<<

Clinical Review, Vol. 16, No. 5, pp. 365-382, 1996 Copyright © 1996 Eisevier Science Ltd Pergamon Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0272-7358/96 $15.00 + .00

PII 80272-7358(96)00006-2

THE MEASUREMENT OF DISSOCIATION IN NORMAL AND CLINICAL POPULATIONS: META-ANALYTIC VALIDATION OF THE DISSOCIATIVE EXPERIENCES SCALE (DES)

Marinus H. van IJzendoorn and Carlo Schuengel

Center for Child and Family Studies, , The

ABSTRACT. The Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) hos now been used in over 100 studies on dissociation. This article reports on a series of meta-analyses to test some of the theoretical assumptions underlying the DES and to examine the instrument's reliability and validity. Studies wilh the DES were identißed through Psychlit, Medline, Social Sciences Citation Index, and Current Contents. Across sludies in similar domains (e.g., studies on multiple personality disorders) combined effect sizes were computed using the Rosenthal-Mullen approach. The DES showed excellent convergent validity wilh other dissociative experiences questionnaires and inter- view schedules (combined effect size: d = 1.82; N = 5,916). The DES also showed impressive pre- dictive validity, in particular concerning dissociative disorders (Multiple Personality Disorder: combined effect size d = 1.05; N = l, 705) and traumatic experiences (post-traumatic stress dis- order: combined effect size d = 0.75; N = 1,099; and abuse: combined effect size d = 0.52; N = 2,108). However, the discriminant validity was less well established. The DES is sensitive to response and experimenter biases. It is recommended to average DES-scores over more points in time and over morejudges. The DES seems to measure the current view on past dissociative expe- riences. The model of dissociation äs a form of autohypnosis failed to receive support from the data. A developmental model to Interpret dissociation is proposed.

DISSOCIATION IS OFTEN considered a psychological defense mechanism for vic- tims of traumatizing events such äs sexual molestation, natural disaster, or combat (Putnam, 1991). It is suggested that it allows these victims to compartmentalize per- ceptions and memories, and to detach themselves from the füll impact of the trauma. At a later stage, dissociation may delay the necessary working through of the traumatic experiences (Spiegel, 1991). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatrie Association, 1994) the essential feature of Dissociative

Correspondence should be addressed to Professor Marinus van IJzendoorn, Center for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, NL-2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands.

365 366 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel

Disorders has been defmed äs a disruption in the usually integrated functions of con- sciousness, memory, identity, and perception of the environment Bernstein and Putnam (1986) developed a self-report measure to assess the degree to which subjects experience dissociation — the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES). It consists of 28 items, and subjects are asked to indicate on a visual analogue scale which percentage of the time they experience dissociative Symptoms. The scale items were con- structed using data from Interviews meeting DSM-III criteria for dissociative disorders and from consultations with clinical experts in the treatment of dissociative disorders. The DES has been used äs a screening Instrument, distinguishing potential dissociative- disorder patients from other psychiatric patients, and äs a research Instrument. Is it possible to measure individual differences in dissociative experiences reliably and validly by means of a self-report Instrument such äs the DES? This question is central to the current meta-analytic validation of the DES. First, we assessed the instrument's con- vergent validity across studies. Several questionnaires and interview schedules have been developed to measure dissociation, dissociative experiences, or related phenomena. Questionnaires, such äs the Perceptual Alteration Scale (PAS; Sanders, 1986), the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS; Teilegen & Atkinson, 1974), the Questionnaire of Experiences of Dissociation (QED; Riley, 1988), the Bliss-scale (Wogan, 1995), the Dissociative Questionnaire (DIS-Q; Vanderlinden, Van Dyck, Vandereycken, Vertommen, & Verkes, 1993), and the dissociation scale of the Trauma Symptom Checklist (TSG40- DIS; Gleaves & Eberenz, 1995), have somewhat different formats and content but their authors all claim to assess some aspect of dissociation or a related phenomenon. Interviews based on the DSM-III-R criteria for dissociative disorders, such äs the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D; Steinberg, 1994) and the Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule (DDIS; ROSS, Heber, Norton, Andersen, Anderson, & Barcher, 1989), measure dissociative Symptoms and syndromes that should be associated with DES scores. Second, the DES should show discriminant validity (Campbell, 1960). Subjects from different socio-economic, racial, and cultural backgrounds should perceive the DES items in similar ways if their scores are used for comparison. Dissociation is not expected to be sex-related. One may expect weak asso- ciations with age (Strick & Wilcoxon, 1991). Lastly, the DES should not be affected strongly by response biases: that is, systematic error variance caused by social desirabili- ty or expectancy effects. Third, the reliability of the DES is an important issue. The data on the internal consistency or alpha reliability of the Instrument are summarized, and the studies on test-retest reliability are discussed. Furthermore, the existence of DES subscales are addressed. The proof of the pudding, however, is in assessing the instrument's predictive valid- ity (Nunnally, 1978): Is the DES related to the syndromes for which it has been devel- oped äs a screening Instrument? As Bernstein and Putnam (1986) predicted, the DES should be related to dissociative disorders, such äs Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD), Dissociative Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (DDNOS), and other Dissociative Disorders (ODD), and at the same time it should not be associated with psychiatric disturbances without dissociative characteristics, such äs Personality Disorders, Affective Disorders, Anxiety Disorders, Schizophrenia, or physical disor- ders such äs Seizure Disorder. Furthermore, äs dissociation is conceptualized äs a defense mechanism, it should be present in cases where defense is functional — at least in the short run. Therefore, the DES may be expected to discriminate Post- Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) or Abuse subjects from other (psychiatric or nor- mal) subjects. There has also been speculation on the basis of clinical reports that Eating Disorders may include certain dissociative components (Demitrack, Putnam, Dissociative Experiences Scale 367

Brewerton, & Brandt, 1990), and that subjects who tend to be hypnotizable also tend to have more frequent dissociative experiences (Bliss, 1986). Lastly, because the DES has been applied in several different diagnostic groups, including more than 12,000 subjects, we found it useful to derive some normative data from the extant studies, and to compare the means of those groups.

METHOD Data Base Pertinent studies were identified through Psychlit, Medline, and the Social Sciences Citation Index, äs well äs through the "snowball" method (Müllen, 1989). Recent studies were identified using Current Contents. Studies were found through comput- er searches using the keywords "dissociative," "experiences," "scale," "DES," and "dis- sociation," and by using the reference to the Bernstein and Putnam (1986) article. Criteria for inclusion of a study in one of the meta-analyses were äs follows: (a) The DES was used to compare nonclinical or clinical groups; (b) Means or medians were reported; and (c) There was sufficient statistical detail to compute effect sizes. The selection procedure yielded 22 studies on the relation between DES and alter- native measures for dissociation, 34 studies on the discriminant validity of the DES, and 79 studies on the predictive validity of the DES. Only published papers were included; because a separate Journal for studies on dissociation started at around the same time äs the DES became available (Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders), probably most DES studies have been published. In 1993, Carlson, Putnam, ROSS, and Torem (1993) published their multicenter study on the validity of the DES äs a screen- ing Instrument for MPD. This study was not included in our meta-analysis, because their sample and the other — separately reported — samples in the meta-analysis would almost certainly overlap. The number of hypothesis tests for the separate meta- analyses varied between 11 and 53, which is sufficient for the computation of com- bined effect sizes (Müllen, 1989; Van IJzendoorn, 1995). The data that were extracted for the meta-analyses from the individual studies are available from the first author.

