Meta-Analytic Validation of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (Des)

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Meta-Analytic Validation of the Dissociative Experiences Scale (Des) Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 16, No. 5, pp. 365-382, 1996 Copyright © 1996 Eisevier Science Ltd Pergamon Printed in the USA. All rights reserved 0272-7358/96 $15.00 + .00 PII 80272-7358(96)00006-2 THE MEASUREMENT OF DISSOCIATION IN NORMAL AND CLINICAL POPULATIONS: META-ANALYTIC VALIDATION OF THE DISSOCIATIVE EXPERIENCES SCALE (DES) Marinus H. van IJzendoorn and Carlo Schuengel Center for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, The Netherlands ABSTRACT. The Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) hos now been used in over 100 studies on dissociation. This article reports on a series of meta-analyses to test some of the theoretical assumptions underlying the DES and to examine the instrument's reliability and validity. Studies wilh the DES were identißed through Psychlit, Medline, Social Sciences Citation Index, and Current Contents. Across sludies in similar domains (e.g., studies on multiple personality disorders) combined effect sizes were computed using the Rosenthal-Mullen approach. The DES showed excellent convergent validity wilh other dissociative experiences questionnaires and inter- view schedules (combined effect size: d = 1.82; N = 5,916). The DES also showed impressive pre- dictive validity, in particular concerning dissociative disorders (Multiple Personality Disorder: combined effect size d = 1.05; N = l, 705) and traumatic experiences (post-traumatic stress dis- order: combined effect size d = 0.75; N = 1,099; and abuse: combined effect size d = 0.52; N = 2,108). However, the discriminant validity was less well established. The DES is sensitive to response and experimenter biases. It is recommended to average DES-scores over more points in time and over morejudges. The DES seems to measure the current view on past dissociative expe- riences. The model of dissociation äs a form of autohypnosis failed to receive support from the data. A developmental model to Interpret dissociation is proposed. DISSOCIATION IS OFTEN considered a psychological defense mechanism for vic- tims of traumatizing events such äs sexual molestation, natural disaster, or combat (Putnam, 1991). It is suggested that it allows these victims to compartmentalize per- ceptions and memories, and to detach themselves from the füll impact of the trauma. At a later stage, dissociation may delay the necessary working through of the traumatic experiences (Spiegel, 1991). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatrie Association, 1994) the essential feature of Dissociative Correspondence should be addressed to Professor Marinus van IJzendoorn, Center for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, P.O. Box 9555, NL-2300 RB Leiden, The Netherlands. 365 366 M. H. van IJzendoorn and C. Schuengel Disorders has been defmed äs a disruption in the usually integrated functions of con- sciousness, memory, identity, and perception of the environment Bernstein and Putnam (1986) developed a self-report measure to assess the degree to which subjects experience dissociation — the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES). It consists of 28 items, and subjects are asked to indicate on a visual analogue scale which percentage of the time they experience dissociative Symptoms. The scale items were con- structed using data from Interviews meeting DSM-III criteria for dissociative disorders and from consultations with clinical experts in the treatment of dissociative disorders. The DES has been used äs a screening Instrument, distinguishing potential dissociative- disorder patients from other psychiatric patients, and äs a research Instrument. Is it possible to measure individual differences in dissociative experiences reliably and validly by means of a self-report Instrument such äs the DES? This question is central to the current meta-analytic validation of the DES. First, we assessed the instrument's con- vergent validity across studies. Several questionnaires and interview schedules have been developed to measure dissociation, dissociative experiences, or related phenomena. Questionnaires, such äs the Perceptual Alteration Scale (PAS; Sanders, 1986), the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS; Teilegen & Atkinson, 1974), the Questionnaire of Experiences of Dissociation (QED; Riley, 1988), the Bliss-scale (Wogan, 1995), the Dissociative Questionnaire (DIS-Q; Vanderlinden, Van Dyck, Vandereycken, Vertommen, & Verkes, 