An Introduction to English Morphology: Words and Their Structure

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

An Introduction to English Morphology: Words and Their Structure An Introduction to English Morphology: Words and Their Structure Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy Edinburgh University Press 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page i An Introduction to English Morphology 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page ii Edinburgh Textbooks on the English Language General Editor Heinz Giegerich, Professor of English Linguistics (University of Edinburgh) Editorial Board Laurie Bauer (University of Wellington) Derek Britton (University of Edinburgh) Olga Fischer (University of Amsterdam) Norman Macleod (University of Edinburgh) Donka Minkova (UCLA) Katie Wales (University of Leeds) Anthony Warner (University of York) An Introduction to English Syntax Jim Miller An Introduction to English Phonology April McMahon An Introduction to English Morphology Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page iii An Introduction to English Morphology Words and Their Structure Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy Edinburgh University Press 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page iv To Jeremy © Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy, 2002 Edinburgh University Press Ltd 22 George Square, Edinburgh Typeset in Janson by Norman Tilley Graphics and printed and bound in Great Britain by MPG Books Ltd, Bodmin A CIP Record for this book is available from the British Library ISBN 0 7486 1327 7 (hardback) ISBN 0 7486 1326 9 (paperback) The right of Andrew Carstairs-McCarthy to be identified as author of this work has been asserted in accordance with the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. Contents Acknowledgements viii 1 Introduction 1 Recommendations for reading 3 2 Words, sentences and dictionaries 4 2.1 Words as meaningful building-blocks of language 4 2.2 Words as types and words as tokens 5 2.3 Words with predictable meanings 6 2.4 Non-words with unpredictable meanings 9 2.5 Conclusion: words versus lexical items 12 Exercises 13 Recommendations for reading 14 3 A word and its parts: roots, affixes and their shapes 16 3.1 Taking words apart 16 3.2 Kinds of morpheme: bound versus free 18 3.3 Kinds of morpheme: root, affix, combining form 20 3.4 Morphemes and their allomorphs 21 3.5 Identifying morphemes independently of meaning 23 3.6 Conclusion: ways of classifying word-parts 26 Exercises 27 Recommendations for reading 27 4 A word and its forms: inflection 28 4.1 Words and grammar: lexemes, word forms and grammatical words 28 4.2 Regular and irregular inflection 31 4.3 Forms of nouns 34 4.4 Forms of pronouns and determiners 38 4.5 Forms of verbs 39 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page vi vi AN INTRODUCTION TO ENGLISH MORPHOLOGY 4.6 Forms of adjectives 40 4.7 Conclusion and summary 42 Exercises 42 Recommendations for reading 43 5 A word and its relatives: derivation 44 5.1 Relationships between lexemes 44 5.2 Word classes and conversion 45 5.3 Adverbs derived from adjectives 48 5.4 Nouns derived from nouns 49 5.5 Nouns derived from members of other word classes 50 5.6 Adjectives derived from adjectives 52 5.7 Adjectives derived from members of other word classes 53 5.8 Verbs derived from verbs 54 5.9 Verbs derived from member of other word classes 55 5.10 Conclusion: generality and idiosyncrasy 56 Exercises 57 Recommendations for reading 58 6 Compound words, blends and phrasal words 59 6.1 Compounds versus phrases 59 6.2 Compound verbs 60 6.3 Compound adjectives 61 6.4 Compound nouns 61 6.5 Headed and headless compounds 64 6.6 Blends and acronyms 65 6.7 Compounds containing bound combining forms 66 6.8 Phrasal words 67 6.9 Conclusion 68 Exercises 68 Recommendations for reading 69 7 A word and its structure 71 7.1 Meaning and structure 71 7.2 Affixes as heads 71 7.3 More elaborate word forms: multiple affixation 72 7.4 More elaborate word forms: compounds within compounds 76 7.5 Apparent mismatches between meaning and structure 79 7.6 Conclusion: structure as guide but not straitjacket 82 Exercises 83 Recommendations for reading 84 01 pages i-viii prelims 18/10/01 3:42 pm Page vii CONTENTS vii 8 Productivity 85 8.1 Introduction: kinds of productivity 85 8.2 Productivity in shape: formal generality and regularity 85 8.3 Productivity in meaning: semantic regularity 88 8.4 Semantic blocking 91 8.5 Productivity in compounding 93 8.6 Measuring productivity: the significance of neologisms 95 8.7 Conclusion: ‘productivity’ in syntax 97 Exercises 98 Recommendations for reading 99 9 The historical sources of English word formation 100 9.1 Introduction 100 9.2 Germanic, Romance and Greek vocabulary 100 9.3 The rarity of borrowed inflectional morphology 102 9.4 The reduction