Forced Exposure and Psychological Reactance: Antecedents and Consequences of the Perceived Intrusiveness of Pop-Up Ads Steven M. Edwards, Hairong Li, and Joo-Hyun Lee

This paper explores forced viewing of 'bop-up ads" on the Internet to understand better how viewers come to define ads as irritating and decide to avoid them. Perceived intrusiveness was suggested as the underlying mechanism by which the process occurs. Antecedents of intrusiveness wwe identified that perceptions of ads as interruptions, including congruence of the advertisement content with the current task and intensity of at the moment the ad pops up. The consequences of intrusiveness were shown to be caused by feel- ings of irritation and ad avoidance. The results provide an understandir~gof how consumers experience forced exposure situations in interactive environments and highlight implications for udvertisers seeking to increase the effectiveness of on-line advertising.

Steven M. Edwards (Ph.D., The idea that, for every action, there is an equal and opposite reaction has University of Texas at Austin) is assistant professor, Department been termed reactance in literature. Reactance theory is a social Advertising, Michigan State psychological theory that explains human behavior in response to the per- University ceived loss of freedorn in an environment (Brehm 1966). Reactance is postu- Hairong Li (Ph.D.,Michigan State lated to be experienced in response to the environment and used to help University) is an associate professor, Departn~entof' Advertising, Ivlichigari persons reestablish freedom and control of a situation. When there is a State Cniversity. threat to a person's freedom, that person will attempt to restore the freedom Joo-Hyun Lee is a doctoral by exhibiting opposition or resisting pressures to conform (Brehm and Brehm candidate in the Department of 1981).Similarly, consumers have becm shown to interpret cominercial mes- Advertising, Michigan State University. sages actively and react against threats of to further their own This research was funded by a grant goals. This paper explores Web surfers' reactions when they are forced to view from the American Academy of advertising. Reactance may be especially important in light of new advertising Advertising. delivery techniques developed for the Internet, such as pop-up ads. The purpose of this research is twofbld: First, we investigate what charac- teristics of pop-up ads are perceived as intrusive. Specifically, the study inves- tigates four aspects of ads themselves that may moderate perceptions of pop-up advertisements as intrusive: timing of the display, duration of the ad, congru- ence with editorial content, and perceived informational and entertainment value. Second, we hope to understand better the relationship between the antecedents and consequences of ads being perceived as intrusive by examin- ing reactions such as irritation and ad avoidance. We believe that, if the point at which advertising becomes intrusive can be identified, strategies for reduc- ing irritation and the avoidance of advertising can be formulated

