SPECIFIC AIMS Our Recently Completed Prevalence Study

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

SPECIFIC AIMS Our Recently Completed Prevalence Study 1994 Continuing Review SPECIFIC AIMS Our recently completed prevalence study of dementia and Alzheimer's Disease (AD) in Indianapolis and Ibadan revealed in initial data analysis a significantly lower prevalence of both dementia and AD in the Yoruba speaking community sample, age 65 years and older in Ibadan as compared to the African American combined community and nursing home sample age 65 years and older in Indianapolis. (Ibadan age adjusted prevalence for dementia 1.76%, for AD 1.11%, Indianapolis age adjusted prevalence for dementia 5.74% community only, 9.06% combined sample for AD, 3.04% community only, 5.68% combined sample). The purpose of this study is to explore possible explanations for this finding by conducting longitudinal follow- up studies of individuals diagnosed cognitively impaired and demented and by conducting a second incidence study in year three. The two communities of related ethnic origin, with different environments and cultures offer a unique opportunity for these studies. The specific aims of the proposal are: Primary 1.To conduct a second incidence study of dementia, AD and cognitive impairment in the two samples, three years after the first incidence study. Hypothesis: There will be a lower incidence of both AD and dementia and of cognitive impairment in Ibadan as compared to Indianapolis. 2.To conduct an annual follow-up study for five years of individuals identified as demented or as cognitively impaired during the initial prevalence and incidence studies. Hypothesis: There will be a slower progression of the dementing symptoms both functional impairment and cognitive decline in the Ibadan subjects as compared to the subjects in Indianapolis. A smaller proportion of cognitively impaired individuals will develop dementia in Ibadan than in Indianapolis. 3. To follow all subjects to determine age specific mortality rates in the two samples. Hypothesis: Age specific mortality rates will be higher in Ibadan than in Indianapolis in individuals with the diagnosis of cognitive impairment and dementia as compared to the mortality rates for the non- cognitively impaired group. 4. To collect blood and type for ApoE alleles in all subjects in both samples. Hypothesis:a) The presence of the ApoE-ε4 allele will be more strongly associated with AD and with cognitive impairment in Indianapolis than in Ibadan. b) There will be statistically significant interaction between ApoE genotyping and some risk factors such as smoking in the two sites. 5. To continue brain autopsy studies in both sites. Hypothesis: Plaques, tangles and other histopathological hallmarks of AD will occur in lower frequency and severity in the clinically diagnosed demented patients in Nigeria than in the African American patients. 6. To complete the data analysis of the current prevalence and incidence study of dementia. Hypothesis:a) The incidence of both dementia and cognitive impairment will be lower in Indianapolis than in Ibadan. b) Risk factors for dementia and cognitive impairment will be identified that are site specific as well as occurring in both sites. Developmental Aims: 1. To continue to collect data on other putative risk factors. 2.To collect blood samples from the subjects in both sites which will be available to allow other genotyping or other biochemical measurements such as lipid measurements to occur as hypotheses are generated. 3.To construct an algorithm from our screening instrument which will be able to identify with a high degree of accuracy both dementia and AD in the two populations. 4.To refine the criteria for cognitive impairment based upon predicted value in determining outcome. 5.To assess the effects of education, occupation life time learning and social interaction on dementia in the two sites. Data analyses is not quite complete in Ibadan with approximately 60 clinical assessments not having full consensus diagnosis. The final analysis will be available prior to review. Our results will be shared for 1994 Continuing Review multi-site comparisons with the members of the WHO age associated dementia study (Dr. Amaducci, P.I.) and with Drs. Levy and Richards who are conducting a study of Jamaican subjects living in London (See accompanying letters). BACKGROUND AND SIGNIFICANCE Alzheimer's Disease is likely to be caused by a combination of aging, genetic and environmental factors. The search for risk factors for AD would be greatly expedited if populations could be identified with significantly lower or significantly higher rates of the disease. However the evidence that such populations exist so far has been scanty. While different rates of dementia and AD have been reported in different countries for example Japanese studies generally report lower prevalence rates of AD than North America and European studies, few studies have used identical methodologies, although the Ni-Hon-Sea study is now applying standardized protocols to Japanese samples (Graves and Kukull, 1994). There also remains the still not well explained differences in the prevalence of dementia, between New York and London in a study which used identical methods and criteria in both sites (Gurland et al, 1983). There are only a few studies comparing rates of dementia in African Americans to whites in the United States with two reports suggesting that in biracial community populations the prevalence of dementia is higher among blacks than among whites (Schoenberg et al, 1985; Heyman et al, 1991). We have now demonstrated significant differences in prevalence rates of both dementia and AD between the African American sample of Indianapolis and the Yoruba speaking sample