2 Statistical matching has been widely used by practitioners without always adequate theoreti- cal underpinnings. The work of Kadane (1978) has been a notable exception and the present article extends his insights. Kadane's 1978 article is reprinted in this JOS issue. Modern com- puting can make possible, under techniques described here, a real advance in the application of statistical matching. Key words: Multivariate normal; complex survey designs; robustness; resampling; variance- covariance structures; and application suggestions. 1. Introduction Many government policy questions, whether on the expenditure or tax side, lend them- selves to microsimulation modeling, where ``what if'' analyses of alternative policy options are carried out (e.g., Citro and Hanushek 1991). Often, the starting point for such models, in an attempt to achieve a degree of verisimilitude, is to employ information contained in several survey microdata ®les. Typically, not all the variables wanted for the modeling have been collected together from a single individual or family. However, the separate survey ®les may have many demographic and other control variables in common. The idea arose, then, of matching the separate ®les on these common variables and thus creating a composite ®le for analysis. ``Statistical matching,'' as the technique began to be called, has been more or less widely practiced since the advent of public use ®les in the 1960's. Arguably, the desire to employ statistical matching was even an impetus for the release of several of the early public use ®les, including those involving U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • A Machine Learning Approach to Census Record Linking∗
    A Machine Learning Approach to Census Record Linking∗ James J. Feigenbaumy March 28, 2016 Abstract Thanks to the availability of new historical census sources and advances in record linking technology, economic historians are becoming big data geneal- ogists. Linking individuals over time and between databases has opened up new avenues for research into intergenerational mobility, the long run effects of early life conditions, assimilation, discrimination, and the returns to edu- cation. To take advantage of these new research opportunities, scholars need to be able to accurately and efficiently match historical records and produce an unbiased dataset of links for analysis. I detail a standard and transparent census matching technique for constructing linked samples that can be repli- cated across a variety of cases. The procedure applies insights from machine learning classification and text comparison to record linkage of historical data. My method teaches an algorithm to replicate how a well trained and consistent researcher would create a linked sample across sources. I begin by extracting a subset of possible matches for each record, and then use training data to tune a matching algorithm that attempts to minimize both false positives and false negatives, taking into account the inherent noise in historical records. To make the procedure precise, I trace its application to an example from my own work, linking children from the 1915 Iowa State Census to their adult-selves in the 1940 Federal Census. In addition, I provide guidance on a number of practical questions, including how large the training data needs to be relative to the sample. ∗I thank Christoph Hafemeister, Jamie Lee, Christopher Muller, Martin Rotemberg, and Nicolas Ziebarth for detailed feedback and thoughts on this project, as well as seminar participants at the NBER DAE Summer Institute and the Berkeley Demography Conference on Census Linking.
    [Show full text]
  • Stability and Median Rationalizability for Aggregate Matchings
    games Article Stability and Median Rationalizability for Aggregate Matchings Federico Echenique 1, SangMok Lee 2, Matthew Shum 1 and M. Bumin Yenmez 3,* 1 Division of the Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA; [email protected] (F.E.); [email protected] (M.S.) 2 Department of Economics, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA; [email protected] 3 Department of Economics, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467, USA * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: We develop the theory of stability for aggregate matchings used in empirical studies and establish fundamental properties of stable matchings including the result that the set of stable matchings is a non-empty, complete, and distributive lattice. Aggregate matchings are relevant as matching data in revealed preference theory. We present a result on rationalizing a matching data as the median stable matching. Keywords: aggregate matching; median stable matching; rationalizability; lattice 1. Introduction Following the seminal work of [1], an extensive literature has developed regarding matching markets with non-transferable utility. This literature assumes that there are agent-specific preferences, and studies the existence of stable matchings in which each Citation: Echenique, F.; Lee, S.; agent prefers her assigned partner to the outside option of being unmatched, and there Shum, M.; Yenmez, M.B. Stability and are no pairs of agents that would like to match with each other rather than keeping their Median Rationalizability for assigned partners. Aggregate Matchings. Games 2021, 12, In this paper, we develop the theory of stability for aggregate matchings, which we 33.