1993), and the dissociation scale of the Trauma Symptom Checklist (TSG40- DIS; Gleaves & Eberenz, 1995), have somewhat different formats and content but their authors all claim to assess some aspect of dissociation or a related phenomenon. Interviews based on the DSM-III-R criteria for dissociative disorders, such äs the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D; Steinberg, 1994) and the Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule (DDIS; ROSS, Heber, Norton, Andersen, Anderson, & Barcher, 1989), measure dissociative Symptoms and syndromes that should be associated with DES scores. Second, the DES should show discriminant validity (Campbell, 1960). Subjects from different socio-economic, racial, and cultural backgrounds should perceive the DES items in similar ways if their scores are used for comparison. Dissociation is not expected to be sex-related. One may expect weak asso- ciations with age (Strick & Wilcoxon, 1991). Lastly, the DES should not be affected strongly by response biases: that is, systematic error variance caused by social desirabili- ty or expectancy effects. Third, the reliability of the DES is an important issue. The data on the internal consistency or alpha reliability of the Instrument are summarized, and the studies on test-retest reliability are discussed. Furthermore, the existence of DES subscales are addressed. The proof of the pudding, however, is in assessing the instrument's predictive valid- ity (Nunnally, 1978): Is the DES related to the syndromes for which it has been devel- oped äs a screening Instrument? As Bernstein and Putnam (1986) predicted, the DES should be related to dissociative disorders, such äs Multiple Personality Disorder (MPD), Dissociative Disorder Not Otherwise Specified (DDNOS), and other Dissociative Disorders (ODD), and at the same time it should not be associated with psychiatric disturbances without dissociative characteristics, such äs Personality Disorders, Affective Disorders, Anxiety Disorders, Schizophrenia, or physical disor- ders such äs Seizure Disorder. Furthermore, äs dissociation is conceptualized äs a defense mechanism, it should be present in cases where defense is functional — at least in the short run. Therefore, the DES may be expected to discriminate Post- Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) or Abuse subjects from other (psychiatric or nor- mal) subjects. There has also been speculation on the basis of clinical reports that Eating Disorders may include certain dissociative components (Demitrack, Putnam, Dissociative Experiences Scale 367 Brewerton, & Brandt, 1990), and that subjects who tend to be hypnotizable also tend to have more frequent dissociative experiences (Bliss, 1986). Lastly, because the DES has been applied in several different diagnostic groups, including more than 12,000 subjects, we found it useful to derive some normative data from the extant studies, and to compare the means of those groups. METHOD Data Base Pertinent studies were identified through Psychlit, Medline, and the Social Sciences Citation Index, äs well äs through the "snowball" method (Müllen, 1989). Recent studies were identified using Current Contents. Studies were found through comput- er searches using the keywords "dissociative," "experiences," "scale," "DES," and "dis- sociation," and by using the reference to the Bernstein and Putnam (1986) article. Criteria for inclusion of a study in one of the meta-analyses were äs follows: (a) The DES was used to compare nonclinical or clinical groups; (b) Means or medians were reported; and (c) There was sufficient statistical detail to compute effect sizes. The selection procedure yielded 22 studies on the relation between DES and alter- native measures for dissociation, 34 studies on the discriminant validity of the DES, and 79 studies on the predictive validity of the DES. Only published papers were included; because a separate Journal for studies on dissociation started at around the same time äs the DES became available (Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders), probably most DES studies have been published. In 1993, Carlson, Putnam, ROSS, and Torem (1993) published their multicenter study on the validity of the DES äs a screen- ing Instrument for MPD. This study was not included in our meta-analysis, because their sample and the other — separately reported — samples in the meta-analysis would almost certainly overlap. The number of hypothesis tests for the separate meta- analyses varied between 11 and 53, which is sufficient for the computation of com- bined effect sizes (Müllen, 1989; Van IJzendoorn, 1995). The data that were extracted for the meta-analyses from the individual studies are available from the first author. Meta-Analytic Procedures In primary-level studies the unit of analysis is the subject; in a meta-analysis of several primary-level studies the unit of analysis is the outcome of those studies. Because of this difference in unit of analysis, the meta-analytic approach has to be based on a dif- ferent set of statistical techniques. These techniques should, for example, take into account the fact that data in a meta-analysis are usually based on different