of Ibadan, Nigeria. (See progress report). As genetic and environmental factors are not likely to be acting independently this finding affords us the opportunity to study the interaction of genetic and environmental factors in two populations originating from the similar geographic area and now living in two very different environments, although we recognize that intermarriage has occurred between African Americans and other American groups. The evidence that AD is at least in part genetically based has increased dramatically over the past two years. Previously familial forms of early onset AD had been linked to two loci on chromosome 21 (St. George Hyslop et al, 1987) and on chromosome 14 (Schellenberg et al, 1992, 1993). These subtypes however accounted for only a very small proportion of the overall individuals with AD. More recently considerable evidence has accumulated that the ε4 allele of ApoE gene on chromosome 19 constitutes a major susceptibility factor for the development of the familial and sporadic forms of late onset AD (Saunders et al, 1993). The risk for AD is higher and the age of onset lower for ε4 heterozygotes and especially for ε4 homozygotes (Corder et al, 1993). We have now extended this association to African Americans where earlier studies using smaller samples had failed to show an association. (See progress report). The finding of the association between ε4 allele and the risk for AD has been described by the Alzheimer's Association National Public Policy Forum as "the most exciting research breakthrough since Congress expanded its commitment to Alzheimers research." As pointed out by several senior investigators the results demonstrate more than ever the need for investigations that are community based, longitudinal in design and involve more than one ethnic group in order to understand the true risk susceptibility for and biological significance of the ApoE-ε4 allele in the etiology of AD. It should also be pointed out that in our study as in the other studies a large percentage of individuals with AD (in our study 44%) did not possess the ε4 allele and some individuals with the ε4 allele (in our study 22%) did not develop AD suggesting that other risk factors are also likely to be involved in the pathogenesis of the disease. The frequency of the ApoE-ε4 allele is reported to be high (30%) in Nigerians (Sephernia et al, 1989) yet the presence of AD appears to be low (Osuntokun et al, 1994). It should be noted that ApoE-ε4 allele predispose also to atherosclerosis which again occurs as of now in relatively low frequency in Nigeria suggesting the presence of an environmental/genetic interaction also in this disease (Ogunnowo et al, 1989). Apart from family history and age, little is known with any certainty about other risk factors for AD although associations with the disease have been reported for severe head trauma, depression, hypothyroidism and a history of Down syndrome or Parkinson's disease (Breteler et al, 1992). The relationship between education and occupation with AD has been controversial. Several cross sectional studies have found an association between AD and limited educational attainment (Zhang et al, 1990, Fratiglione et al, 1985). It has been suggested that advanced educational attainment may supply a brain 1994 Continuing Review reserve either through an acquired set of skills or repertoire or as a result of increased synaptic density in the neocortex on the basis of stimulation which delays the onset of the symptoms for four to five years (Katzman, 1993; Friedland, 1993). This hypothesis has been supported by the finding of an inverse relationship between education and parieto-temporal perfusion deficits in cerebral blood flow studies on AD subjects (Stern et al, 1992). Other prevalence studies do not support this association however (Knoefel et al, 1991). It is interesting to note that the two most recent incidence studies
Recommended publications
  • The Very Process of Taking a Pill May Create Expectations in a Patient Which May Affect Reactions to the Pill
    Ch 1 Pause for Thought Questions p. 36 1- Why is it important to use placebos and a double-blind approach in some studies? The very process of taking a pill may create expectations in a patient which may affect reactions to the pill. To help eliminate the role of patient expectation during testing, a placebo (or fake medicine) is used on subjects in a control group. A researcher’s expectations may also affect results. Therefore, double-blind studies are often used. In such studies, neither the researcher nor the participants know which group is which. 2- Assume that researchers find that people’s memories are sharpest right after they’ve eaten lunch. What hidden variables may have affected these results? Other variables that may play a role (hidden variables): Time of Day Type of food eaten Meaning of “lunch” - May mean “time away from work” - Subjective State of feeling free (not food) helps memory. Socializing at lunch may make a person more alert and then able to take-in more info and retain it longer. 3- How might you use the scientific method to study factors that affect obedience? Devise a simple study, and identify the following: hypothesis, subjects, independent variable, dependent variable, experimental group, and control group. Hypothesis: If employee fears/believes they will be punished if they don’t follow directives, then they will be obedient to directives of bosses/people in “higher” positions than themselves in the workplace. Subjects: Employees Independent Variable: Directive given with harsh consequences for not following through. Dependent Variable: Obedience (Level of) Experimental Group: Group with “harsh” bosses (very by the book; give extreme consequences) present during their work day.