    [Show full text]
  • Report on Exact and Statistical Matching Techniques
    Statistical Policy Working Papers are a series of technical documents prepared under the auspices of the Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards. These documents are the product of working groups or task forces, as noted in the Preface to each report. These Statistical Policy Working Papers are published for the purpose of encouraging further discussion of the technical issues and to stimulate policy actions which flow from the technical findings and recommendations. Readers of Statistical Policy Working Papers are encouraged to communicate directly with the Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards with additional views, suggestions, or technical concerns. Office of Joseph W. Duncan Federal Statistical Director Policy Standards For sale by the Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office Washington, D.C. 20402 Statistical Policy Working Paper 5 Report on Exact and Statistical Matching Techniques Prepared by Subcommittee on Matching Techniques Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology DEPARTMENT OF COMMERCE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA U.S. DEPARTMENT OF COMMERCE Philip M. Klutznick Courtenay M. Slater, Chief Economist Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards Joseph W. Duncan, Director Issued: June 1980 Office of Federal Statistical Policy and Standards Joseph W. Duncan, Director Katherine K. Wallman, Deputy Director, Social Statistics Gaylord E. Worden, Deputy Director, Economic Statistics Maria E. Gonzalez, Chairperson, Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology Preface This working paper was prepared by the Subcommittee on Matching Techniques, Federal Committee on Statistical Methodology. The Subcommittee was chaired by Daniel B. Radner, Office of Research and Statistics, Social Security Administration, Department of Health and Human Services. Members of the Subcommittee include Rich Allen, Economics, Statistics, and Cooperatives Service (USDA); Thomas B.
    [Show full text]
  • Alternatives to Randomized Control Trials: a Review of Three Quasi-Experimental Designs for Causal Inference
    Actualidades en Psicología, 29(119), 2015, 19- 27 ISSN 2215-3535 http://revistas.ucr.ac.cr/index.php/actualidades DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.15517/ap.v29i119.18810 Alternatives to Randomized Control Trials: A Review of Three Quasi-experimental Designs for Causal Inference Alternativas a las Pruebas Controladas Aleatorizadas: una revisión de tres diseños cuasi experimentales para la inferencia causal Pavel Pavolovich Panko1 Jacob D. Curtis2 Brittany K. Gorrall3 Todd Daniel Little4 Texas Tech University, United States Abstract. The Randomized Control Trial (RCT) design is typically seen as the gold standard in psychological research. As it is not always possible to conform to RCT specifications, many studies are conducted in the quasi-experimental framework. Although quasi-experimental designs are considered less preferable to RCTs, with guidance they can produce inferences which are just as valid. In this paper, the authors present 3 quasi-experimental designs which are viable alternatives to RCT designs. These designs are Regression Point Displacement (RPD), Regression Discontinuity (RD), and Propensity Score Matching (PSM). Additionally, the authors outline several notable methodological improvements to use with these designs. Keywords. Psychometrics, Quasi-Experimental Design, Regression Point Displacement, Regression Discontinuity, Propensity Score Matching. Resumen. Los diseños de Pruebas Controladas Aleatorizadas (PCA) son típicamente vistas como el mejor diseño en la investigación en psicología. Como tal, no es siempre posible cumplir con las especificaciones de las PCA y por ello muchos estudios son realizados en un marco cuasi experimental. Aunque los diseños cuasi experimentales son considerados menos convenientes que los diseños PCA, con directrices estos pueden producir inferencias igualmente válidas.
    [Show full text]
  • Matching Via Dimensionality Reduction for Estimation of Treatment Effects in Digital Marketing Campaigns
    Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence (IJCAI-16) Matching via Dimensionality Reduction for Estimation of Treatment Effects in Digital Marketing Campaigns Sheng Li Nikos Vlassis Jaya Kawale Yun Fu Northeastern University Adobe Research Adobe Research Northeastern University Boston, MA, USA San Jose, CA, USA San Jose, CA, USA Boston, MA, USA [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract units in the control group are interpreted as counterfactuals, and the average treatment effect on treated (ATT) is estimated A widely used method for estimating counterfactu- by comparing the outcomes of every matched pair. als and causal treatment effects from observational data is nearest-neighbor matching. This typically One of the widely used matching methods is Nearest involves pairing each treated unit with its nearest- Neighbor Matching (NNM) [Rubin, 1973a]. For each treated in-covariates control unit, and then estimating an unit, NNM finds its nearest neighbor in the control group average treatment effect from the set of matched to generate a matched pair, and the ATT is then estimated pairs. Although straightforward to implement, this from the set of matched pairs. Different NNM methods are estimator is known to suffer from a bias that in- characterized by the choice of a distance measure for de- creases with the dimensionality of the covariate termining such a match. Some of the popularly used dis- space, which can be undesirable in applications tance measures are Exact Matching and its variant Coars- that involve high-dimensional data. To address this ened Exact Matching (CEM) [Iacus et al., 2011], Maha- problem, we propose a novel estimator that first lanobis distance matching [Rubin, 1979], and Propensity projects the data to a number of random linear sub- Score Matching (PSM) [Rosenbaum and Rubin, 1983].
