Running head: A Motivational Atlas

Navigating Motivation: A Semantic and Subjective Atlas of 7 Motives

Gabriele Chierchia*1,4, Marisa Przyrembel*1, Franca Parianen Lesemann1, Steven

Bosworth2,3, Dennis Snower2 & Tania Singer1

1. Max Planck Institute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Social , Leipzig;

2. Kiel Institute for World Economy, Kiel;

3. University of Reading, Reading, UK;

4. University College London, UK.

* These authors contributed equally to the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest:

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Motivational Atlas

Abstract

Research from , neurobiology and behavioral economics indicates that a binary view of motivation, based on approach and avoidance, may be too reductive and that at least seven crucial motives are likely to affect human decisions, such as "consumption/resource seeking", "care", "affiliation", "achievement", "status-power", "threat approach" (or anger), and "threat avoidance" (or fear). As a preliminary investigation of the conceptual distinctness and relatedness of these motives, we conducted a semantic categorization task, in which participants were to assign provided words to one of the motives. Results suggest that, for each motive, subjects converge on how to make these differential assignments. Moreover, principal component analysis of these assignments suggests that the semantic inter-relations of the motives can be represented on a two dimensional space, or a “semantic atlas”. This atlas suggests that, while care and affiliation are conceptually close, affiliation is closer to threat-avoidance (or fear), while on opposite to these motives, we find achievement, consumption and power, with the latter lying closer to threat-approach (or anger). In a second study, we asked participants to rate how well motive-specific words described their currently experienced feelings. We find that semantically close motives also are more likely to be experienced together, that is, we replicate most of the semantic relations in a “subjective atlas”. In addition to these motivational atlases, we provide a database of motive-specific words, together with the valence and arousal scores, to be used for future research on the influence of motives on decision making.

Key words: affiliation, care, motives, motivation psychology, semantic categorization

2

Motivational Atlas

Introduction

Standard economic and decision theory is typically agnostic as regards to the types of goals that agents generally pursue (Samuelson, 1938; Stigler & Becker, 1977). Moreover, several neurobiological theories of decision making draw from behaviorism and associated models of reinforcement (O’Doherty, Kringelbach, Rolls, Hornak, & Andrews, 2001;

Rangel, Camerer, & Montague, 2008; Seymour & Dolan, 2008), a view on motivation most closely resembling motivation psychology’s most prevalent distinction between approach and avoidance or reward and punishment (see also Pessoa, 2014). In contrast, decades of psychological research have suggest that many decision are driven by particular motives, such as the motivation for achievement, power or affiliation (Deci & Moller, 2005; Heckhausen &

Heckhausen, 2010; Lewin, 1936; McClelland, Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1955; McDougall,

1932; Mowrer, 1960; Thorndike, 1898). These motives are frequently conceived as both trait- related dispositions and context-sensitive state-based responses that lead subjects to experience particular types of incentives as pleasurable and rewarding, to strive for certain types of goals, and hence to activate particular behavioral tendencies and related decisions

(Atkinson, 1964; Emmons & McAdams, 1991; McClelland, 1965; Pang, 2010; Rheinberg &

Engeser, 2010; Schultheiss & Strasser, 2012). Similarly, recent advances in neurobiology have also begun to move beyond classic approach/avoidance interpretations of motivation, enriching it with a more motive-related vocabulary (Bromberg-Martin, Matsumoto, &

Hikosaka, 2010; Daw, Kakade, & Dayan, 2002; Eisenegger, Haushofer, & Fehr, 2011;

Terburg & van Honk, 2013). Finally, behavioral game theorists (e.g., Camerer, 2003), social dilemma researchers (Dawes, 1980; & Fischbacher, 2003; Kollock, 1998; Lange,

Balliet, Parks, & Vugt, 2013) and neuroeconomists (Glimcher & Fehr, 2013) have also documented that humans often pursue ends other than payoff maximization in economic interactions (Frank, 1988; Loewenstein & O’Donoghue, 2004) and speculated on their 3

Motivational Atlas potential evolutionary functions (e.g., Robson 2001). Despite this rich knowledge, a coherent motive-based framework of decision making, that may integrate findings from motivation psychology, biology and behavioral economics is still missing.

To lay the grounds for such an integrated motivation-based framework we focus here on seven motives that we identify to be crucial in one or all three of the literatures mentioned above (albeit sometimes with different terminology): achievement, power-status, affiliation, care, threat avoidance (anger), threat approach (fear) and an exploratory “consumption” motive, which we propose as a possible parallel to the motivation implied by the standard

“homo economicus” model. Following seminal efforts in the literature (e.g., Russell,

1980), a first aim of the present paper is to investigate the semantic relations of these motives, by asking whether and how subjects coherently differentiate between them in a semantic categorization task. Then, we aim to explore which dimensions might allow them to do so, by applying principal component analysis to such categorization judgments. In second study, we aim to investigate whether the observed semantic inter-relations hold at the level of subjective experience, that is, whether semantically “close” motives are also more likely to co-occur in subjectively reported experience, and whether, vice versa, semantically distant motives may be more likely to emerge in an antagonistic fashion. Ultimately, we aim to produce and compare a semantic and subjective “atlas” of motives, which we hope will reduce the complexity of navigating motives in a framework that goes beyond approach and avoidance.

In the following, we introduce the motives of interests from a motivation psychology and neurobiological perspective, briefly illustrating their potential relevance in behavioral economics. We then pass to describing two empirical studies that address the semantic and subjective relations between such motives. We conclude by providing a database of motive- specific words (together with their valence and arousal scores). These could be used in future studies, to better qualify the relationship between motives and decision making. 4

Motivational Atlas

Motives in psychology

Three motives, namely, “achievement”, “power” and “affiliation” are among the most recognized motives in motivation psychology (Weiner, 1990). Achievement has been defined as the desire to do something better or more efficiently than before (Atkinson & Feather,

1966; Heckhausen & Heckhausen, 2010; McClelland et al., 1955; Weiner, 1990), and. A power and/or status-seeking motive has been defined as the desire to have an impact (Fodor &

Riordan, 1995; Fodor, Schultheiss, & Brunstein, 2010), to be strong, and to influence others

(Heckhausen & Heckhausen, 2010; McClelland, 1953; Reiss, 2004). Achievement and power are likely to be relatively similar motives (Bakan, 1966a), insofar as related concepts have been proposed to be part of a more general “agency” (Abele, Uchronski, & Suitner, 2008;

Diehl, Owen, & Youngblade, 2004) or “competence” dimension of motivation (Cuddy, Fiske,

& Glick, 2007; Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). Agency is often described as “a person’s striving to be independent, to control one’s environment, and to assert, protect and expand one’s self”, while communion refers to “a person’s striving to be part of a community, to establish close relationships with others, and to subordinate individual needs to the common good” (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966b; Diehl et al., 2004). Indeed, proponents of the agency/communion division have suggested that achievement and power could constitute standard and “excessive” forms of agency, respectively (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966a;

Diehl et al., 2004).

Motives that can be subsumed under the “communion” dimension of motivation are for example intimacy (McAdams, 1980), nurturance (Murray, 1938a), prosocial altruism (J.

Heckhausen, 2000), help (Heckhausen, 1989b), idealism and family (Reiss, 2004), and affiliation (McClelland, 1953). Among these, the most prominent motive is affiliation, which has been defined as the need to be liked or the need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; J. 5

Motivational Atlas

Heckhausen & Heckhausen, 2010; Jackson, 1974; McClelland et al., 1955; Murray, 1969).

This has been suggested (Weinberger, Cotler, & Fishman, 2010) to have a “bright” side, related to the joy of social approval/acceptance, and a “darker” component, related to the fear of social isolation/rejection (e.g., Eisenberger, 2012). In both cases, affiliation has frequently been associated with a variety of socially compliant behaviors (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004;

Langner & Winter, 2001; Over & Carpenter, 2009). Moreover, there is evidence from several psychological approaches that this social motivation can further be differentiated into two subsystems: one that expresses the need to belong and to affiliate, and one that deals with intimacy, care giving, nurturance, and compassion (also see Weinberger et al., 2010). Indeed, psychologists have frequently differentiated between wanting to be accepted, versus wanting to accept and nourish others. For instance, Murray (1969) contrasts nurturance and affiliation,

McAdams (1980) distinguishes an intimacy motivation from affiliation, Heckhausen (2000) discriminates affiliation vs. altruism/help and Jackson (1974) distinguishes between affiliation and nurturance. Interestingly, some agency/communion proponents have also recognized a gradient within the communion dimension, with notions related to “care” on one end and concepts related to “lack of independence”, which are closer to the affiliation motive, on the other (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966b; Diehl et al., 2004). Using the term ‘affiliation’ for both of these phenomenologically and psychologically differing sub-motives might be too general, and thus, we propose a distinction between two subsystems, namely care and affiliation (Weinberger et al., 2010).

In addition to classical motives such as achievement, power and affiliation discussed in motivation psychology, some have advocated a motivational threat system encompassing fear and anger – “harm avoidance” (Murray, 1938b), “avoidance” (Hull, 1943; Lewin, 1936;

Thorndike, 1898), “fear” (Heckhausen 1989), “anxiety” (Trudewind, 2000), as opposed to

“aggression” (Heckhausen, 1989b), “rage” (McDougall, 1932; J Panksepp, 2006), 6

Motivational Atlas

“vengeance” (Reiss, 2004), respectively. In the traditional literature, these two motives often bare the names of primary . However, to stress the motivational dimension of anger and fear (Roseman -2011 - calls them "emotivations") we will speak of “threat approach” and

“threat avoidance” (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009).

This brief review on the concepts of motives, as discussed in personality and motivation psychology, suggests that at least five distinct context-sensitive motives, namely achievement, power, affiliation, threat avoidance (fear) and threat approach (anger), have been described consistently and have been empirically proven to differentially influence human decision- making. The distinction between affiliation and care is slightly less acknowledged, however, as we will see in what follows, the notion of a care-related motive is more prominent in biology (Alcaro, Huber, & Panksepp, 2007; Carter, 1998; Hall, Stanton, Schultheiss,

Schultheiss, & Brunstein, 2010; Insel & Young, 2001; Jaak Panksepp, 2005; O. Schultheiss &

Brunstein, 2010; Singer & Klimecki, 2014). Similarly, the optimization of one's own utility and welfare (i.e., the selfish accumulation of consumption or goods), which is typically assumed to be the backbone of neo-classical economic models, has received relatively less attention in the motivation psychology literature (Kasser, 2006; Kasser et al., 2014) but has often been associated with terms such as "wanting", “desire-seeking”, or “foraging” in the animal and neurobiological literature (Berridge, Ho, Richard, & DiFeliceantonio, 2010;

Berridge & Robinson, 1998; de Waal, 2007; Delgado, Gillis, & Phelps, 2008; Depue &

Collins, 1999; Ikemoto & Panksepp, 1999; Lea & Webley, 2006; Panksepp, 2006).

Neurobiological Evidence for motivational systems

Typically, neurobiological research on motivation and affect does not refer to distinct motives, but rather to more general antagonistic approach-avoidance motivation associated to reward and punishment (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010; Dayan & Balleine, 2002; Dayan & 7

Motivational Atlas

Berridge, 2014; Lang & Bradley, 2010; Montague, Hyman, & Cohen, 2004; Schultz, 2010;

Seymour, Singer, & Dolan, 2007). However, several researchers have started to characterize motivation on a more fine-grained level (Alcaro et al., 2007; Hall et al., 2010; Panksepp,

2005; Rodrigues, LeDoux, & Sapolsky, 2009; Schultheiss et al., 2008; Toates, 1986; Wright

& Panksepp, 2012). For example, Panksepp (Alcaro & Panksepp, 2011; Nelson & Panksepp,

1998; Panksepp, 2005) discusses seven distinct biological systems – seeking, rage, fear, care, panic, lust and play – which are proposed to be characterized by dissociable brain networks and neuromodulatory systems, as well as distinct psycho-behavioral functions and evolutionary purposes. In line with this multi-faceted approach to motivation, on the system neuroscience level, researchers have begun to investigate care-related motivation and behavior using imaging techniques, e.g. by studying maternal love (Bartels & Zeki, 2000,

2004; Strathearn, Fonagy, Amico, & Montague, 2009) and the effects of loving kindness and compassion training (Bornemann, Kok, Böckler, & Singer, 2016; Engen & Singer, 2015;

Hildebrandt, McCall, & Singer, 2017; Klimecki, Leiberg, Ricard, & Singer, 2014; Klimecki,

Leiberg, Lamm, & Singer, 2012; Valk et al., 2017; Weng et al., 2013), which is designed to enhance prosocial motivation and behavior (Leiberg, Klimecki, & Singer, 2011). These studies have highlighted a network of brain regions that frequently include the medial orbitofrontal cortex, the ventral tegmental area and striatum including nucleus accumbens.