in inflectional morphology 104 9.5 Characteristics of Germanic and non-Germanic derivation 106 9.6 Fashions in morphology 108 9.7 Conclusion: history and structure 110 Exercises 111 Recommendations for reading 113 10 Conclusion: words in English and in languages generally 114 10.1 A puzzle: disentangling lexemes, word forms and lexical items 114 10.2 Lexemes and lexical items: possible reasons for their overlap in English 115 10.3 Lexemes and lexical items: the situation outside English 116 10.4 Lexemes and word forms: the situation outside English 118 Recommendations for reading 119 Discussion of the exercises 120 Glossary 141 References 148 Index 150 Acknowledgements I would like to thank Heinz Giegerich for inviting me to write this book, and him and Laurie Bauer for useful comments on a draft version. I must admit that, when I set out to write what is intended as an introductory text on an extremely well-described language, I did not expect to learn anything new myself; but I have enjoyed discovering and rediscovering both new and old questions that arise from the study of morphology and its interaction with syntax and the lexicon, even if I cannot claim to have provided any conclusive new answers. The Library of the University of Canterbury has, as always, been efficient in supplying research material. I would also like to thank my partner Jeremy Carstairs-McCarthy for constant support and help. viii 02 pages 001-152 18/10/01 3:43 pm Page 1 1 Introduction The term ‘word’ is part of everyone’s vocabulary. We all think we understand what words are. What’s more, we are right to think this, at some level. In this book I will not suggest that our ordinary notion of the word needs to be replaced with something radically different. Rather, I want to show how our ordinary notion can be made more precise. This will involve teasing apart the bundle of ingredients that go to make up the notion, showing how these ingredients interact, and introducing ways of talking about each one separately. After reading this book, you will still go on using the term ‘word’ in talking about language, both in everyday conversation and in more formal contexts, such as literary criticism or English language study; but I hope that, in these more formal contexts, you will talk about words more confidently, knowing exactly which ingredients of the notion you have in mind at any one time, and able where necessary to use appropriate terminology in order to make your meaning absolutely clear. This is a textbook for students of the English language or of English literature, not primarily for students of linguistics. Nevertheless, what I say will be consistent with mainstream linguistic views on word- structure, so any readers who go on to more advanced linguistics will not encounter too many inconsistencies. A good way of teasing apart the ingredients in the notion ‘word’ is by explicitly contrasting them. Here are the contrasts that we will be looking at, and the chapters where they will be discussed: • words as units of meaning versus units of sentence structure (Chapters 2, 6, 7) • words as pronounceable entities (‘word forms’) versus more abstract entities (sets of word forms) (Chapters 3, 4, 5) • inflectionally related word forms (forms of the same ‘word’) versus deriva- tionally related words (different ‘words’ with a shared base) (Chapters 4, 5) 1 02 pages 001-152 18/10/01 3:43 pm Page 2 2 AN INTRODUCTION TO ENGLISH MORPHOLOGY • the distinction between compound words and phrases (Chapters 6, 7) • the relationship between the internal structure of a word and its mean- ing (Chapter 7) • productive versus unproductive word-forming processes (Chapter 8) • historical reasons for some of the contemporary divisions within English morphology, especially Germanic versus Romance word- formation processes (Chapter 9). These various contrasts impact on one another in various ways. For example, if one takes the view that the distinction between compound words and phrases is unimportant, or is even perhaps a bogus distinction fundamentally, this will have a considerable effect on how one views the word as a unit of sentence-structure. Linguistic scholars who specialise in the study of words (so-called ‘morphologists’) devote considerable effort to working out the implications of different ways of formulating these distinctions, as they strive to discover the best way (that is, the most illuminating way, or the way that seems to accord most accurately with people’s implicit knowledge of their native languages). We will not be exploring the technical ramifications of these efforts in this book. Never- theless, I will need to ensure that the way I draw the distinctions here yields a coherent overall picture, and some cross-referencing between chapters will be necessary for that. Each of Chapters 2 to 9 inclusive is provided with exercises.
