<<

journal ofNeurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiany 1994;57:73-78 73

Selective deficit of visual size perception: two J Neurol Neurosurg : first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from cases of hemimicropsia

Laurent Cohen, Fran9oise Gray, Christian Meyrignac, Stanislas Dehaene, Jean-Denis Degos

Abstract pathways, such as the chiasmatic tumor Hemimicropsia is a rare disorder of reported by Bender and Savitsky," can cause characterised by an . Micropsia of neurological origin is apparent reduction of the size of objects most frequently reported as a manifestation when presented in one hemifield. We of .'213 It then affects report two cases of hemumicropsia either the entire , or the object that resulting from focal lesions. The the patient fixates at the moment of the first patient was an art teacher and could . It is accompanied by a broad variety accurately depict his abnormal visual of temporal epileptic SyMptomS.7 8 More perception. He subsequently died and his rarely, micropsia can be part of purely visual brain was examined post mortem. In the seizures. It then affects only one half of the second patient, micropsia was assessed visual field, and is accompanied by other by a quantified size comparison task. cerebral visual disturbances, such as meta- The size of a given object is normally morphopsia or dyschromatopsia.415 Apart perceived as constant across any spatial from epileptic phenomena, transient microp- position. Hemimicropsia may thus be sia can also result from ,'6 or from considered a limited violation of the size the action of mescaline and other hallucino- constancy principle. Behavioural and genic drugs. anatomical data are discussed in relation Permanent dysmetropsia following focal to the neural basis of visual object per- cerebral lesions is rare and affects lateral ception in humans. homonymous segments of the visual field.'718 It often may be overlooked, because of severe (7Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:73-78) associated visual impairments,'9 20 or because of the mildness of the functional disability.7 The only probable case of permanent dys- It is now widely acknowledged that different metropsia with satisfactory localisation data types of visual information are processed in was recently reported by Ebata et al,2' with an the brain along anatomically distinct path- unexpected retrosplenial lesion. We describe ways.' Neuropsychology has long provided two cases of pure hemimicropsia following data suggestive of this organisation, through posterior cerebral damage. the description of deficits selectively affecting http://jnnp.bmj.com/ the perception of-for example, colour or Service de Neurologie, movement.2' Building upon these initial clini- Case 1 H6pital de la studies and brain functional The patient was a 50-year-old right-handed Salpetriere, Paris, cal data, animal France imaging in humans have recently allowed a man with no history of psychiatric disorders, L Cohen more systematic and detailed study of these working as an art teacher. Since the age of Service de processing modules.45 There is, however, about 40, he had suffered occasional attacks Neuropathologie,

only scant evidence about the neural basis of of ophthalmic migraine. His mother and one on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. Hopital Henri Mondor, Cr6teil, size constancy, one major property of the nor- brother had similar ophthalmic migraine. At France mal visual system.6 The size of a given object the age of 44, he had suffered a myocardial F Gray is perceived as constant, whatever its location infarct. On his way to a routine cardiological D6partement de and distance. A correction process allows for consultation, the patient experienced sudden Neurosciences, CHU the location and the size of the retinal projec- left homonymous hemianopia. At the same Henri Mondor, Cr6teil, France tion in the course of object perception. time, he noticed that he could not recognise C Meyrignac Dysmetropsia (also called dysmegalopsia), the face of a friend who was with him at that J-D Degos is a disorder of visual perception charac- moment. He was able to find his way to the Laboratoire de terised by an apparent modification of the hospital. He did not recognise his usual car- Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, size of perceived objects. Objects appear diologist, nor the other members of the med- INSERM and CNRS, either shrunk (micropsia) or enlarged ical staff. One hour later, right-sided Paris, France (), relative to their normal size. An throbbing began, and lasted for S Dehaene overview of published reports shows that about two hours. The patient was admitted to Address correspondence to Dr L Cohen, Service de micropsia and macropsia result from similar the hospital. Two days later, left hemianopia Neurologie, H6pital de la causes,7- but micropsia occurs much more had almost completely disappeared but the Salpetriere, 47 Boulevard de I'H6pital, 75651 Paris frequently. prosopagnosia was still severe. The patient CEDEX 13, France. Monocular micropsia can result from reti- could not identify the members of his family Received 27 November nal oedema causing a dislocation of the visually, although he recognised them readily 1992 and in revised form 8 February 1993. receptor cells.'0 Exceptionally, lesions affect- upon hearing their voices. In addition, the Accepted 15 February 1993 ing other parts of the extracerebral visual patient had some difficulty analysing complex 74 Cohen, Gray, Meyrignac, Dehaene, Degos