Recommended publications
  • Problematic Cost–Utility Analysis of Interventions for Behavior Problems in Children and Adolescents
    DOI: 10.1002/cad.20360 REVIEW Problematic cost–utility analysis of interventions for behavior problems in children and adolescents Marinus H. van IJzendoorn1,2 Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg3 1 Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands Abstract 2 University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cost–utility analyses are slowly becoming part of randomized control trials evaluating physical and 3 Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands mental health treatments and (preventive) inter- ventions in child and adolescent development. The Correspondence British National Institute of Health and Care Excel- Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Erasmus Univer- lence, for example, insists on the use of gains in sity,Rotterdam, Netherlands. Email: [email protected] Quality Adjusted Life Years (QALYs) to compute the “value for money” of interventions. But what counts Funding information as a gain in quality of life? For one of the most widely The European Research Council; the Dutch used instruments, the EuroQol 5 Dimensions scale Ministry of Education, Culture, and Science; (EQ-5D), QALYs are estimated by healthy individ- the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO grant number 024.001.003) uals who provide utility scores for specific health states, assuming that the best life is a life without self-experienced problems in five domains: mobil- ity, self-care, usual activities, pain/discomfort, and anxiety/depression. The worst imaginable outcome is defined as “a lot of problems” in each of these five domains. The impact of the individual’sproblems on the social network is not weighted, and important social–developmental domains (externalizing prob- lems, social competence) are missing. Current cost– utility computations based on EQ-5D favor physi- cal health over mental health, and they rely on adult weights for child and adolescent quality of life.
    [Show full text]
  • Aging Holocaust Survivors and Their Offspring Facing New Challenges Ayala Fridmanab, Marian J
    Aging & Mental Health Vol. 15, No. 2, March 2011, 232–242 Coping in old age with extreme childhood trauma: Aging Holocaust survivors and their offspring facing new challenges Ayala Fridmanab, Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburgb, Abraham Sagi-Schwartza* and Marinus H. Van IJzendoornb aCenter for the Study of Child Development, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel; bCentre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands (Received 21 February 2010; final version received 10 June 2010) Objective: The Holocaust has become an iconic example of immense human-made catastrophes, and survivors are now coping with normal aging processes. Childhood trauma may leave the survivors more vulnerable when they are facing stress related to old age, whereas their offspring might have a challenging role of protecting their own parents from further pain. Here we examine the psychological adaptation of Holocaust survivors and their offspring in light of these new challenges, examining satisfaction with life, mental health, cognitive abilities, dissociative symptoms, and physical health. Methods: Careful matching of female Holocaust survivors and comparison subjects living in Israel was employed to form a case-control study design with two generations, including four groups: 32 elderly female Holocaust survivors and 47 daughters, and 33 elderly women in the comparison group, and 32 daughters (total N ¼ 174). Participants completed several measures of mental and physical health, and their cognitive functioning was examined. The current study is a follow-up of a previous study conducted 11 years ago with the same participants. Results: Holocaust survivors showed more dissociative symptomatology (odds ¼ 2.39) and less satisfaction with their life (odds ¼ 2.79) as compared to a matched group.
