The Domestication Hypothesis for Dogs• Skills with Human Communication

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Domestication Hypothesis for Dogs• Skills with Human Communication Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) e1–e6 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Animal Behaviour journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/anbehav Forum Articles The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008) Brian Hare a,*, Alexandra Rosati a, Juliane Kaminski b, Juliane Bra¨uer b, Josep Call b, Michael Tomasello b a Department of Evolutionary Anthropology & Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC, U.S.A. b Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany article info Article history: Received 7 February 2009 Initial acceptance 15 June 2009 Final acceptance 25 June 2009 Available online 29 August 2009 MS. number: AF-09-00115R Keywords: cognitive evolution dog cognition domestication social cognition Domestic dogs have special skills in comprehending human puppies and must be explicitly trained to express dog-like skills in communicative behaviours (Hare & Tomasello 2005; Miklosi 2008). response to human communicative cues (Agnetta et al. 2000; Hare Dogs across a range of breeds use human communicative cues such et al. 2002; Viranyi et al. 2008). This conclusion holds even for as pointing or physical markers to find food that is hidden in one of wolves reared in identical conditions with a group of dogs for the two hiding places (controls rule out the use of olfactory cues; purpose of comparing their social skills with humans. In summary, Cooper et al. 2003; Hare & Tomasello 2005; Miklosi & Soproni it is unlikely that dogs simply inherited their unusual skills from 2006). In direct comparisons, dogs are even more skilled than their last common ancestor with wolves (Viranyi et al. 2008). chimpanzees at using human communicative cues when searching Additionally, dogs develop their ability to use human commu- for food (Hare et al. 2002; Bra¨uer et al. 2006). Moreover, a number nicative cues, such as pointing cues or gaze cues, as young puppies of studies suggest that dogs understand human gestures commu- regardless of rearing history. Even puppies as young as 6–9 weeks nicatively, as a number of possible low-level explanations have of age can use a human’s communicative cues, including unadopted been ruled out (e.g. only responding to movement; reflexively co- puppies still living with their littermates and having little exposure orienting; using only familiar cues, etc.; Hare et al. 1998; Agnetta to humans beyond routine care (Hare et al. 2002; Riedel et al. 2008; et al. 2000; Soproni et al. 2001; Miklosi 2008). Viranyi et al. 2008). This suggests that dogs’ use of human The sophistication that dogs show in using human communi- communicative cues does not require extensive exposure to cative cues led researchers to investigate the origins of these abil- humans (e.g. Hare et al. 2002). ities and to conclude that these social skills are not simply inherited These two major findings led researchers to explore the possi- from wolves nor are they simply learned as a result of exposure to bility that dogs evolved their unusual ability to use human humans in ontogeny, but rather they have evolved as a result of communicative cues not only during, but as a direct result of, domestication (Hare & Tomasello 2005). Although wolves can domestication (Miklosi et al. 2003; Hare & Tomasello 2005). potentially learn to use human communicative cues (much like Comparisons between experimentally domesticated foxes and nonhuman primates), they do not show these skills as young a matched control line of foxes support the hypothesis that domestication, or selection against aggression towards humans, can, as a by-product, lead to enhanced skills in comprehending human gestures. Experimental foxes bred over 40 generations to * Correspondence: B. Hare, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology & Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708, U.S.A. approach humans without fear were more skilled at using human E-mail address: [email protected] (B. Hare). gestures than were control foxes that were not bred over the same 0003-3472/$38.00 Ó 2009 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.031 e2 B. Hare et al. / Animal Behaviour 79 (2010) e1–e6 period for their reaction to humans. Crucially, the control and (2008), were tested by caregivers in familiar outdoor enclosures, experimental foxes participated in the experiments at similar levels but unlike in Hare et al.’s (2002) study, the caregiver in Udell et al.’s (i.e. made equal numbers of choices within test sessions) and, when (2008) study tested the wolves while standing inside the subjects’ tested with a nonsocial cue, the control foxes were actually more enclosure. Three of the dog groups consisted of family-reared pet skilled than the experimental foxes, effectively ruling out the