Meta-Analytic Procedures In primary-level studies the unit of analysis is the subject; in a meta-analysis of several primary-level studies the unit of analysis is the outcome of those studies. Because of this difference in unit of analysis, the meta-analytic approach has to be based on a dif- ferent set of statistical techniques. These techniques should, for example, take into account the fact that data in a meta-analysis are usually based on different sample sizes and, therefore, lack the homogeneity of variance required for conventional statistics (Rosenthal, 1991; Müllen, 1989). In the present meta-analysis the statistical tests asso- ciated with the pertinent studies were transformed into a few common metrics for effect size: the correlation coefficient (r), and the standardized difference between the means of two groups (Cohen's d). The meta-analytic procedures applied to these common metrics have been described in detail by Müllen (1989), and they were suc- cessfully used in earlier meta-analyses (e.g., Van IJzendoorn, 1995). Effect sizes were combined according to the weighted Stouffer method (Müllen, 1989), tests for homo- geneity were performed, and predictor variables were used to explain the variability of effect sizes (Müllen, 1989). When one study yielded several effect sizes, a separate meta-analysis was performed to combine these effect sizes. The following predictors of Variation in study outcomes were included: publication year, sample size, sex ratio, and some features of the study designs. For the meta-analysis 368 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schwengel on convergent validity, the type of measure was included äs predictor (questionnaire or interview). For the meta-analysis on MPD, type of diagnostic measure (structured inter- view vs. clinical diagnosis), äs well äs type of comparison group (normal, students, epilep- tics, other psychiatric disorders, other physical disorders) were included äs predictors. For the meta-analysis on PTSD, whether or not a structured interview was used to estab- lish PTSD was included äs a predictor. In the meta-analysis on abuse, type of abuse (sex- ual, physical, or unspecified), and the method of measurement of abuse (questionnaire or interview) were used äs predictors. For the meta-analysis on Eating Disorders, type of Eating Disorder was included (Anorexia, Bulimia, or unspecified). It may be expected that in meta-analysis the different flaws and biases of the individual studies will cancel each other out. Nevertheless, for some systematic difFerences it is possible to assess the influence statistically (Müllen, 1989; Rosenthal, 1991).

RESULTS Convergent Validity In Table l, the meta-analytic results for the relation between the DES and alternative measures for dissociation are summarized. The overall combined correlation across dif- ferent measures was r= 0.67. The overall mean Cohen's d= 1.82 (N= 5,916). According to Cohen's (1988) criteria for a weak (d = 0.20), medium (d = 0.50), and strong (d= 0.80) effectsize, this convergent validity figure is quite impressive. The DES did not only correlate with similar questionnaires (PAS, TAS, QED, BLISS), but also with mea- sures based on Interviews (SCID-D and DDIS). In fact, the association between the DES and the interview measures appeared to be stronger than the association between the DES and the questionnaires (d = 2.05 and d = 1.81 respectively; p = 0.003, for the dif- ference). The relevant SCID-D and DDIS subscales showed a considerable overlap with the DES in terms of item content. Interviewing may reduce error variance that is intro- duced with paper-and-pencil methodology. The DES was only moderately correlated with the Peri-traumatic Dissociative Experiences Questionnaire (PDEQ) (Marmar et al., 1994), but the PDEQdoes not measure lifetime dissociation. Discriminant Validity In 19 studies the association between sex of subjects and the DES was reported (N= 4,074). The combined effect size of these studies was Cohen's d= —0.01; that is,

TABLE 1. Convergent Validity of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES)

Convergent Number Measure of Studies N r Cohen's d P

PAS 7 1627 0.63 1.64 0.0000 TAS 6 1421 0.54 1.30 0.0000 QED 4 1903 0.80 2.68 0.0000 Bliss scale 2 355 0.53 1.24 1.28 E -22 SCID-D 2 117 0.76 2.33 1.51 E- 19 DDIS 3 137 0.68 1.83 1.29 E -14 TSC-40 DIS 1 125 0.73 2.14 6.13 E -20 PDEQ 1 231 0.41 0.90 6.86 E -11

Total 26 5916 0.67 1.82 0.0000 Dissociative Experiences Scale 369 females did not appear to be more dissociative than male subjects. As expected, we found a weak association between the age of the subjects and their scores on the DES in 14 studies (N = 2,474). The combined effect size was: Cohen's d= -0.24, which is comparable to a correlation of r = -0.12. Younger subjects appeared to report more dissociative experiences than older subjects, but one has to keep in mind that the DES has not been developed for use with children (Putnam, Helmers, & Trickett, 1993). The youngest subjects were adolescents and they appeared to be responsible for the age effect (see below). In five studies (Berger, Saito et al., 1994; Boon & Draijer, 1993; ROSS, Joshi, & Currie, 1990; Saxe et al., 1994; Torem, Hermanowski, & Curdue, 1992) the associations between level of education and the DES scores appeared to be non- significant. In one study (Dünn, Paolo, Ryan, & Van Fleet, 1993), a correlation of r = -0.10 (p < .05) was found, that is, a higher educational level tended to be associ- ated with somewhat lower DES scores. In the same study, Dünn and his colleagues (1993) found a significant correlation between IQ and the DES (r= -0.28; p< .0005). The reason for the association between higher IQ and lower DES scores may be the reading level the DES requires from the respondents. Paolo, Ryan, Dünn, and Van Fleet (1993) found that minimally a high school reading level was required to under- stand the items of the DES. This may also be one of the explanations for the higher scores of African American subjects on the DES (Dünn et al., 1993). Zatzick, Marmar, Weiss, and Metzler (1994), however, found that initial differences in DES scores between African American, Caucasian, and Hispanic war veterans disappeared after adjusting for war zone stress exposure. More studies are needed to replicate these results and to be able to address these discriminant validity issues meta-analytically. In the studies of Sandberg and Lynn (1992), and De Silva and Ward (1993), the DES was not associated with social desirability äs measured through the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). From the perspective of discriminant validity, psychiatric, and physical disorders without a dissociative component should not be associated with high DES-scores. This issue will be addressed in the last paragraph of this section. It should be noted that the DES has been found to correlate strongly with measures for general distress, for exam- ple, the General Health Questionnaire (Walker, Katon, Neraas, Jemelka, & Massoth, 1992) (r= 0.69, df= 35), the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) F-scale (Dünn et al., 1993) (r= 0.40, df= 263), and the Hopkins Symptom Check List- 90 (HSCL-90) subscales for phobic-anxiety (r= 0.58, df= 117), anger-hostility (r= 0.67, df= 117), somatization (r= 0.66, df= 117), depression (r= 0.62, df= 117) (Norton et al., 1990), and the Global Severity Index (GSI) of the number and intensity of symp- tons experienced (r= 0.67, df= 88) (Zlotnick et al., 1995). Furthermore, the DES was associated with the somatization scale of the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents (r= 0.34, df= 31) (Atias, Wolfson, & Lipschitz, 1995). The DES is also asso- ciated with depression äs assessed with Beck's Depression Inventory (Gleaves & Eberenz, 1995: r = 0.51, df= 123; Segal & Lynn, 1993: r= 0.28, df= 83) but not with depression äs measured by the Hamilton Depression Scale (McCarthy et al., 1994).

Reliabiüty In 16 studies the mean alpha reliability was 0.93. The DES is a highly consistent scale. This may also be derived from studies on the factor structure of the 28 DES-items. Although Bernstein and Putnam (1986) originally suggested three factors (absorp- tion, amnesia, and depersonalization), later studies by Fischer and Elnitsky (1990) and Marmar and colleagues (1994) have shown that only one factor could be derived 370 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel

from the items. Re-analyzing the original Bernstein and Putnam (1986) data, Waller (in press) found that the skewness of the DES distribution may spuriously cause three factors to be derived from the one-dimensional DES. In a Tobit factor analysis the evi- dence in favor of a three-dimensional model was lacking (Waller, Putnam, & Carlson, 1995). In this meta-analytic validation of the DES, data on the DES-subscales will, therefore, not be included. Bernstein and Putnam (1986) found a test-retest correlation of 0.84 in 26 normal subjects across 4-8 weeks. Dubester and Braun (1995) found a test-retest correlation of 0.93 over a period of 2 weeks in their sample of 78 inpatients at a dissociative dis- orders clinic. Frischholz, Braun, Sachs, and Hopkins (1990) found a test-retest corre- lation of 0.93 in 30 patients with a dissociative disorder across a 4-week period. Sanders (1992) found a stability of 0.79 in 46 students across 6-8 weeks, and 0.90 in 16 students across 11 weeks. Putnam, Chu, and Dill (1992) even demonstrated that the DES was quite stable across a 1-year period in a sample of 83 mothers (r= 0.78).