    [Show full text]
  • Association Between DNA Methylation and ADHD Symptoms from Birth to School Age: A
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/806844; this version posted October 16, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. TITLE PAGE Association between DNA methylation and ADHD symptoms from birth to school age: A prospective meta-analysis AUTHORS’ NAMES Alexander Neumann, PhD1,2,17*; Esther Walton, PhD3,4*; Silvia Alemany, PhD5,6,7*; Charlotte Cecil, PhD1; Juan Ramon González, PhD5,6,7; Dereje Demissie Jima, MSc8,9; Jari Lahti, PhD10,11; Samuli T. Tuominen, MSc11 ; Edward D. Barker, PhD12,13; Elisabeth Binder, MD, PhD14,16; Doretta Caramaschi, PhD3; Ángel Carracedo, PhD15; Darina Czamara, PhD16: Jorunn Evandt, PhD17; Janine F. Felix, PhD18,19; Bernard F. Fuemmeler, PhD20,21; Kristine B. Gutzkow, PhD22; Cathrine Hoyo, PhD23,24; Jordi Julvez, PhD5,6,7; Eero Kajantie, MD, PhD25,26; Hannele Laivuori, MD, PhD27,28; Rachel Maguire, MPH23; Léa Maitre, PhD5,6,7; Susan K. Murphy29; Mario Murcia, PhD7,30; Pia M. Villa, MD, PhD27; Gemma Sharp, PhD13; Jordi Sunyer, PhD, MD5,6,7; Katri Raikkönen, PhD11; Marian Bakermans-Kranenburg, PhD31; Marinus van IJzendoorn, PhD32; Mònica Guxens, MD, PhD1,5,6,7; Caroline L. Relton, PhD3; Henning Tiemeier, MD, PhD1,33 * These authors contributed equally to this work AUTHORS’ AFFILIATIONS 1 Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry/Psychology, Erasmus University Medical Center Rotterdam, the Netherlands 2 Lady Davis Institute for Medical Research, Jewish General Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada 3 Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, UK 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/806844; this version posted October 16, 2019.
    [Show full text]
  • Attachment Theory and Research: Overview with Suggested Applications to Child Custody
    INVITED MONOGRAPH ATTACHMENT THEORY AND RESEARCH: OVERVIEW WITH SUGGESTED APPLICATIONS TO CHILD CUSTODY Mary Main, Erik Hesse, and Siegfried Hesse* The term “attachment” is now in common usage and, as the readers of this Special Issue are aware, is referenced in a rapidly increasing variety of contexts involving child custody (McIntosh & Chisholm, 2008). The aim of this article is to provide judges, lawyers, mediators and mental health professionals involved in custody assessment with an overview of the history of the field of attachment and its principal measures, together with a clear description of what the term “attachment” does—and does not—mean to attachment researchers and theoreticians. Implications for normative separations that do not involve custody- related assessment or the intervention of courts or mediators are also considered. With respect to contested custody cases, we consider the use of standardized attachment measures, and note that sufficient validation for most such measures in clinical contexts is still developing. We describe three measures taken from the research literature (the Strange Situation procedure, the Attachment Q-sort and theAdultAttachment Interview), each subjected to meta-analyses and widely regarded as “gold standard” methods in research.These three methods come closest at this point in time to meeting criteria for providing “scientific evidence” regarding an individual’s current attachment status. Limitations on widespread use include the need for substantiating meta-analyses on father-child relationships, and further validation across a wider spread of children’s ages. We are confident that these restrictions can be solved by new research. In the interim, we argue that increased familiarity with the above measures will assist custody evaluators both in standardizing their assessment procedures and their capacity to gain more from the observational data available to them.
    [Show full text]
  • Attachment-Based Intervention for Enhancing Sensitive Discipline In
    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 74, No. 6, 994–1005 0022-006X/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994 Attachment-Based Intervention for Enhancing Sensitive Discipline in Mothers of 1- to 3-Year-Old Children at Risk for Externalizing Behavior Problems: A Randomized Controlled Trial Jantien Van Zeijl, Judi Mesman, Hans M. Koot Marinus H. Van IJzendoorn, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Femmie Juffer, and Mirjam N. Stolk Leiden University Lenneke R. A. Alink Leiden University The home-based intervention program Video-feedback Intervention to promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive Discipline (VIPP-SD) was tested in a randomized controlled trial with 237 families screened for their 1- to 3-year-old children’s relatively high scores on externalizing behavior. VIPP-SD, based on attachment theory and coercion theory, focuses on mirroring and discussing actual parent–child inter- actions in six 1.5-hr sessions with individual families at home. VIPP-SD proved to be effective in enhancing maternal attitudes toward sensitivity and sensitive discipline and in promoting sensitive discipline interactions in the intervention group as compared with the control group. Moreover, in families with more marital discord and in families with more daily hassles, the intervention resulted in a decrease of overactive problem behaviors in the children. The authors conclude that VIPP-SD should become an important module in attachment-based interventions. Keywords: attachment, intervention, sensitivity, discipline, externalizing problems Supplemental data: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.994.supp One of the most compelling research themes regarding the Zeijl et al., 2006).