Many of these regions are also rich in receptors of the peptide hormone oxytocin (OT), as well as dopamine and opioids (e.g., Dölen, Darvishzadeh, Huang, & Malenka, 2013), and OT has been suggested to play an important role in mediating a range of caring and affiliative behaviors, such as parental care and social bonding in several species including humans

(Bakermans-Kranenburg & van Ijzendoorn, 2013; Carter, 1998; Feldman, 2012; Gordon,

Martin, Feldman, & Leckman, 2011; Insel & Young, 2001; Cade McCall & Singer, 2012;

Rilling, 2013; Ross & Young, 2009). For instance, intranasal administration of OT in humans has been frequently shown to increase trust and prosocial decisions in economic decision 8

Motivational Atlas making tasks (Baumgartner, Heinrichs, Vonlanthen, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2008; Declerck,

Boone, & Kiyonari, 2010; E Fehr, 2009; Israel, Weisel, Ebstein, & Bornstein, 2012; Kosfeld,

Heinrichs, Zak, Fischbacher, & Fehr, 2005; Rilling et al., 2012). On the other hand, the steroid hormone testosterone (T) has been associated with different competitive or dominant behaviors related to power-status (Burnham, 2007; C Eisenegger et al., 2011; Christoph

Eisenegger, Naef, Snozzi, Heinrichs, & Fehr, 2010; Hall et al., 2010; Mazur & Booth, 1998;

Salvador, 2005; Stanton, O’Dhaniel, & McLaurin, 2011). For instance, administration of T has been related to decreased trust in economic games (Boksem et al., 2013; Bos, Hermans,

Ramsey, & Honk, 2012), lower offers in bargaining problems (Zak et al., 2009) and increased risk taking (Stanton, Liening, & Schultheiss, 2011). In line with the “darker side of affiliation” mentioned above (Weinberger et al., 2010), Panksepp (Panksepp, 2006) distinguishes, among others, between “care” and “panic/separation distress”, with the latter revolving around the anterior cingulate and the periaqueductal gray. Eisenberger and colleagues (Eisenberger, 2012; Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, 2003) found that similar regions are recruited when experiencing both the affective response to physical pain and the one elicited through social rejection. Overall, the neuronal bases of threat avoidance (fear) and threat approach (anger) are rather well explored, with the implicated brain regions and neurotransmitters for the two systems overlapping considerably (Adolphs, 2013; Bond &

Wingrove, 2010; LeDoux, 2014; Nelson & Trainor, 2007; Potegal & Stemmler, 2010;

Rodrigues et al., 2009; Siever, 2008). Finally, a large body of empirical evidence has suggested that the mesolimbic dopamine (DA) system plays a crucial role in mediating consumption-related appetitive, incentive wanting/seeking states and behaviors (Alcaro et al.,

2007; Berridge et al., 2010; Berridge & Robinson, 1998; Depue & Collins, 1999; Schultz,

2007).

This very brief review suggests that motivation should not be reduced to approach and

9

Motivational Atlas avoidance. Rather, different sectors of neuroscience suggest that characteristic neurobiological systems facilitate behaviors related to distinct motives, such as consumption, caring, fear of social rejection, fight/flight, and dominance.

Motivation in Economic Decision Making

Motivation psychology and neurobiology thus suggest that distinct motives or motivational systems could facilitate distinct action tendencies and decisions. This notion is not explicitly invoked in classical economic theories, which generally hold that decisions are determined by stable, context-insensitive and exogenously given "preferences" (Camerer & Loewenstein,

2004; Mas-Colell, Whinston, & Green, 1995; Samuelson, 1938; Varian, 2006). In spite of this, motive-related vocabulary has frequently been invoked to explain economic behavior.

For example, fear has been frequently been associated to risk-, loss-, or ambiguity aversion in economic decision making (Canessa & Crespi, 2013; Hartley & Phelps, 2012; Lerner &

Keltner, 2001; Maner & Gerend, 2007; Martino & Camerer, 2010; Porcelli & Delgado, 2009;

Raghunathan & Pham, 1999), while Anger has been associated with increased risk taking

(Fessler, Pillsworth, & Flamson, 2004; Kugler, Kausel, & Kocher, 2012; Lerner & Keltner,

2001), with so-called altruistic punishment towards experienced unfairness (Carpenter &

Matthews, 2009; Seip, Dijk, & Rotteveel, 2014; Strobel et al., 2011; Yamagishi et al., 2009), as well as with decreased generosity, trust and cooperation in game-theoretic paradigms

(Eimontaite, Nicolle, Schindler, & Goel, 2013; Polman & Kim, 2013; Small & Lerner, 2008).

In the broader economic literature, the closest construct to a care or affiliation motive is plausibly the notion of altruism (Andreoni, 1990; Becker, 1974; Bruce & Waldman, 1990;

Fehr & Fischbacher, 2003b; Hamilton, 1964; Palfrey & Rosenthal, 1988; Sally, 2001), which has been frequently been invoked to explain charitable donations (Harbaugh, Mayr, &

Burghart, 2007; Tusche & Böckler, 2016), cooperation (Batson & Ahmad, 2001; Batson &

Moran, 1999; Ernst Fehr & Fischbacher, 2004), and trust (Baumgartner et al., 2008; Kosfeld 10

Motivational Atlas et al., 2005; McCall, Steinbeis, Ricard, & Singer, 2014; Singer & Steinbeis, 2009). The relevance of status and power concerns for economic decision making has also been frequently argued for in the economic literature (Cole, Mailath, & Postlewaite, 1992; Frank,

1985; Robson & Samuelson, 2010), and a Power-related motive has been associated with a variety of economic decision making patterns, from impatience to receiving monetary goods

(Joshi & Fast, 2013), to heightened reward sensitivity (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002), increased risk-taking (Anderson & Galinsky, 2006), norm-enforcement (Chierchia, Lesemann, Snower,

Vogel, & Singer, 2017; Straub & Murnighan, 1995; Yamagishi et al., 2012), and both prosocial decisions (Guinote, Cotzia, & Sandhu, 2015; Kumru & Vesterlund, 2010) - such as contributing more to public goods in social dilemmas (Galinsky, Gruenfeld, & Magee, 2003) - and selfish decisions - such as taking more from common resources (Lammers, Galinsky,

Gordijn, & Otten, 2008)1. Moreover, both seminal psychological studies on achievement motivation (Atkinson, 1957) and well-known economic theories (Gilboa & Schmeidler, 2001;

Gómez-Miñambres, 2012; Selten, 1998) have highlighted that, rather than maximizing mere monetary payoffs, experimental participants frequently behave as if pursuing subjective goals, where every outcome below that goal is considered a loss/failure, and every outcome above it a gain/success (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Selten, 1998). However, as discussed above, we suggest that the standard motivation of economic theory to maximize one’s own resources is captured by what we refer to as a "consumption motive", which is related to the "wanting" or

"resource-seeking" systems discussed in neurobiology. In synthesis, preliminary evidence suggests that many of the proposed psychological motives could underlie important decision making patterns that have been documented in the behavioral economics literature. However, before experimentally testing such a motive-based decision making model, we first aimed

1 This differential impact of Power-motivation could be due to power increasing agentic and approaching behavior (Galinsky et al., 2003).

11

Motivational Atlas with the present paper, to explore the relations between these motives on a semantic and subjective level.

A semantic and subjective atlas of seven motives

Based on an integrative approach of motivation psychology, neurobiology and behavioral economics, we have suggested that at least seven motives are likely to drive decision making:

1) Consumption/Resource-Seeking, 2) Care, 3) Affiliation, 4) Power-Status, 5) Achievement,

6) Threat Avoidance (or fear) and 7) Threat Approach (or anger). Clearly, this framework is to be viewed as a pragmatic starting point and will have to be expanded and refined, plausibly by investigating additional motives. However, as a first step towards integrating those fields and developing a common motive-related language, we first asked whether subjects can indeed semantically differentiate between each of these motives in similar ways. For example, do people agree on how concepts of affiliation and care differ from one another, or are these categories too similar to be semantically distinguished? What is the commonly conceived relationship between achievement, a well-known motive in psychology, and consumption, an important motivational system in neurobiology? In short, we asked whether people are able to distinguish between these seven proposed motives and how these relate to each other in semantic space.

To investigate these questions we conducted a semantic categorization task in which we asked subjects to ascribe a number of motive-related words to the seven motivational categories posited above. This approach is reminiscent of a long tradition of psychological studies on emotions, which assess subjects’ conceptualization of affective states (Kuppens & Tuerlinckx,

2013, for a review). Such studies typically assume that, throughout their daily interactions, subjects learn about emotions and form “mental maps”, which enable them to understand how emotions relate to one another and to behavior (Russell, 1980). Though such “laymen psychologists” may not be able to explicitly characterize their knowledge of emotions, their 12

Motivational Atlas conceptual maps can be accessed by asking them to make judgments on emotions. For instance, in a seminal paper, Russell (1980) asked subjects to categorize 28 emotion-related words to pre-defined emotional categories and found that 2 dimensions, namely valence and arousal, could predict most categorization judgments. In line with this, many psychologists

(e.g., Barrett, 2004; Shaver, Schwartz, & Kirson, 1987) have capitalized on categorization agreements/disagreements to spatially represent “closeness” of emotion-related concepts on a single (multidimensional) space. To our knowledge, these approaches have frequently been applied to emotions, but never to motives.

Our semantic categorization task thus had two complementary objectives: 1) to investigate whether the motives posited by our framework are conceptually differentiated across subjects in a coherent manner: if not, subjects should seldom (or never) choose the same words to distinguish one motive from its nearest neighbours. On the other hand, if subjects do have similar semantic representations of motives, it should be found that they agree on which words best distinguish one motive from each of the others. 2) As a second goal, by applying a principle component analysis (“PCA”) to the categorization judgments, we aimed to reconstruct subjects’ mental maps of motives, and thus to show how subjects generally think motives relate to one another on a single “semantic atlas”. Importantly, Russell (1980) argues that the cognitive structure of emotions should reflect the structure of actually experienced emotions. If the same were true for motives, than the conceptual-semantic proximity of motives could predict their experienced proximity (e.g., an increased likelihood of co- occurrence). In addition, Russell’s frequently corroborated circumplex model of emotions

(Kuppens & Tuerlinckx, 2013; Posner & Russell, 2005) suggests that many emotion judgments and experiences can be adequately described by valence and arousal, which were found to constitute the two main dimensions of his space. Along these lines, we aimed to assess whether the same may holds for motives. Correspondingly, after the categorization 13

Motivational Atlas task, we asked participants to rate each of the tested motive-relevant words on valence and arousal, to assess whether these dimensions alone could constitute the basis on which subjects made their categorization judgments.

Moreover, we expected the conceptual-semantic relations between motives to display at least four additional characteristics: first, we expected “care” and “affiliation”-specific words to be conceptually closer to one another, as we previously mentioned that these are often conflated within a broader “communion” dimension of social attitudes (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan,

1966a). Moreover, as mentioned above, psychologists focusing on the agency/communion distinction have frequently illustrated a gradient within the communion dimension, spanning notions related to care, on one side, to those related to “lack of independence” (i.e., affiliation), on the other (Abele et al., 2008). Second, we expected that words attributed to affiliation would have some conceptual proximity to fear, as research on the affiliation motive has made a link between the need to belong and fear of rejection, social pain and separation distress (Eisenberger, 2012; Panksepp, 2006; Weinberger et al., 2010). Third, following agency/communion theorists, we expected that power-status and achievement words would be relatively close (at least closer to one another than care/affiliation), as they are both related to agency, with words related to achievement on one end of a hypothetical agency spectrum, and words related to power on the other (Abele et al., 2008). Fourth, we expected words related to threat approach (i.e., anger) and threat avoidance (i.e., fear) to be relatively close, as these motives are both related to perceived threats. Moreover, as threat-approach (or anger) has been associated to feelings of agency, we expected it to be closer to the agency dimension than threat-avoidance, i.e., thus closer to power/achievement (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009).

Finally, as consumption was thought of as a parallel to the self-interested motivation traditionally assumed in economics, we expected consumption-specific words to be relatively

14

Motivational Atlas close to agentic motives such as achievement or power/status and distant from words related to care.