Recommended publications
  • A Tutorial on Lexical Classes
    A tutorial on lexical classes Ricardo Bermúdez-Otero University of Manchester INTRODUCTION Goals of the tutorial §1 Our brief: L to address the key issues arising from exponence phenomena in which the lexicon is divided into arbitrary subsets, notably (a) inflectional classes and (b) cophonologies. §2 Our focus: L to explore the advantages and drawbacks of theories that model such phenomena by means of diacritic features. Overview §3 The discussion will touch upon the following questions: • Architectural issues How must the grammar be organized in order to guarantee the syntactic inertness of inflectional class features? Can syntactic features like gender be sensitive to inflectional class membership, and can inflectional class membership be determined by phonological properties? • Perspectives on inflectional class features (1): denial To what extent can inflectional class features be eliminated by (a) storing complex items in the lexicon and (b) invoking replacive mechanisms of exponence (‘overwriting’)? • Perspectives on inflectional class features (2): strengthening Instead of eliminating inflectional class features, do we rather need a more elaborate theory (e.g. one based on feature decomposition) capable of accounting for a wider range of phenomena (e.g. cross-class syncretism)? • Class features in phonology: cophonologies Can patterns of overlap between cophonologies be captured through the decomposition of cophonology diacritics, just like syncretisms between inflectional classes? Can derived environment effects in phonology be analysed in terms of the percolation of cophonology diacritics within morphological structures? http://www.bermudez-otero.com/WOTM4.pdf Workshop on Theoretical Morphology 4, Großbothen, 20 June 2008 2 ARCHITECTURAL ISSUES Basic properties of inflectional classes §4 Inflectional classes are traditionally defined by two properties: • arbitrariness and • syntactic inertness.
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION This Chapter Presents Background of The
    CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION This chapter presents background of the research, statement of problem, research objective, research significance, and definition of key terms. 1.1 Background Morphology is a branch of linguistics which deals with the study of a word and its internal structure which affects the change of meaning. Moreover, Geert (2005: 7) states that morphology term refers to a study of the structure of the word, systematic word, the formation of word that has a relationship with its meaning. Morphology also covers how users of a particular language understand complex words, and find lexical items. Thus, Morphological studies in this research are used to analyze the word and how the word is formed that the word has a form and a meaning. Morphological process is the formation of a word in the form and meaning according to need in a single act of substitutions. Moreover, morphological process is a process of word formation by connecting one morpheme to other morphemes. The definition of morphological process according to Chaer (2012: 25) is the formation of a word from a base form of words through affixation and reduplication process. The formation of words will undergo another one of two processes, inflection and derivation. Derivation in particular is a process of forming a word that produces new lexeme. According to Ba’dulu and Herman (2006:12) 1 2 morphological derivation takes one word and turns it into another word. In other words, derivation is creating a new lexical word or creating a word from another syntactic category. Derivation is uses lexical meaning to make (or derive) new words and to change a word from grammatical class to another.