Figure 1 Sample gyrus and the inferior part of the middle J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from drawings ofsymmetrical objects by thefirst patient. occipital gyrus (fig 3). This region corre- The left half is consistently sponds to the lower part of the lateral aspect larger than the right half. '..I\\ of areas 18 and 19. More anteriorly, the '.\I1\ .'. I infarct successively affected the depth of the k. inferior temporal sulcus, part of the middle .f- " 4' ,1;, .. temporal gyrus, and the depth of the superior f ;- I- iI.... b I -.. temporal sulcus. The occipitotemporal gyrus I -~~tI and the angular gyrus were spared. The cervi- cal and intracranial arteries were normal. is/ I' Ni This completely haemorrhagic watershed 4 infarct was interpreted as a consequence of 11IPISI .1I, transient arterial vasospasm having occurred simultaneously in the carotid and verte- brobasilar systems. A \I/ Case 2 ,\,V/ . The patient was a 60-year-old right-handed woman, working as a secretary, with no his- tory of previous neurological or psychiatric visual scenes. He would describe isolated disorders. She underwent surgery for a devi- parts of the scene, mostly picking out the ated nasal septum. On the fourth day follow- region on the right handside, but had ing surgery, she presented with progressively difficulties perceiving the visual field as a increasing visual disorders. Firstly, she com- coherent whole, a behaviour suggestive of plained that objects and faces would disap- simultanagnosia with slight left spatial pear from her view although she could see neglect.22 Finally, he complained that he had part of them. She could see her daughter's special difficulty perceiving depth, propor- earring, but not her daughter's face, or a hook tions, and symmetry. There was no sensory in the wall between two windows, but not the or motor deficit, aphasia, alexia, or apraxia. windows themselves. Having got up from her One week later, prosopagnosia and simul- bed, she turned back but could not see the tanagnosia had receded. Visual field was nor- bed any more. Secondly, she would miss mal on Goldmann perimetry. The patient objects she tried to reach, such as a glass of complained, however, that objects falling in water. She was unable to follow a moving tar- his left visual field appeared somewhat shrunk get visually. In addition to these complex and compressed. He felt it particularly diffi- visual impairments, she was confused, disori- cult to appreciate the symmetry of pictures. ented in time and space, and had anterograde When drawing, he spontaneously tended to amnesia. The condition worsened until the compensate for his perceptual asymmetry by second day. Then confusion, disorientation drawing the left half of objects slightly larger and amnesia receded rapidly. than the right half (fig 1). He was also pre- One week later, simultanagnosia was still sented with truly symmetrical patterns, which present: the patient could not visually grasp http://jnnp.bmj.com/ he perceived as smaller on the left than on the the whole of an object, although she per- right. When asked to correct them so as to ceived isolated details. Visual field was grossly make them look symmetrical, he either normal on confrontation. The ability to reach expanded the left part of the pattern, or for targets in the right visual hemifield was reduced its right part (fig 2). In a sample of severely impaired, indicating unilateral optic six spontaneous or corrected drawings of ataxia. The patient spontaneously reported

