Those Verbs Which Allow Or Require the Present Subjunctive In

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Those Verbs Which Allow Or Require the Present Subjunctive In ON TRANSITIVE VERB PHRASE COMPLEMENTATION* SHUJI CHIBA Tsuda College Those verbs which allow or require the present subjunctive in the embedded clause may generally be expected, from their semantic properties, to take a transitive VP complement. However, this is not always the case. Besides transitive VP complements, some of them will be found to take other kinds of infinitival complements; we will find that these present subjunctive verbs can be divided into several different types as to their subcategorization properties. This paper explores these syntactic properties of some of those subjunctive- taking verbs, especially in the framework of the Government and Binding theory. 1. INTRODUCTION. Subcategorization properties of verbs may be expected to be completely predicted by the semantic characteristics of those verbs. However, since we lack, at present, the complete description of the semantic characteristics of verbs, we cannot predict their subcategorization properties to the smallest detail. For example, as Williams (1974: 38) points out, 'the three verbs decree, order, and demand are similar semantically, but their complement struc- tures differ radically,' as shown in 1: {decreed (1) a. John *ordered that Bill leave.1 demanded} {*decreed b. John *ordered of Bill that he leave. demanded} * This is a revised version of sections 1-7 of my paper, Chiba 1984 and is based on my talk at the annual meeting of the English Linguistic Society of Japan which was held at Tsukuba University in November, 1984. I am grateful to those people who gave me invaluable comments on my talk there. I would also like to thank Akira Ota, whose insightful comments helped shape this paper. Of course, the responsibility for remaining inadequacies is entirely my own. 1There is a dialect variation concerning the possibility of the subjunctive clause after the verb order, as we will see more closely in section 4. -81- 82 ENGLISH LINGUISTICS, VOLUME 2 (1985) (*decreed c. John ordered Bill to leave. *demanded} {*decreed d. John *ordered to leave. demanded} The purpose of this paper is to investigate, in the framework of the Government and Binding theory (henceforth the GB-theory), the subcategorization properties of some of those verbs which have generally been considered to be transitive VP complementation verbs. Especially our attention will be drawn to those verbs which allow or require the present subjunctive in the embedded clause, as in 1a above. These verbs, it will be shown, do not behave in the same way as to the syntactic structures which they can take, although they are the same in that they take the subjunctive clause. The verbs which we take up in this paper are the following: advise, ask, beg, command, demand, dictate, insist, order, request, require, and urge. (In section 8, we will consider the case of force, although it usually does not allow the present subjunctive clause.) In the following sections we will pay attention to some of the important subcategorization properties of these verbs and try to describe these properties in the framework of the GB-theory. 2. SOME IMPORTANT GENERAL PRINCIPLES IN THE GB-THEORY. Before proceeding directly to this task of ours, it would be appropriate to briefly mention here some of the important general principles of the GB-theory which we will appeal to in the following sections, when we try to describe the subcategorization properties of those verbs mentioned above. In the GB-theory it is assumed that the properties and the distribu- tion of empty categories (i.e. PRO and trace) can be accounted for by the theories of government, binding, and Case. The theory of govern- ment contains the Empty Category Principle (ECP): (2) ECP: nonpronominal empty categories must be properly governed By way of illustration, consider the following examples: (3) a. Bill was believed [s t to be competent] b. *Bill was wanted [s'[s t to be competent]] In 3a, the trace t is properly governed by the main verb, while in 3b, t is ON TRANSITIVE VERB PHRASE COMPLEMENTATION 83 not so governed because of the boundary S'. (Note that believe is one of those verbs which can delete the S' category by S'-deletion.) Thus, the ill-formedness of 3b can be ascribed to the ECP. Next, as a second general principle in the GB-theory, let us consider the Binding Theory, especially its principles A and B: (4) Binding Theory (A) An anaphor is bound in its governing category (B) A pronominal is free in its governing category To see how principle A can account for the ill-formedness of relevant examples, compare the following two sentences: (5) a. It was believed that John is competent. b. *John was believed [s' (that) [s