    [Show full text]
  • Studying Word-Formation in English
    VYTAUTAS MAGNUS UNIVERSITY FACULTY OF HUMANITIES DEPARTMENT OF ENGLISH PHILOLOGY Jūratė Ruzaitė Studying Word-Formation in English A resource book Vytautas Magnus University Kaunas, 2012 UDK 811.111(075.8) Ru218 This resource book was approved for publication at the meeting of the Department of English Philology, Faculty of Humanities on 23 May 2012 (Protocol No. 2) and the meeting of the Committee of the Faculty of Humanities, Vytautas Magnus University on 19 June 2012 (Protocol No. 3). Reviewed by Assoc. Prof. Dr. Violeta Kalėdaitė ISBN 978-9955-12-801-4 e-ISBN 978-9955-12-802-1 © Jūratė Ruzaitė, 2012 © Vytautas Magnus University, 2012 Table of Contents Introduction ...............................................................................................4 Part 1: Theoretical and methodological preliminaries .........................9 1.1. Abstract morphological facts, background, and beliefs .......10 1.2. Morphology in action ...........................................................15 1.3. Studying word formation: an introduction to morphological analysis ....................................................17 1.3.1. Important distinctions in studying word formation ....17 1.3.2. What is a word? ..........................................................21 1.3.3. Analytic principles of morphological analysis .........25 Part 2: Inflectional morphology ............................................................29 Part 3: Studying complex words ...........................................................33 3.1. Analysis of complex
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter One Introduction
    CHAPTER ONE INTRODUCTION 0 Chapter One Introduction 1.0 Background :- In spite of the fact that English morphology studies have been incepted lately in linguistics; these studies have acquired an overwhelming importance in the field of English language learning and acquisition. According to Leiber (2009) ‘morphology is the study of word formation, including the ways new words are coined in the languages of the world and the way forms of words are varied depending on how they are used in sentence’. Thus morphology studies the formation of words in English language and how this formation is largely dependent on sentence structure in which the word is deployed. Forming words in English language takes two main ways; deriving new lexemes from other words by adding a prefix or a suffix, e.g. “happy”, “unhappy” and “happiness”. This process happens to be called derivational morphology which is concerned with creating a new word though a related one. The other way is forming words using inflectional processes, which result in a modified version of a word (Delahunty & Garvey: 76). These processes are often applied to words in sentences in order to meet the grammatical needs of the sentences such as tense, gender or number. For instance the variant forms of the verb ‘to walk’ ‘walk’ and ‘walks’ are used for the present simple whereas “walked” is used for the past simple. 1 As stated above, words may be constructed of more than one morpheme, a morpheme is the smallest form (i.e, spoken or written units) that has meaning or grammatical function (Delahunty & Garvey:2010 ; p 76).
    [Show full text]
  • Between Inflection and Derivation Paradigmatic Lexical Functions in Morphological Databases
    Between Inflection and Derivation Paradigmatic Lexical Functions in Morphological Databases Maarten Janssen ILTEC, Lisboa, Portugal [email protected] Abstract This article shows the use of lexical functions in the MorDebe lexical database system. Lexical functions are used in MorDebe to bridge the gap between derivation and inflection – to model relations between word-forms that are derivational from one perspective but inflectional from another. However, lexical functions in MorDebe range over lemmas rather than word-meanings and are hence significantly different from the normal lexical function. Абст‡кт В этой ст‡тье обсужд‡етсfl использние лексических функций ‚ лексической б‡зе д‡нных MorDebe. Лексические функции используютсfl ‚ MorDebe длfl того, чтобы сок‡тить ‡зы‚ между сло‚ооб‡знием и сло‚оизменением – длfl моделиниfl отношений между сло‚офом‡ми, котоые можно ‡ссм‡т肇ть с одной точки зениfl к‡к де肇ционные, ‡ с дугой - к‡к сло‚оизменительные. Одн‡ко, лексические функции ‚ MorDebe опеделflютсfl скоее длfl лексем, т.е. слных ‚ходо‚, ‡ не длfl смысло‚ых единиц и, следтельно, отлич‡ютсfl от лексических функций ‚ обычном поним‡нии. 1. Introduction Despite its central role in linguistics, the distinction between inflection and derivation is far from clear-cut. Modern day morphological theories often posit in-between cases - behaving partly like inflections, but partly like derivations. However, the introduction of marginal cases is problematic from the perspective of morphological database: an actual database which lists inflections and/or derivations, either has to treat them on a par, or model any individual form either as derivational or inflectional. This article will show how this problem manifests itself in the MorDebe morphological database, and how lexical functions are used to (partially) overcome the problem.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 on Agent Nominalizations and Why They Are Not Like Event