symmetrical objects, linear measures in the that people's left eye (the one she saw on her on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. left half were on the average 16% larger than right) seemed to be smaller and lower than the corresponding measures in the right half. their right one. Her difficulties in dressing The patient did not mention any anomaly of and eating apparently resulted from her colour or movement perception, which were impaired ability to reach rather than from not further explored. ideomotor apraxia. There was no prosopag- Ten days after onset, CT showed a hypo- nosia and colours were perceived normally. dense area in the right occipital region. A There was no sensory motor deficit; oral lan- diagnosis of migrainous stroke was pro- guage comprehension and production were posed.23 The patient died 27 months later normal. The patient could also read correctly, because of disseminated pancreatic carci- except that, when arriving at the end of a line, noma, and a pathological study of the brain she had great difficulty finding the beginning was performed. of the next one. CT showed bilateral occipi- CAFE de Is toparietal hypodense areas suggestive of bilat- PA% r POSTMORTEM EXAMINATION eral cerebral infarction. Visually evoked Postmortem examination revealed an old, potentials showed normal Pl00 waves for the Figure 2 Examples of haemorrhagic, watershed infarct in the right two hemifields of each eye. actually symmetrical cerebral hemisphere, at the boundary Two years after the stroke, the patient only patterns corrected by the of minor first patient so as to make between the territories of the middle and pos- complained sequelae. Firstly, them look symmetrical to terior cerebral arteries. The most obvious part because of residual optic ataxia, she was him. of the lesion destroyed the inferior occipital slightly inaccurate when reaching with either Selective deficit ofvisual size perception 75

Figure 3A Lateral aspect micropsia objectively, we submitted her to a J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from ofthefirst patient's right controlled size comparison task. On each hemisphere, showing the was occipital lesion, with a trial, a pair of horizontally aligned circles more anterior temporal presented on a computer screen, and the extension. patient had to decide which circle was larger. The patient was seated in front of the screen, at a distance of about 55 cm. She was moder- ately long sighted and was wearing her usual during testing. The mean diameter of the two circles was 24 mm, and their centres were 72 mm apart. The largest circle was on the left in half the trials, and on the right in the other half. The target circles were pre- ceded by a central fixation cross, and were flashed for 150 ms to avoid ocular move- ments. The patient was asked to press a key with her left hand if the circle on the left was hand for objects falling in her right visual larger, and to press another key with her right field. She could prevent misreaching by care- hand if the circle on the right was larger. She ful visual fixation of the object she was aiming was informed that the two circles were never at. Secondly, the right half of symmetrical identical, and that she had to respond as objects, such as faces, pairs of hands or tele- rapidly and as accurately as possible. In a first phone sets, seemed consistently smaller than set of trials, the difference between the diam- their left half. The patient did not mention eters of the two circles was 5%, 10%, 15% or any anomaly of colour or movement percep- 20% of the mean diameter. Each of the eight tion, which were not further explored. There possible targets was presented a total of 24 was a partial right inferior homonymous fasci- times in random order. A second set included cular deficit on Goldman perimetry, sparing trials with identical circles and trials with the central 15° of the visual field. Automated diameter differences of 5% and 10%. Still, static perimetry was normal along the hori- the patient was instructed that the two circles zontal meridian. were never identical. Each of the five possible MRI showed a limited left hemispheric targets was presented a total of 30 times in lesion affecting the lower part of areas 18 and random order. 19 and the underlying white matter (fig 4). On the first set of trials, the overall pattern On the right, there was a small spot of high of responses displayed a normal distance signal intensity in the white matter posterior effect: the more similar the two circles, the to the lateral ventricle. higher the number of errors (Kendall's t = Contrary to the first patient, the present -0 801, p = 0 0023). Performance was far patient was unable to represent her distorted better than chance on all trials (all x2 (1 df) visual perception graphically. To evaluate her >1 0-6), except when the diameter of the right circle was larger by 5% (fig 5). In this latter condition, the patient did not respond better than chance (X2(l df) = 0-667, p = 0-414). http://jnnp.bmj.com/ This high error rate could not simply result from the difficulty to discriminate two circles whose diameter difference was only 5%, as performance was far better than chance when the left circle was larger by the same amount (X2(I df) = 13-5, p < 0-001). The perfor-