Forcing Exposure to Internet Advertising The declining click-through rates of conventional banner ads and rising doubts about Web sites7advertising business models are driving concerned advertisers beyond banner ads. As advertisers scramble to find alternatives, rich media are quickly becoming the standard by which the sophistication of Journal of Ad~ertzszng, Internet advertising is being judged. Rich media ads contain content created Volume XXXI, Nurr~ber3 with new technologies (e.g., Java, JavaScript, Macromedia Flash, Macromedia Fall 2002 Shockwave, (D)HTML, VRML) and are used to deliver high aural and visual The Journal ofAdvertising impact. Use of rich media is growing at an annual enhance recall, they also may result in negative atti- rate of 53% and is forecasted to reach $34 billion by tude formation (Ha 1996) or avoidance of the ads 2004 (Bowen 2001). With the increase in the popular- altogether (Abernethy 1991).Thus, an important theo- ity of rich media ads, new delivery techniques have retical and practical issue for pop-up ads on-line is been developed to solve bandwidth problems associ- how to minimize the negative perceptions while tak- ated with larger rich media file sizes. ing advantage of the potential effective benefits. Pop-ups are one of the popular techniques to de- liver rich media ads that are able to contain more The Perceid Intrusiveness ofAduertising sophisticated messages on the Web (Milward Brown Interactive 1999a). According to Interactive Adver- In a survey of U.S. consumers, Bauer and Greyser tising Bureau's (2001) guidelines, pop-ups (formally (1968) identify as the main reasons people criticize termed interstitials) refer to a form of rich media ads advertising the annoyance or irritation it causes, that "automatically launch in a new browser window which is believed to lead to a general reduction in when a Web page is loaded." Pop-unders are another advertising effectiveness (Aaker and Bruzzone 1985). form of interstitials that load behind the users' Web However, research also indicates that consumers' criti- browser so that they may be seen after users close the cisms of advertising are generally directed at the tac- browser window (Taylor 2001). Although different tics advertisers employ that make the experience of forms of interstitials can be programmed, they are processing advertising negative, rather than at the distinct from conventional banner ads in the manner institution of advertising itself (Bauer and Greyser in which they are displayed. Banner ads appear when 1968; Ducoffe 1996; Sandage and Leckenby 1980). viewing Web pages, usually at the top or along the Therefore, developing a better understanding of these sides of the page. Because banner ads are generally "annoying" or "irritating" tactics should allow for the displayed on the periphery, they do not interrupt the creation of more effective advertisements. activity of Web viewers. However, interstitials can be A few studies have identified aspects of advertising programmed to appear when entering or exiting a that lead to negative feelings. For example, some be- Web page, after a certain amount of time on a Web lieve that irr~tationoccurs as a function of the adver- page, or when a link is selected. The window then can tising content and level of stimulation. Content that be programmed to remain for a predetermined length talks down to consumers, is overly exaggerated, or of time or until the user chooses to close the window. makes confusing statements has been identified as When faced with interstitials, Web users are inter- irritating to consumers (Bauer and Greyser 1968). rupted and forced to react to unrequested commercial Advertisemerlts that excessively stimulate consum- messages. In some cases, viewers have the option to ers' senses can also elicit feelings of irritation. Con- "zap" the advertisement by closing the pop-up win- sumers can become overwhelmed if the ads are too dow, but newer interstitial ad formats expand within long, too loud, or too big (Aaker and Bruzzone 1985; a Web page and do not offer such an opportunity. Bauer and Greyser 1968). Consumers may also feel Web surfers are forced to view a short commercial overstimulated when viewing many ads in a short message if they wish to see the Web site. In either time or seeing a single ad too frequently (Bauer and case, interruptions force users to respond cognitively, Greyser 1968).The likely result is a retreat away from affectively, or behaviorally, possibly resulting in ei- the source of irritation, or ad avoidance (Kennedy 1971; ther positive or negative outcomes for the advertiser. Krugman 1983; Park and McClung 1986; Soldow and This type of forced exposure may elicit a viewer's Principe 1981).Abernethy (1991) finds that consumers involuntary attention, as described by Kahneman often leave the room or change channels to avoid adver- (19731, which would result in positive effects such as tising. Others have shown that television viewers sim- greater processing and increased memory for the ad ply ignore ads (Clancey 1994; Krugman and Johnson message. Industry studies have shown increases in 1991). However, what is not clear is why the same ad recall, awareness, and purchase intention for advertising is annoying to some but not all consumers. interstitials compared with conventional banner ads A possible explanation for why consumers view only (Milward Brown Interactive 1999b). However, forced some advertising as irritating is the concept of intru- exposure often interrupts a viewer's normal viewing siveness. Ha (1996) defines intrusiveness as the in- process, and rich media content may cause a delay in terruption of editorial content. Because the first downloading due to large file sizes. Both situations objective of advertising is to get noticed, by defini- could lead to a negative perception of the advertising tion, advertisements seek to interrupt editorial con- as intrusive. Although intrusive advertisements may tent. By interfering with the goals of consumers, Fall 2002 advertising effectively limits the number of actions attempts intrude on a person's freedom determine that consumers can take to attain their goals. Consum- that consumer's response. Brehm (1966) terms this ers must reevaluate their goals to include advertising feeling reactance and proposes that it occurs to the (acquiesce),or negative reactions are likely to result in degree that (11 the behavior threatened is important, the avoidance of advertising in some way. Aaker and (2) the severity of the threat to the behavior increases, Bruzzone (1985) suggest that negative reactions to ad- (3) the threat affects other freedoms, and (4) the per- vertisements occur to the degree that they cause impa- son ever actually enjoyed the freedom. Clee and tience. To the extent that advertisements are recognized Wicklund (1980) describe reactance as a boomerang as disturbing, negative outcomes such as irritation and effect in which the perception of coercion is met with avoidance may result (Kennedy 1971; Krugrnan 1983; an equal but opposite influence, which is used by Park and McClung 1986; Soldow and Principe 1981). consumers to restore their freedom of choice. This Therefore, though irritation is a possible emotional re- effect can foster a desire to engage in the threatened action and avoidance is a potential behavioral outcome behavior even more strongly (rebellion)or can be mani- in response to intrusive advertising, the perception of fested as an change in the person's belief an ad as intrusive is something different. that the activity is important (acquiescence). The perception of an advertisement as intrusive Several studies demonstrate that hard-sell tactics should be considered a cognitive evaluation of the are less persuasive than soft-sell tactics (Clee and degree to which the advertisement interrupts a Wicklund 1980; Reizenstine 1971), and Brehm and person's goals. If we define what is intrusive accord- Brehm (1981) point out that hard-sell messages re- ing to the person, advertising itself is not intrusive. veal the intent of the persuader and therefore should Rather, intrusiveness is defined by the degree to which be met with grrater resistance. Robertson and Rossiter a person deems the presentation of information as (1974) find that perceptions of persuasion correlated contrary to his or her goals (either functional or he- with less favorable attitudes toward the product be- donic). As such, intrusiveness should be considered ing sold. To the degree that advertisements are rec- distinct from the emotional or behavioral outcomes ognized as simply attempts at persuasion, they could that may result. Therefore, it becomes important to evoke a mild form of reactance. understand the means by which perceptions of intru- Although perhaps not as strong as other forms of siveness can be limited to reduce the negative out- direct coercion, advertisements may be perceived as comes that are likely to result. an interruption and elicit a similar feeling. To the Pasadeos (1990) finds that, when ads are perceived degree that radio listeners are enjoying music, the as valuable (containing useful information),they elicit threat of advertising may result in channel surfing to less irritation and avoidance. According to Ducoffe regain the freedom to listen to music. To the degree (1995), advertising value is best understood as an that the option of changing stations is taken away or overall representation of the worth of advertising to all stations are playing ads at the same time, in- consumers. Ducoffe's (1995, 1996) studies indicate creased psychological reactance should be manifest that ad value is positively correlated with the infor- against the interruption (advertising) and perhaps mativeness and entertainment value of an ad and radio stations themselves. Multiple theories of media that both information and entertainment value are interactions indicate that consumers are wary of per- essential for communication exchanges between ad- suasion. Consumers actively interpret the techniques vertisers and consumers. Therefore, the perception of that ads use to persuade (Friestad and Wright 1994) intrusiveness may be affected when an ad offers the and form counterarguments against ad claims when viewer either utilitarian or aesthetic value or both. they are highly involved (Petty and Cacioppo 19791, To the degree that advertising does not provide value, all in an effort to defend themselves or react against it may be perceived as coercive and unwelcome. It is persuasive messages. this feeling of intrusiveness that may drive negative If consumers' reactions to advertising are defen- reactions toward ads that are recognized as trying to sive, it may not be an effective strategy to force them persuade. to view advertisements. Reactance theory would dic- tate that forced exposure will result in negative con- Reactance sequences for advertisers; however, not all forced advertising may be unwelcome. Discovery of the Brehm and Brehm (1981) describe attempts to means by which reactance can be minimized may change behavior as involving both persuasion and increase the efficacy of advertising that has the po- coercion and believe that the degree to which these tential to threaten viewers' perceptions of freedom. The Journal ofAdvertising