    On agent nominalizations and why they are not like event nominalizations1 Mark C. Baker and Nadya Vinokurova Rutgers University and Research Institute of Humanities -Yakutsk Abstract: This paper focuses on agent-denoting nominalizations in various languages (e.g. the finder of the wallet), contrasting them with the much better studied action/event- denoting nominalizations. In particular, we show that in Sakha, Mapudungun, and English, agent-denoting nominalizations have none of the verbal features that event- denoting nominalizations sometimes have: they cannot contain adverbs, voice markers, expressions of aspect or mood, or verbal negation. An apparent exception to this generalization is that Sakha allows accusative-case marked objects in agentive nominalizations. We show that in fact the structure of agentive nominalizations in Sakha is as purely nominal as in other languages, and the difference is attributable to the rule of accusative case assignment. We explain these restrictions by arguing that agentive nominalizers have a semantics very much like the one proposed by Kratzer (1996) for Voice heads. Given this, the natural order of semantic composition implies that agentive nominalizers must combine directly with VP, just as Voice heads must. As a preliminary to testing this idea typologically, we show how a true agentive nominalization can be distinguished from a headless subject relative clause, illustrating with data from Mapudungun. We then present the results of a 34-language survey, showing that indeed none of these languages allow clause-like syntax inside a true agentive nominalization. We conclude that a generative-style investigation into the details of particular languages can be a productive source of things to look for in typological surveys.
    [Show full text]
  • Obtaining a Better Understanding of Distributional Models of German Derivational Morphology
    Obtaining a Better Understanding of Distributional Models of German Derivational Morphology Max Kisselew∗ Sebastian Pado´∗ Alexis Palmer∗ Jan Snajderˇ † ∗Institut fur¨ maschinelle Sprachverarbeitung, Stuttgart University, Germany †Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, University of Zagreb, Croatia kisselmx,pado,palmeras @ims.uni-stuttgart.de [email protected] { } Abstract Predicting the (distributional) meaning of derivationally related words (read / read+er) from one another has recently been recognized as an instance of distributional compositional meaning construc- tion. However, the properties of this task are not yet well understood. In this paper, we present an analysis of two such composition models on a set of German derivation patterns (e.g., -in, durch-). We begin by introducing a rank-based evaluation metric, which reveals the task to be challenging due to specific properties of German (compounding, capitalization). We also find that performance varies greatly between patterns and even among base-derived term pairs of the same pattern. A regression analysis shows that semantic coherence of the base and derived terms within a pattern, as well as coherence of the semantic shifts from base to derived terms, all significantly impact prediction quality. 1 Introduction Derivation is a major morphological process of word formation (e.g., read read+er), which is typically → associated with a fairly specific semantic shift (+er: agentivization). It may therefore be surprising that the semantics of derivation is a relatively understudied phenomenon in distributional semantics. Recently, Lazaridou et al. (2013) proposed to consider the semantics of a derived term like read+er as the result of a compositional process that combines the meanings of the base term read and the affix +er.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphological Representation and Semantic Interpretation
    Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics, Vol. 11, 1983, 01-18 doi: 10.5774/11-0-104 MORPHOLOGICAL REPRESENTATION AND SEMANTIC INTERPRETATION Rudolf P. Botha Introduction TI1is paper deals with the tenability of the following general linguistic condition on morphological representation: (1) TIle Compositionality Condition The morphological representation assigned to a complex word must provide the formal structure required by an optimal specification of the semantic interpretation of the word. This condition entails, on the one hand, that if the semantic interpreta­ tion (or meaning) of a complex word can be specified as a simple function of the meanings of its constituents, these constituents must be bracketed and labelled in such a way as to make such a specification possible. On the other hand, the condition disallows a bracketing and/or labelling which, for this specification, requires objectionable devices. A device is objectionable if it (a) has unacceptable empirical consequences either in the grammar or in the general linguistic theory, (b) is ad hoc in a specific sense, (c) represents or creates a conceptual redundancy within the total grammar or general linguistic theory, or (d) is insuf­ ficiently constrained in regard to descriptive power. The use of devices with one or more of these properties makes a specification of the semantic interpretation of a word non-optimal. And morphological representations necessitating the use of such devices are suspect. Condi tion (1)' clearly attaches more value to morphological representations which maximize com­ positionality in the specification of the meaning of complex words, hence the name "Compositionality Condition". Of course, the condition cannot be enforced in the case of complex words which have idiosyncratic elements of meaning that cannot be predicted on the basis of the meanings of their .