mance was significantly better when the left on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. circle was 5% larger, than when the right cir- cle was 5% larger (X2(l df) = 5-17, p = 0-023). The results of the second set repli- cated those of the first one. As fig 5 shows, responses were far better than chance in all conditions with different circles (all X2(1 df) >8.5), except when the right circle was larger by 5% (X2(l df) = 2-13, p = 0-144). The per- formance was significantly better when the left circle was 5% larger than when the right circle was 5% larger (X2(1 df) = 9.77, p < 1A 'qA' 0 002). The existence of a perceptive bias is I-s therefore clearly demonstrated. When the p, right circle was slightly larger than the left cir-

-1, cle, the patient perceived the two circles as identical, and randomly chose the left or the right key. It should be noted that when the circles Figure 3B (Top) Frontal sections ofthefirst patient's right and (bottom) were identical, the patient showed no bias corresponding reconstruction using templates from Damasio and Damasio. 33 towards responding that the right one was 76 Cohen, Gray, Meyrignac, Dehaene, Degos J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from

21

Figure 4 (Top) MRI transversal sections ofthe secondpatient's brain, and (bottom) corresponding reconstrction using templatesfrom Damasio and Damasio33. The lesioned left hemisphere is represented on the right side.

smaller (Xf2(1 df) = 0 f13, p = 0'7 15). This fact (about 2 5%). As stimuli were flashed very is apparently at odds with the spontaneous briefly, the patient could not reach a decision illusory perception of symmetrical objects as threshold for such small perceptual differ- asymmetrical. It can be speculated that the ences, and therefore responded randomly. By http://jnnp.bmj.com/ actual amplitude of the bias is some propor- contrast, the spontaneous of asymme- tion situated between 0% and 5%. Pairs of try occurred under natural conditions with identical circles, as well as pairs with the right unlimited viewing time.24 circle larger by 5%, would then be perceived by the patient as very slightly asymmetrical Discussion

We have presented clinical, behavioural, and on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. anatomical data concerning two patients showing isolated stable hemimicropsia. Figure 5 The second Figures 3 and 4 show that the lesions overlap patient's performance on in areas 18 and 19 and underlying white mat- the size comparison task ter. In both cases, the deficit apparently (triangles: first set; circles: resulted from a lesion affecting the unimodal second set). When the right circle was 5% larger than visual association cortex. Vascular lesions are the left one, she equally -C rarely restricted to this cortical region, which often responded 'larger 0 could explain the of right' and 'larger left', infrequency hemimicrop- probably perceiving the two 0. sia. Usually, lesions encompassing this area circles as identical. also yield or hemianopia, preclud- ing the expression of a disorder of size per- ception. In our patients, the lesions did not az U) correspond to conventional arterial territories. Such is also the case for previously reported cases, where the underlying lesion was neo- plastic,'7 18 traumatic20 or haemorrhagic.2' How does the locus of the lesions fit cur- 5 -10 -5 0 5 10 15 rent concepts about the organisation of the Right-left size difference (%) visual system? A parallel can be drawn Selective deficit ofvisual size perception 77