    [Show full text]
  • Frequency Matching Case-Control Techniques: an Epidemiological Perspective
    Frequency Matching case-control techniques: an epidemiological perspective. Authors: Hai Nguyen, MS (1st and Corresponding Author) Research Assistant Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Illinois at Chicago Phone: 1-312-355-4471 Email: [email protected] Trang Pham, MS Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Illinois at Chicago [email protected] Garth Rauscher, PhD Division of Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of Illinois at Chicago [email protected] Abstract In many cohort and case-control studies, subjects are matched to intend to control confounding and to improve study efficiency by improving precision. An often-used approach is to check to see that the frequency distributions in each study group are alike. Being alike in the frequency distributions of key variables would provide evidence that the groups are comparable. However, there are instances where the overall distributions could be alike, but the individual cases could vary substantially. While there are no methods that can guarantee comparability, individual case matching has often been used to provide assurances that the groups are comparable. We propose an algorithm build a macro in SAS to match control for the case given a set of matching criteria, including match exact on site, on the year, exact or 5-year interval match on age. Cases will be matched with a large number of controls. A large dataset from 2000-2017 from Metropolitan Chicago Breast Cancer Registry with more than 485000 women obtaining breast screening or diagnostic imaging (more than 15000 cases:30000 controls) was applied.
    [Show full text]
  • Package 'Matching'
    Package ‘Matching’ April 14, 2021 Version 4.9-9 Date 2021-03-15 Title Multivariate and Propensity Score Matching with Balance Optimization Author Jasjeet Singh Sekhon <[email protected]> Maintainer Jasjeet Singh Sekhon <[email protected]> Description Provides functions for multivariate and propensity score matching and for finding optimal balance based on a genetic search algorithm. A variety of univariate and multivariate metrics to determine if balance has been obtained are also provided. For details, see the paper by Jasjeet Sekhon (2007, <doi:10.18637/jss.v042.i07>). Depends R (>= 2.6.0), MASS (>= 7.2-1), graphics, grDevices, stats Suggests parallel, rgenoud (>= 2.12), rbounds License GPL-3 URL http://sekhon.berkeley.edu/matching/ NeedsCompilation yes RoxygenNote 7.1.1 Repository CRAN Date/Publication 2021-04-13 22:00:15 UTC R topics documented: balanceUV . .2 GenMatch . .4 GerberGreenImai . 11 ks.boot . 13 lalonde . 15 Match . 16 MatchBalance . 22 Matchby . 26 1 2 balanceUV qqstats . 30 summary.balanceUV . 32 summary.ks.boot . 33 summary.Match . 34 summary.Matchby . 34 Index 36 balanceUV Univariate Balance Tests Description This function provides a number of univariate balance metrics. Generally, users should call MatchBalance and not this function directly. Usage balanceUV(Tr, Co, weights = rep(1, length(Co)), exact = FALSE, ks=FALSE, nboots = 1000, paired=TRUE, match=FALSE, weights.Tr=rep(1,length(Tr)), weights.Co=rep(1,length(Co)), estimand="ATT") Arguments Tr A vector containing the treatment observations. Co A vector containing the control observations. weights A vector containing the observation specific weights. Only use this option when the treatment and control observations are paired (as they are after matching).
    [Show full text]
  • A Comparison of Different Methods to Handle Missing Data in the Context of Propensity Score Analysis
    European Journal of Epidemiology https://doi.org/10.1007/s10654-018-0447-z (0123456789().,-volV)(0123456789().,- volV) METHODS A comparison of different methods to handle missing data in the context of propensity score analysis 1 1,2 1,3 Jungyeon Choi • Olaf M. Dekkers • Saskia le Cessie Received: 21 May 2018 / Accepted: 25 September 2018 Ó The Author(s) 2018 Abstract Propensity score analysis is a popular method to control for confounding in observational studies. A challenge in propensity methods is missing values in confounders. Several strategies for handling missing values exist, but guidance in choosing the best method is needed. In this simulation study, we compared four strategies of handling missing covariate values in propensity matching and propensity weighting. These methods include: complete case analysis, missing indicator method, multiple imputation and combining multiple imputation and missing indicator method. Concurrently, we aimed to provide guidance in choosing the optimal strategy. Simulated scenarios varied regarding missing mechanism, presence of effect modification or unmeasured confounding. Additionally, we demonstrated how missingness graphs help clarifying the missing structure. When no effect modification existed, complete case analysis yielded valid causal treatment effects even when data were missing not at random. In some situations, complete case analysis was also able to partially correct for unmeasured confounding. Multiple imputation worked well if the data were missing (completely) at random, and if the imputation model was correctly specified. In the presence of effect modification, more complex imputation models than default options of commonly used statistical software were required. Multiple imputation may fail when data are missing not at random.