Predictive Validity In Table 2 the data on the association between the DES, and MPD, PTSD, abuse, hyp- notizability, and Eating Disorders have been summarized.

Dissociative Disorders In 19 studies on 1,705 subjects, the relation between the DES and MPD was addressed. The overall effect size was: Cohen's d = 1.05, comparable to a correlation coefficient of r= 0.46. In terms of Cohen's (1988) criteria, this is a large effect size. MPD patients have not only been compared with normal adults or students but also with other psy- chiatric patients, äs well äs with physically impaired patients (seizure disorder). If the studies including normal adults were compared with studies including other compar- ison groups, the combined effect size for the difference between MPD and normale (d = 0.63) was smaller than that for the difference between MPD and the other groups (d = 1.06; p = 0.034). In contrast, the combined effect size for epileptics versus MPD (d = 1.16) was significantly larger than that for the other groups versus MPD (d = 1.02; p =0.01).

TABLE 2. Predictive Validity of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES)

Number Validity Issue of Studies N r Cohen's d />

MPD 19 1705 0.46 1.05 1.46 E -39 PTSD 12 1099 0.35 0.75 2.89 E -21 Abuse 26a 2108 0.25 0.52 1.06 E -21 Sexual 26 1550 0.21 0.42 9.87 E -12 Physical 20 1360 0.21 0.42 7.03 E -10 Sexual/Physical 7 809 0.28 0.58 2.12 E -12 Hypnotizability 10 2513 0.13 0.27 6.83 E -10 Eating disorders 12 1488 0.22 0.44 8.92 E -12 aSome studies were divided into several samples or included tests for the relation between the DES and sexual abuse äs well äs physical or sexual/physical abuse, on the same sample or sev- eral samples within one study. Dissociative Experiences Scale 371

In the meta-analysis on MPD and DES, the DES-scores of MPD patients were some- times compared with the DES-scores of patients with somewhat less severe dissociative disorders such äs DDNOS (Frischholz et al., 1990; Draijer & Boon, 1993). Therefore, a meta-analysis was performed in which all types of dissociative disorders were com- pared with nondissociative groups. The combined Cohen's d= 1.08 (N= 1,895), which is comparable to a correlation of r = 0.47. The type of measure for the dissociative disorder diagnosis was an important pre- dictor of the Variation of effect sizes between the studies. If one of the structured interview schedules for dissociative disorders was used, the combined effect size (d = 1.32) was significantly larger than if other diagnostic criteria such äs the clinical interview were used (d = 0.96; p = 0.015). Publication year or sample size were not related to differences in effect sizes.

Trauma tic Experiences In the states-of-consciousness model of dissociation (Putnam, 1991) a dissociative bias is supposed to be preserved or enhanced by traumatic experiences for which dissoci- ation serves äs a defense mechanism. If the DES measures dissociation, it should be associated with the pathological sequelae of traumatic experiences, such äs combat experiences and abuse. In 12 studies on 1,099 subjects, the relation between the DES and PTSD was investigated. The combined effect size for the 12 studies was: Cohen's d = 0.75 (p= 2.89 E - 21). The combined effect size is quite large and robust. However, the studies on veterans with and without PTSD (Marmar et al., 1994; Bremner, Southwick, Brett, & Fontana, 1992; Hyer, Albrecht, Boudewyns, Woods, & Brandsma, 1993; Orr et al., 1990; Orr, Pitman, Lasko, & Herz, 1993) and Warshaw et al.'s (1993) study with a comparison group that experienced trauma make it clear that trauma may not be a sufficient cause for high DES-scores. Larger samples appeared to yield smaller effect sizes (p = 0.01) (Rosenthal, 1991). Publication year and sex of subjects did not explain Variation of effect sizes between studies. In 26 studies, the association between the DES and physical or sexual abuse experiences has been reported (N= 2,108). The combined effect size was d = 0.52, which was highly significant (p = 1.06 E - 21). It was a medium effect size, which was independent of type of abuse: for sexual abuse d = 0.42, for physical abuse d = 0.42, and for abuse that could have been sexual, physical, or both d - 0.58. Whether ques- tionnaires or Interviews were used to assess abuse made no difference: the 13 ques- tionnaire studies yielded an effect size of d = 0.52, whereas the 11 interview studies showed a combined effect size of d = 0.56 (p = 0.07). Sample sizes were negatively asso- ciated with effect sizes (p = 0.02), that is, smaller samples showed larger effect sizes (Müllen, 1989).

Hypnotizability In the autohypnosis model of dissociation (Bliss, 1986) a relation between hypnotiz- ability and dissociation is assumed to exist. This issue has been investigated in 10 stud- ies (N= 2,513). The combined effect size was quite modest: d=0.27 (p= 6.83 E-10). Most studies included nonclinical subjects, in particular students. The only study with a clinical sample — bulimics — showed a much larger effect size, but further studies on clinical subjects are needed to establish a consistent trend. Because of the modest set of studies and the homogeneity of their results, the predictors are not reported on statistically. One exception may be allowed: In the past it has been hypothesized that the association between hypnotizability and dissociation would be inflated if subjects 372 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel knew that they would participate in a hypnosis study (demand characteristics äs response bias). This effect did indeed seem to exist: In the studies in which there was no control of the potential contamination, the effect size was d = 0.29, whereas in the studies in which the subjects were kept in ignorance of the content of the experiment, the effect size was d = 0.19. The difference was significant (p = 0.014).

Eating Disorders From the very beginning, eating disorders have been linked to dissociative experiences (Covino, Jimerson, Wolfe, Franko, & Frankel, 1994). The dissociative experiences of amnesia, timelessness, depersonalization and derealization are seen äs common char- acteristics of a binge episode (Covino, et al., 1994). There were 12 studies on 1,488 sub- jects investigating the relation between dissociation and eating disorders. The combined effect size for this set of studies amounted to d = 0.44 (p = 8.92 E -12). There were no differences between effect sizes of the studies focusing on bulimia and those focusing on anorexia (p = 0.07). Dissociation tended to be somewhat more strongly related to anorexia than bulimia. It should be noted that in the only study with depressed com- parison subjects the effect size was zero (Greenes, Fava, Cioffi, & Herzog, 1993). Furthermore, in the Valdiserri and Kihlstrorn (1995) study dissociation appeared to be more strongly related to aspects of ego dysfunction than to abnormal eating, per se.

Means of Different Diagnostic Groups In 85 studies on almost 12,000 subjects, the means or medians of different diagnostic groups have been reported. In Figure l, the means of 16 different groups, each based on several samples, are presented. If means were absent, medians were inserted äs a reasonable approximation of the means. This happened for only 10 studies. From Figure l it can be derived that the dissociative disorders scored highest (MPD, DDNOS, and dissociative disorder patients of unknown description) followed by sub- jects with a traumatic history (PTSD and abuse). Subjects with personality disorders, affective disorders, eating disorders, anxiety disorders, schizophrenia, and psychiatric disorders without dissociative components did indeed show much less high means. The seizure disorders group even scored somehat lower than the normal groups, whereas the war veterans scored relatively high. In Table 3, the means of the diagnos- tic groups are presented. If parametric data (means and Standard deviations) were available, they are presented on the right side of the table. These data are required to test whether new samples show comparable or deviating distributions of DES scores. For comparison, the mean score of normal subjects was 11.57 (SD = 10.63), and for students the mean score was 14.27 (SD = 11.54). Sizes of the Standard deviations were strikingly large in all groups except the dissociative disordered categories. Undiscovered dissociation and comorbidity may be the causes for the large Variation of DES scores in these groups. The distribution of means was not bimodal. Against the background of potential comorbidity and undiscovered dissociation, the means for normals and nondissociative patients were remarkably similar. The contrasts between the different groups were not statistically tested, äs the data refer to samples, not to subjects.