    [Show full text]
  • Attachment in Surinam-Dutch Families: a Contribution to the Cross-Cultural Study of Attachment
    INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 1990,13 (3) 333-344 Attachment in Surinam-Dutch Families: A Contribution to the Cross-Cultural Study of Attachment V Marinus H. van IJzendoorn Center for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, The Netherlands A study on attachment in indigenous Dutch families and in families who came to the Netherlands from Surinam—a former Dutch colony in South America is described. Sixty-five mothers and their infants participated, including 26 mothers who had immigrated from Surinam. Attachment be- haviours and maternal responsiveness were recorded in the Strange Situation and in free play. The Infant Characteristics Questionnaire, and a question- naire on child-rearing attitudes were completed. In the Surinam-Dutch group, maternal responsiveness was related to quality of attachment, although the Surinam-Dutch mothers scored significantly lower on the responsiveness scale than the Dutch mothers. The attachment classification distribution of the Surinam-Dutch dyads did not significantly differ from Dutch or global distributions. However, Surinam-Dutch mothers appeared to be more anxious about child-rearing than Dutch mothers. Surinam-Dutch mothers who had recently arrived in Holland tended to show less responsive- ness, and were more anxious about child-rearing than Surinam-Dutch mothers who had immigrated several years ago. These data may reflect strains that arise from the transition from one culture to another. INTRODUCTION t In attachment theory it is hypothesised that the readiness to establish an attachment relationship, defined äs a relatively durable affective relation- Requests for reprints should be sent to Marinus H. van IJzendoorn, Center for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, P.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae (Dec 2012)
    Curriculum Vitae (Dec 2012) Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg Paulus Buysstraat 13 2334 CH Leiden Born 19-06-1965, Alphen aan den Rijn, The Netherlands Three children, born 1993 (Jacob), 1994 (Maartje), and 1998 (Anne) e-mail: [email protected] Degrees MA degree (March 1989): Leiden University, Center for Child and Family Studies Main subject: Attachment and maternal structuring PhD degree (October 1993): Leiden University, Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences; Supervisor (‘Promotor’): Prof. dr M.H. van IJzendoorn Title of thesis: Het Gehechtheidsbiografisch Interview (‘The Adult Attachment Interview: Psychometric analyses’) Work experience since graduating 1993- 2004, Assistant Professor, Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University 2004 - 2007, Associate Professor, Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University 2007 - present, Full Professor, Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University Research Summary of main research interests As part of my PhD study on the reliability and discriminant validity of the Adult Attachment Interview we invited about 90 first-time mothers to the institute. Three of them appeared to have more than one child: they had twins. Our interest had been aroused: would these twin siblings show similar attachment behaviours to their mothers (and corresponding to their mothers’ attachment representations)? The larger group of mothers was followed up in order to observe attachment behaviours of the second-born children. Results for the siblings have been reported in Van IJzendoorn et al. (2000). A study on attachment in twins was a rational next step in this line of research, and we studied the relative effects of genetic and environmental factors on infant-mother attachment (Bokhorst et al., 2003), infant-father attachment (Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Bokhorst, & Schuengel, 2004), and on the association between maternal sensitivity and infant attachment security (Fearon et al., 2006).