dogs that were tested by different combinations of familiar or possibility that the control foxes were too fearful or unmotivated to unfamiliar experimenters and testing locations: dogs in the ‘home participate in all human-led tasks (Hare et al. 2005). The findings unfamiliar’ group were tested at their homes by an experimenter; from these experiments with foxes provide further support that the dogs in the ‘outdoor familiar’ group were tested in a less familiar domestication of dogs actually enhanced their ability to use outdoor location by a familiar human; and dogs in the ‘outdoor communicative cues provided by humans. unfamiliar’ group were tested in a less familiar outdoor location by Recently, two papers challenge the ideas (1) that dogs outper- an experimenter. The final group consisted of dogs living at a local form wolves in using human communicative gestures (Udell et al. shelter that were tested in a less familiar room by an unfamiliar 2008) and (2) that dogs require very limited human exposure to human experimenter. show initial skill in using such communicative cues (Wynne et al. Unlike all previous studies, Udell et al. (2008) gave subjects four 2008). In the first of these studies, Udell et al. (2008) present warm-up trials in which they placed food on top of one of two findings suggesting that wolves are more skilled than dogs in using hiding locations in view of subjects so that, once placed, the food human communicative cues in a food retrieval task. Specifically was visible to the subject (usually in a warm-up phase with they found that wolves were more skilled than shelter dogs and pet primates or dogs, food is hidden under different hiding locations as dogs tested outdoors, and equally as skilled as pet dogs tested the subjects watch). During this warm-up, food was always indoors. In a second study, Wynne et al. (2008) critiqued an completely visible and all subjects correctly retrieved the visible experiment by Riedel et al. (2008) that examined the use of human food in all four trials. After the warm-up, all subjects were then communicative cues by dog puppies. Riedel and colleagues repor- given 10 experimental test trials. However, the methodology used ted a series of three studies showing that domestic dog puppies in the test trials was very different from that of previous studies. In comprehended a human pointing gesture as early as 6 weeks of age. Udell et al.’s (2008) study, no food was hidden, but rather the Wynne and colleagues suggest that this study did not rule out the experimenter pointed and ‘when a subject indicated a correct possibility that human experience plays a decisive role. In both choice, the experimenter clicked [a clicker device] and then drop- papers this team of authors claims that their evidence and rean- ped a piece of food on the chosen container’ (page 3). This task alysis refute the domestication hypothesis, and they suggest that differs greatly from the traditional method in which a human differences among canid species in social skills is largely due to indicates the location of hidden food to a dog (reviewed in: Hare & environmental factors during rearing rather than being a result of Tomasello 2005; Miklosi 2008). dog domestication. The performance of each individual subject was compared to To evaluate the evidence presented in these studies, we first chance using binomial probabilities (authors assigned chance to discuss several methodological concerns that we have about the 50%), one-sample t tests, one-way ANOVAs and post hoc compar- approach of Udell et al. (2008), then we reanalyse their data based isons (Bonferroni tests). In addition to the experimental trials, after on these methodological concerns. We also present a test of shelter every second experimental trial, a control trial was run that was dogs naı¨ve to cognitive testing to examine whether it is the case identical to the experimental trial with the exception that no that shelter dogs are less skilled at using human communicative pointing cue was provided (these trials acted as control for olfac- cues than other groups of dogs. Finally, we directly rebut the tory cues that the subjects might be using). Unlike other studies, if critique of Wynne et al. (2008) and argue that there remains no subjects made incorrect choices for more than three trials consec- evidence of significant differences in performance between dogs of utively they were given another warm-up trial in which food was different ages in their use of human communicative cues. We placed on one of the two hiding locations as the animal watched; conclude that the domestication hypothesis remains the best this was done to rule out the possibility that the subjects’ failures explanation for dogs’ special skills for communicating with were due to lack of food motivation. humans. Based on their analysis of the data, Udell et al. (2008) concluded that the performance of the five groups of subjects differed REANALYSIS OF UDELL ET AL.