    [Show full text]
  • Design of Prospective Studies
    Design of Prospective Studies Kyoungmi Kim, Ph.D. June 14, 2017 This seminar is jointly supported by the following NIH-funded centers: Seminar Objectives . Discuss about design of prospective longitudinal studies . Understand options for overcoming shortcomings of prospective studies . Learn to determine how many subjects to recruit for a follow-up study What is a prospective study? Study subjects of disease‐free at enrollment are followed for a period of time and periodically checked for progress to see who/when gets the outcome in question ‐thus be able to establish a temporal relationship between exposure & outcome. What is a prospective study? During the follow‐up, data is collected on the factors of interest, including: •When the subject develops the condition •When they drop out of the study or become “lost” •When they exposure status changes •When they die Example: Framingham Study . An original cohort of 5,209 subjects from Framingham, MA between the ages of 30 and 62 years of age was recruited and followed up for 20 years. A number of hypotheses were generated and described by Dawber et al. in 1980 listing various presupposed risk factors such as increasing age, increased weight, tobacco smoking, elevated blood pressure and cholesterol and decreased physical activity. It is largely quoted as a successful longitudinal study owing to the fact that a large proportion of the exposures chosen for analysis were indeed found to correlate closely with the development of cardiovascular disease. Example: Framingham Study . Biases exist: – It was a study carried out in a single population in a single town, brining into question the generalizability and applicability of this data to different groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Study Designs in Biomedical Research
    STUDY DESIGNS IN BIOMEDICAL RESEARCH INTRODUCTION TO CLINICAL TRIALS SOME TERMINOLOGIES Research Designs: Methods for data collection Clinical Studies: Class of all scientific approaches to evaluate Disease Prevention, Diagnostics, and Treatments. Clinical Trials: Subset of clinical studies that evaluates Investigational Drugs; they are in prospective/longitudinal form (the basic nature of trials is prospective). TYPICAL CLINICAL TRIAL Study Initiation Study Termination No subjects enrolled after π1 0 π1 π2 Enrollment Period, e.g. Follow-up Period, e.g. three (3) years two (2) years OPERATION: Patients come sequentially; each is enrolled & randomized to receive one of two or several treatments, and followed for varying amount of time- between π1 & π2 In clinical trials, investigators apply an “intervention” and observe the effect on outcomes. The major advantage is the ability to demonstrate causality; in particular: (1) random assigning subjects to intervention helps to reduce or eliminate the influence of confounders, and (2) blinding its administration helps to reduce or eliminate the effect of biases from ascertainment of the outcome. Clinical Trials form a subset of cohort studies but not all cohort studies are clinical trials because not every research question is amenable to the clinical trial design. For example: (1) By ethical reasons, we cannot assign subjects to smoking in a trial in order to learn about its harmful effects, or (2) It is not feasible to study whether drug treatment of high LDL-cholesterol in children will prevent heart attacks many decades later. In addition, clinical trials are generally expensive, time consuming, address narrow clinical questions, and sometimes expose participants to potential harm.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Do We Need Longitudinal Survey Data?
    HEATHER JOSHI University College London, UK Why do we need longitudinal survey data? Knowing people’s history helps in understanding their present state and where they are heading Keywords: life chances, inequality, mobility, gender, cohort, panel studies ELEVATOR PITCH Work experiences of men and women explain little of the Information from longitudinal surveys transforms gap in their hourly pay in the UK Cohorts snapshots of a given moment into something with a Born 1946 time dimension. It illuminates patterns of events within Age 26 31 an individual’s life and records mobility and immobility 43 Born 1958 between older and younger generations. It can track Age 23 33 the different pathways of men and women and people 42 of diverse socio-economic background through the life Born 1970 Gap explained by differences Age 26 in work experience & education course. It can join up data on aspects of a person’s life, 30 Unexplained gap 34 health, education, family, and employment and show how 0510 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 these domains affect one another. It is ideal for bridging Men’s pay minus women’s pay as % of men’s pay the different silos of policies that affect people’s lives. Source: [1]; Table 7.12. KEY FINDINGS Pros Cons Longitudinal surveys form a record of Longitudinal survey data are expensive to collect continuities and transitions of a life as they and challenging to analyze. happen, information not reliably gained through Data must be collected over a long timeframe to recollection or cross-section surveys. show relevant, long-term results.