Hypotheses tions, such as escapism, diversion, esthetic enjoyment, or emotional release (McQuail 1983). This psycho- As has been argued, intrusiveness can be thought logical force, in opposition to reactance, has been of as a psychological consequence that occurs when termed positive . Clee and Wicklund an audience member's cognitive processes are inter- (1980) posit that persuasive communications elicit rupted and that may result in the avoidance of adver- both reactance and positive social change and that tising. However, advertising may not always be the net result of any persuasive communication must perceived as equally intrusive. Perceived intrusive- take into account both forces. Given the existence of a ness may be moderated by the intensity or focus of a positive social interpretation of persuasive communi- cognitive process. By the intensity of a cognitive pro- cation, the degree to which viewers perceive benefits cess, we mean the degree to which an audience is or gratifications from persuasion attempts should mentally engaged in an activity, not cognition itself. counter the perception of intrusiveness. For example, a television commercial would be more One possible positive social influence that may affect intrusive when inserted in the middle of a highly the perception of intrusiveness is the perceived value of involving show than at the end of a less arousing an advertisement. More specifically, perceived intru- program. Similarly, the timing of exposure to rich siveness may be lessened for ads that are deemed of media ads during normal surfing sessions on the Web high value. When faced with advertising on-line, users may differentially affect perceptions of intrusiveness. may regard the degree of congruity between the adver- For example, viewers' cognitive intensity is likely to tising content and the editorial content as valuable. be higher when they are viewing a content page than For example, exposure to advertising for Chevrolet whlle when taking a cognitive pause to switch pages. Simi- surfing the Internet for information about a new car larly, cognitive intensity should be higher when tak- purchase would probably be perceived as less intrusive ing a cognitive pause to switch pages than when than would an ad for Microsoft in the same situation. finished surfing and closing the browser. Therefore, Ads that are congruent with expectations and/or cur- an ad displayed under different cognitive loads may rent cognitive activities could be perceived as positive be perceived as different in terms of intrusiveness. social influences and thus not be considered threats to We hypothesize that the consumer's freedom. In contrast, ads not congruent Hla: Ads that interrupt content pages will be with expectations activate divergent knowledge struc- perceived as more intrusive than will ads tures and create added mental processing demands. displayed between breaks in content pages. These added demands threaten the freedom to con- Hlb: Ads displayed between breaks in con- tinue with current processing activities and may result tent pages will be perceived as more in considerable reactance. Therefore, intrusive than will ads displayed upon H3: Ads that are congruent with the editorial closing the browser. content will be perceived as less intru- Similarly, the duration of the interruption may af- sive than ads that are not congruent. fect the perceived level of intrusiveness. The longer H4: Ads that are congruent with the editorial an interruption, the more intrusive it may be per- content will be perceived as more infor- ceived to be. Theoretically, extended interruptions mative than ads that are not congruent. should be perceived as greater threats to freedom H5: Ads that are perceived as more informative than should shorter interruptions, which should re- will be rated as less intrusive than ads that sult in greater psychological discomfort and greater are perceived as less informative. reactance. Therefore, A second positive social influence identified by Bauer H2: Longer ads will be perceived as more and Greyser (1968) and Ducoffe (19951is entertain- intrusive than will shorter ads in forced ment. To the degree that advertisements are per- exposure situations. ceived as entertaining, they should provide value to Holding the intensity of cognition constant, there the viewer. To the degree that the entertainment is may be other aspects of advertisements that moder- welcome, it should not be perceived as interrupting the ate the perception of intrusiveness. Persuasion at- cognitive goals of the viewer, and, therefore, it should tempts are not always perceived as intrusive and met garner less psychological reactance than advertising with skepticism, counterarguments, or source dero- judged less entertaining. Thus, it is expected that gations; they can be met with support arguments H6: Ads that are perceived as more entertain- (Petty and Cacioppo 1979). Advertisements often pro- ing will be rated as less intrusive than ads vide relevant information and/or consumer gratifica- that are perceived as less entertaining. Fall 2002

As reviewed previously, theories of psychological either 10 or 20 seconds. Although participants could reactance suggest that, in response to a loss of free- close the interstitial at will, it was assumed that a dom, viewers will feel uncomfortable and attempt to longer interstitial would be more intrusive than a regain control of their experience. When advertising shorter one if watched entirely. interrupts the goals of consumers, consumers are Cognitive intensity of the viewer when seeing the likely to seek their freedom either passively, by ig- interstitial on a Web site was the third independent noring the interruption (Clancey 1994; Krugman and variable. In the first condition, an interstitial was Johnson 1991), or actively, by dispensing with it programmed to pop up 20 seconds after the experi- (Abernethy 1991; Speck and Elliott 1997).The degree mental page was opened. The experimental pages for to which viewers seek freedom will be directly pro- either movie reviews or financial aid were pretested portional to the perception of the ad as an intrusive and took more than two minutes to read. This condi- threat to that freedom. The perception of the intru- tion was designed to be the most intrusive, as it inter- siveness of an ad will likely result in feelings of irrita- rupted participants at a time they were actively tion and ultimately the avoidance of that ad, if processing information that was needed to complete possible. Therefore, perceived intrusiveness, level of the assigned task. In the second condition, the same irritation experienced, and avoidance behaviors should content as appeared in the first condition was posi- all be related. On the basis of the reviewed literature, tioned on two Web pages with the interstitial placed we believe that between them. The ad opened only when the partici- H7: The perceptiori of intrusiveness will be pants chose to move from the first to the second page. positively related to feelings of irrit at' ion. Providing participants the opportunity to take a "cog- H8: The perception of intrusiveness will be psi- nitive pause" from their reading was designed to re- tively related to advertising avoidance. duce the level of intrusiveness felt in response to the H9: Feelings of irritation will be positively advertisement Participants had to request the sec- related to advertising avoidance. ond page of information, thereby giving them more control over the situation. The third condition was Methods similar to the first condition, but the interstitial was shown only when the participants finished reading Participants the articles and exited the experimental page. By placing the advertisement at the end of the task, A total of 379 participants was recruited from un- participants were no longer actively processing the dergraduate courses at a Midwestern U.S. university information at the Web site and therefore should not to participate in the experiment. The courses were have experienced reactance. campuswide electives, so the participants represented a variety of majors and backgrounds. Women consti- Experimental Stimuli tuted 58% of the sample, men 42%. Two Web sites were created for the experiment: Experimental Design one site contained movie reviews and the other finan- cial aid information. The site themes were selected The experiment used a 2 x 2 x 3 factorial design in from a pretest in which 16 topics were rated for stu- which participants were asked to find out as much dent interest. The selected topics were rated as mod- information as they could about either financial aid erately interesting by a sample of both male and or current movies using a Web site provided. The female students (n=35)who did not participate in the three independent variables manipulated were edito- main experiment. The content was adapted from ex- rial-ad congruence, duration of interruption, and in- isting Web sites. All experimental conditions were tensity of cognition when faced with a pop-up ad. Two identical in structure, font size, color, and the num- conditions of editorial-ad congruence were created by ber and length of the articles. The movie review site placing a pop-up ad for a movie on either a site that reviewed the movies Beautiful and Cyberworld 30, featured movie reviews or a site that featured finan- and the financial aid site contained articles about cial aid information. When an interstitial was incon- "Stafford Loans" and "Loan Consolidation." gruent, it was assumed to be more intrusive than Two interstitials (created using Macromedia Flash) when the advertisement was congruent with the site advertised a fictitious movie titled l'he Good Days. content. Duration of interruption of the interstitial Both interstitials had identical format and content was manipulated by providing content that lasted and varied only in length. The 20-second interstitial 88 The Journal ofAdvertising