DISCUSSION The meta-analytic validation of the DES suggests that this Instrument has many strengths and some weaknesses. The DES shows excellent convergent validity with similar questionnaires and established interview schedules. The DES also shows Dissociative Experiences Scale 373

ίι *J co ω ° * ο5 w i. Π3 1 ω =« üt ^* ο -σ D ·- * ο C/3 co (Ο D_ b •Ό

FIGURE 1. Pooled Means of Diagnostic Croups. (Mean DES-scores for the different diagnostic groups were arrived at by pooling the means [or in a few cascs the medi- an] for the samples that consisted of persons from these groups. All the diagnostic criteria were derived from the DSM-III-R or the DSM-HI. Number of studies on which the means are based are included in parentheses after each diagnostic label.) impressive predictive validity, in particular in the area of dissociative disorders (MPD, DDNOS, ODD) and traumatic experiences (PTSD, abuse). Many severe psychiatric syndromes and disorders (Schizophrenia, Personality Disorders, Anxiety Disorders, Affective Disorders) do not appear to be associated with high scores on the DES. It should be noted, however, that samples with subjects completing the DES without being aware of their Status äs MPD patients (Berger et al., 1994; McCallum, Lock, Kulla, Rorty, & Wetzel, 1992; ROSS, Ryan, Voigt, & Eide, 1991; Sandberg & Lynn, 1992; 374 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel

TABLE 3. Means of Different Diagnostic Groups

Parametric or Nonparametric Data Parametric Data

Number Number Diagnostic Group of Studies N M of Studies N M SD

Anxiety disorder 5 468 10.16 3 407 10.32 9.99 Eating disorder 11 345 14.51 6 203 14.82 11.49 Personality disorder 7 462 16.80 5 349 19.61 16.24 Schizophrenia 4 63 19.10 1 20 17.10 15.30 Affective disorder 3 81 19.43 PTSD 9 259 32.58 4 121 32.01 19.18 DDNOS 6 121 35.29 3 64 39.49 15.83 Diss. disorder unspec. 4 143 41.15 3 140 41.22 21.99 MPD 18 472 45.63 12 355 44.64 20.26 Normal 11 1578 11.05 7 1458 11.57 10.63 Student/ Adolescent 21 5676 14.40 18 4687 14.27 11.54 Seizure disorder 3 130 8.12 1 20 6.70 6.80 Psychiatrie patient 16 1302 16.39 14 1174 16.66 16.41 War Veteran 3 62 20.58 3 62 20.58 21.63 Abused 3 238 27.06 2 141 29.17 20.99 Other 14 541 12.65 Overall 85 11914 16.45

Saxe et al., 1993) show weaker associations between DES and MPD than samples with subjects who know that they are MPD patients (d = 0.53 and d = 1.11 respectively, p = 0.028). It would be preferable if studies with the DES should try to keep the sub- jects in ignorance of the goals of the study, and those of the DES assessment in par- ticular. In many cases, however, this may be impossible because of the popularity of both the concept and measure of dissociation (Hacking, 1995). The discriminant validity äs well äs the reliability of the Instrument deserve even more attention. Some studies reported strong associations between the DES and self- report measures of general psychological and physical distress (e.g., Walker, Katon, Neraas, Jemelka, & Massoth, 1992) which may decrease the instrument's discriminant validity. Few studies addressed the critical issue of response biases affecting the DES, and their results are not unequivocal. Gilbertson, Torem, Cohen, and Newman (1992) demonstrated experimentally that the DES is susceptible to malingering. Normal subjects are able to fake "good" or "bad" on the DES depending on what the experimenter asked them to do. Convicted sex offenders are able to present them- selves äs an extremely normal population when asked to complete the DES (Dwyer, Rosser, & Sawyer, 1992). The questionnaire is usually completed äs a whole, and all of the questions are slanted in the same direction. It is, therefore, clear to the subjects what the Instrument aims at (Wiener, 1992). If dummy items with neutral content are inserted in the DES, the resulting average score for dissociation appears to be several scale points lower in a Student population (Ensink & Van Otterloo, 1989). Furthermore, the DES seems to be somewhat dependent on IQ and it appears to require at least high school reading level. Further research is needed to explore the limits of com- prehensibility of the DES items in disturbed populations. Its generalizability to other races and cultures than U.S. Caucasian persons is not self-evident. Dissociative Experiences Scale 375

To enhance the instrument's reliability and validity and to overcome some of the weaknesses mentioned above, averaging the DES scores over more points in time and over more judges is recommended. Although the DES is a rather stable Instrument, test-retest reliability is not maximal, and averaging across several assessments enhances the systematic variance (Crocker & Algina, 1986). Furthermore, selfjudgments are intrinsically deficient because judgment errors cannot be averaged out. Following Hofstee (1994), the recommended procedure for assessing dissociative experiences is to also give the DES — phrased in the third person singular — to those who know the target person best. In fact, this is in line with the procedure followed in the Child Dissociative Checklist (CDC; Putnam, Helmers, & Trickett, 1993), which is an observ- er report measure of dissociation in children. Recently, Waller et al. (1995) introduced a brief eight-item Version of the DES (DES-T) which would assess the category of pathological dissociative experiences. The DES-T, however, still relies on self-report. The brief questionnaire will be highly transparent äs to what it is meant to assess because it contains the most extreme items, and it will, therefore, be even more susceptible to response biases than the DES. Normal subjects do not indicate to have experienced the kind of extreme dissociative states that the DES-T presents, which seems to be characteristic of the MPD patients. Finding a categorical response pattern seems circular. The PTSD patients, however, show a less clear-cut categorical response pattern, which casts some doubt on the validity of the DES-T, in particular, its ability to discriminate pathological from non- pathological dissociation. If the DES-T is going to be used äs a diagnostic tool instead of a screening device, it should be validated more thoroughly in independent groups. The question remains äs to what exactly the DES measures (Pribor, Yutzy, Dean, & Wetzel, 1993). The validity of the DES, of course, is limited by the validity of the dis- sociation theory on which it is based. The construct of dissociation can easily be over- stretched to include almost every defense mechanism (Frankel, 1990). If dissociation were a "container" concept, the validity of the DES would be problematic. The meta- analysis on hypnotizability shows how the data resist supporting the alleged link between dissociation and hypnosis. The two phenomena may show some superficial, phenotypical similarities, the underlying dynamics may, however, be very different. The same is true for eating disorders. The rather modest meta-analytic effect size for the DES and eating disorders should not be interpreted to mean that eating disorders are some type of dissociative disorder (Covino et al., 1994). An alternative explana- tion would be that, in some patients, childhood abuse experiences lead to an eating disorder äs well äs to a dissociative disorder. The study by McCarthy et al. (1994) sug- gests that this may be the case. Dissociation and binge eating or substance abuse may also be alternative but distinct strategies to distract and separate oneself from the rec- ollection of traumatic experiences. Because the DES is an efficient and convenient measure, researchers are tempted to include the DES routinely in their clinical stud- ies. This may occasionally lead to "significant" results without a clear theoretical ratio- nale, and this approach equals the proverbial "fishing expedition." Putnam (1991) described four theoretical models of dissociation. The autohypno- sis model (Bliss, 1986), which conceptualizes clinical dissociation äs self-hypnosis gone awry, does seem to be somewhat problematic in view of the rather modest rela- tion between the DES and scales for hypnotizability. The neurological model, which emphasizes the neurological substratum of dissociative experiences, does not receive support from several studies on dissociation in seizure disorders but research is lack- ing on other neurological disturbances. The Strategie role enactment or malingering model has found partial support in studies showing that respondents are able to fake 376 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel

"good" or "bad" depending on what the Situation requires. However, it is difficult to believe that the large differences between the dissociative disorders groups and the other psychiatrically disturbed groups can be explained solely by referring to malin- gering or response set. The states-of-consciousness model, which proposes that the core phenomena of dis- sociative disorders such äs the amnesias and disturbances of sense of seif, arise out of traumatically induced disruptions of capacities for modulation of states of consciousness and Integration of seif across highly discrete states of consciousness (Putnam, 1991), has, in fact, not been tested thoroughly. Behavioral genetic studies on the presumed innate bias for dissociative disorders are absent, and litüe is known about strong tendencies to dissociate in childhood. The link between traumatic experiences and dissociation is not based on convincing data that is independent of the respondents' autobiographical memory. In one of the few studies in which abuse was derived from hospital records, the relation between the DES and abuse was negative (Sanders & Giolas, 1991). In this respect, it may be counterproductive to identify the DES with the frequency of dissocia- tive experiences äs they really happened. Instead, the DES seems to assess the respon- dents' current perception of past (dissociative) experiences. This perception may change in the course of working through negative experiences (Warshaw et al., 1993). This is in line with current äs well äs older perspectives on autobiographical memory, which describe "normal" autobiographical memory äs the continuous reconstraction of past experiences in the light of new experiences. It may often be a better reflection of current views and personality than of objective past events (Rubin, Wetzler, & Nebes, 1986; Van Der Kolk & Van Der Hart, 1991; Wagenaar, 1991). If the DES is interpreted äs measuring "perceived dissociative experiences," the cru- cial question would logically arise how childhood dissociative experiences accumulate to evolve into a dissociative disorder. In speculating about a developmental model of dis- sociation we would like to draw on attachment theoiy (Bowlby, 1985; Main & Hesse, 1990; Liotti, 1992). In attachment theoiy there is a growing body of research on the effects of parental trauma. Parents who have experienced loss of an attachment figure or another potential trauma, and who show through their discourse about those past experiences that they are still unresolved and disoriented with respect to these negative experiences, appear to raise children who display disorganized and disoriented — dis- sociative — behavior in stressful situations (Main & Hesse, 1990; Van IJzendoorn, 1995). It is speculated that the parents' uncontrolled remembrances or reliving of episodes of past traumatic experiences in the presence of their children may unad- vertently be frightening for these children, who at the same time have to rely on these parents äs their primary haven of security (Main & Hesse, 1990). To escape from this paradoxical Situation, the children are assumed to enter a momentary state of disor- ganization of their own that may develop into a tendency to dissociate under stressful circumstances. The developmental bias to dissociate may in its turn prepare the ground for dissociation äs a defense mechanism in confrontation with personal expe- riences of loss or other trauma. The developmental model would explain why some people develop dissociative disorders in response to trauma and others do not. Only prospective longitudinal studies may provide evidence for the developmental model. At present there is no evidence for causal relationships between dissociation and other phenomena. The designs are mainly correlational and retrospective, relying almost exclusively on autobiographical memory. For example, the association between the DES and PTSD appears to be a replicated fact. How should this fact be interpreted? Three variables are involved: traumatic experience, the DES^core, and the PTSD diagnosis. Without knowing what the subjects' Status was before the trau- DissociativeExpenences Scale 377 matic experience, a myriad of causal models are possible. The traumatic experience may, for example, trigger a dissociative tendency which in the short run ameliorates the trauma. In the long run, however, it may hamper the constructive processing of the traumatic experiences. The simple causal model would be: Trauma—»Dissociation —>PTSD. Another possibility would be that a traumatic experience provokes dissocia- tion, and independent of this short-term defense mechanism, it may also result in PTSD in the long run. In that case, the association between the DES and PTSD may be strong but spurious. Yet, another causal model would mclude a causal effect of trauma on PTSD, äs well äs a causal effect of PTSD on the DES (for example because the diagnosis and thera- py of PTSD would stimulate scoring high on the DES): Trauma—»PTSD—>Dissociation. Only prospective, longitudinal, and quasi-experimental studies may shed light on the problem of causality. As yet, these studies are few (see Malinoskyrummell & Hoier, 1991, for a study that compared sexually abused children with nonabused children on observer-rating scales of dissociation) To our knowledge there are no studies of fac- tors that predict individual differences in dissociative reactivity to similar forms of childhood trauma. Expenmental Intervention studies that systematically change the level of dissociation in subjects with a traumatic experience to see how this affects the development of PTSD (or other dissociative disorders) have not yet been published either. It is this type of longitudinal and experimental studies that may move the study of the DES — and dissociation in general — beyond the current descnptive and exploratory stage. This may very well require the development of a second generation (Putnam et al., 1993) of Instruments to measure dissociation, beyond the current pre- dominance of self-report questionnaires.

Acknowledgements — This study was supported by a PIONEER award from the Netherlands Orgamzation for Scientific Research (NWO, grant no PCS 59-256 to the first author)

REFERENCES Studies preceded by an asterisk (*) were included in the meta-analysis. Studies pre- ceded by a plus sign (+) were included in the computation of mean DES scores of the different diagnostic groups.

American Psychiatrie Associaüon (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed ) Washington, DC Author +Anderson, G .Yasemk, L , & ROSS, C A (1993) Dissociative experiences and disorders among women who idenüfy themselves äs sexual abuse survivors and Neglect, 17, 677-686 +Armstrong,J G , & Loewenstem, R J (1990) Charactensücs of paüents with multiple personality and dis sociative disorders on psychological tesüng Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 178, 448—454 +*Atlas, J A , & Hiott, J (1994) Dissociative experience in a group of adolescents with history of abuse Perceptual and Motor Stalls, 78, 121-122 +*A(las,J A , Wolfson, M A , & Lipschitz, D S (1995) Dissociation and somaüzation in adolescent mpa- üents with and without hislory of abuse Psychological Reports, 76,1101-1102 +Berenbaum, H , & James, T (1994) Correlates and retrospectively reported antecedents of alexithymia Psychosomalic , 56, 353—359 +Berger, D, Ono, Y, Nakajima, K, & Suematsu, H (1994) Dissociative Symptoms in Japan [letter] Amencan Journal of , 151, 148-149 *Berger, D , Saito, S , Ono, Y, Tezuka, I , ShirahaseJ , Kuboki, T, & Suematsu, H (1994) Dissociaüon and child abuse histones in an eatmg disorder cohort in Japan Acta Psychiatnca Scandinamca, 90, 274-280 +*Bernstem, E M , & Putnam, F W (1986) Development, rehability and validity of a dissociation scale Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 727-735 378 ΜΗ van IJzendoorn and C Schwengel