t is competent]] Principle A requires t in 5b, which is an anaphor, to find its antecedent in its governing category, the embedded S in this case. This requirement, however, is not satisfied; hence principle A of the Binding Theory is violated. (You will find that 5b violates the ECP also.) PRO, a pronominal without a phonetic matrix, is considered a pronominal anaphor. From this dual characteristics of PRO, we eventually get the following derivative principle: (6) PRO is ungoverned Since PRO is governed in 7b-c (by the main verb in the former case, and by the agreement element AGR in the embedded S in the latter case), both of the sentences are ungrammatical: (7) a. We wanted [s'[s PRO to be competent]] b. *We believed [s PRO to be competent] c. *We believed [s'[s PRO is competent]] On the other hand, 7a is grammatical, because PRO is not governed. The last general principle we consider here is the Case Filter, which is stated as follows: (8) The Case Filter *NP, where NP has phonetic content, but no Case The Case Filter has the effect of blocking a sentence which contains an NP with a phonetic matrix which, however, has no Case. Consider, for example, the following sentences: (9) a. John expected [s Bill to help Mary] b. *John tried [s'[s Bill to help Mary]] In 9a, Bill can be assigned the objective Case by the main verb expected, because the category S' has been deleted by S'-deletion. In 9b, however, Bill will be left with no Case assigned to it, since it 84 ENGLISH LINGUISTICS, VOLUME 2 (1985) does not appear in a position to which Case can be assigned. (Note that try is not an S'-deletion verb.) So the sentence is blocked by the Case Filter. With these general principles as our guidelines, let us now turn to the description of those subjunctive verbs which we mentioned above. 3. THE VERBS Demand, Dictate, AND Insist. In this section, let us investigate the syntactic characteristics of the verbs demand, dictate, and insist. First, let us consider the following examples: {a) demanded (10) *John b) dictated Tom to leave immediately. c) insisted} As these examples show, demand, dictate, and insist cannot appear in this sentence structure. Next consider the following examples: {a) demanded (11) *Tom was b) dictated to leave immediately. c) insisted} The fact that the sentences in 11 are ungrammatical implies that the S-structures in 12a, b cannot be appropriate for these verbs: (12) a. Tom was Ven [NPt] [s' PRO to leave immediately] b. Tom was Yen [s t to leave immediately] c. Tom was Yen [s' [s t to leave immediately]] The reason for this is that t in 12a, b and PRO in 12a satisfy the general principles shown in section 2; if the S-structure of 11a-c is either 12a or 12b, the sentences in 11 would wrongly be predicted grammatical. On the other hand, the S-structure in 12c would correctly predict the ill-formedness of these sentences. That is, t in 12c is not properly governed, which is a violation of the ECP; cf. 3b in section 2. This structure would also go well with the fact that 10a, b, c, which are repeated below as 13a, b, c, respectively, with their presumed S-structure, are ungrammatical: {a) demanded (13) *John b) dictated [s'[s Tom to leave immediately]] c) insisted} That is, in 13a-c, the embedded subject Tom cannot be assigned a proper Case, thus a violation of the Case Filter; cf. 9b in section 2. This structure also seems to explain the fact that the sentences in 14 are ungrammatical: ON TRANSITIVE VERB PHRASE COMPLEMENTATION 85 {a) demand (14) *Who did John b) dictate [s'[s t to leave immediately]]? c) insist} Note that, in 14a-c, t must be properly governed, according to the ECP, which, however, is not satisfied in this structure; thus 14a-c are ungrammatical.2 So far, so good. However, consider the following sentences in 15 with their supposed structure shown in 16: {a) demanded (15) John b) *dictated to leave immediately. c) *insisted} {a) demanded (16) John b) dictated [s'[s RPO to leave immediately]] c) insisted} Supposing, as we did above, that the embedded structure [s'[s...]] is correct for these verbs, it should predict that all of these sentences are grammatical, since PRO in this structure satisfies the principle in 6 in section 2. But, in reality, only the a-sentence is grammatical. To be especially noticed here is that we cannot regard the S-structure of sentences like 15b, c as the one in 17: {a) dictated (17) John b) insisted}[NPPROi] [s'PROi to leave immediately] A structure like this is generally prohibited, because here the first PRO is governed, which is a violation of the general principle concerning PRO. Given these properties of demand, dictate, and insist, it would be natural to assign the structure [s/[s...]] only to demand among these three, and to regard the other two as those verbs which do not occur in any infinitival constructions.
Recommended publications
  • Common and Distinct Neural Substrates for Pragmatic, Semantic, and Syntactic Processing of Spoken Sentences: an Fmri Study
    Common and Distinct Neural Substrates for Pragmatic, Semantic, and Syntactic Processing of Spoken Sentences: An fMRI Study G.R.Kuperberg and P.K.McGuire Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/12/2/321/1758711/089892900562138.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Institute of Psychiatry, London E.T.Bullmore Institute of Psychiatry, London and the University of Cambridge M.J.Brammer, S.Rabe-Hesketh, I.C.Wright, D.J.Lythgoe, S.C.R.Williams, and A.S.David Institute of Psychiatry, London Abstract & Extracting meaning from speech requires the use of difference between conditions in activation of the left-super- pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic information. A central ior-temporal gyrus for the pragmatic experiment than the question is:Does the processing of these different types of semantic/syntactic experiments; (2) a greater difference be- linguistic information have common or distinct neuroanatomi- tween conditions in activation of the right-superior and middle- cal substrates? We addressed this issue using functional temporal gyrus in the semantic experiment than in the syntactic magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure neural activity experiment; and (3) no regions activated to a greater degree in when subjects listened to spoken normal sentences contrasted the syntactic experiment than in the semantic experiment. with sentences that had either (A) pragmatical, (B) semantic These data show that, while left- and right-superior-temporal (selection restriction), or (C) syntactic (subcategorical) viola- regions may be differentially involved in processing pragmatic tions sentences. All three contrasts revealed robust activation of and lexico-semantic information within sentences, the left- the left-inferior-temporal/fusiform gyrus.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-Occurrence Outline
    Introducing LexIt Building Distributional Profiles Ongoing Work Conclusions Outline Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-occurrence 1 Introducing LexIt A Computational Lexical Resource for Italian Verbs The Project Distributional Profiles 1 2 Gabriella Lapesa ,AlessandroLenci 2 Building Distributional Profiles Pre-processing 1University of Osnabr¨uck, Institute of Cognitive Science Subcategorization Frames 2 University of Pisa, Department of Linguistics Lexical sets Selectional preferences Explorations in Syntactic Government and Subcategorisation nd University of Cambridge, 2 September 2011 3 Ongoing Work 4 Conclusions Gabriella Lapesa, Alessandro Lenci Modeling Subcategorization Through Co-occurrence 2/ 38 Introducing LexIt Introducing LexIt Building Distributional Profiles The Project Building Distributional Profiles The Project Ongoing Work Distributional Profiles Ongoing Work Distributional Profiles Conclusions Conclusions Computational approaches to argument structure LexIt: a computational lexical resource for Italian The automatic acquisition of lexical information from corpora is a longstanding research avenue in computational linguistics LexIt is a computational framework for the automatic acquisition subcategorization frames (Korhonen 2002, Schulte im Walde and exploration of corpus-based distributional profiles of Italian 2009, etc.) verbs, nouns and adjectives selectional preferences (Resnik 1993, Light & Greiff 2002, Erk et al. 2010, etc.) LexIt is publicly available through a web interface: verb classes (Merlo & Stevenson 2001, Schulte im Walde 2006, http://sesia.humnet.unipi.it/lexit/ Kipper-Schuler et al. 2008, etc.) Corpus-based information has been used to build lexical LexIt is the first large-scale resource of such type for Italian, resources aiming at characterizing the valence properties of predicates cf. VALEX for English (Korohnen et al. 2006), LexSchem for fully on distributional ground French (Messiant et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Acquiring Verb Subcategorization from Spanish Corpora
    Acquiring Verb Subcategorization from Spanish Corpora Grzegorz ChrupaÃla [email protected] Universitat de Barcelona Department of General Linguistics PhD Program “Cognitive Science and Language” Supervised by Dr. Irene Castell´onMasalles September 2003 Contents 1 Introduction 5 2 Verb Subcategorization in Linguistic Theory 7 2.1 Introduction . 7 2.2 Government-Binding and related approaches . 7 2.3 Categorial Grammar . 9 2.4 Lexical-Functional Grammar . 11 2.5 Generalized Phrase-Structure Grammar . 12 2.6 Head-Driven Phrase-Structure Grammar . 14 2.7 Discussion . 17 3 Diathesis Alternations 19 3.1 Introduction . 19 3.2 Diathesis . 19 3.2.1 Alternations . 20 3.3 Diathesis alternations in Spanish . 21 3.3.1 Change of focus . 22 3.3.2 Underspecification . 26 3.3.3 Resultative construction . 27 3.3.4 Middle construction . 27 3.3.5 Conclusions . 29 4 Verb classification 30 4.1 Introduction . 30 4.2 Semantic decomposition . 30 4.3 Levin classes . 32 4.3.1 Beth Levin’s classification . 32 4.3.2 Intersective Levin Classes . 33 4.4 Spanish: verbs of change and verbs of path . 35 4.4.1 Verbs of change . 35 4.4.2 Verbs of path . 37 4.4.3 Discussion . 39 4.5 Lexicographical databases . 40 1 4.5.1 WordNet . 40 4.5.2 VerbNet . 41 4.5.3 FrameNet . 42 5 Subcategorization Acquisition 44 5.1 Evaluation measures . 45 5.1.1 Precision, recall and the F-measure . 45 5.1.2 Types and tokens . 46 5.2 SF acquisition systems . 47 5.2.1 Raw text .
    [Show full text]
  • Features from Aspects Via the Minimalist Program to Combinatory Categorial Grammar
    Á. Gallego & D. Ott (eds.). 2015. 50 Years Later: Reflections on Chomsky’s Aspects. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL. Available for purchase at http://mitwpl.mit.edu © The Authors FEATURES FROM ASPECTS VIA THE MINIMALIST PROGRAM TO COMBINATORY CATEGORIAL GRAMMAR NEIL SMITH ANNABEL CORMACK University College London 1 Background One major contribution of Aspects (Chomsky 1965)1 was to initiate the development of a theory of syntactic features. There is no use of features, either syntactic or morphophonemic, in Chomsky’s earliest work The Morphophonemics of Modern Hebrew (1951/79); they do not appear in his monumental The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory (1955/75); nor in Syntactic Structures (1957) (except for category labels); nor, except for phonological distinctive features, in Current Issues in Linguistic Theory (1964). In Aspects features play a crucial role. Chomsky suggests (p.214) that “We might, then, take a lexical entry to be simply a set of features, some syntactic, some phonological, some semantic”. But even here the use of features was somewhat haphazard, apparently unconstrained, and a terminological mess. In this contribution we want to trace – in broad outline – the development of a theory of morphosyntactic features and propose a fully Merge-based and parsimonious version of such a theory. Our work is intended as a contribution to Chomsky’s Minimalist Program, pursuing the same ends on the basis of some different background assumptions. These assumptions and the specific analyses derived from them have been developed using an austere version of Combinatorial Categorial Grammar, but are of wider applicability. All current theories use phonological features and ‘semantic’ features which we will largely ignore except to point out that the term ‘semantic’ is used in radically different ways to cover truth-theoretic, conceptual or encyclopaedic distinctions.
    [Show full text]
  • Glossary for Syntax in Three Dimensions (2015) © Carola Trips
    Glossary for Syntax in three Dimensions (2015) © Carola Trips A - B - C - D - E - F - G - H - I - J - K - L - M - N - O - P - Q - R - S - T - U - V - W - X - Y – Z A abstract case Abstract Case (or "Deep Case") is an abstract notion of case distinct from the morphologically marked case (cf. "case") used in Case theory (subtheories of Generative Grammar). Abstract Case is present even in languages which lack morphological case on noun phrases; it is usually assumed to be congruent with morphological case when such features are present. For example in German dative case is marked morphologically as in Ich helfe ihm ('I help him') whereas no such case marking is found in the English sentence (but Case is still there). academy In previous centuries the need for an academy dealing with linguistic matters was discussed and debated in England (on the model of the French académie française). Due to the increase in the production of grammars in the 17th and 18th centuries and the rising number of grammarians, calls for an academy to codify the English language by publishing an authoritative grammar and dictionary dwindled. acceptance Step in the process of standardization: a selected variety of a particular language must gain acceptance by a group of speakers/users who are capable of influencing other speakers. actants In valency grammar, a functional unit determined by the valency of the verb. Actants are required by the valence of the verb and are the equivalents of arguments (in generative grammar). © Carola Trips 1 active voice Term used in grammatical analysis of voice, referring to a sentence, clause or verb form where from a semantic point of view the grammatical subject is typically the actor in relation to the verb.
    [Show full text]
  • Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 4 Issue 2 Article 11 1997 Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings Edward Kako University of Pennsylvania Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Kako, Edward (1997) "Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 4 : Iss. 2 , Article 11. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol4/iss2/11 Subcategorization Semantics and the Naturalness of Verb-Frame Pairings Edward Kako 1. Introduction Do subcategorization frames have meanings independent of the verbs which appear in them? Advocates of the Projectionist position have answered “no” to this question, arguing that subcat frames serve only to satisfy the structural demands imposed upon sentences by the semantics of verbs (e.g., Chomsky, 1981; Pinker, 1989; Levin, 1993). Proponents of the Constructionist position, by contrast, have answered “yes” to this question, arguing that whether a verb and frame can peaceably cohabit in a sentence depends upon whether the two are “compatible” (e.g., Goldberg,
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Sense and Verb Subcategorization Probabilities Douglas Roland Daniel Jurafsky University of Colorado University of Colorado Department of Linguistics Dept
    To appear in Suzanne Stevenson and Paola Merlo’s volume of papers from the 1998 CUNY Sentence Processing Conference (Benjamins) Verb Sense and Verb Subcategorization Probabilities Douglas Roland Daniel Jurafsky University of Colorado University of Colorado Department of Linguistics Dept. of Linguistics, Computer Boulder, CO 80309-0295 Science, and Inst. of Cognitive Science [email protected] Boulder, CO 80309-0295 [email protected] 1 Introduction The probabilistic relation between verbs and their arguments plays an important role in psychological theories of human language processing. For example, Ford, Bresnan and Kaplan (1982) proposed that verbs like position have two lexical forms: a more preferred form that subcategorizes for three arguments (SUBJ, OBJ, PCOMP) and a less preferred form that subcategorizes for two arguments (SUBJ, OBJ). Many recent psychological experiments suggest that humans use these kinds of verb-argument preferences as an essential part of the process of sentence interpretation. (Clifton et al. 1984, Ferreira & McClure 1997, Garnsey et al. 1997, MacDonald 1994, Mitchell & Holmes 1985, Boland et al. 1990, Trueswell et al. 1993). It is not completely understood how these preferences are realized, but one possible model proposes that each lexical entry for a verb expresses a conditional probability for each potential subcategorization frame (Jurafsky 1996, Narayanan and Jurafsky 1998). Unfortunately, different methods of calculating verb subcategorization probabilities yield different results. Recent studies (Merlo 1994, Gibson et al. 1996, Roland & Jurafsky 1997) have found differences between syntactic and subcategorization frequencies computed from corpora and those computed from psychological experiments. Merlo (1994) showed that the subcategorization frequencies derived from corpus data were different from the subcategorization data derived from a variety of psychological protocols.
    [Show full text]
  • Valency Dictionary of Czech Verbs: Complex Tectogrammatical Annotation
    Valency Dictionary of Czech Verbs: Complex Tectogrammatical Annotation Marketa´ Stranˇak´ ova-Lopatk´ ova,´ Zdenekˇ Zabokrtskˇ y´ Center for Computational Linguistics Faculty of Mathematics and Physics, Charles University Malostranske´ nam.´ 25, CZ-11800 Prague, Czech Republic stranak,zabokrtsky ¡ @ckl.mff cuni.cz http://ckl.mff.cuni.cz Abstract A lexicon containing a certain kind of syntactic information about verbs is one of the crucial prerequisities for most tasks in Natural Language Processing. The goal of the project described in the paper is to create a human- and machine-readable lexicon capturing in detail valency behavior of hundreds most frequent Czech verbs. Manual annotation effort consumed at this project limits the speed of its growth on the one hand, but guarantees significantly higher data consistency than that of automatically acquired lexicons. In this paper, we outline the theoretical background on which the lexicon is based, and describe the annotation schema (lexicon data structure, annotation tools, etc.). Selected quantitative characteristics of the lexicon are presented as well. 1. Introduction The noun phrase jeho bratra [his brother] preceded by The verb is traditionally considered to be the center of no preposition can be Gen.sg or Acc.sg. The verb ptat´ the sentence, and thus the description of syntactic behav- se [to ask] allows only the former possibility. ior of verbs is a substantial task for linguists. A syntactic ¢ Syntactic analysis (considering a dependency ori- lexicon of verbs with the subcategorization information is ented formalism, syntactic analysis can be informally obviously crucial also for many tasks in Natural Language expressed as ‘determining which word depends on Processing (NLP) domain.
    [Show full text]
  • Dependency Grammar
    Formal & Computational Aspects of Dependency Grammar { Argument structure in DG { Geert-Jan M. Kruijff Computational linguistics University of the Saarland Saarbrucken¨ Germany [email protected] h i Contents First Last Prev Next J Goals Today we will have a look at two levels of argument structure, namely sub- • categorization and valency. At each level, we look at argumenthood from different viewpoints: • { Role of dependency: heads and dependents, types of relations, the meaning of relations. { Different dimensions of 'argumenthood': obligatoriness versus optionality, core versus free modification. { Instantiations of these notions in various dependency- and phrase structure- grammar frameworks. We argue that subcategorization and valency describe distinct levels of struc- • ture in a grammar. { We examplify why these levels are not isomorphic. { We develop a typological theory of abstract case to relate them. Contents First Last Prev Next J Contents Goals .................................................... 2 Subcategorization and valency .............................. 5 Subcategorization and valency are not isomorphic ....... 6 Consequences of non-isomorphy ....................... 8 Remarks on subcategorization in DG ........................ 9 Between subcategorization and surface word order ...... 12 Surface word order in DG ............................ 13 Separating dependency and linearization ............... 15 Resulting notions of subcategorization in DG ........... 17 Valency .................................................. 18
    [Show full text]
  • MITA Working Papers in Psycholinguistics, Volume 2. INSTITUTION Keio Univ., Tokyo (Japan)
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 358 719 FL 021 283 AUTHOR Otsu, Yukio, Ed. TITLE MITA Working Papers in Psycholinguistics, Volume 2. INSTITUTION Keio Univ., Tokyo (Japan). Inst. of Cultural and Linguistic Studies. PUB DATE 89 NOTE 102p.; Volume 1 is unavailable. For selected individual papers, see ED 331 302, ED 323 759, and FL 021 286-290. Publication partly supported bya grant from the Japanese Ministry of Education and Culture and the INS Corporation. AVAILABLE FROM Institute of Cultural & Linguistics Studies, Keio University, 2-15-45 Mita, Minato-ku, Tokyo 108 Japan. PUB TYPE Collected Works General (020) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC05 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS College Students; *English (Second Language); English for Academic Purposes; Error Patterns; Foreign Countries; Higher Education; High School Freshmen; High Schools; High School Students; *Language Processing; Linguistic Theory; *Second Language Learning; Semanti, ; Sentence Structure; Speech Acts; Verbs; Writing (Co-position) IDENTIFIERS Clauses; Connectives (Grammar); Japan; Reflexives; Universal Grammar ABSTRACT Seven original research papers are presented. The titles and authors are as follows: "Acquisition of the Argument-Structure of Verbs" (Mika Endo); "A Note on Semantic Selection" (Yoshio Endo); "The Governing Category Parameter in Second Language" (Makiko Hirakawa); "The Use of Connectives in English Academic Papers Written by Japanese Students" (Yasuko Kanno); "A Note on English As-Clauses" (Yoshihara Kumagai); "Structure-Dependence in Second Language Acquisition" (Kazuhiro Naoi); and
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Subcategorization Frequencies: American English Corpus Data, Methodological Studies, and Cross-Corpus Comparisons
    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 2004, 36 (3), 432-443 Verb subcategorization frequencies: American English corpus data, methodological studies, and cross-corpus comparisons SUSANNE GAHL University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, Illinois DAN JURAFSKY Stanford University, Stanford, California and DOUGLAS ROLAND University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California Verb subcategorization frequencies (verb biases) have been widely studied in psycholinguistics and play an important role in human sentence processing. Yet available resources on subcategorization fre- quencies suffer from limited coverage, limited ecological validity, and divergent coding criteria. Prior estimates of verb transitivity, for example, vary widely with corpus size, coverage, and coding criteria. This article provides norming data for 281 verbs of interest to psycholinguistic research, sampled from a corpus of American English, along with a detailed coding manual. We examine the effect on transi- tivity bias of various coding decisions and methods of computing verb biases. The frequency of linguistic structures, such as pho- duction (Gahl & Garnsey, in press). The experimental lit- nemes or words, has long been known to affect language erature on verb biases shows that, other things being equal, processing. Increasingly, research in sentence processing sentences that conform to a verb’s bias are easier to pro- has focused on the frequencies of more complex linguis- cess than sentences that violate a verb’s bias. Indeed, this tic structures.
    [Show full text]
  • Automatic Extraction of Subcategorization from Corpora
    Automatic Extraction of Subcategorization from Corpora Ted Briscoe John Carroll Computer Laboratory Cognitive and Computing Sciences University of Cambridge University of Sussex Pembroke Street, Cambridge CB2 3QG, UK Brighton BN1 9QH, UK ejb@cl, cam. ac. uk j otto. carroll@cogs, susx. ac. uk Abstract Several substantial machine-readable subcatego- rization dictionaries exist for English, either built We describe a novel technique and imple- largely automatically from machine-readable ver- mented system for constructing a subcate- sions of conventional learners' dictionaries, or manu- gorization dictionary from textual corpora. ally by (computational) linguists (e.g. the Alvey NL Each dictionary entry encodes the relative Tools (ANLT) dictionary, Boguraev et al. (1987); frequency of occurrence of a comprehen- the COMLEX Syntax dictionary, Grishman et al. sive set of subcategorization classes for En- (1994)). Unfortunately, neither approach can yield a glish. An initial experiment, on a sample genuinely accurate or comprehensive computational of 14 verbs which exhibit multiple comple- lexicon, because both rest ultimately on the manual mentation patterns, demonstrates that the efforts of lexicographers / linguists and are, there- technique achieves accuracy comparable to fore, prone to errors of omission and commission previous approaches, which are all limited which are hard or impossible to detect automatically to a highly restricted set of subcategoriza- (e.g. Boguraev & Briscoe, 1989; see also section 3.1 tion classes. We also demonstrate that a below for an example). Furthermore, manual encod- subcategorization dictionary built with the ing is labour intensive and, therefore, it is costly to system improves the accuracy of a parser extend it to neologisms, information not currently by an appreciable amount 1.
    [Show full text]