between size perception and colour percep- variations of distance and position. In both J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from tion, although the latter domain is better patients the relative size of objects within one understood at present. The spectral composi- hemifield was seemingly normal, indicating tion of the light reflected by an object differs only limited impairment of the size correction according to the spectrum of the ambient processes. We may suggest that lesions affect- light. The brain, however, takes into account ing this fimctional module only resulted in a the characteristics of the ambient light and mistuning affecting the size correction computes a constant subjective colour over process for stimuli within one hemifield. This wide illumination changes.25 Similarly, the impairment would therefore yield interhemi- size of the retinal projection of a given object spheric discrepancy in size perception. depends on its distance and angular position relative to the eye. Object perception requires 1 Livingstone M. Segregation of form, color, movement, that the image be corrected for these parame- and depth processing in the visual system: Anatomy, ters, to compute a constant representation of physiology, art, and illusion. In: Cohen B, Bodis- Wollner L, eds. Vision and the Brain. New York: Raven the actual size of the object. In this respect, Press, 1990:119-38. the property of size constancy is largely com- 2 Zeki S. A century of cerebral achromatopsia. Brain 1990;113: 1721-77. parable to colour constancy. It may appear 3 Zihl J, Von Cramon D, Mai N, Schmid CH. Disturbance natural that the ventral occipitotemporal of movement vision after bilateral posterior brain dam- age. Brain 1991;114:2235-52. visual pathway, which is responsible for 4 Desimone R, Schein SJ. Visual properties of neurons in object vision, including probably colour con- area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. J Neurophysiol 1987;57:835-68. stancy,2 should also mediate perception of 5 Haxby JV, Grady CL, Ungerleider LG, Horwitz B. size.2627 Accordingly, Hart, Lesser and Lee28 Mapping the functional neuroanatomy of the intact human brain with brain work imaging. Neuropsychologia have reported a patient who, under electrical 1991 ;29:539-55. stimulation in the posterior part of the left 6 Sedgwick HA. Space perception. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP, eds. Handbook of perception and human middle temporal gyrus, could not make ver- performance, Vol 1. New York: John Wiley & Sons, bal size judgments-for example, "Is a tree 1986:21/1-21/57. 7 Mouren P, Tatossian A. Les visuo-spatiales: bigger than an ant?". The authors suggest Etude clinique. Encephale 1963;52:439-80. that this very selective deficit resulted from a 8 Muller C. Mikropsie und makropsie. Monatschr Psychiat Neurol 1956; Suppl.98:1-60. disconnection between superior temporal lan- 9 Miller NR. Central disorders of visual integration. In: guage areas and inferior temporal regions Walsh and Hoyt's clinical neuro-. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1982:153-71. responsible for the processing of visual size. 10 Frisen L. and visual field tests: There is further experimental evidence from Psychophysical versus pathophysiological objectives. In: Kennard C, Clifford Rose I, eds. Physiological aspects of animal studies pointing to the involvement of neuro-ophthalmology. London: Chapman & Hall, the occipitotemporal pathway in the percep- 1991:3-26. 11 Bender MB, Savitsky N. Micropsia and teleopsia limited tual equivalence of objects across translations to the temporal fields of vision. Arch Ophthalmol of retinal position,29 and across size modifica- 1943;29:904-8. 12 Mullan S, Penfield W. Illusions of comparative interpreta- tions.'0 More specifically, cells in area V4 tion and emotion. Arch Neurol Psychiat 1959;81:269-84. have been shown to be tuned to the length or 13 Gloning I, Gloning K, Hoff H. Neuropsychological symp- toms and syndromes in lesions of the occipital lobe and the to the width of visual stimuli within large adjacent areas. Paris: Gauthier-Villars, 1968. receptive fields,4 and removal of this area 14 Teuber HL, Battersby WS, Bender MB. Visualfield defects after penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Cambridge results in impaired size discrimination.'2 Harvard University Press, 1960. In our patients, as in other cases of perma- 15 Mooney AJ, Carey P, Ryan M, Bofin P. Parasagittal 18 21 parieto-occipital meningioma with visual . http://jnnp.bmj.com/ nent dysmetropsia,17 the anomaly was Am _Ophthalmol 1965;59:197-205. restricted to homonymous segments of the 16 Wilson SAK. Dysmetropsia and its pathogenesis. Trans Ophthalmol Soc UK 1916;36:412-44. visual field. The micropsia affected the left 17 Bregeat P, Klein M, Thiebaut F, Bouniol. Hemi-macrop- hemifield in the first patient, and the right sie homonyme droite et tumeur occipitale gauche. Rev Oto-Neuro-Ophtalmol 1947;19:238-40. hemifield in the second patient. These obser- 18 Thiebaut F, Matavulj N. Hemi-micropsie relative vations suggest that each hemisphere is in homonyme droite en quadrant inferieur. Rev Oto-Neuro- Ophtalmol 1949;21:245-6. charge of size processing only for contralat- 19 Bender MB, Teuber HL. Spatial organization of visual