Recommended publications
  • Human Nature, Part II
    Human Nature, Part II Yom Kippur 2020 Rabbi David J Fine, PhD Besides masks to prevent the spread of covid-19, the other thing that’s hard to spot in ultra- Orthodox neighborhoods are images of Barney the Dinosaur. The fossil record of dinosaurs is hard to explain to children when one is committed to a six-day creation timeline. But children are always fascinated by dinosaurs. There is something about their immense size and power that is irresistible. And there is another thing about dinosaurs that captures the imagination: the fact, and mystery, of their disappearance. How could beings of such strength and such size die out, whereas we were left to inherit the earth. Or we and even smaller beings like ants, as I spoke about on the second day of Rosh Hashanah. The extinction of the dinosaurs seems counterintuitive to what we have been led to understand about nature, that the strong and the mightiest are the ones who survive, the survival of the fittest. Humans grew to “fill the earth and master it,” as the Book of Genesis has God command us, through our intelligence, our ability to build tools and weapons that put us at the top of the food chain. Charles Darwin’s theory that nature favors, or selects, those who help themselves, has been interpreted, or misinterpreted I should say, to support the racist abuses of fellow humans that I talked about on the first day of Rosh Hashanah. The idea that strength prevails has been used to justify the worst acts of hatred and oppression in history.
    [Show full text]
  • Michael Tomasello [March, 2020]
    CURRICULUM VITAE MICHAEL TOMASELLO [MARCH, 2020] Department of Psychology & Neuroscience Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology Duke University; Durham, NC; 27708; USA Deutscher Platz 6; D-04103 Leipzig, GERMANY E-MAIL: [email protected] E-MAIL: [email protected] EDUCATION: DUKE UNIVERSITY B.A. Psychology, 1972 UNIVERSITY OF GEORGIA Ph.D. Experimental Psychology, 1980 UNIVERSITY OF LEIPZIG Doctorate, honoris causa, 2016 EMPLOYMENT: 1980 - 1998 Assistant-Associate-Full Professor of Psychology; Adjunct Professor of Anthropology, EMORY UNIVERSITY 1982 - 1998 Affiliate Scientist, Psychobiology, YERKES PRIMATE CENTER 1998 - 2018 Co-Director, MAX PLANCK INSTITUTE FOR EVOLUTIONARY ANTHROPOLOGY 1999 - 2018 Honorary Professor, Dept of Psychology, University of Leipzig 2001 - 2018 Co-Director, WOLFGANG KÖHLER PRIMATE RESEARCH CENTER 2016 - James Bonk Professor of Psychology & Neuroscience, DUKE UNIVERSITY - Director of Developmental Psychology Program - Secondary App’ts: Philosophy, Evol. Anthropology, Linguistics 2016 - Faculty of Center for Developmental Science, UNC AD HOC: 1987 - 1988 Visiting Scholar, HARVARD UNIVERSITY 1994 (summer) Instructor, INTERNATIONAL COGNITIVE SCIENCE INSTITUTE 1994 (summer) Visiting Fellow, BRITISH PSYCHOLOGICAL SOCIETY 1995 (spring) Visiting Professor, UNIVERSITY OF ROME 1996 (spring) Visiting Professor, THE BRITISH ACADEMY 1998 (spring) Visiting Scholar, MPI FOR PSYCHOLINGUISTICS 1999 (summer) Instructor, INTERNATIONAL COGNITIVE SCIENCE INSTITUTE 2001 (winter) Instructor, LOT (DUTCH GRADUATE
    [Show full text]
  • DISCOVERING the DUKE CANINE COGNITION CENTER October 28-30, 2016
    DISCOVERING THE DUKE CANINE COGNITION CENTER October 28-30, 2016 Forever Learning. Forever Duke. oin fellow alumni, family, and friends for an Learn what your dog is thinking when they stare at Jeducational weekend spent unlocking the secrets you, what senses they rely on most, and whether of the canine mind with evolutionary anthropologist they can read your mind. Bring every question and dog cognition expert, Brian Hare, at the Duke you have ever had about Canine Cognition Center (DCCC) on Duke’s campus. your dog and in a series of workshops, learn how Established in 2009, the DCCC is the first dog to develop a game that research center on a college campus in the United might lead to an answer. States and is internationally recognized for the Of course, there will be cutting edge studies investigating what goes on adorable dogs that will inside the minds of man’s best friend. The DCCC