    [Show full text]
  • Recommendations for Naming Study Design and Levels of Evidence
    Recommendations for Naming Study Design and Levels of Evidence for Structured Abstracts in the Journal of Orthopedic & Sports Physical Therapy (JOSPT) Last revised 07-03-2008 The JOSPT as a journal that primarily focuses on clinically based research, attempts to assist readers in understanding the quality of research evidence in its published manuscripts by using a structured abstract that includes information on the “study design” and the classification of the “level of evidence.” While no single piece of information or classification can signify the quality and clinical relevance of published studies, a consistent approach to communicating this information is a step towards assisting our readers in evaluating new knowledge. Outline of the structured abstract for research reports published in JOSPT Study design Objectives Background Methods and Measures Results Conclusions Level of Evidence Suggestions for Naming Study Designs We recommend that authors attempt to use the following terminology when naming their research designs. Use of consistent terminology will make it easier for readers to identify the nature of the study and will allow us to track the type of studies published more easily. We recognize that this list is not all-inclusive and that more appropriate descriptors might be suitable for some studies. In those cases, investigators are encouraged to pick the most appropriate descriptors for their study. These suggestions are provided as a means of encouraging consistency where it would be both useful and informative and their use is expected where they do apply. Quantitative Clinical Study Categories include (Therapy, Prevention, Etiology, Harm, Prognosis, Diagnosis, Differential Diagnosis, Symptom Prevalence, Economic Analysis, and Decision Analysis).
    [Show full text]
  • Choosing the Right Study Design
    Choosing the right study design Caroline Sabin Professor of Medical Statistics and Epidemiology Institute for Global Health Conflicts of interest I have received funding for the membership of Data Safety and Monitoring Boards, Advisory Boards and for the preparation of educational materials from: • Gilead Sciences • ViiV Healthcare • Janssen‐Cilag Main types of study design BEST QUALITY Randomised controlled trial (RCT) EVIDENCE Cohort study Case‐control study Cross‐sectional study Case series/case note review ‘Expert’ opinion WORST QUALITY EVIDENCE Experimental vs. Observational Experimental study Investigator intervenes in the care of the patient in a pre‐planned, experimental way and records the outcome Observational study Investigator does not intervene in the care of a patient in any way, other than what is routine clinical care; investigator simply records what happens Cross‐sectional vs. Longitudinal Cross‐sectional study Patients are studied at a single time‐point only (e.g. patients are surveyed on a single day, patients are interviewed at the start of therapy) Longitudinal study Patients are followed over a period of time (days, months, years…) Assessing causality (Bradford Hill criteria) • Cause should precede effect • Association should be plausible (i.e. biologically sensible) • Results from different studies should be consistent • Association should be strong • Should be a dose‐response relationship between the cause and effect •Removal of cause should reduce risk of the effect Incidence vs. prevalence Incidence: proportion
    [Show full text]
  • The Design of Observational Longitudinal Studies
    The Design of Observational Longitudinal Studies Xavier Basagana˜ 1;2;3;4, Donna Spiegelman4;5 1Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology (CREAL) 2 Municipal Institute of Medical Research (IMIM-Hospital del Mar) 3 CIBER Epidemiologia y Salud Publica (CIBERESP) 4Department of Biostatistics, Harvard School of Public Health 5Department of Epidemiology, Harvard School of Public Health Summary This paper considers the design of observational longitudinal studies with a continu- ous response and a binary time-invariant exposure, where, typically, the exposure is unbalanced, the mean response in the two groups differs at baseline and the measure- ment times might not be the same for all participants. We consider group differences that are constant and those that increase linearly with time. We study power, number of study participants (N) and number of repeated measures (r), and provide formulas for each quantity when the other two are fixed, for compound symmetry, damped ex- ponential and random intercepts and slopes covariances. When both N and r can be chosen by the investigator, we study the optimal combination for maximizing power subject to a cost constraint and minimizing cost for fixed power. Intuitive parameteri- zations are used for all quantities. All calculations are implemented in freely available software. 1 Introduction Sample size and power calculation in longitudinal studies with continuous outcomes that compare two groups have been considered previously (Yi and Panzarella, 2002; Schouten, 1999; Galbraith and Marschner, 2002; Frison and Pocock, 1992, 1997; Daw- son and Lagakos, 1993; Raudenbush and Xiao-Feng, 2001; Overall and Doyle, 1994; Hedeker et al., 1999; Jung and Ahn, 2003; Schlesselman, 1973; Liu and Liang, 1997; Kirby et al., 1994; Rochon, 1998).