Finally, another important question we aimed to address is whether similar relationships will also be reflected in a “subjective atlas”. Thus, although concepts that are thought of as conceptually close generally also co-occur in self-reported experience (Russell, 1980)2, this needn’t always be the case. For instance, compassion tends to be prototypically conceptualized as a positive emotion, though when actually experienced (e.g., when witnessing the suffering of others) it can be accompanied by positive as well as negative affect (Condon & Feldman Barrett, 2013). Analog to such approaches in emotion research, we investigated here whether in the domain of motives, semantic categorization of motives and subjectively experienced motives would fall onto similar or distinct dimensions. To answer this question, participants self-reported (by providing ratings) in a second study how the seven motives of interest described their spontaneously occurring motives and feelings: if semantically close motives are also subjectively close (Russell, 1980), we expected semantically close motives to also be more likely to co-occur at the level of experience.

Study 1

Methods

Participants

79 participants (40 males, mean age = 32.4, SD=13) took part in one of 10 experimental sessions. Experimental protocols were approved by local ethics committee (of the Medical

Faculty of the University of Leipzig, n. 090-15-090032015) and all participants provided informed consent for the anonymized treatment of their data.

2 Russell suggests that this occurs because self-reports of affective states are also the result of a cognitive or “interpretative” process, which relies on semantic representations and their corresponding structures.

15

Motivational Atlas

Procedures

Sessions were conducted in the computer lab of the Department of at the

Max Planck Institute in Leipzig, Germany. Participants took part in the tasks from individually shielded computer cubicles. Participants of sessions 1 through 6 (n=38, 21 males, mean age=41.3, SD=18.2) took part in the categorization task only (see below), while participants of sessions 7 through 10 (n=41, 20 males, mean age=26.4, SD=3.8) additionally took part in the valence and arousal ratings (after the categorization task).

Word pre-selection

To pre-select motive-related words to use in the categorization task, we drew from a previously existing “free-association” database (Nelson, McEvoy, & Schreiber, 2004), which was created by requiring participants to name the “first word that came to mind that was meaningfully related or strongly associated to the presented cue word”. This database provides over 72.000 “cue-associate” pairs and to draw motive-related words from it we proceeded in two steps. First, for each of our motives, we searched the database for one or two most closely related words. We called these “anchor words”. Second, we searched the database for adjectives that had been associated to the anchor words in either the 1st or 2nd degree3. For most of our motives, the identification of anchor words was trivial. Specifically, we took the word “care” as an anchor word for the care motive, the word “power” for power- status, “fear and fearful” for threat-avoidance (i.e., fear), “anger/angry” for threat-approach

(i.e., anger) and “achieve/achievement” for achievement. Two of the posited motives, namely,

“affiliation” and “consumption”, did not have obvious equivalents in the database. For affiliation we thus chose the anchor words “friend/friendly”, while for consumption we chose

3 For example, for our care motive we selected among associates of the anchor word “care” (1st degree), as well as among the associates of these associates – 2nd degree.

16

Motivational Atlas the anchor word “want”. After having obtained a preliminary set of motive-associated words, we expanded this set through online synonym vocabularies. A list of 174 words was used in sessions 1 through 6, and this list was extended to 305 words for sessions 7 through 10.4

Semantic categorization task

The pre-selected words were then used in the main categorization task of interest. Here, subjects were required to ascribe each word to one of the seven motives posited by our framework. Visually, participants were shown a list of candidate motive-related words (in randomized order), on the left side of the screen, and seven labelled “boxes” on the right (see figure 1 in the supplementary material, section A). All box labels began with the phrase

“motivated by…” and ended with one of the following: “…care for others”, “…the desire to belong”, “…power”, “…achievement”, “…anger”, “…fear” and “…the desire for possession and consumption”5. Subjects were then asked to drag and drop each word into one of the boxes. No instructions were provided as to how many words could be placed in each category

(see supplementary material, section B, for the specific instructions). The results of the semantic categorization task are available in the supplementary material (see also supplementary material section E, for a description of the data frame).

Valence, arousal and frequency scores

After the categorization task, participants of sessions 7 through 10 (n=41) also rated each of the 305 words on two scales: a valence scale and an arousal scale. Scales consisted of continuous scrolling bars, which could take values between 0 and 100 (in table 1 we subtract

4 With one exception (due to a programming error for the word “gemein”= mean), all of the words used in the first 6 sessions were also used in the last 4 ones. 5 For sessions 6 through 10, we slightly changed the label of this category to “motivated by wanting”. We did so to control for a possible overly negative connotation of the words consumption and possession. This label change led to no identifiable difference in the results (see supplementary material, section D, for details).

17

Motivational Atlas

50 from the mean valence and arousal scores, so that t-tests against 0 may suggest which words are statistically high/low in valence or arousal). The instructions for the tasks were taken from Kanske & Kotz (Kanske & Kotz, 2010). English translations in Table 1 were carried out by professional translators.

Analysis

We ran two types of analyses. The first searched for “motive-specific” words, that is, words that were attributed more frequently to one motive than any other. To obtain these, for each word, we took the two categories in which the word had been most frequently placed in and we then ran a chi-square test to assess whether the resulting proportion could be due to chance6. If it could not (p<0.05), we took the resulting word to be “motive-specific” and we took it to be “ambiguous” otherwise 7. We call words that satisfied this property “motive- specific”. Second, we ran a principal component analysis (“PCA”) taking the seven motivation-categories as variables/descriptors, and the single words as items. The PCA was conducted with the “PCA” function of the “Factominer” package (Lê, Josse, & Husson, 2008) in R (R Core team, 2013). As supplementary variables of interest we took: i) the results of the first analysis (namely, a vector indicating which motive – if any - each word had been specifically ascribed to); and ii) valence and arousal scores. Supplementary variables enable to relate variables to PCA space, without affecting its solution (Lê et al., 2008). As regards valence and arousal, we also ran parametric (t-tests) against 0 (i.e., neutrality) and non- parametric tests (Mann-Whitney), and compared each set of motive specific words to one another on these two dimensions. However, as these results added little to our supplementary valence and arousal results, we report them in the supplementary material (section D).

6 This could be considered a statistical parallel to Russell’s “precision” (which took a value of 0, if a given word was distributed randomly over the provided categories, and 1, if all subjects attributed a given word to the same category) (Russell, 1980). 7 P-values were also corrected for multiple comparisons (using the Hochberg method). In Table 1, we append an asterisk to the words that survived this more stringent criterion.

18

Motivational Atlas

Results

Motive specific words

For each one of the tested motives, we found a subset of words that significantly (p<0.05) differentiated this motive from each of the other presented motives. This was observed for

210 (68%) of the 305 words and an average of 30 words (SD=8.56) per motivational category. This suggests that participants can indeed semantically distinguish between the seven tested motives and, more specifically, that participants tacitly agree on which words best characterize each of the seven distinctions we proposed. The “motive-specific” words are presented in the database (see Table 1). Within each motivational category they are ordered based on how frequently they were placed in that category8.

word motive valence arousal hard-working [leistungsbereit] achievement*** 8.55 (24.09) 2.73 (27.4) industrious [fleißig] achievement*** 25 (16.07)*** 4.45 (24.52) capable [leistungsfähig] achievement*** 24.6 (15.83)*** 4.23 (26.29) productive [produktiv] achievement*** 22.83 (15.57)*** 1.77 (26.73) overambitious [streberhaft] achievement*** -6.35 (22.68) -11.75 (25.18) ambitious [ehrgeizig] achievement*** 13.25 (18.6)** 10.17 (22.12) pressure-to-achieve [Leistungsdruck] achievement*** -19.43 (18.8)*** 3.77 (30.8) talent [Talent] achievement*** 31.35 (14.35)*** 3.1 (30.85) success-driven [erfolgsorientiert] achievement** 9.38 (22.74) 2.95 (26.69) self-improvement [Selbstverbesserung] achievement** 18.7 (18.96)*** 5.65 (25.82) self [selbst-optimierend] achievement** 15.42 (22.18)** -9.67 (28.76) efficient [effizient] achievement** 18.78 (19.15)*** -5.48 (30.11) motivated [motiviert] achievement** 29.9 (13.61)*** 12.9 (23.84) determined [strebend] achievement** 12.48 (20.92)* 2.15 (26.91) drive [Eigenantrieb] achievement** 23.9 (16.6)*** 7.9 (24.5)

8 The slight label difference of one of the categories between the first 6 and last 4 sessions did not appear to elicit notable differences in the categorization results (see Supplementary material 4 for more details).

19

Motivational Atlas

pressure-to-succeed [Erfolgsdruck] achievement* -19.98 (18.47)*** -4.12 (31.02) gift [Begabung] achievement* 29.85 (13.42)*** 9.58 (26.44) competitive [wetteifernd] achievement* -2.23 (21.37) 4.98 (26.53) active [tatkräftig] achievement 23.92 (16.93)*** 15.9 (21.69)* outstanding [herausragend] achievement 24.47 (18.84)*** 16.3 (20.79)** obstinate [hartnäckig] achievement 10.17 (16.39)* 10.67 (23.07) rational [rational] achievement 11.02 (18.17)* -17.92 (25.17)* realistic [realistisch] achievement 18.88 (16.98)*** -11.42 (27.8) persistent [beharrlich] achievement 15.75 (16.28)*** 2.27 (21.16) victory-oriented [siegesorientiert] achievement 7.52 (21.83) 1.8 (27.01) well-calculated [wohlkalkuliert] achievement 9.33 (15.08)* -15.38 (23.71) self-critical [selbstkritisch] achievement 20.22 (20.01)*** 6.62 (26.16) conventional [angepasst] affiliation*** -13.98 (17.11)** -18.75 (24.38)** ingratiating [einschmeichelnd] affiliation*** -13.52 (19.67)* -13.05 (23.12) entertaining [unterhaltsam] affiliation*** 27.58 (12.81)*** 11.58 (25.31) peer-pressure [Gruppendruck] affiliation** -29 (18.52)*** -9.2 (26.53) excluded [ausgeschlossen] affiliation* -23.35 (29.77)** -2 (29.47) attached [anhänglich] affiliation* -10.67 (15.73)* -10.67 (22.01) popularity [Beliebtheit] affiliation* 23.25 (12.55)*** 7.48 (24.5) sycophantic [anbiedernd] affiliation -26.32 (17.36)*** -12.05 (28.09) assimilated [assimiliert] affiliation -10.65 (17.34) -14.17 (21.66) conformist [konformistisch] affiliation -16.92 (17.57)*** -19.43 (20.65)*** obliging [gefällig] affiliation -4.55 (18.82) -11.12 (22.22) inexperienced [unbedarft] affiliation 0.7 (20.17) -10.27 (23.38) affable [umgänglich] affiliation 27.62 (13.98)*** -1.25 (29.9) obedient [folgsam] affiliation -7.6 (19.21) -14.55 (22.26) talkative [gesprächig] affiliation 20.88 (17.11)*** 4.8 (27.71) popular [beliebt] affiliation 26.65 (13.82)*** 6.88 (27.15) communicative [mitteilsam] affiliation 15.03 (14.45)*** 2.45 (23.07) gregarious [gesellig] affiliation 30.5 (13.49)*** 6.58 (25.56) sociable [kontaktfreudig] affiliation 30.7 (14.87)*** 9.25 (27.91) livid [fuchsteufelswild] anger*** -23.98 (17.09)*** 12.08 (32.67) irritable [reizbar] anger*** -24.1 (15.7)*** 8.98 (25.9) excitable [erregbar] anger*** -2.3 (20.57) 18.47 (24.98)** quick-tempered [hitzig] anger*** -10.65 (17.89) 16.75 (23.58)* impulsive [impulsiv] anger*** -2.2 (18.79) 22.85 (20.46)*** short-tempered [aufbrausend] anger*** -20.95 (18.51)*** 14.35 (27.79) hateful [hasserfüllt] anger*** -42.33 (10.78)*** 8.12 (36.86) aggressive [aggressiv] anger*** -31 (18.46)*** 8.55 (34.89) argumentative [streitlustig] anger*** -21.62 (22.96)*** 7.05 (30.6)