has help test your theories tested over two thousand dogs from Durham and and demonstrate their the surrounding communities with non-invasive, abilities. On Sunday, creative skill assessments that measure their Vanessa Woods, co- abilities and aptitudes in a variety of ways. With author of Dr. Hare’s New the highest acceptance rate and cheapest tuition York Times bestselling at Duke, the DCCC offers dog owners work, The Genius of the opportunity to participate in Dogs, will share new Who's here scientific discovery. For the first research from their time, you can also be part of that highly-anticipated for a journey. next book that will leave you belly rub? rethinking domestication and what it means to The weekend begins on Friday be human.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Evolution in Captive Foxes Is a Correlated By-Product of Experimental Domestication
    Current Biology, Vol. 15, 226–230, February 8, 2005, 2005 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved. DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2005.01.040 Social Cognitive Evolution in Captive Foxes Is a Correlated By-Product of Experimental Domestication Brian Hare,1,2,* Irene Plyusnina,3 Natalie Ignacio,1 test; Figure 1). The ten foxes tested in the control condi- Olesya Schepina,3 Anna Stepika,3 tion were unable to find the hidden food when no visual Richard Wrangham,1 and Lyudmila Trut3 cue was provided (t[9] ϭ 0.318, p ϭ NS, one-sample t 1Department of Anthropology test). In addition, when the foxes’ number of correct Harvard University choices from their first nine trials is compared to that Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138 of their second nine trials, there is no evidence that their 2 Max Plank Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology performance improved with experience during the test D-04103 Leipzig session (t ϭ 0.76, df ϭ 10, p ϭ NS, one-sample t test). Germany Similarly, it has been repeatedly shown that dogs, even 3 Institute of Cytology and Genetics as adults, are unable to find hidden food with olfactory Siberian Division of the Russian Academy cues in this same setting, and there is no evidence that of Science performance improves within a test session [5–8]. 630090 Novosibirsk In experiment two, 13 experimental foxes and 13 con- Russia trol foxes approached the experimenter (E) within the 3 min introduction trial. In addition, 17 experimental foxes and 16 control foxes approached the novel object Summary within the introduction trial.
    [Show full text]
  • Brian Hare’S Deutscher Platz
    PERSONAL PORTRAIT EVOLUTIONARY ANTHROPOLOGY Brian BrianHare Hare he clipping from the German ing that fits in well at its location on The Max Planck researcher began Tnewspaper BILD on Brian Hare’s Deutscher Platz. Inside, life is inter- studying at the psychology depart- office door is a real eye-catcher. It national; it is the Max Planck Insti- ment of Emory University in Atlanta shows a photograph of the Ameri- tute with the largest percentage of and was thrilled. There were “cool can researcher with a fox, with a researchers from abroad. The five di- lectures” in psychology and anthro- short text below – “full of mis- rectors alone come from five differ- pology, and the student whose high takes,” as Hare comments. What was ent countries. No one really notices school grades were “not particularly a serious scientist like Hare doing in that Brian Hare speaks only a little outstanding” now garnered only the Germany’s most popular daily German. best marks. At Emory he met his tabloid? The article’s headline re- most important teacher: Michael PRACTICAL TRAINING IN THE veals the reason: “Foxes are the bet- (“Mike”) Tomasello, professor of psy- ECUADORIAN JUNGLE ter dogs,” it says. Brian Hare, born chology. Tomasello is one of the in 1976, investigates the social be- Hare was at the institute earlier, from founding Directors of the institute in havior of dogs, and man’s best 2001 to 2002, to collect data for his Leipzig and, as Director of the De- friend is always a good topic for a Ph.D. In 2004, he returned to Leipzig partment for Developmental Psy- wide audience.