Bhss, E L (1986) Multiple personahty, attwd disorders, and hypnosis New York Oxford Umversity Press +Boon, S , & Drayer, N (1993) Multiple personahty disorder m the Netherlands A chnical investigation of 71 patients Amencan Journal of Psychialry, 150, 489-494 +Boon, S , & Drayer, N (1993) Multiple personahty disorder m the Netherlands A study on reliabihty and validi- ty ofthe diagnoszs Amsterdam Swets & Zeidinger Bowlby.J (1985) Attachment and Loss Vol 3 Loss Sadness and depression London Penguin Books +Bowman, E S , & Coons, P M (1992) The use of electroconvulsive therapy m patients widi dissociative disorders Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 180, 524—528 +Bowman, E S (1993) Etiology and chnical course of pseudoseizures Relaüonship to trauma, depression, and dissociation Psychosomatics, 34, 333-342 +*Branscomb, L P (1991) Dissociation in combat-related posttraumaüc stress disorder Dissociation Pmgress in the Dissociative Disorders, 4, 13—20 +*Bremner, J D , Soudiwick, S , Brett, E , & Fontana, A. (1992) Dissociation and posttraumaüc stress dis- order m Vietnam combat veterans Amencan Journal of Psychialry, 149, 328—332 Campbell, D (1960) Recommendations for APA lest Standards Amencan Psychologtst, 15, 546-553 Carlson, E B , Putnam, F W, ROSS, C A , & Torem, M (1993) Validity of the Dissociative Expenences Scale m screenmg for multiple personality disorder A multicenter study Amencan Journal of Psychtalry, 150, 1030-1036 +*Carlson, E B , & Rosserhogan, R (1991) Trauma expenences, posttraumaüc stress, dissociaüon, and depression in Cambodian refugees Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1548-1551 +*Chu,J A, & Dill, D L (1990) Dissociative Symptoms in relaüon to childhood physical and sexual abuse Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 887-892 Cohen.J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences New York Academic Press Coons, P M (1992) Dissociative disorder not otherwise specified A chnical mvesügaüon of 50 cases with suggesüons for typology and treatment Dissociation Pmgress in the Dissociative Disorders, 5, 187—195 +*Coons, P M , Bowman, E S , Pellow, T A., & Schneider, P (1989) Post-traumaüc aspects ofthe ü-eatment of vicüms of sexual abuse and incest Psychiatnc Clinics ofNorth Amenca, 12, 325—335 +*Covmo, N A , Jimerson, D C , Wolfe, B E , Franko, D L , & Frankel, F H (1994) Hypnoüzabihty, dis- sociation, and buhmia nervosa Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 455-459 Crocker, L , & Algina, J (1986) Introduction to classical and modern lest theory (13th ed ) Fort Worth Holt, Rinehard & Wmston Crowne, D P,&Marlowe,D (1960) A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349—354 +De Silva, P, & Ward, A J M (1993) Personality correlates of dissociative expenences Personality and Indivtdual Differences, 14, 857-859 Demitrack, M A , Putnam, F W, Brewerton, T D , & Brandt, H A (1990) Relaüon of chnical variables to dissociaüve phenomena m eaüng disorders Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1184—1188 +*Demitrack, M A, Putnam, F W, Rubmow, D R, & Pigott, T A (1993) Relaüon of dissociaüve phe- nomena to levels of cerebrospmal fluid monoamme metabolities and beta-endorphin in paüents with eat- ing disorders A pilot study Psychiatry Research, 49, 1-10 +*Devmsky, O , Putnam, F, Grafman, J , Bromfield, E , & Theodore, W H (1989) Dissociaüve states and epilepsy Neurohgy, 39, 835-840 +*DiTomasso, M J , & Routh, D K. (1993) Recall of abuse in childhood and three measures of dissocia- üon Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 477-485 *Draijer, N , & Boon, S (1993) The vahdaüon ofthe dissociative expenences scale agamst the cntenon of the SCID-D usmg receiver operaüng charactensücs (ROC) analysis Dissociation Pmgress m the Dissociative Disorders, 6, 28-37 Dubester, K. A, & Braun, B G (1995) Psychometnc propcrües of the dissociaüve expenences scale Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 183, 231-235 +Dunn, G E , Paolo, A M , Ryan, J J , & Van Fleet, J (1993) Dissociaüve Symptoms in a substance abuse populaüon Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1043-1047 +Dwyer, S M , Rosser, B S , & Sawyer, S (1992) Dissociaüve expenences of sexual offenders A compan- son between two outpaüent groups and those found to be falsely accused Special Issue Sex offender treatment Psychological and medical approaches Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, 18, 49-58 +*Eisen, M (1993) Assessing the hypnoüzabihty of College students from addicüve famihes Contemporary Hypnosis, 10, 11-17 +Ensmk, B J (1992) Confusingreahties A study on child sexual abuse and psychiatnc Symptoms Amsterdam VU Umversity Press +*Ensmk, B J , & Van Otterloo, D (1989) A vahdaüon study of the DES m the Netherlands Dtssociation Pmgress m the Dissociative Disorders, 2, 221-223 Dissociative Expenences Scale 379

+*Evenll,J , Waller, G , & Macdonald, W (1995) Dissociaüon in bulimic and noneatmg-disordered women International Journal ofEating Disorders, 17, 127—134 +*Faith, M , & Ray, W J (1994) Hypnoüzabihty and dissociation in a College age populaüon — Orthogonal mdividual differences Personality andIndmdualDifferences, 77,211-216 *Fischer, D G , & Elmtsky, S (1990) A factor analyüc study of two scales measunng dissociaüon Amencan Journal of Chnical Hypnosis, 32, 201-207 Frankel, F H (1990) Hypnotizabihty and Dissociaüon AmericanJournalofPsychiatry, 147, 823-829 +Frischholz, E J , Braun, B G , Lipman, L S , & Sachs, R (1992) Suggested posthypnoüc amnesia m psy- chiatnc patients and normals Amencan Journal of Chnical Hypnosis, 35, 29—39 +*Fnschholz, E J , Braun, B G , Sachs, R G , & Hopkms, L (1990) The Dissociative Expenences Scale Further replication and validation Dissoaation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 3, 151—153 *Fnschholz, E J , Braun, B G , Sachs, R G , & Schwartz, D R (1991) Construct validity of the Dissociative Expenences Scale (DES) I The relationship between die DES and other self-report measures of DES Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 4, 185—188 *Fnschholz, E J , Braun, B G , Sachs, R G ,& Schwartz, D R (1992) Construct validity of die Dissociative Expenences Scale II Its relationship to hypnotizabihty Amencan Journal ofChnical Hypnosis, 35, 145—152 +*Gilbertson, A D , Torem, M , Cohen, R , & Newman, I (1992) Susceptibihty of common self-report mea- sures of dissociaüon to malmgenng Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 5, 216—220 *Gleaves, D H , & Eberenz, K. P (1995) Assessing dissociaüve Symptoms in eaüng disordered paüents — Construct validation of two seif report measures International Journal ofEating Disorders, 18, 99—102 +*Goff,D C , Brotman, A W, Kmdlon, D , & Waites, M (1991) Self-reports of childhood abuse m chron- ically psychoüc paüents Psychiatry Research, 37, 73—80 +Goff, D C , Ohn, J A , Jenike, M A , Baer, L , & Buttolph, M L (1992) Dissociaüve Symptoms m paüents with obsessive-compulsive disorder Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 180, 332-337 +*Goldner, E M , Cockhill, L A., Bakan, R , & Birmingham, C L (1991) Dissociaüve expenences and eat mg disorders Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1274—1275 +*Greenes, D , Fava, M , Cioffi, J , & Herzog, D B (1993) The relaüonship of depression to dissociaüon m paüents with bulimia nervosa Journal of Psychiatrie Research, 27, 133—137 Hacking, I (1995) Reumting the soul Multiple personahty and the sciences of memory Pnnceton, NJ Pnnceton Umversity Press +Heber, A S , Fleisher, W P, ROSS, C A , & Stanwick, R S (1989) Dissociaüon m alternaüve healers and tradiüonal therapists A comparaüve sttidy Amencan Journal of Psychotherapy, 43, 562—574 +*Herzog, D B , Staley, J E , Carmody, S , & Robbms, W M (1993) Childhood sexual abuse m anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa A pilot study Journal ofthe Amencan Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32, 962-966 Hofstee, W K. B (1994) Who should own the deflmüon of personality? European Journal of Personality, 8, 149-162 +Horen, S A, Leichner, P P, &Lawson,J S (1995) Prevalence of dissociaüve Symptoms and disorders in an adult psychiatnc mpaüent populaüon in Canada Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, 185—191 +Hughes, D J (1992) Differences between trance channeling and mulüple personality disorder on struc- tured interview Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 24, 181—192 +*Hyer, L A , Albrecht,J W, Boudewyns, P A , Woods, M G , & Brandsma, J (1993) Dissociaüve expen- ences of Vietnam veterans with chronic posttraumaüc stress disorder Psychological Reports, 73, 519—530 *Kirby,J S,Chu,J A, & Dill, D L (1993) Correlates of dissociaüve symptomatology m paüents with phys- ical and sexual abuse histones Comprehenswe Psychiatry, 34, 258—263 +Kunzendorf, R G, & Marsden, D (1991) Dissociaüon m ambidextrous students Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73, 778 +*Lauer, J , Black, D W, & Keen, P (1993) Mulüple personality disorder and borderlme personality dis- order Disünct enüües of vanaüons on a common theme' Annals of Chnical Psychiatry, 5, 129-134 +Leavitt, F (1994) Chnical correlates of alleged satanic abuse and less controversial sexual molestaüon Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 387-392 +Leavitt, F, & Braun, B (1991) Histoncal rehability A key to differenüaüng populaüons among paüents presenüng signs of mulüple personality disorder Psychological Reports, 69, 499—510 Liotü, G (1992) Disorgamzed/disonented attachment in the eüology of the dissociaüve disorders Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 5, 196-204 +*Loewenstem, R J , & Putnam, F W (1988) A companson study of dissociaüve Symptoms in paüents with complex parüal seizures, MPD, and posttraumaüc stress disorder Dissociation Progress m the Dissociative Disorders, l, 17-23 Main, M , & Hesse, E (1990) Parents' unresolved traumaüc expenences are related to infant disorganized attachement Status Is fnghtened and/or fnghtening parental behavior the hnking mechanism' In M T 380 M H van IJzendoorn and C Schuengel