20

Motivational Atlas

resentment [Groll] anger*** -30.25 (14.01)*** 2 (31.37) angry [wütend] anger*** -24.7 (18.67)*** 12.17 (30.5) upset [aufgebracht] anger*** -14.15 (15.68)*** 9.42 (26.02) ill-tempered [übellaunig] anger*** -35.08 (11.31)*** -0.1 (29.79) grouchy [grießgrämig] anger*** -29.75 (13.82)*** -9.17 (25.29) outrage [Empörung] anger*** -10.62 (20.64) 10.48 (28.98) raging [rasend] anger*** -24.68 (17.23)*** 13.17 (31.69) annoyed [genervt] anger*** -27.38 (15.11)*** 2.73 (28.29) offended [beleidigt] anger*** -25.75 (13.47)*** -5.38 (25.56) rebellious [rebellisch] anger** 7.95 (18.14) 20.1 (20.35)*** indignant [ungehalten] anger** -15 (17.15)*** 10 (22.86) tempestuous [stürmisch] anger* 1.52 (19.97) 18.15 (25.77)* spirited [temperamentvoll] anger* 9.05 (17.78) 20.45 (19.06)*** hostile [feindselig] anger* -36.8 (13.34)*** 3.3 (29.64) resistance [Widerstand] anger* 8.12 (21.37) 13.73 (20.25)* defiance [Trotz] anger -17.02 (17.27)*** 3.7 (26.81) booming [aufmüpfig] anger -10.92 (15.33)* 7.33 (22.65) willing-to-fight [kampfbereit] anger 7.67 (23.77) 17.08 (23.78)* stubborn [stur] anger -17.83 (20.7)*** 0.3 (25.88) destructive [zerstörerisch] anger -34.25 (18.44)*** 12 (32.11) warm-hearted [warmherzig] care*** 37.47 (10.95)*** 7.2 (28.53) affectionate [liebevoll] care*** 36.75 (11.83)*** 14.8 (29.07) helpful [hilfsbereit] care*** 37.05 (11.23)*** 4.27 (30.44) merciful [barmherzig] care*** 29.7 (15.45)*** -1.4 (30.76) cordial [herzlich] care*** 33.95 (17.02)*** 13.92 (27.63) soft-hearted [weichherzig] care*** 16.75 (15.21)*** -3.05 (27.44) paternal [väterlich] care*** 19.75 (18.27)*** -2.8 (29.76) kind [lieb] care*** 34.2 (14.9)*** 5.23 (30.55) self-sacrificing [aufopfernd] care*** 11.77 (22.95) 5 (24.31) selfless [selbstlos] care*** 18.5 (21.6)*** 2.98 (27.71) consoling [tröstend] care*** 26.3 (15.6)*** 2.65 (28.98) magnanimous [großherzig] care*** 34.28 (13.63)*** 6.3 (29.59) motherly [mütterlich] care*** 24.22 (18.81)*** -0.75 (29.53) caring [fürsorglich] care*** 31.3 (13)*** 0.88 (28.84) sympathetic2 [mitfühlend] care*** 29.55 (12.65)*** 10.17 (28.43) sensitive [feinfühlig] care*** 29.72 (11.55)*** 2.33 (26.72) supportive [unterstützend] care*** 31.17 (15.31)*** 2.73 (31.2) sympathetic [anteilnehmend] care*** 30.28 (12.49)*** -5.35 (27.84) prudent [umsichtig] care** 23.7 (13.37)*** -3.62 (28.1) protective [beschützend] care** 30.22 (12.28)*** 7.25 (26.58)

21

Motivational Atlas

kind-hearted [gutherzig] care** 29.44 (12.37)*** 3.54 (26.5) social [sozial] care** 29.5 (14.58)*** 4.05 (29.81) understanding [nachsichtig] care** 18.58 (12.86)*** -2.5 (25.75) unselfish [uneigennützig] care** 19.95 (21.47)*** -6.33 (30.29) cautious [behutsam] care* 19.2 (15.3)*** -8.25 (25.09) solidarity [Solidarität] care* 31.3 (14.16)*** 4.23 (31.09) pampering [verhätschelnd] care -19.05 (18.44)*** -11.92 (23.36) concerned [besorgt] care -8.4 (17.03) -3.48 (26.42) friendly [freundlich] care 35.17 (11.01)*** 2.02 (29.92) likeable [sympathisch] care 39.28 (11.79)*** 11.05 (29.38) comradely [kameradschaftlich] care 23.3 (22.75)*** 2.27 (28.06) generous [großzügig] care 30.45 (13.25)*** 9.6 (28.29) nice [nett] care 26.25 (15.1)*** -2.25 (30.69) frank [offenherzig] care 28.78 (13.51)*** 8.92 (24.59) consumerist [konsumorientiert] consumption*** -15.17 (19.74)** -14.38 (27.32) desire to buy [Kauflust] consumption*** -5.58 (20.35) 1.02 (25.92) shoppin-mood [Kauflaune] consumption*** -4.25 (19.21) 2.5 (25.89) passion-for-collecting [Sammelleidenschaft] consumption*** -0.88 (19.42) -7.25 (27.07) covetousness [Besitzdrang] consumption*** -12.38 (18.68)* 0.65 (26.86) pleasure-seeking [genusssüchtig] consumption*** -12.77 (20.25)* 12.33 (22.37) materialistic2 [besitzorientiert] consumption*** -15.83 (19.72)** -11.12 (25.97) materialistic [materialistisch] consumption*** -16.92 (22.84)** -7.65 (28.83) commercialized [kommerzorieniert] consumption** -20.7 (20.11)*** -6.4 (27.73) pride-of-ownership [Besitzerstolz] consumption** -0.38 (17.34) 6.23 (22.95) acquisitive [habsüchtig] consumption** -29.52 (14.85)*** 0.65 (27.42) avaricious2 [raffgierig] consumption** -34.45 (13.73)*** 4.98 (29.8) hoarding [hamsternd] consumption** -13.88 (18.76)** -12.83 (23.63) collecting-mania [Sammelwut] consumption** -20.07 (21.45)*** -8.95 (28.7) acquiring [anschaffend] consumption** -8.3 (18.7) -11.65 (21.35) appetite [Appetit] consumption** 16.7 (13.9)*** 10.17 (29.23) amassing [anhäufend] consumption* -13.17 (17.46)** -11.23 (22.16) accumulating [ansammelnd] consumption* -8.27 (15.81) -15.48 (21.07)* to-pocket [einheimsen] consumption* -16.02 (19.21)** -12.95 (22.95) sensuality [Sinnesfreude] consumption* 27.08 (15.5)*** 19.97 (25.58)** acquisitiveness [Besitzgier] consumption* -28.07 (16.6)*** 2.35 (29.56) avaricious [geldgeil] consumption* -29.77 (19.57)*** 2.17 (29.39) bon-vivant [genießerisch] consumption* 23.12 (15.28)*** 10.73 (27.89) ravenous [heißhungrig] consumption* -3.12 (19.8) 21.4 (20.46)*** opulence [Üppigkeit] consumption 0.2 (17.22) 0.62 (24.47) abundance [Überfluss] consumption -7.4 (25.17) -0.35 (27.03)

22

Motivational Atlas

yearning [Verlangen] consumption 2.7 (19.44) 24.55 (17.93)*** temptation [Versuchung] consumption 0.15 (17.76) 17.58 (21.44)** to-store [aufbewahren] consumption 6.77 (9.92)** -14.3 (21.65)* grasping [habgierig] consumption -31.82 (15.48)*** -2.5 (29.69) to-keep [behalten] consumption 1.23 (14.87) -17.52 (21.52)** accumulate [scheffeln] consumption -18.68 (20.04)*** -5.38 (24.96) cravings [Gelüste] consumption 3.95 (18.44) 23.9 (18.17)*** rummaging [stöbernd] consumption 6.2 (16.91) -10.85 (26.51) desire [Begierde] consumption 2.05 (21.21) 23.75 (19.9)*** possession [Besitz] consumption 3.25 (15.11) -1.23 (25.24) excessiveness [Maßlosigkeit] consumption -22.8 (23.72)*** 7.5 (31.16) gluttonous [gefräßig] consumption -26.77 (16.68)*** 2.38 (28.05) profit-maximizing [gewinnmaximierend] consumption -4.1 (28.35) -6.2 (30.34) wanting [wollend] consumption 9.73 (16.31)* 6.27 (23.14) lack of restraint [Hemmungslosigkeit] consumption -10.17 (22.89) 22.53 (25.7)*** addicted [süchtig] consumption -26.35 (16.59)*** 6.67 (30.84) venal [käuflich] consumption -26.55 (19.2)*** -13.52 (23.6) greedy [gierig] consumption -33.33 (14.73)*** 7.33 (26.13) eager [vorfreudig] consumption 29.8 (14.41)*** 20.72 (23.84)*** impatient [ungeduldig] consumption -21.82 (15.06)*** 0.1 (25.19) panic-stricken [panisch] fear*** -31.95 (15.68)*** 10.6 (35.96) apprehensive [ängstlich] fear*** -27.12 (16.11)*** 0.05 (27.16) frightened [verängstigt] fear*** -27.62 (14.09)*** -0.4 (27.64) timid [furchtsam] fear*** -21.2 (15.18)*** -4.35 (23.03) careful [vorsichtig] fear*** 2.3 (16.04) -13.92 (25.13) risk-averse [risikoscheu] fear*** -11.7 (12.8)*** -17.05 (20.83)** afraid [bange] fear*** -20.05 (14.94)*** -8.17 (23.8) distraught [verstört] fear*** -27.6 (13.4)*** 0.62 (27.85) nervous [nervös] fear*** -21.73 (15.98)*** 7.02 (26.36) overcautious [übervorsichtig] fear*** -19.07 (15)*** -8.5 (22.13) worried [beunruhigt] fear*** -17.9 (13.26)*** -0.85 (23.28) subdued [kleinlaut] fear*** -17.95 (13.98)*** -22.32 (19.58)*** alarmed [alarmiert] fear** -9.73 (14.28)* 10.62 (25.83) bemused [irritiert] fear** -11.92 (11.82)*** -3.98 (26.36) desperate [verzweifelt] fear** -33.3 (17.12)*** 5.88 (30.28) vigilant [wachsam] fear* 16.53 (12.79)*** 3.33 (21.53) self-protective [selbstschützend] fear* 15.62 (15.94)*** -1.88 (27.33) tense [angespannt] fear -15.6 (17.15)*** 1.48 (26.24) defensive [defensiv] fear -5.98 (18.39) -18.55 (23.14)** overwhelmed [überfordert] fear -28.07 (15.79)*** -2.75 (29.91)

23

Motivational Atlas

disconcerted [aufgewühlt] fear -16.85 (15)*** 8.95 (23.68) thin-skinned [dünnhäutig] fear -19.6 (13.77)*** -9.92 (25.43) obsequious [unterwürfig] fear -27.15 (17.19)*** -9.15 (25.81) fear of failure [Versagensangst] fear -33.98 (14.65)*** 8.25 (30.65) authoritarian [autoritär] power*** -16.88 (21.31)** 7.9 (27.8) officious [tonangebend] power*** -8.65 (18.7) 9.67 (22.16) mighty [mächtig] power*** -6.45 (20.87) 17.47 (20.01)*** influential [einflussreich] power*** 11.27 (16.6)** 6.33 (21.64) dominant [dominant] power*** -12.33 (20.44) 16.38 (23.66)* firm [bestimmt] power*** -9.98 (16.68) 8.55 (22.39) arrogant [arrogant] power*** -34.1 (14.01)*** -1.9 (31.51) manipulative [manipulativ] power*** -35.08 (11.78)*** -1.15 (31.65) despotic [despotisch] power*** -27.9 (19.84)*** -0.62 (30.6) conceited [eingebildet] power*** -33.65 (13.38)*** -8.15 (30.46) imperious [herrisch] power*** -33.65 (13.49)*** 6.12 (29.47) sly [durchtrieben] power*** -22.62 (24.31)*** 8.23 (30.44) self-centred [egozentrisch] power*** -22.07 (18.82)*** 2.88 (29.57) self-important [wichtigtuerisch] power*** -31.48 (14.48)*** -4.1 (28.07) tyrannical [tyrannisch] power** -43.45 (9.79)*** 5.15 (35.43) condescending [herablassend] power** -35.95 (17.67)*** -1.3 (32.62) stifling [bevormunden] power** -26.9 (14.51)*** -6.88 (25.14) important [gewichtig] power* 3.25 (11.06) -7.33 (23.66) ruthless [rücksichtslos] power* -31.93 (17.28)*** 1.33 (31.26) significant [bedeutend] power* 21.55 (14.39)*** 6.08 (22.16) self-seeking [selbstsüchtig] power* -33.45 (14.65)*** 0.2 (29.29) complacent [selbstgefällig] power -26.35 (16.92)*** -8.38 (25.2) selfwilled [eigenwillig] power 3 (15.42) 5.95 (21.52) vain [eitel] power -24.02 (19.01)*** -6.75 (23.66) boastful [prahlerisch] power -31.88 (13.56)*** -1.7 (28.09) paternalistic [paternalistisch] power -12.85 (17.08)** -8.73 (22.96) assertive [durchsetzungsfähig] power 21.78 (14.3)*** 7.55 (22.56) contemptuous [geringschätzig] power -30.6 (14.49)*** -6.25 (27.18) strong [stark] power 29.17 (13.28)*** 17.35 (24.74)* convincing [überzeugend] power 21.45 (16.76)*** 5.85 (23.35) disparaging [verächtlich] power -30.68 (14.57)*** 0.33 (29.1)

Table 1 Motive-specific words. Words that participants attributed to one motivational category significantly more than any other, together with their mean valence and arousal scores. *=p<0.05, **=p<0.01, ***=p<0.001, Hochberg corrected.