Table 1 Scale Reliabilities

Variable Indicators Alpha N When the ad popped-up, I thought it was... Perceived intrusiveness distracting, disturbing, forced, interfering, intrusive, invasive, and obtrusive .91 373 Perceived irritation irritating, phony, ridiculous, stupid, and terrible .87 373 The movie ad I saw was... Informativeness helpful, unimportant, uninformative, and useless .82 370 Entertainment attractive, enjoyable, entertaining, and fun to watch .94 37 1 used blank filler frames to maintain the same amount obtrusive (Li, Edwards, and Lee 2001). Irritation was of information as the 10-second interstitial. Both measured using five items: irritating, phony, ridicu- interstitials were sized 350 x 350 pixels and set to lous, stupid, and terrible (Wells, Leavitt, and pop up in the center of the computer screen. Specifi- McConville 1971).The perceived informativeness and cally, the ads contained four still pictures represent- entertainment value of the interstitial were measured ing the movie scenes, a soft piano as background using modified scales based on the work of Ducoffe music, and a listing of a fictitious title, director, and (1996). Both informativeness (helpful, unimportant, cast of the movie. uninformative, and useless) and entertainment value (attractive, enjoyable, entertaining, and fun to watch) Procedure were measured using four items. Finally, ad avoid- ance was measured using observational data. Partici- Participants were randomly assigned to 1 of the 12 pants' actual time spent viewing the ad m7asassessed conditions (2 levels of editorial-ad congruence, 2 lev- using screen capture software and later coded for els of duration, and 3 levels of cognitive intensity) analysis. Although each of the measures had been and asked to read an instruction page about the ex- used in previous studies, a confirmatory factor analy- periment. To stimulate interest in the Web content, sis of the items was conducted to investigate their participants were told they would be tested about the validity in this context. Specifically, the measured site content at the end of the viewing session and that constructs (intrusiveness, irritation, advertising en- the person who answered the most questions cor- tertainment, and advertising informativeness) were rectly would win a cash prize. Participants viewed tested using first-order confirmatory factor models in the Web pages individually and were fitted with ear- which every item was confined to load on its specified phones to hear the music used in the ad. The ads factor. In all the models, the item-loading estimates were programmed to remain for either 10 or 20 sec- on their prespecified factors were highly significant onds, but participants were able to close the (p<.001), and goodness-of-fit indices demonstrated the interstitials at will and continue with their assigned quality of the measurement models (intrusiveness: task. After the 10-minute viewing session was over, goodness-of-fit index [GFIl=.984, adjusted goodness- each participant was asked to quit the Web browser of-fit index [AGFI]=.965; irritation: GFI=.997, and fill out a questionnaire and then was debriefed. AGFI= .960; advertising entertainment: GFI=.998, AGFI=.990; advertising informativeness: GFI=.993, Measures AGFI=.963).The questions asked and response items that make up each measure, along with reliability A questionnaire was used to assess perceived intru- coefficients, are presented in Table 1. siveness, irritation, perceived informativeness and entertainment value of the movie ad, and the demo- Results graphics of the participants. All items were answered using seven-point scales with response categories from Manipulatiort Checks strongly agree to strongly disagree. The intrusive- ness measure consisted of seven items: distracting, To assess the validity of our manipulations, a third disturbing, forced, interfering, intrusive, invasive, and sample of 60 undergraduate students was randomly Fall 2002 assigned to 1 of the 12 conditions in the main experi- tance (x=2.64) as did those viewing the 20-second ad ment and asked to assess the perceived congruence of (%=2.90),p>.05.Participants who saw an ad within a the ad with the task, the perceived length of the ad, and page reported greater reactance (T=3.32) than did the intensity of cognition at the moment the ad ap- those who saw an ad between pages (%=2.83),who peared. To check the manipulations of our independent reported greater reactance than did those who saw variables, we first examined the degree to which par- an ad when exiting the browser, F,,.,,, =4.78,p<.05.In ticipants recognized the editorial congruence of the ad. summary, though the manipulations of editorial-ad Participants were asked to agree or disagree with state- congruence and cognitive intensity adequately dis- ments regarding the degree to which the ad helped tinguished between conditions, the strength of the them fulfill their task, provided useful information, reactance was only mild. That is, advertising may not and was relevant to their task on a seven-point scale. warrant strong feelings about the loss of control. Edi- Those who saw a congruent ad reported significantly torial-ad congruence was recognized as congruent or greater relevance G=3.16) than did those who saw a not congruent to the assigned task, as anticipated, noncongruent ad (%=2.31),t ,,,, =2.49,p<.05. but ratings of the threat to freedom were opposite To assess if the length of the ads was recognized, expectations. This finding may indicate that, when participants were asked to agree or disagree with ads are relevant, they elicit greater attention and are statements such as the ad was short, seemed to play perceived as greater threats to freedom than when for a long time, and was over before I knew it. There not relevant. If true, the value that ads provide by was no significant difference between those who saw being relevant must overcome the perceived threat to a 10-second ad (x =2.86) and those who saw a 20- freedom to be perceived as unintrusive. Finally, the second ad (T=2.68),~>.05.Therefore, participants per- manipulation to change the perception of the dura- ceived the ads to be of similar length, which indicates tion of the interruption was unsuccessful and not the failure of our manipulation. expected to explain variance in the proposed model. Finally, participants' levels of cognitive intensity at the moment the ads popped up were assessed. Par- Model Testing ticipants reported that they were more involved with the article, more actively reading the information pre- We tested the 10 hypotheses using structural equa- sented, and more focused on the article when an ad tion analysis with -Amos 4.0. The promise of such an popped up while they were reading the text (T =5.20) approach lies in that, when tested simultaneously, than if it appeared between pages (x =3.97) or upon existing relationships between constructs may change exiting the browser (%=3.87), F,,, ,;,=4.71, p<.05. Post and additional relationships may emerge. Structural hoc analyses revealed significant differences in cogni- equation modeling (SEM)captures these changes and tive intensity when participants were reading the thus provides u better description of the relationships text compared with when they were between pages or among variables (Bollen 1989). exiting the browser. There was no significant differ- All constructs described in the hypotheses and ence reported for between pages and exiting the browser. shown in Figure 1were specified in the initial model. The manipulation resulted in only high and low levels Exogenous variables included cognitive intensity, du- of cognitive intensity, and, therefore, no further dis- ration of the interruption, and editorial congruence. tinction was made between the manipulations that re- Each was a manipulated variable and therefore cat- sulted in similar levels of cognitive intensity. egorical in nature. Although not ideal for SEM analy- To conduct an overall assessment of the degree to sis, categorical variables can be successfully which these variables may have elicited reactance, incorporated into such analysis (Joreskog and Sorbom we measured perceptions of the ad as threatening 1989).The variables were coded so that higher values freedom. The 60 participants were asked to rate the reflect greater cognitive intensity, longer duration, degree to which they felt their freedom was threat- and greater congruence. Five endogenous variables ened, that the ad infringed on their freedom, and that included ad informativeness, ad entertainment, ad the ad forced them to respond. In contrast to expecta- intrusiveness. ad irritation, and ad avoidance. These tions, those who saw a task-relevant ad reported a are each observed, continuous measures. The maxi- greater threat to freedom (:=3.07) than did those who mum likelihood method was used in model estima- saw a nonrelevant ad (2=2.48),F,,, ,,,=3.73, p<.05. tion. The initial analysis indicated a poor fitting model, However, similar to the findings pertaining to the x2=668.09(d.f = 244), a GFI of .87, AGFI of .84, com- perceived duration of the ad, participants viewing a parative fit index (CFI) of .92, and root mean squared 10-second ad experienced a similar amount of reac- error of approximation (RMSEA) of .07. 90 The Journal ofAdvertising