Greenberg, D Cicchetü, & E M Cummmgs (Eds ), Attachment in the preschool years Theory, research and Intervention (pp 161-182) Chicago The University of Chicago Press Malmoskyrummell, R R , & Hoier, T S (1991) Vahdating measures of dissociation in sexually abused and nonabused children Behavior Assessment, 75,341-357 +*Marmar, C R , Weiss, D S , Schienger, W E , Fairbank, J A .Jordan, B K., Kulka, R A., & Hough, R L (1994) Pentraumaüc dissociation and posttraumaüc stress in male Vietnam theater veterans Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 902-907 +*McCallum, K. E , Lock, J , Kulla, M , Rorty, M , & Wetzel, R D (1992) Dissociative Symptoms and disor- ders in patients with eating disorders Dissociation Pmgress in the Dissociative Disorders, 5, 227-235 +*McCarthy, M K., Goff, D C , Baer, L , Cioffi, J , & Herzog, D B (1994) Dissociation, childhood trauma, and the response to fluoxeüne in bulimic patients International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 219—226 +*Miller, D A F, Mccluskeyfawcett, K., & Irvmg, L M (1993) The relaüonship between childhood sexu- al abuse and subsequent onset of bulimia nervosa Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 305-314 Müllen, B (1989) Advanced basic meta-analysts Hillsdale, NJ Erlbaum *Nadon, R , Hoyt, I P, Register, P A, & Kihlstrom, J F (1991) Absorption and hypnoüzabihty Context effects reexammed Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 144—153 *Norton, G R , ROSS, C A,&Novotny, M F (1990) Factors that predict scores on the Dissociative Expenences Scale Journal of Clmical Psycholog), 46, 273-277 Nunnally,J C (1978) Prpchometnc theory (2nd ed ) New York McGraw-Hill +Ogles, B M , Lynn, S J , & Masters, K. S (1993) Runners' cogmtive strategies and motivations Absorption, fantasy style, and dissociative expenences Imagination, Cognitwn and Personality, 13, 163-174 +*Orr, S P, Claiborn, J M , Altman, B , Forgue, D F, Dejong, J B , Pitman, R K., & Herz, L R (1990) Psychometnc profile of posttraumatic-stress-disorder, anxious, and healthy Vietnam veterans — Correlations with psychophysiological responses Journal of Consulting and Clmical Psycholog), 58, 329-335 +*Orr, S P, Pitman, R K., Lasko, N B , & Herz, L R (1993) Psychophysiological assessment of posttrau- maüc-slress-disorder imagery in World War II and Korean combat veterans Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 152-159 Paolo, A M , Ryan,J J , Dünn, G E ,& Van Fleet, J (1993) Readmg level of the Dissociative Expenences Scale Journal of Clmical Psychology, 49, 209-211 +*Pnbor, E F.Yutzy, S H , Dean,J T, & Wetzel, R D (1993) Bnquet's syndrome, dissociation, and abuse Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1507—1511 Putnam, F W (1991) Dissociative phenomena In A Tasman & S M Goldfinger (Eds ), Amencan psychi- atnc press review ofpsychialry (Vol 10, pp 145—160) Washington, DC Amencan Psychiatrie Press Putnam, F W, Helmers, K., & Tnckett, P K. (1993) Development, rehabihty, and validity of a child disso- ciation scale Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 731-741 Putnam, F W, Chu,J A ,& Dill, D L (1992) "The Dissociative Expenences Scale" Reply Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 143-144 •fQuimby, L G , & Putnam, F W (1991) Dissociative Symptoms and aggression in a state mental hospital Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 4, 21—24 *Ray, W J , & Faith, M (1995) Dissociative expenences in a College age populaüon Follow-up with 1,190 subjects Personality and Indwidual Differences, 75,223—230 *Ray, W J , June, K., Turaj, K., & Lundy, R (1992) Dissociative expenences in a College age populaüon A factor analyüc study of two dissociation scales Personality and Indnndual Differences, 13, 417-424 +Richards, D G (1991) A study of the correlations between subjecüve psychic expenences and dissocia- tive expenences Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 4, 83—91 +Richards, P, & Pcrsinger, M A (1991) Temporal lobe signs, the dissociative expenences scale and the hemisphenc quoüent Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72,1139-1142 Riley, K. C (1988) Measurement of dissociation Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 776,449—450 +*Rosen, E F,&Petty, L C (1994) Dissociative states and disordered eating Amencan Journal ofdinical Hypnosis, 36, 266-275 Rosenthal, R (1991) Meta analytic pmcedures ffor social research Beverly Hills, CA Sage +*Ross, C A, & Anderson, G (1988) Phenomenological overlap of multiple personality disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 295-299 +Ross, C A , Anderson, G , Fleishcr, W P, & Norton, G R (1991) The frequency of multiple personality disorder among psychiatnc inpaüents (Scventh Annual International Conference on Multiple Personality/ Dissociaüve States, 1990, Chicago, IL) Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1717-1720 +*Ross, C A , Anderson, G , Heber, S ,& Norton, G R (1990) Dissociaüon and abuse among multiple-per- sonahty patients, prostitules, and exotic dancers Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 328—330 +*Ross, C A , Fast, E , Anderson, G , & Auty, A (1990) Somatic Symptoms in multiple sclerosis and MPD Dissociation Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 3, 102—106 Dissociative Expenences Scale 381