24

Motivational Atlas

Semantic closeness of seven motives

To explore the relative “semantic distance” between motives we ran a PCA on the categorization judgments. The Kaiser rule suggested retaining the first 2 components (as these had eigenvalues higher than unity) of the PCA. The first component explained 26% of the categorization variance, the second 18%, for a total of 44.2%. The 1st component most clearly separated between affiliation, care and fear, on the one side, and power, achievement, and consumption on the other (all loadings were above 0.40 and below -0.40 respectively). The

2nd component distinguished between anger and fear, on the one side, and care, consumption and achievement on the other (all loadings above 0.4 and below -0.3, respectively) (see Fig. 1, left). With regard to the supplementary valence and arousal variables, valence was correlated to both the PCA components, but more strongly to component 2 (r=0.54, p<0.001), while arousal was not clearly related to either of the dimensions (both rs<0.21) (for statistical comparisons of the valence and arousal of each set of motive-specific words see section C of the supplementary material).

Fig. 1. A semantic and subjective atlas of seven motives. Left: A semantic atlas of seven motives. Principle component analysis (“PCA”) of a word categorization task in which participants were to assign words (points) to 1 of 7 motivational categories (arrows). Words more 25

Motivational Atlas frequently attributed to one motivational category than any other (p<0.05) are surrounded by 95% confidence ellipses. Words that did not have this property are labelled “ambiguous”. They were included in the PCA analysis but are not depicted (i.e., in white) to reduce visual clutter. Blue arrows represent how the valence and arousal of all words related to the PCA space. Right: A subjective atlas of seven motives. Principle component analysis (“PCA”) of a self-report task in which participants were rate on a visual analogue scale how well a number of motive-specific words described their spontaneously experienced motives and feelings at that moment.

Study 2

Methods

Participants

Participants (n=310, mean age=27.19, sd=5.97, females=163, males=147) were recruited via email through the Max Planck’s participant database to take part in two separate studies on economic decision making (Chierchia et al., 2017). As soon as participants were welcomed to the experimental sessions, and thus before being introduced to any of the experimental activities of interest for those studies, they took part in the task described below. Assessments were approved by the Research Ethics Committee (agreement number 090-15-09032015) of the University of Leipzig, Germany. All participants provided informed consent for the anonymized analysis of their data. We report all measures, manipulations, and exclusions in these studies, though no participants were excluded from the analysis.

Procedures

Participants were provided with a list of words and for each one, they were asked to rate in how far they felt “currently driven by these motivations, feelings or states”, on a continuous visual analogue scale ranging -350 to 350 (numbers were not visible to participants). These novel questionnaire items probed the seven motives of interest and were taken from the motive-specific words of study 1, above. Specifically, the fear-related items were

“apprehensive”, “afraid”, “timid”, “nervous”, “panic-stricken”, “overcautious”, “frightened”,

“reserved”; the anger-related items were “aggressive”, “angry”, “offended”, “irritable”, 26

Motivational Atlas

“argumentative”, “tempestuous”, “spirited”, care-related items were “caring”, “protective”,

“kind-hearted”, “cordial”, “helpful”, “affectionate”, “sympathetic”, and “consoling”; power- related items were “mighty”, “dominant”, “authoritarian”, “firm”, “influential”,

“condescending” and “officious”; achievement items were “hard-working”, “industrious”,

“capable”, “efficient”, “ambitious”, “success-driven”, “obstinate”, “productive”; affiliation- related items were “entertaining”, “ingratiating”, “excluded”, “conventional”, “attached”,

“obliging”, “popular”; and consumption items were “consumerist”, “hoarding”, “avaricious”,

“greedy”, “materialistic”, “cheap”, “pleasure-seeking”, “acquisitive”, “desire to buy”,

“gluttonous”. In addition to these motive-related constructs, affect was measured by asking participants to provide ratings on a number of items related either to happiness (“content”,

“happy”, “overjoyed”, “pleasant”, “enthusiastic”) or sadness (“sad”, “downcast”). Overall, the questionnaire consisted of 63 items. The order of the items was fully randomized for each participant, who viewed seven items per page.

Analysis

The ratings on items related to each motive displayed acceptable internal reliability: fear

(Chronbach’s α=0.87), anger (α=0.83), power (α=0.86), care (α=0.92), achievement (α=0.88), affiliation (α=0.67), consumption (α=0.84). We thus first ipsatized the ratings for each subject9 and then aggregated over the items related to each motive. This resulted in 7-motive related and 2 affect related measures, for each participant. As for study 1, we then subjected these measures to principal component analysis, focusing on the seven motive-related measures as variables of interest and using the two affect measures as supplementary

9 Ipsatization (i.e., subtracting a subject’s mean rating from each of that subject’s ratings) is recommended for studies employing unipolar ratings on single adjective items, which otherwise often result in all measures loading on a single component (Moeller, 2015; Russell, 1980).

27

Motivational Atlas variables. As we aimed to establish whether the dimensions that explain the most variance at the semantic level (as investigated in study 1) also do so at the subjective level (as investigated here), we focused on the first two components of the PCA solution.

Results

Subjective-experiential closeness of seven motives

Taken together, the first two components of a PCA solution explained 51.6% of the seven spontaneously occurring motives of interest, as measured by ratings on a visual analogue scale (Fig. 1, right). The first of the components (which explained 29.8% of the total variance) most clearly differentiated between fear, anger and consumption on the one hand (all loadings

> 0.56) and achievement and care on the other (loadings< - 0.50). The second component

(which explained 21.8% of total variance) differentiated between more clearly between care and affiliation on the one end (both loadings < -0.56), and power and achievement on the other (loadings > 0.35). As for the supplementary variables, happiness and sadness were both

(oppositely) related to the 1st dimension (with loadings respectively >0.54 and < -0.63), with the exception of consumption, these dimensions clearly resembled those individuated in study

1, as can be observed in Fig. 1 (right) where, to facilitate comparability with the corresponding figure of study 1 (Fig. 1, left), we plotted the 1st component (i.e., the component that explained the most variance) on the y-axis and the 2nd on the x-axis.

Discussion

As a first step towards an integrative framework of a motivation-based decision making model based on the integration of psychological, biological and behavioral economic literature, the main goal of the present paper was to investigate how seven different motives that have recurred in one or all of these fields (Consumption/Resource-Seeking, Care,

28

Motivational Atlas

Affiliation, Power-Status, Achievement, Threat Avoidance and Threat Approach) relate to one another, both on the semantic-conceptual as well as the subjective-experiential level.

First, the analyses of a word-categorization task revealed that subjects coherently differentiated between the identified seven different motives, as they agreed on which words were more related to one motive as compared to any other of the remaining motives. In spite of this general agreement, participants disagreed more frequently on how to categorize words to some motives more than others. This enabled us to obtain a spatial proxy of the relative conceptual overlap of each of the tested seven motives (Figure 1, left). The 1st dimension of the space differentiated between care, fear and affiliation on one side and power, achievement and consumption on the other. As anticipated, this separation is reminiscent of a communion/agency distinction (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966b; Diehl et al., 2004): with care and affiliation words loading on one side, and power, achievement and consumption loading on the other. The 2nd dimension was partially related to valence and especially differentiated threat approach, threat avoidance, and power, on the one side, from care, affiliation, achievement and consumption, on the other. More specifically, the four predictions delineated in the introduction appeared substantiated by the results. Notably, these anticipated inter-relations amongst motives were not only apparent in the semantic but also in the subjective atlas.

First, in both atlases, care and affiliation were indeed conceptualized and experienced as close. However, a previously reported gradient (Abele et al., 2008) that could differentiate between them was observed along the valence related dimension (rather than on a communality/agency one). In fact, most of the negatively valenced words on the alleged communion side of the space were attributed to the affiliation motive (e.g., “conventional”,

“excluded”, “attached/clingy”, “ingratiating”, “conformist” etc.), while most positive 29

Motivational Atlas communion words were attributed to care (e.g., “warm-hearted”, “soft-hearted”, “motherly”,

“kind”, “understanding” etc.). Indeed, care-specific words were almost entirely positive.

Indeed, of the 32 words that were specifically attributed to care, only 1 – “pampering” – had negative valence. On the other hand, many positively valenced affiliation words (e.g.,

“entertaining”, “popularity”, “talkative”, “affable”, “gregarious”, “communicative”,

“sociable”) did not survive correction for multiple comparisons, suggesting that, as affiliation- related words become positive, they also become more “confused” with care. We thus suggest that care and affiliation are partially distinguished by valence, with care almost exclusively linked to positively valenced concepts, and affiliation more clearly (but not exclusively) associated with negative valence. Also, in addition to the differentiation in terms of valence, and from a more conceptual perspective, nearly all care-specific words clearly denoted other- oriented motivation and behaviors (e.g., “selfless”, “generous”, “sacrificing”, “unselfish”,

“helpful”, “protective” etc.) while those related to affiliation (e.g., “clingy”, “popular”) seemed related to a more self-centred need to belong and to be liked. We thus suggest that, while care and affiliation are both plausibly related to communion (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan,

1966b; Diehl et al., 2004), they could have distinct self vs. other oriented focus: words that subjects agreed best differentiate care from affiliation clearly express a motive to protect/help/accept others, while affiliation-specific words could express the need to be protected/helped/accepted by others.

Similarly, and in line with our second prediction, we found that affiliation is semantically conceptualized and subjectively experienced closer to threat-avoidance (or fear) than care. In fact, previous literature has repeatedly linked affiliation to various negatively valenced states of social anxiety, social exclusion or separation distress (N. Eisenberger, 2012; N. I.

Eisenberger et al., 2003). Moreover, a separate line of studies on for pain and altruistic behavior have shown that when distress prevails over empathic concern, subjects 30

Motivational Atlas tend to make more self-protective and avoidant choices rather than altruistic ones (Batson,

Fultz, & Schoenrade, 1987; Batson, O’Quin, Fultz, Vanderplas, & Isen, 1983). Our second finding could thus further qualify with our first one: Affiliation-related concepts may be more negatively valenced than care-related concepts, partly because the motive of affiliation being also connected to fear of separation and not belonging, and thus being more closely connected to the motive of fear. Such a view would further be in line with findings suggesting that fear induces a more selfishly oriented stance and decreases other-related altruistic behaviors

(Batson et al., 1987, 1983).

As for our third prediction, we found that power-status and achievement-related motives were relatively close and opposed to care and affiliation. Indeed, both of these motives have been previously indirectly associated with agency (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966b; Diehl et al.,

2004). In spite of this relative closeness, it has been suggested that agency could span from positive aspects of agency (such as “independence” and “achievement”), to more “excessive” forms of agency, such as “hunger for power and superiority”, culminating “in aggressive or rude behavior” (Abele et al., 2008; Bakan, 1966b; Diehl et al., 2004). Indeed, this agency spectrum appeared to be reflected in our motivational space, with power/status-related words

(e.g., “influential”, “dominant”, “mighty”, “despotic” etc.) standing vertically somewhere in between those related to more positively valenced achievement (“determined”, “success- driven”, “ambitious”, “motivated”, “capable”, “efficient”, “competitive” etc.) and those related to anger (“aggressive”, “hateful”, “hostile”, “argumentative” etc.). In regards to the content of the two sets of words, it also appears that achievement-specific words are more related to performance and seem only “incidentally” social. For instance, among the achievement-specific words, only the word “competitive” appears to imply some social comparison, while most power-related words more explicitly refer to dominance over others.

31

Motivational Atlas

Schematically, achievement could be conceptualized as socially neutral, or a-social, while power appears more frequently anti-social (Honk, Will, Terburg, & Raub, 2016).

Our fourth hypothesis, also substantiated in the results, was that threat-approach and threat- avoidance would be conceptually close to one another. The proximity of these two motives is in line with the notion that they can be considered as two opposing response tendencies

(approach vs. avoidance) to similar perceived threats (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009).

Indeed, anger and fear-related states are generally both low in valence and high in arousal, and they have been frequently found to cluster closely together in previous word sorting tasks

(Kuppens & Tuerlinckx, 2013; Posner & Russell, 2005; Russell, 1980), and are frequently experienced together (Moons, Eisenberger, & Taylor, 2010).

The only aspect on which the semantic and subjective atlases diverged was in regards to our more exploratory consumption motive. In both atlases, consumption loaded on our alleged

“agency” dimension, however, in the semantic atlas, consumption overlapped highly with achievement and less with power, while in the subjective atlas it shifted away from achievement, and towards threat-approach, or anger. Though unanticipated, we speculate that this difference could be due to the fact that consumption-oriented behaviors are frequently conceptualized as linked to achievement (or perhaps even self-justified in the name of achievement). For instance, it has been suggested that the self-interested maximization of profit or money is clearly “an indicator of achievement, respect, and freedom or power” (Lea

& Webley, 2006, p. 170). However, at the level of experience, it has also been suggested that the pursuit of profit (i.e., consumption) can be used as a socially acceptable or ritualized tool to express aggressive instincts (Behrendt, 2006), and that anger can be a “hidden motivator” behind the desire to buy products (Veling, Ruys, & Aarts, 2012). Our finding that anger and consumption are subjectively experienced but not semantically conceptualized as close are in 32

Motivational Atlas line with this notion, that anger and consumption may share a subconscious link (Behrendt,

2006; Veling et al., 2012).

Finally, in addition to showing that subjects agree on which words best characterize each of seven motives, and to characterizing their semantic and subjective-experiential inter-relations, our sorting task also enabled to obtain a list of 215 motive-specific words, together with the normed valence and arousal scores (Table 1). This database corroborates previous questionnaire and database items. For instance, many of our achievement-related and power- related words have been previously grouped together in a number of tasks and/or questionnaires (Abele et al., 2008; Costa & McCrae, 1992; Lachman & Weaver, 1998;

Lefcourt, 1991). Similarly, some of the words most attributed to care (such as “friendly” and cordial”) have been previously related to the "warmth" and "tender-mindedness" personality factors according to the NEO-PIR (Costa & McCrae, 1992); while the words “soft-hearted”,

“sympathetic” and “loving” are part of Batson’s “empathic response scale” (Batson et al.,

1987). Typical fear words (e.g., “apprehensive, worried, careful, risk-averse, tense, nervous etc.”) are reminiscent of the words that appear in paradigmatic anxiety questionnaires

(Spielberger's, 2010 State-Trait Anxiety Questionnaire), while several of our anger words

(e.g., “annoyed, angry, upset raging, short-tempered”) have also previously been used (e.g.,

Buss & Perry's, 1992 Aggression Questionnaire).

However, our findings also highlight the necessity of validating words for distinct motives. In fact, some words that have often been used in the literature may not be optimal to disambiguate between certain motives. For instance the words “excited” and “concerned” are both used in the State Anxiety Inventory, but while “excited” elicited ambiguous associations in our participants (because it was also frequently attributed to anger), the word “concerned” was actually related more closely to care. Similarly, the words “cordial” or “likeable” are 33

Motivational Atlas related to “warmth” in the NEO-PIR but also to affiliation tendency by Mehrabian’s scale of

Affiliative Tendency (Mehrabian, 1994). Our results indicate that, when confronted with the choice, participants indeed might associate these words with care rather than with affiliation.

Also, the word “careful”/”precautious” is used to describe “competence” in the NEO-PIR

(Costa & McCrae, 1992), but by our participants it was associated to care. Researchers juxtaposing self-report measures to (economic) decision making paradigms (Ben-Shakhar,

Bornstein, & Hopfensitz, 2007; Bosman, Sutter, & Winden, 2005; Eimontaite et al., 2013;

Hopfensitz & Reuben, 2009; Maner et al., 2002; Reuben & Winden, 2008) could use our motive-specific words to better identify the motives underlying specific decisions and investigate whether indeed such motives are related to the corresponding decision patterns anticipated above.

5. Conclusion

Integrating literature from psychology, biology and behavioral economics suggests that at least seven distinct motives are likely to be crucial driving forces of human decision making:

“consumption/resource seeking”, “care”, “affiliation”, “achievement”, “status-power”, “threat approach”, and “threat avoidance”. As a first investigation of such a motivational taxonomy, we analysed how participants differentiate between those motives, and how such motives are inter-related on a semantic-conceptual as well as subjective-experiential level. Results suggest that subjects converge in their semantic differentiation of these seven motives and specify which motives are generally conceptualized as “close” or “far” from one other, relations that we spatially illustrate on a “semantic atlas”. We also find that, with only one exception, semantically close motives also tend to co-occur in actual self-reported experience, while others appear to be antagonistic, and we illustrate these experienced relations in a “subjective atlas”. These atlases could help navigate the complexity of human motivation in a framework that goes beyond the traditional dichotomy of approach and avoidance. For example, they 34

Motivational Atlas suggest that, whereas care and affiliation are close, affiliation is closer to threat-avoidance (or fear) and could thus refer to a more self-centred need to belong and fear of social exclusion, while care is more positively valenced and could denote more altruistic orientation. On the other side of the atlases, we find achievement overlapping highly with power, but with power lying closer to threat-approach (or anger).

Overall, our results may inspire the formulation of new decision making models that highlight the importance of contexts in driving decisions (as recently suggested by Bosworth, Singer, &

Snower, 2016) and propose new distinctions in motivation research, involving motives such as care, affiliation, and consumption/resource seeking. Future research could show how each of the seven motives can be induced through contextual primes, and how utility functions may link these different motivational systems to clear computational predictions for psychological or (neuro)economic experiments. Finally, we provide a database of motive-specific words to be used in future research on the motivational basis of decision making, a subset of which has been recently shown to be sensitive to distinct motivational inductions and to be related to differential decision making patterns in game-theoretic paradigms (Chierchia et al., 2017). In conclusion, we prospect a decision making model that goes beyond approach and avoidance, that is psycho-biologically plausible, and that could justify policies focusing on how institutions, contexts and frames can modulate motivation, and possibly harness it to bolster cooperation across groups and the national boundaries it is generally restricted to.

Ethics

All procedures performed in our study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964

Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

35

Motivational Atlas

Data accessibility

The data are provided in the electronic supplementary material.

Authors' contributions

TS and DS conceived of the study, and, together with GC, MP, and FPL, designed the study.

MP provided the theoretical backbone by carrying out a comprehensive literature review on motivation psychology and biology, and SB on economic theory. FPL and GC coordinated the empirical studies, collected the data and carried out the statistical analyses. All authors helped draft the manuscript and gave final approval for publication.

Competing interests

All authors declare no conflict of interest and no competing interests.

Funding

This study was funded by the Institute for New Economic Thinking (INET - Grant ID

#INO13-00036).

Acknowledgements

The support of the Institute for New Economic Thinking to the authors is gratefully acknowledged. We thank Simon Bartke for remarks on earlier drafts of this manuscript, and

Yvonne Melzer for her literature research during early stages of this paper, especially regarding the biological review.

36

Motivational Atlas

6. References

Abele, A., Uchronski, M., & Suitner, C. (2008). Towards an operationalization of the

fundamental dimensions of agency and communion: Trait content ratings in five

countries considering valence and frequency of. European Journal of.

Adolphs, R. (2013). The biology of fear. Current Biology, 23(2), R79–R93.

Alcaro, A., Huber, R., & Panksepp, J. (2007). Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic

dopaminergic system: an affective neuroethological perspective. Brain Res Rev, 56(2),

283–321. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.014

Alcaro, A., & Panksepp, J. (2011). The SEEKING mind: primal neuro-affective substrates for

appetitive incentive states and their pathological dynamics in addictions and depression.

Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1805–1820.

Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. (2002). The experience of power: examining the effects of power

on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social.

Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk­taking. European Journal of

Social.

Andreoni, J. (1990). Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow

giving. The Economic Journal, 464–477.

Atkinson, J. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review.

Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Oxford, England: Van Nostrand.

Atkinson, J. W., & Feather, N. T. (1966). A theory of achievement motivation (Vol. 66).

Wiley New York.

Bakan, D. (1966a). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion.

Bakan, D. (1966b). The duality of human existence: Isolatio1. Bakan D. The duality of human

existence: Isolation and communion in Western man. Beacon Press (MA); 1966. n and

communion in Western man. Beacon Press (MA).

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2013). Sniffing around oxytocin: 37

Motivational Atlas

review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for

pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry, 3, e258. http://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2013.34

Barrett, L. (2004). Feelings or words? Understanding the content in self-report ratings of

experienced emotion. Journal of Personality and .

Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport, 11(17), 3829–

3834.

Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love.

NeuroImage, 21(3), 1155–1166.

Batson, C. D., & Ahmad, N. (2001). Empathy­induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma II:

what if the target of empathy has defected? European Journal of Social Psychology,

31(1), 25–36.

Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively

distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. Journal of

Personality, 55(1), 19–39.

Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy­induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma.

European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(7), 909–924.

Batson, C. D., O’Quin, K., Fultz, J., Vanderplas, M., & Isen, A. M. (1983). Influence of self-

reported distress and empathy on egoistic versus altruistic motivation to help. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 45(3), 706.

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal

attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497.

Baumgartner, T., Heinrichs, M., Vonlanthen, A., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2008).

Oxytocin shapes the neural circuitry of trust and trust adaptation in humans. Neuron,

58(4), 639–650.

Becker, G. (1974). A theory of social interactions.

Behrendt, R.-P. (2006). The desire to obtain money: A culturally ritualised expression of the 38

Motivational Atlas

aggressive instinct. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(2), 178–179.

http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06259041

Ben-Shakhar, G., Bornstein, G., & Hopfensitz, A. (2007). Reciprocity and emotions in

bargaining using physiological and self-report measures. Journal of Economic.

Berridge, K. C., Ho, C. Y., Richard, J. M., & DiFeliceantonio, A. G. (2010). The tempted

brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating disorders. Brain Res, 1350,

43–64. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.003

Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic

impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev, 28(3), 309–

369.

Boksem, M. A., Mehta, P. H., den Bergh, B. Van, van Son, V., Trautmann, S. T., Roelofs, K.,

… Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Testosterone inhibits trust but promotes reciprocity.

Psychological Science, 24(11), 2306–2314.

Bond, A. J., & Wingrove, J. (2010). The neurochemistry and psychopharmacology of anger

(pp. 79–102). Springer.

Bornemann, B., Kok, B. E., Böckler, A., & Singer, T. (2016). Helping from the heart:

Voluntary upregulation of heart rate variability predicts altruistic behavior. Biological

Psychology, 119, 54–63. http://doi.org/10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2016.07.004

Bos, P., Hermans, E., Ramsey, N., & Honk, J. Van. (2012). The neural mechanisms by which

testosterone acts on interpersonal trust. NeuroImage.

Bosman, R., Sutter, M., & Winden, F. van. (2005). The impact of real effort and emotions in

the power-to-take game. Journal of Economic Psychology.

Bosworth, S., Singer, T., & Snower, D. (2016). Cooperation, motivation and social balance.

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 126, 72–94.

Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational

control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron, 68(5), 815–834. 39

Motivational Atlas

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022

Bruce, N., & Waldman, M. (1990). The rotten-kid theorem meets the Samaritan’s dilemma.

The Quarterly Journal of Economics.

Burnham, T. C. (2007). High-testosterone men reject low ultimatum game offers.

Proceedings.Biological Sciences / The Royal Society, 274(1623), 2327–2330.

Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 63(3), 452.

Camerer, C. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton

University Press.

Camerer, C., & Loewenstein, G. (2004). Behavioral Economics: Past, Present, Future. In C.

Camerer, G. Loewenstein, & M. Rabin (Eds.), Advances in Behavioral Economics (pp.

3–51). Princeton University Press. http://doi.org/citeulike-article-id:9087695

Canessa, N., & Crespi, C. (2013). The functional and structural neural basis of individual

differences in loss aversion. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(36), 14307–14317.

Carpenter, J., & Matthews, P. H. (2009). What norms trigger punishment? Experimental

Economics, 12(3), 272–288. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-009-9214-z

Carter, C. S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love.

Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23(8), 779–818.

Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and

implications. Psychol Bull, 135(2), 183–204. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0013965

Chierchia, G., Lesemann, F. H. P., Snower, D., Vogel, M., & Singer, T. (2017). Caring

Cooperators and Powerful Punishers: Differential Effects of Induced Care and Power

Motivation on Different Types of Economic Decision Making. Scientific Reports, 7(1),

11068. http://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-11580-8

Cialdini, R., & Goldstein, N. (2004). Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annu.

Rev. Psychol. 40

Motivational Atlas

Cole, H. L., Mailath, G. J., & Postlewaite, A. (1992). Social Norms, Savings Behavior, and

Growth. Journal of Political Economy, 100(6), 1092–1125.

http://doi.org/10.2307/2138828

Condon, P., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2013). Conceptualizing and experiencing compassion.

Emotion, 13(5), 817–821. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0033747

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and

NEO Five Factor Inventory: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509

Cuddy, A., Fiske, S., & Glick, P. (2007). The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect

and stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social.

Daw, N. D., Kakade, S., & Dayan, P. (2002). Opponent interactions between serotonin and

dopamine. Neural Netw, 15(4–6), 603–616.

Dawes, R. (1980). Social dilemmas. Annual Review of Psychology.

Dayan, P., & Balleine, B. W. (2002). Reward, motivation, and reinforcement learning.

Neuron, 36(2), 285–298.

Dayan, P., & Berridge, K. C. (2014). Model-based and model-free Pavlovian reward learning:

revaluation, revision, and revelation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci, 14(2), 473–492.

http://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0277-8 de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Chimpanzee politics : power and sex among apes. Baltimore, Md.:

Johns Hopkins University Press.

Deci, E. L., & Moller, A. C. (2005). The Concept of Competence: A Starting Place for

Understanding Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determined Extrinsic Motivation.

Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., & Kiyonari, T. (2010). Oxytocin and cooperation under

conditions of uncertainty: the modulating role of incentives and social information.

Hormones and Behavior, 57(3), 368–374.

Delgado, M. R., Gillis, M. M., & Phelps, E. A. (2008). Regulating the expectation of reward 41

Motivational Atlas

via cognitive strategies. Nature Neuroscience, 11(8), 880–881.

http://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2141

Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality:

Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral and Brain

Sciences, 22(3), 491–517.

Diehl, M., Owen, S., & Youngblade, L. (2004). Agency and communion attributes in adults’

spontaneous self-representations. International Journal of.

Dölen, G., Darvishzadeh, A., Huang, K., & Malenka, R. (2013). Social reward requires

coordinated activity of nucleus accumbens oxytocin and serotonin. Nature.

Eimontaite, I., Nicolle, A., Schindler, I., & Goel, V. (2013). The effect of partner-directed

emotion in social exchange decision-making. Frontiers in Psychology, 4.

Eisenberger, N. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural

underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(6), 421–

434.

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An

FMRI study of social exclusion. Science (New York, N.Y.), 302(5643), 290–292.

Eisenegger, C., Haushofer, J., & Fehr, E. (2011). The role of testosterone in social interaction.

Trends Cogn Sci, 15(6), 263–271. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.008

Eisenegger, C., Naef, M., Snozzi, R., Heinrichs, M., & Fehr, E. (2010). Prejudice and truth

about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour. Nature, 463(7279), 356–

359.

Emmons, R. A., & McAdams, D. P. (1991). Personal Strivings and Motive Dispositions:

Exploring the Links. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(6), 648–654.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167291176007

Engen, H. G., & Singer, T. (2015). Compassion-based emotion regulation up-regulates

experienced positive affect and associated neural networks. Social Cognitive and 42

Motivational Atlas

Affective Neuroscience.

Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology of trust. Journal of the European Economic

Association.

Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785–

791.

Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and

Human Behavior, 25(2), 63–87.

Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior,

61(3), 380–391.

Fessler, D. M. T., Pillsworth, E. G., & Flamson, T. J. (2004). Angry men and disgusted

women: An evolutionary approach to the influence of emotions on risk taking.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(1), 107–123.

Fiske, S., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth

and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83.

Fodor, E. M., & Riordan, J. M. (1995). Leader Power Motive and Group Conflict as

Influences on Leader Behavior and Group Member Self-Affect. Journal of Research in

Personality, 29(4), 418–431. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1995.1024

Fodor, E. M., Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2010). Power motivation. Implicit

Motives, 3–29.

Frank, R. H. (1985). The demand for unobservable and other nonpositional goods. The

American Economic Review, 101–116.

Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. (W. W. N. &

Co, Ed.). New York.

Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 453.

Gilboa, I., & Schmeidler, D. (2001). A theory of case-based decisions. 43

Motivational Atlas

Glimcher, P., & Fehr, E. (2013). Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain.

Gómez-Miñambres, J. (2012). Motivation through goal setting. Journal of Economic

Psychology.

Gordon, I., Martin, C., Feldman, R., & Leckman, J. F. (2011). Oxytocin and social

motivation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 471–493.

Guinote, A., Cotzia, I., & Sandhu, S. (2015). Social status modulates prosocial behavior and

egalitarianism in preschool children and adults. Proceedings of the.

Hall, J. L., Stanton, S. J., Schultheiss, O. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2010).

Biopsychological and neural processes of implicit motivation. Implicit Motives, 279–

307.

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of

Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–16. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-

5193(64)90038-4

Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and

voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science (New York, N.Y.),

316(5831), 1622–1625.

Hartley, C., & Phelps, E. (2012). Anxiety and decision-making. Biological Psychiatry.

Heckhausen, H. (1989a). Motivation und Handeln. Berlin: Springer.

Heckhausen, H. (1989b). Motivation und Handlung. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York.

Heckhausen, J. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral

Scientist, 43(6), 1015–1029.

Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2010). Motivation und Entwicklung (pp. 427–488).

Springer.

Hildebrandt, L. K., McCall, C., & Singer, T. (2017). Differential Effects of Attention-,

Compassion-, and Socio-Cognitively Based Mental Practices on Self-Reports of

Mindfulness and Compassion. Mindfulness, 8(6), 1488–1512. 44

Motivational Atlas

http://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0716-z

Honk, J. van, Will, G., Terburg, D., & Raub, W. (2016). Effects of testosterone administration

on strategic gambling in poker play. Scientific.

Hopfensitz, A., & Reuben, E. (2009). The importance of emotions for the effectiveness of

social punishment. The Economic Journal.

Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: an introduction to behavior theory.

Ikemoto, S., & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated

behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain

Research Reviews, 31(1), 6–41.

Insel, T. R., & Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 2(2), 129–136.

Israel, S., Weisel, O., Ebstein, R., & Bornstein, G. (2012). Oxytocin, but not vasopressin,

increases both parochial and universal altruism. Psychoneuroendocrinology.

Jackson, D. N. (1974). Manual for the personality research form. Goshen, NY: Research

Psychologists Press. Josiassen, RC, Shagass, C., Roemer, RA, Ercegovac, DV, &

Straumanis, JJ (1982). Somatosensory Evoked Potential Changes with a Selective

Attention Task. Psychophysiology, 19, 146–159.

Joshi, P. D., & Fast, N. J. (2013). Power and Reduced Temporal Discounting. Psychological

Science. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457950

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk.

Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291. http://doi.org/10.2307/1914185

Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. (2010). Leipzig affective norms for German: A reliability study.

Behavior Research Methods.

Kasser, T. (2006). Materialism and Its Alternatives.

Kasser, T., Rosenblum, K. L., Sameroff, A. J., Deci, E. L., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., …

Dungan, N. (2014). Changes in materialism, changes in psychological well-being: 45

Motivational Atlas

Evidence from three longitudinal studies and an intervention experiment. Motivation and

Emotion, 38(1), 1–22.

Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2012). Functional Neural Plasticity

and Associated Changes in Positive Affect After Compassion Training. Cerebral Cortex.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs142

Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2014). Differential pattern of

functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and

Affective Neuroscience, 9(6), 873–879.

Kollock, P. (1998). Social dilemmas: The anatomy of cooperation. Annual Review of

Sociology, 24, 183–214.

Kosfeld, M., Heinrichs, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2005). Oxytocin increases

trust in humans. Nature, 435(7042), 673–676.

Kugler, T., Kausel, E. E., & Kocher, M. G. (2012). Are groups more rational than

individuals? A review of interactive decision making in groups. {…} Reviews: Cognitive

Science.

Kumru, C., & Vesterlund, L. (2010). The effect of status on charitable giving. Journal of

Public Economic Theory.

Kuppens, P., & Tuerlinckx, F. (2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective

experience. Psychological ….

Lachman, M. E., & Weaver, S. L. (1998). The sense of control as a moderator of social class

differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

74(3), 763.

Lammers, J., Galinsky, A. D., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the

effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19(6), 558–564.

Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M. (2010). Emotion and the motivational brain. Biological

Psychology, 84(3), 437–450. 46

Motivational Atlas

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.10.007

Lange, P. Van, Balliet, D., Parks, C., & Vugt, M. Van. (2013). Social dilemmas:

Understanding human cooperation.

Langner, C. A., & Winter, D. G. (2001). The motivational basis of concessions and

compromise: Archival and laboratory studies. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 81(4), 711.

Lê, S., Josse, J., & Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis.

Journal of Statistical Software.

Lea, S. E. G., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological

psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(2), 161–209.

LeDoux, J. E. (2014). The Psychological Construction of Emotion. (J. A. Barrett, L. F., &

Russell, Ed.). Guilford Publications.

Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control.

Leiberg, S., Klimecki, O., & Singer, T. (2011). Short-term compassion training increases

prosocial behavior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS One, 6(3), e17798.

Lerner, J., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology.

Lewin, K. (1936). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers. The Journal of Nervous

and Mental Disease, 84(5), 612–613.

Loewenstein, G. F., & O’Donoghue, T. (2004). Animal Spirits: Affective and Deliberative

Processes in Economic Behavior. SSRN Electronic Journal.

http://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.539843

Maner, J. K., & Gerend, M. A. (2007). Motivationally selective risk judgments: Do fear and

curiosity boost the boons or the banes? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 103(2), 256–267. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2006.08.002

Maner, J. K., Luce, C. L., Neuberg, S. L., Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S., & Sagarin, B. J. (2002). 47

Motivational Atlas

The effects of perspective taking on motivations for helping: Still no evidence for

altruism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(11), 1601–1610.

Martino, B. De, & Camerer, C. (2010). Amygdala damage eliminates monetary loss aversion.

Proceedings of the.

Mas-Colell, A., Whinston, M. D., & Green, J. R. (1995). Microeconomic theory. New York:

Oxford University Press.

Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain

Sciences, 21(3), 353–363.

McAdams, D. P. (1980). A thematic coding system for the intimacy motive. Journal of

Research in Personality, 14(4), 413–432. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-

6566(80)90001-X

McCall, C., & Singer, T. (2012). The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social

cognition, motivation and behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 681–688.

McCall, C., Steinbeis, N., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2014). Compassion meditators show less

anger, less punishment, and more compensation of victims in response to fairness

violations. Frontiers in , 8, 424.

McClelland, D. C. (1953). The achievement motive, by David C. McClelland [et al.]. New

York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

McClelland, D. C. (1965). Toward a theory of motive acquisition. American Psychologist,

20(5), 321–333.

McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1955). The Achievement

Motive, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1953. Studies in Motivation, New York:

Appleton-Century-Crofts.

McDougall, W. (1932). The energies of man. A study of the fundamentals of dynamic

psychology.

Mehrabian, A. (1994). Evidence bearing on the affiliative tendency (MAFF) and sensitivity to 48

Motivational Atlas

rejection (MSR) scales. Current Psychology.

Moeller, J. (2015). A word on standardization in longitudinal studies: don’t. Frontiers in

Psychology, 6, 1389. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01389

Montague, P. R., Hyman, S. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Computational roles for dopamine in

behavioural control. Nature, 431(7010), 760–767.

Moons, W., Eisenberger, N., & Taylor, S. (2010). Anger and fear responses to stress have

different biological profiles. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity.

Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

Murray, H. A. (1938a). Explorations in personality.

Murray, H. A. (1938b). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty

men of college age.

Murray, H. A. (1969). Explorations in personality : a clinical and experimental study of fifty

men of college age. New York: Oxford University Press.

Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida

free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods,

Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 402–407.

Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment:

contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral

Reviews, 22(3), 437–452.

Nelson, R. J., & Trainor, B. C. (2007). Neural mechanisms of aggression. Nature Reviews

Neuroscience, 8(7), 536–546.

O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J., & Andrews, C. (2001). Abstract

reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nature

Neuroscience, 4(1), 95–102.

Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation

in children. Developmental Science, 12(3), F1–F8. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467- 49

Motivational Atlas

7687.2008.00820.x

Palfrey, T., & Rosenthal, H. (1988). Private incentives in social dilemmas: The effects of

incomplete information and altruism. Journal of Public Economics.

Pang, J. S. (2010). The achievement motive: A review of theory and assessment of n

achievement, hope of success, and fear of failure. Implicit Motives, 1, 30–71.

Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and

humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(1), 30–80.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.10.004

Panksepp, J. (2006). The core emotional systems of the mammalian brain: The fundamental

substrates of human emotions. In About a body: Working with the embodied mind in

psychotherapy (pp. 14–32).

Polman, E., & Kim, S. H. (2013). Effects of anger, disgust, and sadness on sharing with

others. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(12), 1683–1692.

Porcelli, A., & Delgado, M. (2009). Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision

making. Psychological Science.

Posner, J., & Russell, J. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to

affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and.

Potegal, M., & Stemmler, G. (2010). Constructing a neurology of anger (pp. 39–59). Springer.

Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational

influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and

Human Decision Processes, 79(1), 56–77.

Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the

neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545–

556.

Reiss, S. (2004). Multifaceted nature of intrinsic motivation: The theory of 16 basic desires.

Review of General Psychology, 8(3), 179. 50

Motivational Atlas

Reuben, E., & Winden, F. Van. (2008). Social ties and coordination on negative reciprocity:

The role of affect. Journal of Public Economics.

Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2010). Motive training and motivational competence. Implicit

Motives, 510–548.

Rilling, J. K. (2013). The neural and hormonal bases of human parentalcare.

Neuropsychologia, 51(4), 731–747.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.017

Rilling, J. K., DeMarco, A. C., Hackett, P. D., Thompson, R., Ditzen, B., Patel, R., &

Pagnoni, G. (2012). Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative

behavior and associated brain activity in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(4), 447–

461.

Robson, A. (2001). The biological basis of economic behavior. Journal of Economic

Literature.

Robson, A. J., & Samuelson, L. (2010). The evolutionary foundations of preferences. In J.

Benhabib, A. Bisin, & M. O. Jackson (Eds.), Handbook of social economics (Vol. 1, pp.

221–310). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones

on fear circuitry. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 289–313.

Roseman, I. J. (2011). Emotional Behaviors, Emotivational Goals, Emotion Strategies:

Multiple Levels of Organization Integrate Variable and Consistent Responses. Emotion

Review, 3(4), 434–443. http://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410744

Ross, H. E., & Young, L. J. (2009). Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social

cognition and affiliative behavior. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 30(4), 534–547.

Russell, J. A. (1980). a Circumplex Model of Affect. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. http://doi.org/10.1037/h0077714

Sally, D. (2001). On sympathy and games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 51

Motivational Atlas

44(1), 1–30.

Salvador, A. (2005). Coping with competitive situations in humans. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(1), 195–205.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.07.004

Samuelson, P. A. (1938). A Note on the Pure Theory of Consumer’s Behaviour. Economica,

5(17), 61–71. http://doi.org/10.2307/2548836

Schultheiss, O., & Brunstein, J. (2010). Implicit motives. Oxford University Press.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Strasser, A. (2012). Referential processing and competence as

determinants of congruence between implicit and explicit motives. Handbook of Self-

Knowledge, 39–62.

Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Waugh, C. E., Stanton, S. J., Meier, E. A., & Reuter-

Lorenz, P. (2008). Exploring the motivational brain: effects of implicit power motivation

on brain activation in response to facial expressions of emotion. Social Cognitive and

Affective Neuroscience, 3(4), 333–343. http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn030

Schultz, W. (2007). Behavioral dopamine signals. Trends in , 30(5), 203–210.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.007

Schultz, W. (2010). Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: basic and recent data.

Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6(1), 1–9. http://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-6-24

Seip, E., Dijk, W. Van, & Rotteveel, M. (2014). Anger motivates costly punishment of unfair

behavior. Motivation and Emotion.

Selten, R. (1998). Aspiration adaptation theory. Journal of Mathematical Psychology.

Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. (2008). Emotion, Decision Making, and the Amygdala. Neuron,

58(5), 662–671. http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.020

Seymour, B., Singer, T., & Dolan, R. (2007). The neurobiology of punishment. Nature

Reviews Neuroscience, 8(4), 300–311.

Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., & Kirson, D. (1987). Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a 52

Motivational Atlas

prototype approach. Journal of Personality.

Siever, L. J. (2008). Neurobiology of aggression and violence.

Singer, T., & Klimecki, O. M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Current Biology, 24(18),

R875–R878.

Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2009). Differential Roles of Fairness­and Compassion­Based

Motivations for Cooperation, Defection, and Punishment. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1167(1), 41–50.

Small, D. A., & Lerner, J. S. (2008). Emotional policy: Personal sadness and anger shape

judgments about a welfare case. Political Psychology, 29(2), 149–168.

Spielberger, C. D. (2010). State­Trait Anxiety Inventory. Wiley Online Library.

Stanton, S., Liening, S., & Schultheiss, O. (2011). Testosterone is positively associated with

risk taking in the Iowa Gambling Task. Hormones and Behavior.

Stanton, S., O’Dhaniel, A., & McLaurin, R. (2011). Low-and high-testosterone individuals

exhibit decreased aversion to economic risk. Psychological.

Stigler, G., & Becker, G. (1977). De gustibus non est disputandum. The American Economic

Review.

Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment predicts

maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(13),

2655–2666.

Straub, P., & Murnighan, J. (1995). An experimental investigation of ultimatum games:

Information, fairness, expectations, and lowest acceptable offers. Journal of Economic

Behavior & Organization.

Strobel, A., Zimmermann, J., Schmitz, A., Reuter, M., Lis, S., Windmann, S., & Kirsch, P.

(2011). Beyond revenge: neural and genetic bases of altruistic punishment. NeuroImage,

54(1), 671–680.

Terburg, D., & van Honk, J. (2013). Approach–avoidance versus dominance–submissiveness: 53

Motivational Atlas

A multilevel neural framework on how testosterone promotes social status. Emotion

Review, 5(3), 296–302.

Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative

processes in animals. The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 2(4), i-109.

http://doi.org/10.1037/h0092987

Toates, F. M. (1986). Motivational Systems.

Trudewind, C. (2000). Curiosity and anxiety as motivational determinants of cognitive

development.

Tusche, A., & Böckler, A. (2016). Decoding the Charitable Brain: Empathy, Perspective

Taking, and Attention Shifts Differentially Predict Altruistic Giving. The Journal of ….

Valk, S. L., Bernhardt, B. C., Trautwein, F.-M., Böckler, A., Kanske, P., Guizard, N., …

Singer, T. (2017). Structural plasticity of the social brain: Differential change after socio-

affective and cognitive mental training. Science Advances, 3(10), e1700489.

http://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1700489

Varian, H. R. (2006). Revealed preference. In M. Szenberg, L. Ramrattan, & A. A. Gottesman

(Eds.), Samuelsonian economics and the twenty-first century (pp. 99–115).

Veling, H., Ruys, K. I., & Aarts, H. (2012). Anger as a Hidden Motivator. Social

Psychological and Personality Science, 3(4), 438–445.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611425425

Weinberger, J., Cotler, T., & Fishman, D. (2010). The duality of affiliative motivation.

Implicit Motives, 1, 71–89.

Weiner, B. (1990). History of motivational research in education. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 82(4), 616–622. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.616

Weng, H. Y., Fox, A. S., Shackman, A. J., Stodola, D. E., Caldwell, J. Z., Olson, M. C., …

Davidson, R. J. (2013). Compassion training alters altruism and neural responses to

suffering. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1171–1180. 54

Motivational Atlas

Wright, J. S., & Panksepp, J. (2012). An evolutionary framework to understand foraging,

wanting, and desire: the neuropsychology of the SEEKING system.

Neuropsychoanalysis, 14(1), 5–39.

Yamagishi, T., Horita, Y., Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., Li, Y., Shinada, M., … Simunovic, D.

(2012). Rejection of unfair offers in the ultimatum game is no evidence of strong

reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of

America, 109(50), 20364–20368.

Yamagishi, T., Horita, Y., Takagishi, H., Shinada, M., Tanida, S., & Cook, K. S. (2009). The

private rejection of unfair offers and emotional commitment. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(28), 11520–11523.

Zak, P. J., Kurzban, R., Ahmadi, S., Swerdloff, R. S., Park, J., Efremidze, L., … Matzner, W.

(2009). Testosterone administration decreases generosity in the ultimatum game. PLoS

One, 4(12), e8330.

55

Motivational Atlas

56

Motivational Atlas

Supplementary material

A. Supplementary figure 1: Example Screen display during the Semantic Categorization

Task. Participants were shown a list of either 174 or 305 words (random order) on the left and were asked to drag and drop each of the words into one of the seven boxes on the right. Each of the boxes represented one of the seven postulated motives. There were no further instructions regarding time limit or number of words that should be sorted into the boxes.

*

B. Semantic categorization task instructions

“Please think of a situation where you, or another person was driven by one of the following motivations. Which of the words on the left best describes which motivation? Which sentiments or conditions could have accompanied that situation? Please drag the words into the respective boxes. There are no right or wrong answers. We are only interested in your personal opinion.”

57

Motivational Atlas

C. Valence and arousal of motive-specific words

Parametric and non-parametric comparisons against 0 (i.e., neutrality) revealed that words specifically attributed to care were higher on valence than words related to any other motive

(all ps<0.01), with a mean score of 25 (95% CI [21 29], p<0.001). Words related to achievement were also significantly positive (m=13, 95% CI [8 18], p<0.001), but slightly less so than care-related words. Affiliation words were neutral on average (m=2, 95% CI [-6

11], p>0.5), while the remaining motivational categories – anger (m=-16, 95% CI [-22 -11]), fear (m=-16, 95% CI [-22 -10]), power (m=-18, 95% CI [-25 -11]) and consumption (m=-9,

95% CI [-14 -4]) - were all significantly negatively valenced (all ps<0.001). More specifically, fear, power and anger did not significantly differ in terms of negative valence (all ps>0.1), while the same motives were all slightly more negative than consumption (all ps<0.05). As for arousal, words related to anger were most clearly rated as “high-arousal” (m=10, 95% CI [7

13], p<0.001), and indeed more so than any other motive (all ps<0.001). Care-related words were also slightly, but significantly, high on arousal (m=2, CI [0.1 4.7], p<0.05)), while affiliation words were slightly low (m=-5, 95% CI [-9 -0.5], p<0.05). The remaining motives

(power, achievement, fear and consumption) did not significantly differ from “neutrality” (all ps>0.1). Affiliation however was also lower on arousal than all other motives except fear (all ps<0.05), while fear was only significantly lower than power (p<0.05) and differed from achievement at the trend level (p<0.1). Power, consumption and achievement did not significantly differ from one another on arousal. Finally, we observed a positive correlation

(r=0.23, p<0.001) and a weak trend for a quadratic relationship (p=0.09) between valence and arousal scores.

D. Consistency between experimental sessions

Since there was a slight label difference between the 2 experimental sessions for our experimental “consumption” category, we re-ran our analysis on the common set of words 58

Motivational Atlas

(n=174) of each session separately. In session 1, 99 words out of 174 were placed in one motivational category more than any of the others, while this occurred for 118 words in session 2. 86 words resulted to be “motive-specific” in both sessions, and all of these words were ascribed to the same motivational categories across both sessions. Thus 86% of the motive-specific words of session 1 were confirmed by session 2 and 72% of words of session

2 were corroborated by session 1. This speaks to a satisfactory reliability of our measure of motive-specificity across different participant pools (i.e., sessions 1 and 2). Indeed, no word was ascribed to different motivational categories in the two sessions (rather, some words appeared to pass the statistical threshold for motive-specificity in one of the sessions but not for the other). Finally, the number of words that resulted to be motive-specific in one session but not the other were distributed as follows over the seven motivational categories: achievement=7, affiliation=8, anger=3, care=6, fear=3, power=6 and consumption=6. This suggests that changing the label of the “consumption” category from session 1 to session 2 did not affect the categorization results.

59