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistics 1A Morphology 3 Compounding and Derivation
    Linguistics 1A Morphology 3 Compounding and derivation In the previous lecture we saw that it is possible to combine two free morphemes to make one complex word, a compound. In principle, the two morphemes can be of any lexical category. Indeed, many different combinations of lexical categories can be found in English compounds: (1) a. N + N apron string, kitchen towel, bathroom, police car b. A + N high school, smallpox, dimwit, greenbelt c. P + N overdose, underdog, afterthought, uptown d. V + N swearword, rattlesnake, whetstone, scrubwoman (2) a. N + A headstrong, honey-sweet, skin-deep, nationwide b. A + A red-hot, worldy-wise, widespread c. P + A overabundant, underripe, underprivileged d. V + A --- (3) a. N + V colour-code, breast-feed, chain-smoke, pan-fry b. A + V sharpshoot, dry-clean, whitewash c. P + V outperform, underfeed, overreact d. V + V stirfry Some of the combinations are more productive than others, meaning that it is easier to coin new compounds of that type. The N+N type, for example, is completely productive: any combination of two nouns is a possible compound of English. At the other extreme, V+A combinations do not seem to occur at all. This is an arbitrary gap in the English lexicon: a look at other languages quickly reveals that there is nothing intrinsically wrong with a V+A compound, as the Dutch examples in (4) show. (4) fonkel V-nieuw A druip V-nat A shine new drip wet ‘brand new’ ‘soaking wet’ The resulting meaning of the combination of two morphemes is largely unpredictable.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mirror Alignment Principle: 1 Introduction
    The Mirror Alignment Principle: Morpheme Ordering at the Morphosyntax-Phonology Interface Sam Zukoff ([email protected]), MIT MIT Generals Paper 1/26/16 1 Introduction Since at least Baker’s (1985) proposal of the “Mirror Principle”, it has been widely recognized that the linear order of morphemes within a morphologically complex word generally correlates with hierarchical syntactic structure (see also Muysken 1981). In morphologically complex words, morphemes which represent the exponents of morphosyntactic terminals that are lower in the syntactic tree (or, in Baker’s terms, earlier in the syntactic derivation) generally surface closer to the root than those morphemes which are exponents of higher morphosyntactic terminals. A question that Baker does not directly explore in his original proposal is by what formal means this ordering relation is implemented in the grammar. Both from a cross-linguistic perspective and a language-internal perspective, it is generally an arbitrary property (in terms of the synchronic grammar) whether a particular morpheme displays prefixal or suffixal behavior. This makes it difficult if not impossible to directly translate proposals regarding syntactic linearization (e.g., Kayne 1994) into the word-level domain.1 Some other mechanism is needed. This paper proposes such a mechanism: the Mirror Alignment Principle. This paper outlines a proposal whereby the surface order of morphemes is the result of several steps in a modular, feed-forward grammar with the following characteristics.2 The syntax generates a hierarchical structure of morphosyntactic terminals (following generally the principles of Chomsky’s (1995, et seq.) Minimalist Program). This hierarchal structure serves as input to a discrete morphological component (as in Distributed Morphology (DM); Halle & Marantz 1993) which has the ability to perform its own operations on the hierarchical structure (cf.