    [Show full text]
  • Attachment at an Early Age (0-5) and Its Impact on Children’S Development Marinus Van Ijzendoorn, Phd Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands September 2019, Rev
    ATTACHMENT Attachment at an Early Age (0-5) and its Impact on Children’s Development Marinus van IJzendoorn, PhD Erasmus University Rotterdam, Netherlands September 2019, Rev. ed. Introduction What is attachment? Children are considered to be attached if they tend to seek proximity to and contact with a 1 specific caregiver in times of distress, illness and tiredness. Attachment to a protective caregiver helps infants to regulate their negative emotions in times of stress and distress and to explore the environment, even if it contains somewhat frightening stimuli. Attachment, a major developmental milestone in the child’s life, remains an important issue throughout the lifespan. In adulthood, attachment representations shape the way adults feel about the strains and stresses of intimate relationships, in particular parent-child relationships, and the way in which the self is perceived. Development of attachment 1 Attachment is suggested to develop in four phases. In the first phase — indiscriminately orienting and signalling to people — the baby seems “tuned” to certain wave-lengths of signals from the environment. These signals are mostly of human origin (e.g. the sound of voices). During the second phase, probably first by smell and then by sight, the baby develops preference for one or more caregivers — the phase of orienting and signalling to one or several specific persons. Not until the infant is able to show active attachment behaviour, such as actively seeking proximity to and following the attachment figure, does the infant enters the third phase, the phase of attachment proper — staying near a specific person by means of signalling and movement.
    [Show full text]
  • International Journal of Behavioral Development
    International Journal of Behavioral Development http://jbd.sagepub.com/ Impact of institutional care on attachment disorganization and insecurity of Ukrainian preschoolers: Protective effect of the long variant of the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT) Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, Natasha Dobrova-Krol and Marinus van IJzendoorn International Journal of Behavioral Development published online 17 June 2011 DOI: 10.1177/0165025411406858 The online version of this article can be found at: http://jbd.sagepub.com/content/early/2011/06/15/0165025411406858 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development Additional services and information for International Journal of Behavioral Development can be found at: Email Alerts: http://jbd.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://jbd.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav >> Version of Record - Jun 17, 2011 What is This? Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com at Universiteit Leiden \ LUMC on October 6, 2011 International Journal of Behavioral Development 1–8 Impact of institutional care on ª The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permissions: attachment disorganization and sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0165025411406858 insecurity of Ukrainian preschoolers: ijbd.sagepub.com Protective effect of the long variant of the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT) Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg,1 Natasha Dobrova-Krol,1 and Marinus van IJzendoorn1 Abstract Institutional care has been shown to lead to insecure and disorganized attachments and indiscriminate friendliness. Some children, however, are surprisingly resilient to the adverse environment. Here the protective role of the long variant of the serotonin receptor gene (5HTT) is explored in a small hypothesis-generating study of 37 Ukrainian preschoolers reared in institutional settings or in their biological families.
    [Show full text]
  • IX. RESEARCH, PRACTICE, and POLICY PERSPECTIVES on ISSUES of CHILDREN WITHOUT PERMANENT PARENTAL CARE Robert B
    MONOGRAPHS OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT IX. RESEARCH, PRACTICE, AND POLICY PERSPECTIVES ON ISSUES OF CHILDREN WITHOUT PERMANENT PARENTAL CARE Robert B. McCall This chapter presents conclusions, trends, conceptual analyses, hypotheses, and speculations regarding some fundamental issues of research, practice, and policy that are largely unsettled or controversial. As such, the chapter is not a summary of Chapters I–VIII, but rather contains interpretations and opinions of the author intended to elevate the priority of certain issues, suggest hypotheses to be studied, and propose practice and policy steps to be considered. This chapter is primarily aimed at presenting conclusions, trends, con- ceptual analyses, hypotheses, and speculations regarding some fundamental research, practice, and policy issues that are unsettled or controversial. The goal is to examine some of the conceptual forests at a time when the field may be preoccupied with individual trees, raise the priorities of these gen- eral issues on future agenda, and heuristically