eral stimuli. In primates, the size of the recep- perception following injury to the brain. Arch Neurol on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. Psychiat 1947;58:721-39. tive field increases along the pathway from Vl 20 Bender MB, Teuber HL. Spatial organization of visual to V4 and from V4 to inferior temporal cor- perception following injury to the brain. Arch Neurol Psychiat 1948;59:39-62. tex. Up to area V4, neurons show excitatory 21 Ebata S, Ogawa M, Tanaka Y, Mizuno Y, Yoshida M. responses only to contralateral stimuli. By Apparent reduction in the size of one side of the face associated with a small retrosplenial haemorrhage. contrast, inferior temporal neurons also J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1991;54:68-70. respond to stimuli presented in the ipsilateral 22 Damasio AR. Disorders of complex visual processing: agnosias, achromatopsia, Balint's syndrome, and related field.25 It is therefore plausible that hemimi- difficulties of orientation and construction. In: MM cropsia should result from lesions affecting Mesulam, ed. Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: F A Davis, 1985:259-88. the posterior part of the ventral visual path- 23 Rothrock JF, Walicke P, Swenson MR, Lyden PD, Logan way. This idea is compatible with the lesions WR. Migrainous stroke. Arch Neurol 1988;45:63-7. 24 Carlson VR. Instructions and perceptual constancy judg- we have described, which overlap in the lower ments. In: Epstein W, ed. Stability and constancy in part of the lateral aspect of areas 18 and 19. visual perception: Mechanisms and processes. New York: Wiley-Interscience, 1977:217-54. More precise correlations with animal data 25 Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Neural mechanisms of can only be tentative. visual processing in monkeys. In: Boller F, Graftman J, eds. Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. 2. Amsterdam: The two cases we have reported suggest Elsevier, 1989:267-99. that size perception may be dissociated from 26 Mishkin M, Ungerleider LG, Macko KA. Object vision and spatial vision: Two cortical pathways. Trends other aspects of visual perception such as Neurosci 1983;6:414-7. colour or movement. Part of the ventral 27 Plaut DC, Farah MJ. Visual object representation: Interpreting neurophysiological data within a computa- visual association cortex probably computes a tional framework. J Cogn Neurosci 1990;2:320-43. representation of size that is constant across 28 Hart J, Lesser RP, Gordon B. Selective interference with 78 Cohen, Gray, Meyrgnac, Dehaene, Degos

representation of size in the human by direct cortical rhesus monkeys: Effects of pulvinar, prestriate, and J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.57.1.73 on 1 January 1994. Downloaded from electrical stimulation. Jf Cogn Neurosci 1992;4:337-44. inferotemporal lesions. Exp Brain Res 1969;27:251-69. 29 Gross CG, Mishkin M. The neural basis of stimulus 31 Weiskrantz L, Saunders RC. Impairments of visual object equivalence across retinal translation. In: Hamad S, transforms in monkeys. Brain 1984;107:1033-72. Doty R, Jaynes J, Goldberg L, Krauthamer G, eds. 32 Schiller PH, Lee K. The role of the primate extrastriate Lateralization in the nervous system. New York: Academic area V4 in vision. Science 1991;251:1251-3. Press, 1977:109-22. 33 Damasio H, Damasio AR. Lesion analysis in neuropsychol- 30 Ungerleider LG, Ganz L, Pribram KH. Size constancy in ogy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1989. http://jnnp.bmj.com/ on September 30, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright.