Finding Translation Equivalents: an Application of Grammatical Metaphor

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Finding Translation Equivalents: an Application of Grammatical Metaphor FINDING TRANSLATION EQUIVALENTS: AN APPLICATION OF GRAMMATICAL METAPHOR John A. Bateman* USC/Information Sciences Institute 4676 Admiralty Way Marina del Rey, CA 90292 U.S.A. (e-mail: bateman@isi, edu) April 4, 1990 Abstract linguistic structures generated by monolingual gen- erators for distinct languages for a given abstract se- In this paper I describe how a significant class of cases mantic specification to be structurally similar: there- that would involve (possibly complex) structural trans- fore, translations that involve very diverse structures fer in nmchine translation can be handled avoiding trans- are readily obtainable if they are semantically moti- fer. This is achieved by applying a semantic organization vated. Monolingual generation components of the developed for monolingual text generation that is suffi- type described are also independently motivated by ciently abstract to remain invariant, within theoretically language processing tasks that do not involve MT specifiable limits, across different languages. The further and so are ill any case required. Providing for their application of a mechanism motivated from within mono- usage within the MT context also is therefore doubly lingual text generation, 'grammatical metaphor', then beneficial. allows candidate appropriate translations to be isolated. The particular mechanism developed within text The incorporation of these essentially monolingual mech- anisms within the machine translation process promises generation that I will apply to MT problems here to significantly improve translational capabilities; exam- is that of grammatical raelaphor (Ilalliday, 1985); grammatical metaphor was originally developed ples of this are presented for English and German. within the tradition of Systemic-Functional Linguis- tics and is now beginning to be applied within 1 Introduction the PENMAN text generation system (Mann and Matthiessen, 1985; The Penman Project, 1989). Historically there has rather little interaction be- Systemic-functional linguistics posits sets of map- tween work in text generation and machine trans- pings between semantic information to be expressed lation (MT) -- even though language generation and grammatical features. One class of mappings is needs to be an integral component of any cornplete termed 'congruent', in that it offers an unmarked re- MT system. Current text generation systems are, alization of a semantic concept type -- e.g., that however, achieving results which can be beneficially a processual semantic entity is realized as a ver- applied in the MT context. In this paper, I de- bal constituent in the grammar -- while a fur- scribe one such area of possible interaction between ther class of mappings is 'noneongrnent', in that it. mechanisms developed for monolingual text gener- enables marked correspondences between semantics ation and the requirements of MT. In particular, and grammar --e.g., in nominalizations where a the increasing concern that text generation theories processual ,;emantic entity is realized as a nominal show for higher levels of semantics and its realization constituent in the grammar. Below I show how the in linguistic form makes it possible to move away set of linguistic structures related by grammatical from lower-level, 'structural' transfer between lan- metaphor form a useful equivalence class for MT: i.e., guages. In Iket, some of the semantic specifications when seeking an appropriate translation tbr some now being uncovered within text generation arc suf- sentence, one will often be found in the set of tar- ficiently abstract as to capture significant informa- get language sentences formed by generating from tional invariances across languages. Sophisticated the corresponding abstract semantic specification in- monolingual generation components are able to gen- voldng grammatical metaphor for variations in the erate appropriate linguistic structures from such ab- structures generated. stract informational specifications. This avoids, in To show how this works in more detail, I first de- many cases, problems of (possibly complex) struc- scribe the level of abstract semantic information that tural transfer. There is no reason a priori for the is currently used within the PENMAN system -- this we term the upper model- and the user interface to *Much of the work reported here was carried out with the financial support of the Institut fi',. Angcwandte hfformations- the text generation system that it. supports -- which forschung at the University of the Saarland, S~mrbri'mken', ad- is called SPL. Second, 1 provide more concr¢~te exam- ditional support was provided by AFOS[{ contract, F49620-87- pies of two distinct kinds of grammatical metaphor C-0005, and in part by DAI{PA contract MDAg03-87-C-641. that we can now implement compntationally. And The opilfions in this report arc solely those of the author. Thanks are due to Erich Steiner, Jgrg Schi'ltz, and C (~cilc Pro'is finally, I go on to show their applica~:.ion to MT using for significant contributions. examples from English and German. i 13 / // 2 The Upper Model and SPL inalienable-possessions of that actor. 1 The position- ing of the general types and relationships within the The PENMAN tipper model organizes the 'proposi- upper model provides much of the information that tional type' meanings that need to be expressed in a grammar needs for constraining possible surface text; it provides a general semantic taxonomy of realizations. 2 classes of experiences and objects. This classifica- tion can also be seen as an inheritance hierarchy that organizes concepts according to how they may be ex- 3 Grammatical Metaphors pressed. For example, the inheritance of certain roles defines the types of participants that processes may A powerful property of the relationship between the have and the types of qualities that may be ascribed upper model and the grammar is the existence of to particular objects, while class-subclass relations grammatical metaphor. Grammatical metaphor oc- capture generalizations about possible grammatical curs when meanings are realized through 'nontypical' and lexical realizations of concepts. Significantly, selections of grammatical features; such realizations this orientation towards supporting grammatical re- can be described systematically and bring their own alization renders the upper model independent of distinctive contributions to the meaning expressed particular domains -- the semantic taxonomy offers (for a general typology, see Ravelli, 1985). Two types an organization that is required for any domain if it is of grammatical metaphor which we are beginning to to support natural language generation. Rather fine be able to control computationally are rankshifling distinctions are drawn by the current upper model, and complexity metaphors. Figure 1 shows the first which contains approximately 200 concepts; details grammatical decisions that must be made when be- are provided in Bateman, Kasper, Moore, and Whit- ginning generation of a grammatical unit according ney (1990). to a systemic-functional grammar such as that used within PENMAN. For each meaning to be expressed, The upper model supports specifications of sen- tence 'meaning' that may serve as input to a text grammatical decisions must first be made as to the generation system (Kasper, 1989a), or as output rank of the grammatical unit that will be used -- from an analysis component (Kasper, 1989b). Such i.e., clauses, groups (nominal, verbal, adverbial and specifications, which are expressed in the PENMAN prepositional), words, and morphemes -- and the Sentence: Plan Language (SPL), abstract beyond complexity of that grammatical unit -- i.e., whether many syntactic variations; they capture basic mean- the unit is a single unit or some combination of simi- ings -- defined in terms of the upper model -- that lar units (e.g., conjunction). Traditionally, the map- may be given a variety of linguistic realizations de- ping between meanings to be expressed and these pending on other, specifiable, criteria. One conse- grammatical choices has been rather inflexible and quence of the abstractness of this representation is limited to congruenl realizations; i.e., by and large, a that many sentences taken from distinct languages process-type meaning (activity, state, etc.) would be that would require complex structural transfer for realized by a selection of a simple clause, an object- translation simply share a common SPL representa- type meaning by a selection of nominal, etc. Gram- tion, thus requiring no transfer at all. However, it is tactical metaphor makes explicit the fact that this important to note that this is motivated by a com- relationship between meaning and grammatical form monality in linguistic function when a sufficiently ab- is considerably more flexible and many noncongruent stract standpoint is taken rather than on any claims realizations are possible. of universality; further details on the theoretical sta- Consider, for example, the SPL specification for tus of the shared representations are presented in example sentence (2) The discharge of electricity re- Bateman (1989). We will see examples of the avoid- suited in a breakdown of the system. ance of structural transfer in Section 5. ((cO / cause-effect :domain discharge A simple example of an SPL specification using :range breakdown) the upper model is shown below. This shows the (discharge / directed-action SPL specification for the sentence: (1) Mary cut her :actee (electricity / substance)) (breakdown
Recommended publications
  • Systemic Functional Linguistic Discourse Features in the Personal Essay Anna Jones Rollins [email protected]
    Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 1-1-2012 Systemic Functional Linguistic Discourse Features in the Personal Essay Anna Jones Rollins [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Creative Writing Commons, Higher Education and Teaching Commons, and the Liberal Studies Commons Recommended Citation Rollins, Anna Jones, "Systemic Functional Linguistic Discourse Features in the Personal Essay" (2012). Theses, Dissertations and Capstones. Paper 216. This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Marshall Digital Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Marshall Digital Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL LINGUISTIC DISCOURSE FEATURES IN THE PERSONAL ESSAY A Thesis submitted to the Graduate College of Marshall University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in English by Anna Jones Rollins Approved by Hyo-Chang Hong, Ph.D., Committee Chairperson Rachael Peckham, Ph.D. Kelli Prejean, Ph.D. Marshall University May 2012 i Acknowledgments First, I would like to thank my husband, James, for bearing with me, and bearing with me gracefully, throughout the entire process of writing this thesis. Thank you for supporting me, for making dinner on those late nights (and early nights – and, well, pretty much every night), for listening to the thoughts that ―came to me‖ always too early in the morning; thank you for your love. Likewise, I would like to thank my family – my dad, Bob, my mom, Carol, and my brother, Bobby – for supporting me throughout my entire life, and specifically, for listening to each mini triumph and tragedy that occurred during the process of writing this document.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax–Semantics Interface in Systemic Functional Grammar: Halliday’S Interpretation of the Hjelmslevian Model of Stratification
    PREPRINT NOTICE This is a PREPRINT of an article to be published in Journal of Pragmatics. Copyright © Elsevier 2011. For citation, please consult the published version. ↓ P U B L I S H E D Full reference: VERSION Taverniers, Miriam (2011) The syntax–semantics interface in Systemic Functional Grammar: Halliday’s interpretation of the Hjelmslevian model of stratification. Journal of Pragmatics 43(4): 1100–1126. DOI:10.1016/j.pragma.2010.09.003 F U R T H E R http://users.UGent.be/~mtaverni/ INFORMATION & C ONTACT [email protected] The syntax–semantics interface in Systemic Functional Grammar: Halliday’s interpretation of the Hjelmslevian model of stratification Miriam Taverniers (Ghent University) English Department Ghent University Rozier 44 9000 Gent Belgium [email protected] Phone: + 32 9 264 3789 Fax: + 32 9 264 4179 PREPRINT. Taverniers, Miriam (2011) The syntax–semantics interface in Systemic Functional Grammar: Halliday’s interpretation of the Hjelmslevian model of stratification. Journal of Pragmatics 43(4): 1100–1126. Abstract The aim of this article is to explore how exactly the idea of distinguishing different coding levels in language has been theorized in different stages of Hallidayan systemic functional grammar (SFG), focusing on its view of the syntax–semantics interface. This is done by juxtaposing the levels of the Hallidayan model and the various components of Hjelmslev’s model of stratification, on the basis of Halliday’s re-interpretation of Hjelmslev’s theory at various stages in the development of SFG. In this exploration, specific attention is paid to two important theoretical aspects of the design of Hjelmslev’s and Halliday’s models: (1) the different dimensions along which semiotic distinctions are made in the two models, i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Multidimensional Thinking of Systemic Functional Linguistics
    2018 International Conference on Culture, Literature, Arts & Humanities (ICCLAH 2018) Multidimensional Thinking of Systemic Functional Linguistics Hongjie Zhang Xi’an International Studies University, Xi'an, Shaanxi, 710128 Keywords: Systemic Functional Linguistics, Multidimensional Thinking Abstract: Systemic functional linguistics, a language discipline based on certain philosophical ideas, plays an important role in the academic development of today's world. The development of systemic functional linguistics is also long-term, and today many people's cognition of systemic functional linguistics is relatively shallow, and some are even unheard of, but learning system functional linguistics is very helpful to the overall development. Therefore, in order to achieve the goal, it is necessary to first increase the cognition of systemic functional linguistics and clarify the meaning of development. 1. Introduction With the development and progress of the times, new ideas, ideas, technologies and many other aspects have flooded into the society. Under the promotion of these excellent factors, not only has the society developed rapidly, but people’s daily lives have also undergone earth-shaking Variety. Systemic functional linguistics, as a product of social and human development, evolved from the anthropological tradition. The main difference between this discipline and other disciplines is that it emphasizes the sociality of language users and is based on language practice. Application, thus providing better services for human survival and development. Systemic language function, first appeared in the United Kingdom, was founded by the famous British linguist Halliday. From the perspective of theory, systemic linguistics means that language is used as a tool on the basis of social attributes. Practice activities, not just in terms of thinking, at the same time, pay more attention to the analysis of individual languages, and verify theories through observation and statistical means.