    [Show full text]
  • Rachna B. Reddy Curriculum Vitae August 3, 2016
    Rachna B. Reddy Curriculum Vitae August 3, 2016 Department of Anthropology [email protected] University of Michigan (586) 944-4785 1085 S. University Ave. Ann Arbor, MI 48109 EDUCATION 2013-present Ph.D. candidate, Anthropology, University of Michigan 2012 B.A. with Distinction, Evolutionary Anthropology, Duke University POSITIONS HELD 2014 - 2015 Graduate Student Instructor, Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan 2012 - 2013 Lab Manager, Hominoid Psychology Research Group and Duke Canine Cognition Center, Department of Evolutionary Anthropology, Duke University, P.I. Dr. Brian Hare PUBLICATIONS Journal Articles 2016 Sandel AA, Reddy RB, Mitani JC. Adolescent male chimpanzees do not form a dominance hierarchy with their peers. Primates: 10.1007/s10329-016-0553-z 2016 Reddy RB, Krupenye C, MacLean EL, Hare B. No evidence for yawn contagion in lemurs. Animal Cognition 19(5):889-898 2015 Reddy RB, MacLean EL, Sandel AA, Hare B. Social inhibitory control in five lemur species. Primates 56(3): 241-252 2014 MacLean EL, Hare B, Nunn C…Reddy RB….et al. The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111(20): E2140-E2148 2013 MacLean EL, Sandel AA, Bray J, Oldenkamp RE, Reddy RB, Hare B. Group size predicts social but not nonsocial cognition. PLoS One 8(6): 0066359 AWARDS 2015 Outstanding Graduate Student Instructor, Department of Anthropology, University of Michigan 2010 Melcher Family Award for Excellence in Journalism, Duke University FELLOWSHIPS 2014 National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (3 years of funding) 2013 Honorable Mention National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship 2009 Howard Hughes Research Fellows Program, Duke University RESEARCH GRANTS External Grants 2016 National Science Foundation Doctoral Dissertation Research Improvement Grant ($17,146) 2016 L.S.B.
    [Show full text]
  • PETITION Before the Fish and Wildlife Service United States Department
    PETITION Before the Fish and Wildlife Service United States Department of the Interior March 16, 2010 To Upgrade Captive Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from Threatened to Endangered Status Pursuant to the Endangered Species Act of 1973, as Amended Pan troglodytes (Photograph by National Geographic) Prepared by Anna Frostic, Esq. The Humane Society of the United States TABLE OF CONTENTS I. Introduction………………………………………………….…….……….…........4 II. Legal Background………………….…………….………………………………...7 A. Endangered Species Act Background…..…………………………………...7 B. The Listing History of the Chimpanzee……..….…………………………15 III. Argument: Depriving Captive Chimpanzees of Full Protection Under the ESA is Antithetical to Conserving the Species……….………….….……….20 A. The ―Split-Listing‖ Scheme for Pan troglodytes has Contributed to a Proliferation of Privately Owned and Exploited Chimpanzees, Interfering With Conservation of the Species in the Wild..…………....27 1. Chimpanzees in Entertainment.…………………..……..…………....29 a. Chimpanzees Are Widely Exploited for Entertainment in the U.S…………………………………………………………………..29 b. Because Chimpanzees Are So Pervasively Used in Entertainment the Public Believes the Species is Prevalent in the Wild………………………………………………………....69 c. Chimpanzees Used in Entertainment Are Abused and Mistreated................................................................................71 2. Chimpanzees Used as Pets……………………………………………...77 3. Chimpanzees in Biomedical Research Laboratories………..…........87 B. Threats to Wild Chimpanzee Populations Have Only Increased