Figure 1 A Conceptual Model of the Perceived Intrusiveness of Pop-Up Ads

Antecedents Consequences

Ad irr~tat~on

; ~nterruption , L__..i

congruence -..

informativeness

Figure 2 Antecedents and Consequences of Perceived Intrusiveness: The Final Model

Antecedents Consequences

Ad -.35

-- . -- /" /------,\ ---,- .27 Ad irritation ] -.. -... -\ /\ /' . '.. C 69/' -1. - -< ( intrusivenessAd

-- __-'.-L\ 3, ,~--- -'... -- __-- - \\ /dilOriai-ad \.\<*:avoidance I congruence \ - ,,' /) .-._ Ad

/ 1,

To improve the model, the significance of the re- by the perceived intrusiveness of the ad. Therefore, gression weights was first examined for all variables. duration of the interruption was removed from the As expected on the basis of the nonsignificant find- model, as was the link between irritation and ad avoid- ings of the manipulation check, the perceived dura- ance. The revised model was further tested, and modi- tion of the interruption caused by the ad was not fication indices were used to identify any missed significantly related to perceived intrusiveness,p>.05. relationships in the original model. The degree to Unexpectedly, irritation did not significantly predict which an ad was rated as entertaining was shown to ad avoidance, p>.05. Instead, avoidance was driven be related to the level of irritation experienced @<.01) Fall 2002 9 1

Table 2 Final Model Summary

Independent Dependent Unstandardized Standard Standardized Variables Variables Parameters Errors Parameters H 1 Cognitive intensity Ad intrusiveness H2* Duration of interruption Ad intrusiveness H3 Editorial-ad congruence Ad intrusiveness H4 Editorial-ad congruence Ad informativeness H5 Ad informativeness Ad intrusiveness H6 Ad entertainment Ad intrusiveness Added Ad entertainment Ad irritation H7 Ad intrusiveness Ad irritation H8 Ad intrusiveness Ad avoidance H9* Ad irritation Ad avoidance Goodness-of-Fit Indices

x2 433.64 (d.f.=202), pc.001 Joreskog-Sorbom goodness-of-fit index (GFI) .91 Adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) .89 Comparative fit index (CFI) .95 Root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) .06

*Dropped in the final model. and therefore was added to the model. Although we Specifically, two manipulated variables-cognitive expected this relationship to be moderated by intru- intensity and editorial-ad congruence-had a signifi- siveness, much of the literature on irritation points cant impact on perceived intrusiveness, whereas du- out that irritation occurs when ads contain untruth- ration of the ad did not. That is, ads were found to be ful or confusing content or are executed poorly (Aaker more intrusive by participants highly immersed in and Bruzzone 1985; Bauer and Greyser 1968). The the content (2~4.42,S=1.41) than by those who were model with the addition of the new causal path be- less cognitively engaged (Z=3.70, S=1.41),t ,,,,,=4.70, tween entertainment and irritation was tested, and p<.001. The manipulation of cognitive intensity speci- one item (irritation) from the irritation scale with fied in Hlb did not work and therefore could not be excessive covariance was removed. The resulting tested. However, evidence for Hla was found. Unfor- model, presented in Figure 2, was found to fit the tunately, H2 could not be supported. The duration of data well, x2=433.64 (d.f.=202), GFI=.91, AGFI=.89, both 10- and 20-second ads was perceived similarly, CFI=.95, and RMSEA=.06. The significance of regres- and, thus, duration is not related to perceptions of sion weights was examined for all remaining con- intrusiveness, p>.05. Perhaps the existence of the structs, and their associated measures and all interstitial itself triggered feelings of intrusiveness, relationships were found to be significant atp<.Ol.A and once a participant decided to watch the ad, the final model summary is presented in Table 2. length might have not mattered. Editorial congru- The final model provides support for seven of the ence was found to have a negative relationship with ten hypotheses. In addition, a new causal relation- perceptions of intrusiveness, and, therefore, H3 was