    [Show full text]
  • Parametric Versus Functional Explanations of Syntactic Universals
    Parametric versus functional explanations of syntactic universals MARTIN HASPELMATH February 2006 Abstract: This paper compares the generative principles-and-parameters approach to explaining syntactic universals to the functional-typological approach and also discusses the intermediate approach of Optimality Theory. It identifies some fundamental differences between generative parametric explanations and functional explanations. Most importantly, generative explanations assume that cross-linguistic generalizations are due to the innate Universal Grammar, whereas functional explanations assume that language structure can be influenced by regularities of language use through language change. Despite these differences, both approaches to cross-linguistic similarities and differences seem to be guided by a similar vision: That the superficial structural diversity of languages can be reduced to a few basic patterns once one digs below the surface (macroparameters or holistic types). Unfortunately, the evidence for such reductionist constructs has never been very good, so more recently both generativists and functionalists have shifted their interests away from these ambitious goals. However, I argue that there is one type of cross-linguistic generalization for which there is very good empirical evidence: intra-domain universals relating to prominence scales, most of which are straightforwardly explained functionally in terms of processing difficulty. 1. Introduction: the relevance of observable universals Many generative and functional linguists share a primary interest in understanding structural similarities and differences between languages. It is clear that the observed language structures represent only a tiny fraction of the logically possible structures, and for a whole range of syntactic domains linguists have a good initial idea of what the limits on structural variation (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • 1. Functionalist Linguistics: Usage-Based Explanations of Language Structure
    MARTIN HASPELMATH, 15 July 1. Functionalist linguistics: usage-based explanations of language structure 1. What is functionalism? prototypical representatives of "functionalism" in my sense: general: Greenberg 1966, Croft 1990, Paul 1880/1920 syntax: Givón 1984-90, Hawkins 1994, Croft 2001 morphology: Bybee 1985 phonology: Lindblom 1986, Bybee 2001 non-prototypical representatives: Foley & Van Valin 1984, Langacker 1987-91, Dik 1997 fundamental question: Why is language structure the way it is? not: How can language be acquired despite the poverty of the stimulus? primary answer: Language structure (= competence) is the way it is because it reflects constraints on language use (= performance). typically this means: because it is adapted to the needs of language users main problem: H ow can language structure "reflect" language use? There must be an evolutionary/adaptive process, perhaps involving variation and selection. 2. Two examples (A) Vowel systems: The great majority of languages have "triangular" vowel systems as in (1a) or (1b). "Horizontal" or "vertical" systems as in (2a-b) are logically perfectly possible, but unattested. (1) a. [i] [u] b. [i] [u] [a] [e] [o] [a] (2) a. [i] [I] [u] b. [I] [´] [a] This reflects a phonetic constraint on language use: Vowels should be easily distinguishable, and phonetic research has shown that [i], [u], and [a] are the three extremes in the available vowel space (e.g. Lindblom 1986). 1 (B) Implicit infinitival subjects: In many languages, the subject of certain types of complement clauses can be left unexpressed only when it is coreferential with a matrix argument: (3) a. Roberti wants [Øi/*j to arrive in time].
    [Show full text]
  • Genre in Linguistic Traditions: Systemic Functional and Corpus Linguistics
    3 Genre in Linguistic Traditions: Systemic Functional and Corpus Linguistics While current approaches to genre in Rhetoric and Composition stud- ies draw in part from work in literary theory, they draw more so from linguistic, rhetorical, and sociological traditions. In this and the fol- lowing chapter, we will examine genre studies within linguistic tra- ditions, namely Systemic Functional Linguistics, Corpus Linguistics, and English for Specific Purposes. Then in Chapters 5 and 6, we will focus on genre studies within rhetorical and sociological traditions, since Rhetorical Genre Studies (RGS) has been most closely linked with and has most directly informed the study and teaching of genre in Rhetoric and Composition studies. Genre and Systemic Functional Linguistics Systemic Functional approaches to genre have contributed richly to how genre is understood and applied in textual analysis and language teaching over the last twenty-five years. Influenced in large part by the work of Michael Halliday (Halliday; Halliday and Hasan) at the University of Sydney, and applied to genre particularly in the work of J. R. Martin, Frances Christie, Bill Cope and Mary Kalantzis, Gunther Kress, Brian Paltridge, Joan Rothery, Eija Ventola, and others, Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) operates from the premise that language structure is integrally related to social function and context. Language is organized the way it is within a culture because such an organization serves a social purpose within that culture. “Functional” thus refers to the work that language does within particular contexts. “Systemic” re- fers to the structure or organization of language so that it can be used to get things done within those contexts.
    [Show full text]
  • A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya
    University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2013-05-28 Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya Gebregziabher, Keffyalew Gebregziabher, K. (2013). Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. doi:10.11575/PRISM/27453 http://hdl.handle.net/11023/741 doctoral thesis University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya By Keffyalew Gebregziabher A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS CALGARY, ALBERTA May 2013 © Keffyalew Gebregziabher 2013 Abstract In this dissertation, I examine the grammatical expression of possession in Tigrinya, a lesser-studied Semitic language of Ethiopia and Eritrea. I show that possession in Tigrinya is encoded by two strategies, which differ in both structure and function: (i) PREDICATIONAL STRATEGY has the particle nay and is used for alienable possession; (ii) ARGUMENTAL STRATEGY or BARE POSSESSION has no nay and is used for inalienable possession. To account for such differences, I propose different treatments for both types of possession.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of the Faculty of Language from a Chomskyan Perspective: Bridging Linguistics and Biology
    doi 10.4436/JASS.91011 JASs Invited Reviews e-pub ahead of print Journal of Anthropological Sciences Vol. 91 (2013), pp. 1-48 The evolution of the Faculty of Language from a Chomskyan perspective: bridging linguistics and biology Víctor M. Longa Area of General Linguistics, Faculty of Philology, University of Santiago de Compostela, 15782 Santiago de Compostela, Spain e-mail: [email protected] Summary - While language was traditionally considered a purely cultural trait, the advent of Noam Chomsky’s Generative Grammar in the second half of the twentieth century dramatically challenged that view. According to that theory, language is an innate feature, part of the human biological endowment. If language is indeed innate, it had to biologically evolve. This review has two main objectives: firstly, it characterizes from a Chomskyan perspective the evolutionary processes by which language could have come into being. Secondly, it proposes a new method for interpreting the archaeological record that radically differs from the usual types of evidence Paleoanthropology has concentrated on when dealing with language evolution: while archaeological remains have usually been regarded from the view of the behavior they could be associated with, the paper will consider archaeological remains from the view of the computational processes and capabilities at work for their production. This computational approach, illustrated with a computational analysis of prehistoric geometric engravings, will be used to challenge the usual generative thinking on language evolution, based on the high specificity of language. The paper argues that the biological machinery of language is neither specifically linguistic nor specifically human, although language itself can still be considered a species-specific innate trait.
    [Show full text]
  • A Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) Analysis of Yoruba Students’ Narratives Of
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by UWC Theses and Dissertations A Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) Analysis of Yoruba Students’ Narratives of Identity at Three Western Cape Universities Idowu Jacob Adetomokun BA (ENGLISH) OAU Ile-Ife, Nigeria A thesis submitted in fulfillment of the requirements for Masters Degree of Arts In the Department of Linguistics University of the Western Cape Supervisor: Prof. Felix Banda May 2012 ABSTRACT There has been a great deal of research exploring Halliday’s (1978, 1994, 2004) Systemic Functional Linguistic (SFL) approach. However, there has been little work that specifically targets SFL to explore African discourse. Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL) views language as “social semiotic”, that is, language is functional in terms of what it can do or what can be done with it; and semantic in that it is used to make meanings (Halliday’s, 1978). This study explores SFL to analyse narratives of identity as told by Yoruba students at three Western Cape Universities: University of the Western Cape (UWC), University of Cape Town (UCT) and University of Stellenbosch (SUN). This research is both quantitative and qualitative in outlook and results of the study are presented. I used the qualitative method to collect and analyse the data; but a certain amount of quantitative analysis was presented as well in order to determine the predominant identity options favoured by the students. A total of 14 Yoruba students were interviewed for data collection which was analysed with SFL interpersonal metafunction theoretical approach. Specifically, the study examines linguistic choices that the students utilize to maintain and reconstruct their identities in Cape Town.
    [Show full text]
  • The Routledge Handbook of Systemic Functional Linguistics What Is A
    Elissa Asp In contrast, de Saussure (1986: 122) describes paradigmatic relations in terms of absence. What is present in a syntagm gets part of its value from its associates or ‘mnemonic group’, which may include not only morphosyntactic associates such as the inflectional paradigms for verbs or declensions of nouns, but also items related via semantic, lexical, phonological or phonetic features. Consider the following example. (1) Sam slept through an afternoon In example (1), the value of any particular item is partially determined by the absent items with which it is in contrast: the referent Sam versus relevant other participants; past not present tense; contrasts for semantic features of sleep versus, for instance, doze; and so on for the other selections. This value is partial since selection and interpretation may also be affected by items copresent in a syntagm. For example, simple past tense verbs may be ambiguous between habitual and single-instance interpretations. Features of an accompa- nying time adjunct can push interpretation towards one of these. So a partial paraphrase of example (1) with singular indefinite an is on some occasion Sam did this, but the in the same environment leaves the interpretation ambiguous. Thus the selection of items present in a syntagm can be seen as functionally motivated not only by contrast with their absent associ- ates, but also by the syntagmatic environments in which they appear. For de Saussure, these axes – the syntagmatic and the paradigmatic; the arbitrary and motivated – were the central linguistic relations to be accounted for. Curiously, although all linguistic models recognise paradigmatic contrasts to some extent, most frame grammars in terms of syntagmatic structures.
    [Show full text]
  • Systemic Functional Grammar: a First Step Into the Theory
    SYSTEMIC FUNCTIONAL GRAMMAR: A FIRST STEP INTO THE THEORY Christian Matthiessen & M. A. K. Halliday iii/97 © Matthiessen & Halliday. Please do not copy or quote without authors' permission. 2 1. Into systemic-functional theory of grammar 1.1 General: [lexico]grammar & the study of grammar ('grammatics') This is an introductory account of a particular theory of grammar, namely systemic-functional theory. Grammar is one of the subsystems of a language; more specifically, it is the system of wordings of a language. It is a phenomenon that can be studied, just like light, physical motion, the human body, and decision-making processes in bureaucracies; and just as in the case of these and other phenomena under study, we need theory in order to interpret it. So for instance, the physical phenomenon of the atom has been interpreted theoretically in terms of Democritus' theory, Rutherford's theory, Bohr's theory, and so on. We distinguish between the phenomenon itself (the atom) and various theoretical models of it. What kind of thing the atom is thought to be will of course vary considerably as we move from one theory to another. Democritus' atom was very different from Bohr's atom, in that it was indivisible, not a configuration of subatomic particles; that is, Democritus' theory allowed us to see much less of the atom than Bohr's theory does. A well-known example of the way theory determines how we interpret phenomena is light. Light can be interpreted either as particle or as wave; there are two alternative theories. In this case, the alternatives turn out to be complementary, in the sense that each reveals something about light that we need to account for.
    [Show full text]
  • Points of Convergence Between Functional and Formal Approaches to Syntactic Analysis
    Points of convergence between functional and formal approaches to syntactic analysis Tavs Bjerre1, Eva Engels2, Henrik Jørgensen1 & Sten Vikner1 (1 University of Aarhus & 2 University of Oslo) 1. Introduction.................................................................................................................. 1 2. Theoretical and empirical linguistics........................................................................... 2 3. Radicalism within the formal and the functional approaches...................................... 3 4. Clausal architecture in the formal and functional approaches..................................... 4 4.1 Diderichsen's fields and slots ........................................................................... 5 4.2 Generative tree structures................................................................................. 9 5. Points of convergence between the formal and functional approaches..................... 12 5.1 Topological slots and their equvalents in the tree structure ........................... 12 5.2 Topological fields and their equvalents in the tree structure.......................... 16 6. Movement .................................................................................................................. 18 6.1 The position of unstressed object pronouns ................................................... 18 6.2 The position of the finite verb in main and embedded clauses ...................... 20 6.3 The initial position in main clauses...............................................................
    [Show full text]