contribute ideas that may be researched or discussed with greater vigor in the future. Thus, this chapter is not a summary of Chapters I–VIII; some points made in those chapters are highlighted here, but additional material is added, some of which was pre- pared for this project and is still unpublished, and a variety of new issues are raised. The interpretations and opinions expressed are those of the author, not a consensus of project participants. SOCIAL SCIENCE ISSUES—A TOUGH SCIENTIFIC NUT TO CRACK Early adversities, whether poverty, malnutrition, abuse and neglect, in- stitutionalization, or other atypical circumstances, represent unfortunate This paper is supported in part by grant HD050212 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the author and Christina J.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Review: Genetic Vulnerability Or Differential Susceptibility in Child Development: the Case of Attachment
    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48:12 (2007), pp 1160–1173 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01801.x Research Review: Genetic vulnerability or differential susceptibility in child development: the case of attachment Marian J. Bakermans-Kranenburg and Marinus H. van IJzendoorn Centre for Child and Family Studies, Leiden University, The Netherlands Gene–environment interactions interpreted in terms of differential susceptibility may play a large part in the explanation of individual differences in human development. Reviewing studies on the behavioral and molecular genetics of attachment, we present evidence for interactions between genetic and envir- onmental factors explaining individual differences in attachment security and disorganization. In particular, the DRD4 7-repeat polymorphism seems associated with an increased risk for disorganized attachment, but only when combined with environmental risk. Gene–environment (G · E) interactions may be interpreted as genetic vulnerability or differential susceptibility. We found support for the differential susceptibility hypothesis predicting not only more negative outcomes for susceptible children in unfavorable environments, but also positive outcomes for susceptible children in favorable environments. Keywords: Attachment, gene–environment interaction, differential susceptibility, disorganized attachment, dopamine, behavior problems, genetics. Attachment theory is an important theoretical Hesse, 1999). Maternal sensitivity, however, framework for studying infant–parent relationships accounts for only a third of the association between and individual differences in emotion regulation. parental attachment representation and infant Attachment is the evolutionary-based, innate attachment security, leaving a transmission gap of predisposition of a child to seek proximity to and unexplained variance in infant attachment security contact with a specific figure, notably when the child (Van IJzendoorn, 1995; Belsky, 2005).
    [Show full text]
  • Disorganized Attachment in Infancy: a Review of the Phenomenon and Its Implications for Clinicians and Policy-Makers
    Attachment & Human Development ISSN: 1461-6734 (Print) 1469-2988 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rahd20 Disorganized attachment in infancy: a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy-makers Pehr Granqvist, L. Alan Sroufe, Mary Dozier, Erik Hesse, Miriam Steele, Marinus van Ijzendoorn, Judith Solomon, Carlo Schuengel, Pasco Fearon, Marian Bakermans-Kranenburg, Howard Steele, Jude Cassidy, Elizabeth Carlson, Sheri Madigan, Deborah Jacobvitz, Sarah Foster, Kazuko Behrens, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Naomi Gribneau, Gottfried Spangler, Mary J Ward, Mary True, Susan Spieker, Sophie Reijman, Samantha Reisz, Anne Tharner, Frances Nkara, Ruth Goldwyn, June Sroufe, David Pederson, Deanne Pederson, Robert Weigand, Daniel Siegel, Nino Dazzi, Kristin Bernard, Peter Fonagy, Everett Waters, Sheree Toth, Dante Cicchetti, Charles H Zeanah, Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Mary Main & Robbie Duschinsky To cite this article: Pehr Granqvist, L. Alan Sroufe, Mary Dozier, Erik Hesse, Miriam Steele, Marinus van Ijzendoorn, Judith Solomon, Carlo Schuengel, Pasco Fearon, Marian Bakermans- Kranenburg, Howard Steele, Jude Cassidy, Elizabeth Carlson, Sheri Madigan, Deborah Jacobvitz, Sarah Foster, Kazuko Behrens, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Naomi Gribneau, Gottfried Spangler, Mary J Ward, Mary True, Susan Spieker, Sophie Reijman, Samantha Reisz, Anne Tharner, Frances Nkara, Ruth Goldwyn, June Sroufe, David Pederson, Deanne Pederson, Robert Weigand, Daniel Siegel, Nino Dazzi, Kristin Bernard, Peter Fonagy, Everett Waters, Sheree Toth, Dante Cicchetti, Charles H Zeanah, Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Mary Main & Robbie Duschinsky (2017) Disorganized attachment in infancy: a review of the phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy-makers, Attachment & Human Development, 19:6, 534-558, DOI: 10.1080/14616734.2017.1354040 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2017.1354040 © 2017 The Author(s).
    [Show full text]