    [Show full text]
  • Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition”
    Journal of Intelligence Review Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not “One Cognition” Juliane Bräuer 1,2,*, Daniel Hanus 3, Simone Pika 4, Russell Gray 1 and Natalie Uomini 1 1 Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Department of Linguistic and Cultural Evolution, Kahlaische Strasse 10, 07745 Jena, Germany; [email protected] (R.G.); [email protected] (N.U.) 2 Department of General Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-University, Am Steiger 3, 07743 Jena, Germany 3 Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Department of Developmental and Comparative Psychology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany; [email protected] 4 Institute of Cognitive Science, Comparative BioCognition, University of Osnabrück, Artilleriestrasse 34, 49076 Osnabrück, Germany; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] Received: 26 March 2020; Accepted: 22 June 2020; Published: 2 July 2020 Abstract: Using the comparative approach, researchers draw inferences about the evolution of cognition. Psychologists have postulated several hypotheses to explain why certain species are cognitively more flexible than others, and these hypotheses assume that certain cognitive skills are linked together to create a generally “smart” species. However, empirical findings suggest that several animal species are highly specialized, showing exceptional skills in single cognitive domains while performing poorly in others. Although some cognitive skills may indeed overlap, we cannot a priori assume that they do across species. We argue that the term “cognition” has often been used by applying an anthropocentric viewpoint rather than a biocentric one. As a result, researchers tend to overrate cognitive skills that are human-like and assume that certain skills cluster together in other animals as they do in our own species.
    [Show full text]
  • Transcript Animal Cognition with Dr
    1 S2E4: Transcript Animal Cognition with Dr. Brian Hare Tavia Gilbert: Welcome to Stories of Impact. I’m producer Tavia Gilbert, and in every episode of this podcast, journalist Richard Sergay and I bring you a conversation about the newest scientific research on human flourishing, and how those discoveries can be translated into practical tools. In every episode of this season of the Stories of Impact podcast, we’re asking the questions: What are diverse intelligences, what do we learn by studying them, and how can those revelations support human virtue? Meet Dr. Brian Hare. He’s a core member of the Center of Cognitive Neuroscience and Professor in Evolutionary Anthropology and Psychology and Neuroscience at Duke University. Not only is he an expert in diverse animal intelligences and a canine specialist, along with co-author Vanessa Woods, he’s the writer of The Genius of Dogs and Survival of the ​ ​ ​ Friendliest: Understanding Our Origins and Rediscovering Our Common Humanity. ​ In today’s episode, Dr. Hare shares how investigating diverse intelligences offers human beings insight into our own psychology. He’ll also describe the powerful new online tool he’s building to spark the acceleration of our understanding about animal — and human — cognition. And he’ll reveal why he is so drawn to study dogs and their particular genius. Brian Hare: So why dogs as a species? Why would you focus on dogs? For several reasons. One is they share an evolutionary history with us for the last 25,000-40,000 years. Their evolutionary story I think in many ways tells us about our own story.
    [Show full text]
  • Dogs Hijack the Human Bonding Pathway Oxytocin Facilitates Social Connections Between Humans and Dogs
    Broad-spectrum antivirals p. 282 The cells in our skinkin INSIGHTS that heal p. 284 PERSPECTIVES on February 2, 2017 http://science.sciencemag.org/ Look into my eyes. Changes in oxytocin concentration in a dog might elicit similar changes in a human and vice versa. EVOLUTION Downloaded from Dogs hijack the human bonding pathway Oxytocin facilitates social connections between humans and dogs By Evan L. MacLean1 ,2 and Brian Hare1 ,2, 3 which dogs win our hearts—and we win than many might have imagined (3). This theirs in return. is particularly true when it comes to how ens of thousands of years ago, a wolf- Until recently, most research on human dogs understand the social world. Even like predator gave rise to a more social and cognitive evolution concentrated as puppies, dogs spontaneously respond docile lineage, which soon became on our closest primate relatives. Mean- to cooperative human gestures, such as our trusted fireside companions ( 1). while, sitting at our feet was a remarkable pointing cues, to find hidden food or toy How did dogs become so embedded case of evolutionary convergence. Inspired rewards. By contrast, great apes must have in human societies? Why do we feel by developmental psychologists studying extensive experience with people to show Tgenuine friendship, love, and social at- human infants, comparative psychologists similar skills (4). This use of social cues tachment in our relationships with dogs? began studying family dogs. It quickly be- extends to a wide range of social gestures, On page 333 in this issue, Nagasawa et al. came apparent that dogs have much more including gaze direction and even the use ( 2) reveal a powerful mechanism through to tell us about cognition, and ourselves, of arbitrary communicative markers.
    [Show full text]
  • Bonobo Cognition and Behaviour
    Bonobo Cognition and Behaviour Brian Hare and Shinya Yamamoto - 978-90-04-30417-8 Heruntergeladen von Brill.com10/14/2019 09:14:20AM via free access Brian Hare and Shinya Yamamoto - 978-90-04-30417-8 Heruntergeladen von Brill.com10/14/2019 09:14:20AM via free access Bonobo Cognition and Behaviour Edited by Brian Hare and Shinya Yamamoto Leiden · Boston 2015 Brian Hare and Shinya Yamamoto - 978-90-04-30417-8 Heruntergeladen von Brill.com10/14/2019 09:14:20AM via free access This is an open access title distributed under the terms of the prevail- ing CC-BY-NC License at the time of publication, which permits any non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited. An electronic version of this book is freely available, thanks to the support of libraries working with Knowledge Unlatched. More information about the initiative can be found at www.knowledgeunlatched.org. Library of Congress Control Number: 2015956161 ISBN: 978 90 04 30416 1 E_ISBN: 978 90 04 30417 8 © Copyright 2015 by Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, The Netherlands. Koninklijke Brill NV incorporates the imprints Brill, Brill Hes & De Graaf, Brill Nijhoff, Brill Rodopi and Hotei Publishing. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, translated, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without prior written permission of the publisher. Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use is granted by Brill provided that the appropriate fees are paid directly to Copyright Clearance Center, 222 Rosewood Drive, Suite 910, Danvers, MA 01923, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • Human-Like Social Skills in Dogs?
    TICS 369 DTD 5 ARTICLE IN PRESS Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.xx No.xx Monthxxxx Human-like social skills in dogs? Brian Hare and Michael Tomasello Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, Leipzig, Germany Domestic dogs are unusually skilled at reading human comparative work has begun to identify the selection social and communicative behavior – even more so than pressures that drove the evolution of these skills, and, our nearest primate relatives. For example, they use further, suggests that a similar process played a role in human social and communicative behavior (e.g. a shaping human social skills as well. pointing gesture) to find hidden food, and they know what the human can and cannot see in various situ- Human-like social skills in dogs? ations. Recent comparisons between canid species The test is simple. Hide a piece offood or an attractive object suggest that these unusual social skills have a heritable in one of several opaque containers, and then look at or point component and initially evolved during domestication to that location in an attempt to help the subject find the as a result of selection on systems mediating fear and hidden object. Human infants find this task trivially easy aggression towards humans. Differences in chimpanzee from around 14 months of age, as they are just beginning to and human temperament suggest that a similar process learn language [5]. However, perhaps surprisingly, chim- may have been an important catalyst leading to the panzees, so impressive in solving so many other social evolution of unusual social skills in our own species.
    [Show full text]