ship (ad entertainment -+ ad irritation) emerged and supported. Ads were perceived as less intrusive when was added to the model. This addition is conceptually related to the participant's task (2=3.64, S=1.42)than sound, in that previous studies have shown that en- when not related to the task (%=4.24,S=1.43), tertaining ads are perceived as valuable by audiences t,,,,!=4.04, p<.001. Editorial congruence also had a (Alwitt and Prabhaker 1992; Biel and Bridgwater positive impact on the perception of the ad as infor- 1990; Ducoffe 1996). To the degree that viewers are mative, in support of H4. Those who saw the congru- entertained by ads, they are less likely to be irritated. ent ad reported it to be more informative (2=4.17, All relationships in the final model seem reasonable S=1.32)than did those who saw the noncongruent ad and are in accordance with the literature reviewed. (T=3.77, S=1.15),ti3711=3.14, p<.01. The Journal ofAdvertising

Hypotheses 5 and 6 were related to the degree to up ads. Recent practices in the on-line advertising which ads that are perceived as informative and en- industry to use pop-unders or interstitials (ads that tertaining reduce perceptions of the ad as intrusive. appear in the main Web browser when users attempt Both variables were negatively related to perceptions to move from one page to another) would be justified of intrusiveness, indicating that the more value (in- by the findings from the current study. formation or entertainment) perceived in an ad, the Second, another means of limiting perceptions of less intrusive it is perceived. Both hypotheses are intrusiveness involves increasing the relevancy of pop- thus supported. Hypothesis 7 proposed a connection up advertisements by using content placement strat- between perceptions of intrusiveness and feelings of egies. Although increasing relevance was found to irritation. A strong positive relationship was found in increase reactance, participants actually reported feel- support of the relationship. However, the added rela- ing less intrusiveness. A possible explanation for this tionship between the perception of an ad as enter- finding is that relevant ads could not be as easily taining and feelings of irritation now means that discounted as meaningless. So, though they raised entertainment has both a direct and an indirect effect significantly greater reactance than nonrelevant ads, on irritation. Hypothesis 8 specified that perceptions meaningful information tempered feelings of intru- of intrusiveness would be positively related to the siveness and irritation. This finding highlights the avoidance of the ad. The model shows that avoidance importance of content placement strategies, which is caused by the degree to which an ad is judged to be have been used in print media (Janiszewski 1990; Yi intrusive. Finally, the lack of support for H9 is interest- 1990) and television (Murry, Lastovicka, and Singh ing because feelings of irritation were not significantly 1992; Park and McClung 1986; Singh and Churchill related to ad avoidance. Similar to the findings of Cronin 1987; Soldow and Principe 1981). The same rationale and Menelly (19921, this suggests that ad avoidance is being used on-line (Sherman and Deighton 2001). may take place upon recognition of the ad as intrusive, If consumers are surfing "cars.com" and an automo- even though the viewer is not yet irritated. bile ad is seen, it is likely to be more relevant than an ad for Budweiser and thus less likely to elicit feelings Discussion of intrusiveness and irritation. Third, the final strategy to reduce intrusiveness is to The current study provides evidence that, when increase the value viewers receive from ads. Information ads are perceived as intrusive, feelings of irritation deemed important or interesting or an ad that is enter- are elicited and advertisements are avoided. Appar- taining rewards the viewer, who is thereby less likely to ently, perceptions of interstitials as intrusive are re- feel irritated by the interruption. Through strategic use lated to the level of cognitive intensity with which and placement of commercial messages, resistance of con- viewers pursue their goals. When viewers are focused, sumers can be lessened, which is likely to result in less they perceive interruptions as more severe than when irritation and greater message effectiveness. they are not focused. However, through creative ad- However, there are several limitations to the cur- vertisement placement strategies, perceptions of in- rent study that should be addressed. First, the ma- trusiveness may be moderated. When ads are nipulation of ad length was not perceived to differ requested or provide value, either in the form of in- among participants. Therefore, we cannot comment formation or entertainment, they are perceived as on previous claims that longer ads are more intrusive less of an interruption, are less irritating, and may be or irritating than shorter ads. Subjects had a modi- less likely to be dismissed as nuisances. cum of control during the experiment, in that they The variables found to limit perceptions of intru- could close the pop-up ad at will. Although partici- siveness involve (1)targeting viewers when their cog- pants exposed to the 20-second ad left the ad open nitive effort is low, (2) increasing the relevancy of the significantly longer than did those exposed to the 10- advertising, and (3) providing value to viewers. First, second ad, there was no perceived difference in the strategies that seek to minimize the interruption of length of the ads. Perhaps the participants were not in viewers' current activities are likely to meet with less a hurry to complete the task and therefore did not resistance. Therefore, viewers should be exposed to deem the length of the pop-up ad important. Alterna- pop-up ads only at breaks in content. In the current tive procedures could include providing an incentive study, switching between pages offered such a break. for timely completion of the task or choosing very short However, Web pages could be designed with pictures and very long ads to maximize perceptions of ad length. or large banner ads separating sections of content, Second, the degree to which participants felt in thereby providing the breaks needed to launch pop- control of the timing of the appearance of the ad may Fall 2002 be a limitation. Participants in the "timed" cognitive (1996) distinguish goal-directed behavior from expe- intensity condition had no control over when the ad riential or ritual behavior on the Web. Whereas the popped up, whereas participants in the second and former refers to activities such as information seek- third conditions were required to click a link (either ing and on-line shopping, the latter is less directed. to go to the next page or close the browser) before the Because intrusiveness has been defined as the inter- ad popped up. Because of the importance of control in ruption of cognitive processing, it would be worth- assessing perceived interactivity, teasing out the ef- while to investigate the effects of intrusiveness during fects of control from cognitive intensity seems like an experiential behavior. For example, when users are important next step in this line of research. mindlessly surfing through various Web sites without Third, the use of categorical exogenous variables in any specific purposes, an interstitial may cause less the tested SEM is unorthodox. Although acceptable intrusiveness than it does in a goal-directed context. analytically, cognitive intensity, duration of the inter- Unlike many Web sites with interstitials, only one ruption, and editorial congruence were manipulated to interstitial popped up at the experimental Web site. create experimental conditions and therefore limited Future studies may wish to examine multiple the variation and explanatory power of the model. Fu- interstitials and vary the frequency with which ads ture research should note this limitation and measure pop up; their cumulative effects may exacerbate per- perception&of such manipulations to facilitate greater ceptions of intrusiveness. The type of message dis- explanation with continuous exogenous variables. played in a pop-up ad may also interact with the act of popping up itself. Static banners may not provide Conclusion the entertainment that rich media can offer. Alterna- tively, some consumers may be irritated by having to We have provided a framework for understanding wait for rich media and prefer a simpler message. negative responses to Internet advertising, such as In a similar vein, the method of placing ads in Web feelings of irritation and ad avoidance. The proposed sites should be investigated in more depth. The cur- model is an initial step in understanding the rela- rent study manipulates cognitive intensity by placing tionships among intrusiveness, irritation, and ad the ad at different locations within the Web page: at avoidance in forced exposure situations. The proposed the beginning, in the middle, and at the end. How- model offers several unique characteristics, in that it ever, technological advances provide