+*Ross, C A , Heber, S , Andersen, G , & Norton, G R (1989) Differenüaüng mulüple personahty disor- der and complex partial seizures General Hospital Psychiatry, 11, 54-58 ROSS, C A, Heber, S , Norton, G R , Anderson, D , Anderson, G , & Barcher, P (1989) The dissociaüve dis- orders interview schedule A structured interview Dissociation Pmgress tn the Dissociatwe Disarders, 2,169—189 +Ross, C A., Joshi, S, & Curne, R (1990) Dissociaüve expenences in the general populaüon Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 1547-1552 +Ross, C A, Kronson, J , Koensgen, S , Barkman, K., ClarK, P, & Rockman, G (1992) Dissociaüve comor- bity m 100 chemically dependent paüents Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 43, 840-842 +Ross, C A , Miller, S D , Reagor, P, & Bjornson, L (1990) Structured interview data on 102 cases of mul- tiple personahty disorder from four centers Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 596—601 +*Ross, C A, Norton, G R , & Anderson, G (1988) The Dissociaüve Expenences Scale A rephcaüon study Dissociatton Pmgress tn the Dissociative Disorders, l, 21—22 +Ross, C A , Ryan, L , Anderson, G , & ROSS, D (1989) Dissociaüve expenences in adolescents and College students Dissociation Pmgress in the Dissociattve Disorders, 2, 239-242 *Ross, C A , Ryan, L , Voigt, H , & Eide, L (1991) High and low dissociators m a College Student popula- üon Dissociation Pmgress m the Dissociative Disorders, 4, 147—151 Rubin, D C.Wetzler, S E , & Nebes, R D (1986) Autobiographical memory across the hfespan In D C Rubin (Ed ), Autobiographical memory (pp 202-221) Cambridge Cambndge Umversity Press +Rynearson, E K., & McCreery.J M (1993) Bereavement after homicide A synergism of trauma and loss Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 258—261 +*Sandberg, D A, & Lynn, S J (1992) Dissociaüve expenences, psychopathology and adjustment, and child and adolescent maltreatment m female College students Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 717—723 Sanders, B (1992) "The Dissociaüve Expenences Scale" Reply Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 144 +*Sanders, B , & Giolas, M H (1991) Dissociaüon and childhood trauma in psychologically disturbed ado- lescents Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 50—54 +*Sanders, B , McRoberts, G , & Tollefson, C (1989) Childhood stress and dissociaüon in a College popu- laüon Dissociatton Pmgress m the Dissociatwe Disorders, 2, 17—23 Sanders, S (1986) The Perceptual Alteraüon Scale a scale measunng dissociaüon Amencan Journal of Climcal Hypnosis, 29, 95-102 *Saunders, E A (1991) Rorschach mdicators of chronic childhood sexual abuse in female borderlme mpaüents Bulletin ofthe Mennmger Chnic, 55, 48-71 *Saxe, G N , Van der Kolk, B A , Berkowitz, R , & Chmman, G (1993) Dissociaüve disorders in psychiatnc mpaüents (144th Annual Meeüng of the Amencan Psychiatnc Associaüon, 1991, New Orleans, LA) Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1037-1042 Saxe, G N , Van der Kolk, B A., Berkowitz, R, Chmman, G , Hall, K., Lieberg, G , & Schwartz, J (1994) Diagnosmg the dissociaüve disorders — Reply Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1251 *Segal, D , & Lynn, S J (1993) Predicüng dissociaüve expenences Imagmaüon, hypnoüzability, psy- chopathology, and alcohol use Imagination, Cognitum and Personality, 12, 287—300 +*Shearer, S L (1994) Dissociaüve phenomena m women with borderlme personality disorder Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 1324-1328 *Silva, C E , & Kirsch, I (1992) Interpreüve sets, expectancy, fantasy proneness, and dissociaüon äs pre- dictors of hypnoüc response Journal of Personahty and Social Psychology, 63, 847—856 +*Smyser, C H ,& Baron, D A (1993) Hypnoüzability, absorpüon, and subscales of the dissociaüve expe- nences scale m a nonchmcal populaüon Dissociation Pmgress in the Dissociatwe Disorders, 6, 42—46 *Spanos, N P, Arango, M , & Degroot, H P (1993) Context äs a moderator m relaüonships between attnbute vanables and hypnoüzability Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 71-77 Spiegel, D (1991) Dissociaüon and trauma In A Tasman & S M Goldfinger (Eds ), Amencan psychiatnc press retnew ofpsychiatry (Vol 10, pp 261-266) Washington, DC Amencan Psychiatnc Press Steinberg, M (1994) Structured clmical interview forDSM-Γνdissociatwe disorders — Revised (SCID-D, 2nd ed ) Washington, DC Amencan Psychiatnc Press +*Stemberg, M , Rounsaville, B , & Cicchetü, D (1991) Detecüon of dissociaüve disorders m psychiatnc paüents by a screenmg Instrument and a structured diagnosüc interview Amencan Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1050-1054 +*Stnck, F L , & Wilcoxon, S A. (1991) A companson of dissociative expenences in adult female outpaüents with and without histones of early incestuous abuse Dissociation Pmgress m the Dissoaative Disorders, 4,193-199 +*Swett, C , & Halpert, M (1993) Reported history of physical and sexual abuse in relaüon to dissociaüon and other symptomatology m women psychiatnc mpaüents Journal of Interpersonal Vwlence, 8, 545—555 +Tanabe, H , & Ogawa, T (1992) On the measurement of dissociaüve expenences Develpment of the dis- sociaüve expenences scale with a populaüon of Japanese umversity students Tsukuba Psychology Research, 14, 171-178 382 M H van IJzendoorn and C Schwengel

Teilegen, A , & Atkmson, G (1974) Openness to absorbmg and seif altenng expenences ("absorption") A trait related to hypnoüc susceptibihty Journal of Abnormal Psycholog), 83, 268-277 +*Torem, M S, Hermanowski, R W, & Curdue, K. J (1992) Dissociation phenomena and age Stress Medicme, 8, 23-25 +*Valdisern, S, & Kihlstrom, J F (1995) Abnormal eaüng and dissociaüve expenences International Journal of Eatmg Disorders, 17, 373-380 Van IJzendoorn, M H (1995) Adult attachment representaüons, parental responsiveness, and mfant attachment A meta-analysis on the predicüve validity of the Adult Attachment Interview Psychological Bulletin, 777,387-403 *Van der Kolk, B A., Perry,J C,&Herman,J L (1991) Childhood ongins of self-destrucüve behavior Amencan Journal of Psychtatry, 148, 1665—1671 Van der Kolk, B A, & Van der Hart, O (1991) The mtrusive past The flexibility of memory and the engraving of trauma Amencan Imago, 48, 425—454 Vanderhnden, J , Van Dyck, R , Vandereycken, W, Vertommen, H , & Verkes, R J (1993) The dissociation quesüonnaire (DIS-Q) Development and charactensücs of a new self-report questionnaire Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, l, 21-27 Wagenaar, W A (1991) Remembenng my worst sms How autobiographical memory serves the updating of the conceptual seif In M A Conway, D C Rubin, H Spinnler, & W A. Wagenaar (Eds ), Theoretical perspectives on autobwgraphical memory (pp 263—274) Dordrecht Kluwer +*Waldinger, R J , Swett, C , Frank, A , & Miller, K. (1994) Levels of dissociaüon and histones of reported abuse among women outpatients Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 182, 625—630 +*Walker, E A , Katon, W J , Neraas, K., Jemelka, R P, & Massoth, D (1992) Dissociaüon m women with chronic pelvic pain Amencan Journal of Psychtatry, 149, 534—537 Waller, N G (in press) The Dissociative Expenences Scale Twelflh Mental Measurements Yearbook Lincoln, NE Büros Institute of Mental Measurement Waller, N G , Putnam, F W, & Carlson, E B (1995) Types of dissociatwn and dtssociattve types A taxometnc analysis of dissociative expenences Unpublished manuscnpt +*Warshaw, M G , Fierman, E , Pratt, L , Hunt, M , Yonkers, K. A, Massion, A O , & Keller, M B (1993) Quality of hfe and dissociation in anxiety disorder paüents with histones of trauma or PTSD Amencan Journal of Psychtatry, 150, 1512-1516 Wiener, A. (1992) The Dissociaüve Expenences Scale Amencan Journal of Psychtatry, 149, 143 Wogan, M (1995) The Bltss scale Development, reliabihty, and validity Unpublished manuscnpt Zatzick, D F,Marmar, C R.Weiss, D S , & Metzlcr, T (1994) Does traumahnked dissociaüon vary across ethnic groups' Journal ofNervous and Mental Disease, 182, 576—582 +Zlotmck, C , Shea, M T, Zaknski, A , Costello, E , Begin, A, Pearlstem, T, & Simpson, E (1995) Stressors and close relaüonships dunng childhood and dissociaüve expenences m survivors of sexual abuse among mpaüent psychiatnc women Comprehensive Psychtatry, 36, 207—212 +*Zweigfrank, H , Pans, J , & Guzder, J (1994) Dissociation m male paüents with borderhne and nonbor- derhne personality disorders Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 210—218 +*Zweigfrank, H , Pans, J , & Guzder, J (1994) Dissociaüon in femalc paüents with borderlme and non borderhne personality disorders Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 203—209