    [Show full text]
  • Performance of Logistic Regression, Propensity Scores, and Instrumental Variable for Estimating Their True Target Odds Ratios In
    Performance of Logistic Regression, Propensity Scores, and Instrumental Variable for Estimating Their True Target Odds Ratios in the Absence and Presence of Unmeasured Confounder Tianqi Yu West China Hospital of Sichuan University Chengyu Ke Southern Methodist University Wentao Xu West China Hospital of Sichuan University Jing Li ( [email protected] ) West China Hospital of Sichuan University Research Article Keywords: bias, unmeasured confounder, logistic model, propensity score, instrumental variable Posted Date: December 28th, 2020 DOI: https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-133868/v1 License: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. Read Full License Yu et al. RESEARCH Performance of logistic regression, propensity scores, and instrumental variable for estimating their true target odds ratios in the absence and presence of unmeasured confounder Tianqi. Yu1, Chengyu. Ke2 , Wentao. Xu1 and Jing. Li1* *Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract 1 Department of Evidence-Based Medicine and Clinical Background: A lot of studies have compared the ability of statistical methods to Epidemiology, Sichuan University control for confounding. However, a majority of studies mistakenly assumed these West China Hospital, Chengdu, China methods estimate the same effect. The aim of this study was to use Monte Carlo Full list of author information is simulations to compare logistic regression, propensity scores and instrumental available at the end of the article variable analysis for estimating their true target odds ratios in terms of bias and precision in the absence and presence of unmeasured confounder. Methods: We established the formula allowing us to compute the true odds ratio of each method. We varied the instrument’s strength and the unmeasured confounder to cover a large range of scenarios in the simulation study.
    [Show full text]
  • An Empirical Evaluation of Statistical Matching Methodologies
    Southern Methodist University SMU Scholar Historical Working Papers Cox School of Business 1982 An Empirical Evaluation of Statistical Matching Methodologies Richard A. Barry Southern Methodist University William H. Stewart Oklahoma State University - Main Campus J. Scott Turner Oklahoma State University - Main Campus Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.smu.edu/business_workingpapers Part of the Business Commons This document is brought to you for free and open access by the Cox School of Business at SMU Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Historical Working Papers by an authorized administrator of SMU Scholar. For more information, please visit http://digitalrepository.smu.edu. Research and Development Scbool of "P us i ness Administraticm Southern Methodist Univeraitv Dallas. Texas 75275 · AN EMPIRICAL EVALUATION OF STATISTICAL MATCHING METHODOLOGIES Working Paper 82-804* by Richard A. Barr William H. Stewart J. Scott Turner Richard A. Barr Edwin L. Cox School of Business Southern Methodist University Dallas, Texas 75275 William H. Stewart Oklahoma State University Stillwater, Oklahoma J. Scott Turner Oklahoma State University Stillwater, Oklahoma *This paper represents a draft of work in progress by the authors and is bei ng sent to you for inf ormation and review. Responsibility for the contents rests solely wi th the authors . This working paper may not be reproduced or distributed wi thout the written consent of the authors. Please address all correspondence to Richard A. Barr. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We wish to acknowledge the strong support and cooperation of the follow­ ing organizations: the Office of Evaluation and Technical Analysis, De­ partment of Health and Human Services; the Office of Research and Sta­ tistics, Social Security Administration; the Internal Revenue Service and Office of Tax Analysis, Department of the Treasury; and our home universities.
    [Show full text]
  • The Essential Role of Pair Matching in Cluster-Randomized Experiments, with Application to the Mexican Universal Health Insuranc
    Statistical Science 2009, Vol. 24, No. 1, 29–53 DOI: 10.1214/08-STS274 © Institute of Mathematical Statistics, 2009 The Essential Role of Pair Matching in Cluster-Randomized Experiments, with Application to the Mexican Universal Health Insurance Evaluation Kosuke Imai, Gary King and Clayton Nall Abstract. A basic feature of many field experiments is that investigators are only able to randomize clusters of individuals—such as households, com- munities, firms, medical practices, schools or classrooms—even when the individual is the unit of interest. To recoup the resulting efficiency loss, some studies pair similar clusters and randomize treatment within pairs. However, many other studies avoid pairing, in part because of claims in the litera- ture, echoed by clinical trials standards organizations, that this matched-pair, cluster-randomization design has serious problems. We argue that all such claims are unfounded. We also prove that the estimator recommended for this design in the literature is unbiased only in situations when matching is unnecessary; its standard error is also invalid. To overcome this problem without modeling assumptions, we develop a simple design-based estimator with much improved statistical properties. We also propose a model-based approach that includes some of the benefits of our design-based estimator as well as the estimator in the literature. Our methods also address individual- level noncompliance, which is common in applications but not allowed for in most existing methods. We show that from the perspective of bias, efficiency, power, robustness or research costs, and in large or small samples, pairing should be used in cluster-randomized experiments whenever feasible; failing to do so is equivalent to discarding a considerable fraction of one’s data.
    [Show full text]