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 13: Cohort Studies (Kanchanaraksa)
    This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike License. Your use of this material constitutes acceptance of that license and the conditions of use of materials on this site. Copyright 2008, The Johns Hopkins University and Sukon Kanchanaraksa. All rights reserved. Use of these materials permitted only in accordance with license rights granted. Materials provided “AS IS”; no representations or warranties provided. User assumes all responsibility for use, and all liability related thereto, and must independently review all materials for accuracy and efficacy. May contain materials owned by others. User is responsible for obtaining permissions for use from third parties as needed. Cohort Studies Sukon Kanchanaraksa, PhD Johns Hopkins University Design of a Cohort Study Identify: Exposed Not exposed follow: Develop Do not Develop Do not disease develop disease develop disease disease 3 Cohort Study Totals Exposed a + b First, identify Not c + d exposed 4 Cohort Study Then, follow to see whether Disease Disease does not develops develop Totals Exposed a b a + b First, identify Not c d c + d exposed 5 Cohort Study Calculate Then, follow to see whether and compare Disease Disease does not Incidence develops develop Totals of disease a Exposed a b a + b a+b First, identify Not c c d c + d exposed c+ d a c = Incidence in exposed = Incidence in not exposed a+ b c+ d 6 Cohort Study Then, follow to see whether Calculate Do not Develop develop Incidence CHD CHD Totals of disease Smoke 84 84 2916 3000 First, cigarettes 3000 select Do not 87 smoke 87 4913 5000 cigarettes 5000 84 = 0.028 = Incidence in 'smoke cigarettes' 3000 87 = 0.0174 = Incidence in 'not smoke cigarettes' 5000 7 Design of a Cohort Study Begin with: Defined population N o n - r a n d o m i z e d Identify : Exposed Not exposed follow: Develop Do not Develop Do not disease develop disease develop disease disease 8 Comparison of Experimental vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Sampling Strategies to Measure the Prevalence of Common Recurrent
    Schmidt et al. Emerging Themes in Epidemiology 2010, 7:5 http://www.ete-online.com/content/7/1/5 EMERGING THEMES IN EPIDEMIOLOGY METHODOLOGY Open Access Sampling strategies to measure the prevalence of common recurrent infections in longitudinal studies Wolf-Peter Schmidt1*, Bernd Genser2, Mauricio L Barreto2, Thomas Clasen1, Stephen P Luby3, Sandy Cairncross1, Zaid Chalabi4 Abstract Background: Measuring recurrent infections such as diarrhoea or respiratory infections in epidemiological studies is a methodological challenge. Problems in measuring the incidence of recurrent infections include the episode definition, recall error, and the logistics of close follow up. Longitudinal prevalence (LP), the proportion-of-time-ill estimated by repeated prevalence measurements, is an alternative measure to incidence of recurrent infections. In contrast to incidence which usually requires continuous sampling, LP can be measured at intervals. This study explored how many more participants are needed for infrequent sampling to achieve the same study power as frequent sampling. Methods: We developed a set of four empirical simulation models representing low and high risk settings with short or long episode durations. The model was used to evaluate different sampling strategies with different assumptions on recall period and recall error. Results: The model identified three major factors that influence sampling strategies: (1) the clustering of episodes in individuals; (2) the duration of episodes; (3) the positive correlation between an individual’s disease incidence and episode duration. Intermittent sampling (e.g. 12 times per year) often requires only a slightly larger sample size compared to continuous sampling, especially in cluster-randomized trials. The collection of period prevalence data can lead to highly biased effect estimates if the exposure variable is associated with episode duration.
    [Show full text]
  • Relative Efficiency of Longitudinal, Endpoint, and Change Score
    Relative efficiency of longitudinal, endpoint, and change score analyses in randomized clinical trials Yuanjia Wang∗ Department of Biostatistics, Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University Email: [email protected] and Naihua Duan Department of Psychiatry and Department of Biostatistics Columbia University, New York, NY 10032 Abstract In the last two decades, the design of longitudinal studies, especially the sample size determination, has received extensive attention (Overall and Doyle 1994; Hedeker et al. 1999; Roy et al. 2007). However, there is little discussion on the relative efficiency of three strategies widely used to analyze randomized clinical trial data: a full longitudinal analysis using all data measured over time, an endpoint analysis using data measured at the endpoint (the primary time point for outcome evaluation), and a change score analysis using data measured at the baseline and the endpoint. When designing randomized clinical trials, investigators usually need to decide whether they would collect the interim data and if so, which type of analysis among the three would be the primary analysis. In this work, we compare the relative efficiency of detecting an intervention effect in randomized clinical trials using longitudinal, endpoint, and change score analysis, assuming linearity of the outcome trajectory and several commonly used within-individual correlation structures. Our analysis reveals an important and ∗Corresponding author 1 somewhat surprising finding that a full longitudinal analysis using all available data is often less efficient than an endpoint analysis and the change score analysis if the correlation among the repeated measurements is not particularly strong and the drop out rate is not high.
    [Show full text]
  • FORWARD: a Registry and Longitudinal Clinical Database to Study Fragile X Syndrome Stephanie L
    FORWARD: A Registry and Longitudinal Clinical Database to Study Fragile X Syndrome Stephanie L. Sherman, PhD, a Sharon A. Kidd, PhD, MPH,b Catharine Riley, PhD, MPH,c Elizabeth Berry-Kravis, PhD,d, e, f Howard F. Andrews, PhD,g Robert M. Miller, BA, b Sharyn Lincoln, MS, CGC, h Mark Swanson, MD, MPH,c Walter E. Kaufmann, MD, i, j W. Ted Brown, MD, PhDk BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVE: Advances in the care of patients with fragile X syndrome (FXS) abstract have been hampered by lack of data. This deficiency has produced fragmentary knowledge regarding the natural history of this condition, healthcare needs, and the effects of the disease on caregivers. To remedy this deficiency, the Fragile X Clinic and Research Consortium was established to facilitate research. Through a collective effort, the Fragile X Clinic and Research Consortium developed the Fragile X Online Registry With Accessible Research Database (FORWARD) to facilitate multisite data collection. This report describes FORWARD and the way it can be used to improve health and quality of life of FXS patients and their relatives and caregivers. METHODS: FORWARD collects demographic information on individuals with FXS and their family members (affected and unaffected) through a 1-time registry form. The longitudinal database collects clinician- and parent-reported data on individuals diagnosed with FXS, focused on those who are 0 to 24 years of age, although individuals of any age can participate. RESULTS: The registry includes >2300 registrants (data collected September 7, 2009 to August 31, 2014). The longitudinal database includes data on 713 individuals diagnosed with FXS (data collected September 7, 2012 to August 31, 2014).
    [Show full text]
  • Time-Varying, Serotype-Specific Force of Infection of Dengue Virus
    Time-varying, serotype-specific force of infection of dengue virus Robert C. Reiner, Jr.a,b,1, Steven T. Stoddarda,b, Brett M. Forsheyc, Aaron A. Kinga,d, Alicia M. Ellisa,e, Alun L. Lloyda,f, Kanya C. Longb,g, Claudio Rochac, Stalin Vilcarromeroc, Helvio Astetec, Isabel Bazanc, Audrey Lenharth,i, Gonzalo M. Vazquez-Prokopeca,j, Valerie A. Paz-Soldank, Philip J. McCallh, Uriel Kitrona,j, John P. Elderl, Eric S. Halseyc, Amy C. Morrisonb,c, Tadeusz J. Kochelc, and Thomas W. Scotta,b aFogarty International Center, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20892; bDepartment of Entomology and Nematology, University of California, Davis, CA 95616; cUS Naval Medical Research Unit No. 6 Lima and Iquitos, Peru; dDepartment of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109; eRubenstein School of Environment and Natural Resources, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT 05405; fDepartment of Mathematics and Biomathematics Graduate Program, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695; gDepartment of Biology, Andrews University, Berrien Springs, MI 49104; hLiverpool School of Tropical Medicine, Liverpool, Merseyside L3 5QA, United Kingdom; iEntomology Branch, Division of Parasitic Diseases and Malaria, Center for Global Health, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, GA 30333; jDepartment of Environmental Sciences, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322; kGlobal Health Systems and Development, School of Public Health and Tropical Medicine, Tulane University, New Orleans, LA 70112; and lInstitute for Behavioral and Community Health, Graduate School of Public Health, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182 Edited by Burton H. Singer, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, and approved April 16, 2014 (received for review August 15, 2013) Infectious disease models play a key role in public health planning.
    [Show full text]