    [Show full text]
  • Collective and Feminine in Tocharian. In: Multilingualism and History of Knowledge
    Olav Hackstein 2011/2012: Collective and Feminine in Tocharian. In: Multilingualism and History of Knowledge. Vol. II: Linguistic Developments along the Silk Road. Archaism and Innovation in Tocharian. Edited by Olav Hackstein and Ronald I. Kim. Wien. 143-177. (= Österreichische Akademie der Wissenschaften, phil.-hist. Klasse Sitzungsberichte, 834. Band.). * COLLECTIVE AND FEMININE IN TOCHARIAN 1.1.1. The morphological markers of the Indo-European feminine gender arose from a word-formation suffix with collective meaning *-h2 and various derivatives formed with it, including thematic *-e-h2 and athematic * -i-h2. This insight goes back to Johannes SCHMIDT and was later substantiated in many ways by advances in the reconstruction of Proto-Indo-European nominal morphology, most notably NUSSBAUM 1986 and HARÐARSON 1987a, b. The origin of the collective morpheme *-h2 is hinted at by a wealth of traces of the pre-inflectional, purely word-formational use of *-h2, which survive into the individual Indo-European lan- guages. The evidence includes the placement of *-h2- before derivational suffixes in complex formations and before inflectional endings, which accords with the usual behavior of derivational morphemes, cf. e.g. w w PIE * k e-h2-nt-o- > Lat. quantus; PIE * te-h2-nt-o- > Lat. tantus; PIE * k i-h2 ⇒ w *k i-h2-ent - > Skt. kíyant- (SIMS -WILLIAMS 1997: 318); PIE * ker-h2 ⇒ * ker-h2-s- ⇒ * ker-h2-s-ro- ⇒ * ker-h2-s-re-h2 > Lat. cerebra ‘brains’ (for the semantics cf. Germ. Gehirn ); PIE * dru-h2 ‘wood’ ⇒ singulative * dru-h2-s ‘single tree’ > Gk. δρKL 28ALLES 200.a: .A, NIKOLAEV 2010a: 192E on the derivation of singulative formations cf.
    [Show full text]
  • The Syntax, Semantics and Derivation of Bare Nominalisations in English
    BOŻENA CETNAROWSKA The Syntax, Semantics and Derivation of Bare Nominalisations in English Uniwersytet Śląski Katowice 1993 The Syntax, Semantics and Derivation of Bare Nominalisations in English Prace Naukowe Uniwersytetu Śląskiego w Katowicach nr 1385 BOŻENA CETNAROWSKA The Syntax, Semantics and Derivation of Bare Nominalisations in English Uniwersytet Śląski Katowice 1993 Editor of the Series: Neophilological Linguistics CZESŁAWA SCHATTE Reviewer ANNA MALICKA-KLEPARSKA Publikacja finansowana z funduszy na badania własne Wydziału Filologicznego Uniwersytetu Śląskiego Executive Editor Jerzy Stencel Technical Editor Alicja Zajączkowska Ł A Proof-reader Barbara Kuźniarowska .é wwww i»* łtófc \ g Bp / & / Copyright © 1993 by Uniwersytet Śląski All rights reserved ISSN 0208-6336 ISBN 83-226-0535-8 Published by Uniwersytet Śląski ul. Bankowa 12B, 40-096 Katowice First impression. Edition: 220 + 50 copies. Printed sheets: 9.5. Publishing sheets: 12.5. Passed to the Printing Works in October, 1993. Signed for printing aid printing finished in December, 1993. Order No. 1109/93. Price: zl 40000,— Computer-generated composition: Agencja Poligraficzno-Wydawnicza "Compal" ' Printing and binding: Zakład Poligraficzny Ośrodka Wydawniczego "Augustana" pl. ks. M. Lutra 3, 43-300 Bielsko-Biała CONTENTS P R E F A C E ............................................................................................................................. 11 CHAPTER 1 PRELIMINARY REMARKS ON BARE NOMINALISATIONS 1.0. Introductory.........................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]