many more op- conceptualizes the role and nature of intrusiveness in tions for specifying ad placement. These different understanding responses to advertising and demon- placements vary in the degree of forced exposure. For strates that perceptions of intrusiveness may be mod- example, pop-unders can be loaded under the browser erated by other factors. None of these issues has and be seen upon closing the browser. Ads can also be received explicit attention in previous literature. By made to float on the browser window without disap- bringing them together in a framework, we aim to pearing, be full-screen ads that prevent people from guide future empirical research and theoretical work. viewing other content, or have the appearance of tra- It must be noted that our findings cannot be gener- ditional banner ads but expand without leaving the alized to all Web site viewing behaviors. This study current Web site. Other ads are programmed to ap- examines the impact of perceived intrusiveness in pear only when users wait for the Web pages to load. the context of task-oriented, extrinsically motivated These diverse tactics and new technologies are allow- behavior. Participants were assigned to learn as much ing for greater control over advertising placement, as possible about their given tasks, and thus, the and it will be necessary to test how people perceive underlying the Web surfing task may limit those new ad formats. the generalizability of the results. Intrinsically moti- A third area that needs to be addressed in future vated, task-oriented surfing behaviors could be ex- research is the trade-off between good and bad expo- amined by providing participants with a range of sure. The old saying that "any publicity is good pub- topics from which to select. Intrinsically motivated licity'' illustrates this point. Even if viewers respond behavior could result in more or less reactance, de- negatively to forced exposure advertisements, they pending on the strength of the motivation. Therefore, are still exposed to the message. This exposure is it would be interesting to examine the relationship likely to elicit increased levels of attention and should among motivation, cognitive intensity, and the per- facilitate memory for the advertisement. Therefore, ception of intrusiveness. it becomes important to consider the goal of the cam- It should also be noted that not all Web site viewing paign when studying about the effects of intrusive- behavior is task- or goal-oriented. Hoffman and Novak ness. For example, advertisers seeking positive 94 The Journal of Advertising attitude formation as a campaign objective may wish Ducoffe, Robert H. (1995), "How Consumers Assess the Value of to avoid intrusive ads, which are likely to result in Advertising." Journal of Current Issues and Research in Ad- vertising, 17 (I), 1-18. irritation. However, if recall or recognition is a goal, (1996), "Advertising Value and Advertising on the intrusive ads could be more effective than nonintrusive Web," Jourrlal of Aduertising Research, 36 (5),21-35. ads. Exploration of the relationship between intru- Friestad, Marian and Peter Wright (19941, 'The Persuasion Knowl- siveness and memory is needed to understand the edge Model. How People Cope with Persuasion Attempts," Jourr~alof (:onsurner Research, 21 (I), 1-31. benefits and drawbacks of using forced exposure ads. Ha, Louisa (199(i),"Advertising Clutter in Consumer Magazines: Overall, the current research shows that judgments Dimensions and Effects," Jourrzal ofAduertisir~gResearch, 36 about ad interruptions may be changed by manipu- (July!Auyst), 76-83. lating the antecedents of intrusiveness. These find- Hoffman, Donna L. and Thomas P. Novak (19961, "Marketing in Hypermedia Computer-Mediated Environments: Conceptual ings add to our understanding of how viewers come to Foundations," Joz~rlzalof Marketing, 60 (31, 50-68. define when an ad is intrusive. Furthermore, the re- Interactive Advertising Bureau (2001!, Rich Medla Ad Unit Guide- search seeks to distinguish the intrusiveness of an lines, Interactive Advertising Bureau, (August 6), [http:I! advertisement from the consequences of the inter- www.iab.net/rich~contentiindex.html]. Janiszewslu, Chris (19901, "The Influence of Nonattended hlate- ruption itself. By demonstrating that intrusiveness is rial on the l'rocessing of Advertising Clainls," ,Journal of Mar- a precursor to feelings of irritation and avoidance keting Resenrch, 27 (34 263-278. behaviors, we have added to the understanding of the Joreskog, Karl (;. and Dag Sorbom (19891, LISREL 7: A Gurde to underlying mechanism by which negative reactions the Progrartl and Applications, 2d ed., Chicago: SPSS Inc. Kahneman, Daniel (19731, Atter~tiorlad Effort, Englewood Cliffs, to advertising occur. By providing this evidence in a NJ: Prentice Hall. single model, we begin to understand how both the Kennedy, John It. (1971), "How Program Environment Affects TV antecedents and consequences of forced exposure ad- Commercials," Journal ofddvertising Research, 11 (I),33-38. vertising are best understood by examining the con- Krugman, Dean M. and Keith F. Johnson (1991), ''Differences in the Consumption of Traditional Broadcast and VCR Movie Rental," struct of intrusiveness. Jour~lcllofBrDadcastit~g &Electronic Media, 35 (Spring),213-232. Krugman, Herbert E. (1983), 'Television Program Interest and Com- mercial Interruption: Are Commercials on Interesting Programs References Less Effective?" Journal of Advertising Research, 23 (11, 21-23. Li, Hairong, Steven M. Edwards, and Joo-Hwn Lee (20011, "Mea- Aaker, David S. and Donald E. Bruzzone (1985 I, "Causes of Irrita- suring the Intrusiveness of Internet Advertising: Scale Devel- tion in Advertising," Journal of Mariietillg, 49 (21, 47-57. opment and Validation," in Proceedings of the 2001 Conference Abernethy, Avery M. (19911, "Physical and Mechanical Avoidance of the Arnerican Acaclerr~jofAd~ler-tising, Charles K. Taylor, ed., of Television Commercials: An Exploratory Study of Zipping, Villanova, I'A. American Academy of Advertising, 25-26. Zapping and Leaving," in Proceedirlgs of the An~erzcanAcad- McQuail, Dennis (1983), Mass Conz~rzurzicationTheoiy: An It~tro- erny of Aduertising, Rebecca Holman, ed., New York: The ductaon, Thousand Oaks, CA. Sage. American Academy of Advertising, 223-231. Milward Brown Interactive (1999a1, The Wired Digital Rich Media Alwitt, Linda F. and Paul R. Prabhaker (19921, "Functional and Stud.v, San Francisco, CA: ward Brown Interactive, http:!:I Belief Dimensions of Attitudes to Television," Jourr~alof Ad- www.intelhquest.convsearcWresults.asp?vpath=/press!'releasesi uertising Research, 32 (51, 30-42. mbi-release05.aspl. Bauer, Raymond A and Stephen A. Greyser (19681, Advertising in (1999b), Evaluating the Effectiveness of the hlericar~The Consunler View, Boston, kL4: Harvard University. Superstitzuls, Millward Brown Interactive, (October,, [http:'; Biel, Alexander L. and Carol A. Bridgwater (1990!, "Attributes of www.unicast.comidownloads!mbi.pdfl. Likable Television Cominercials," Journal of Advertiszng Re- Murry, John P.. Jr., John L. Lastovicka, and Suiendra N. Singh search, 30 i3), 38-44. (1992 1, "Feeling and Liking Responses to Television Programs: Bollen, Kenneth A. (1989),Structural Equations with Latent Vari- An Examination of Two Explanations for Media Context Ef- ables, New York: John Wiley & Sons. fects," Journal of Consurner Research, 18 (March), 441-451. Bowen, John (2001), "Analyst View: The Emergence of Rich Media," Park, C. \Vhan and Gordon W. McClung (1985), 'The Effect of TV Red Herring, (January 24). [http://wu~w.redherring.com/ Program Involvement on Involvement with Commercials," in Pro- inde~.asp?layout=story&channel=20000002&did=12100175211. ceedings ~f~bsociationof Consu~rler Research, Richard J. Lutz, ed., Brehm, Jack W. (1966),A Theory of Psychological Reactance, New Las Vegas, NV. Association of Consumer Research, 544-547. York: Academic Press. Pasadeos, Yorgo (19901, "Perceived Informativeness of and Irritation Brehm, Sharon S. and Jack W. Brehm (1981),Psycholog2cal Reactance: with Local Advertising," Journalism Quarterly, 67 (1),35-39. A Theory of Freedorn and Control, New York: Academic Press. Petty, Richard E:. and John T. Cacioppo (19791, "Issue Involvement Clancey, Maura (19941, 'The Television Audience Examined." Jour- Can Increase or Decrease Persuasion by Enhancing Message- nal ofAduertising Research, 39 (51, 27-37. Relevant Cognitive Responses," Journal of Person,ality and Clee, Mona A. and Robert A. Wicklund (19801, "Consumer Behav- Social Psyc~hology,37 (lo), 1915-1926. ior and Psychological Reactance," Journal of Consurner Re- Reizenstine, Richard C. (19711, "A Dissonance Approach to Mea- search, 6 (4), 389. suring the Effectiveness of Two Personal Selling Techniques Cronin, John J. and Nancy E. Menelly (19921, "Discrimination vs. Through 1)ecision Reversal," Proceedings of the Conference of Avoidance: 'Zipping' of Television Commercials," Journal of thehzerican Marketing Association, Chicago: American Mar- Aduertising, 21 (Summer), 1-7. keting Association, 176-180. Fall 2002 95

Robertson, Thomas S. and John R. Rossiter (19741, "Children and Speck, Paul Surg and Michael T. Elliott (19971, "Predictors of Commercial Persuasion: An Attribution Theory Analysis," Advertising Avoidance in Print and Broadcast Media," Jour- Journal of Consumer Research, 1 (11, 13. nal of Advertising, 26 (31, 61-76. Sandage, Charles H. and John Leckenby (1980), "Student Attitude Taylor, Catharine P. (20011, "The Crackle Over 'Pop Unders,"' toward Advertising: Institution vs. Instrument," Journal of AdvertisingAge, 72 (July 161, 36. Advertising, 9 (21, 29-33. Wells, William D., Clark Leavitt, and Maureen McConville (19711, Sherman, Lee and John Deighton (20011, "Banner Advertising: "A Reaction Profile for TV Commercials," Journal of Aduertis- Measuring Effectiveness and Optimizing Placement," Jour- ing Research, 11 (December), 11-17. nal of Interactive Marketing, 15 (2), 60-64. Yi, Youjae (19901, "Cognitive and Affective Primmg Effects of the Con- Singh, Surendra N. and Gilbert A. Churchill Jr. (19871, " and text for Print Advertising," Joud ofAduerfising, 19 (2),40-48. Advertising Effectiveness," Journal ofAduertising, 16 (1),4-10. Soldow, Gary F. and Victor Principe (19811, "Response to Commer- cials as a Function of Program Context," Journal ofddvertis- ing Research, 21 (21, 59-65.