Panksepp 2011 Why Does Depression Hurt Diminished Brain Reward

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Panksepp 2011 Why Does Depression Hurt Diminished Brain Reward Psychiatry 74(1) Spring 2011 5 Why Does Depression Hurt? Panksepp and Watt Why Does Depression Hurt? Ancestral Primary- Process Separation-Distress (PANIC/GRIEF) and Diminished Brain Reward (SEEKING) Processes in the Genesis of Depressive Affect Jaak Panksepp and Douglas Watt What can affective neuroscience add to ry-process nature). Although every aspect of the discussion of the genesis of depression? the affective life can be influenced by depres- Among other contributions, it may begin to sion, depression is intimately related to 1) answer the question of why depression feels sustained overactivity of the separation-dis- so bad. Since it is the only basic neuroscience tress PANIC system that can, if prolonged, approach that specifically aims to take the lead to a downward cascade of psychological affective infrastructure of the evolved mind despair (a theoretical view originally formu- as its central focus, it offers testable hypoth- lated by John Bowlby); and 2) the despair eses concerning the affective imbalances that phase that follows the acute PANIC response contribute to clinical depression (Solms & which is characterized by abnormally low Panksepp, 2011). A critical question about activity of the SEEKING system. In terms genesis of depression is: Which negative of animal modeling, depression reflects the affect-generating networks of mammalian behavioral agitation of separation distress brains are most important for understanding followed by emotional shutdown. The initial depressive “pain” and what new therapeu- behaviorially agitated panic state may include tics might such knowledge engender? SEEKING arousal, followed by dramatically Affective neuroscience has outlined diminished SEEKING during the depressive seven primary process (i.e., genetically pro- “despair phase.” From this perspective, de- vided) emotional systems. All are subcorti- pression may have evolutionary advantages, cally situated (Panksepp, 1998), where ani- such as conservation of resources follow- mal models allow causal (vs. correlational) ing unalleviated separation distress. A more analysis, not afforded by human research, detailed exposition of this view, along with including modern brain imaging. These pri- seven expert commentaries, is available in mary functions consist of SEEKING, RAGE, Watt and Panksepp (2009). Here, we briefly FEAR, sexual LUST, maternal CARE, sepa- summarize the implications of this perspec- ration-distress PANIC/GRIEF (henceforth, tive for research and therapeutics. simply PANIC) and joyful PLAY (neural sys- Many stressors used to evoke depres- tems are capitalized to highlight their prima- sive phenotypes—from physical to psycho- Jaak Panksepp is affiliated with the Department of VCAPP, College of Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman. Douglas Watt is with Cambridge City Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge, Mas- sachusetts. This work was supported by the Hope for Depression Research Foundation to JP. Address correspondence to Jaak Panksepp, Department of VCAPP, College of Veterinary Medicine, Washington State University, Pullman, WA 99164-6351. E-mail: [email protected]. © 2011 Guilford Publications, Inc. 6 Why Does Depression Hurt? logical (e.g., social-defeat in adult aggressive Depression research during the last encounters)—remain to be clearly linked four decades of the 20th century focused most to specific emotional network activities, al- heavily on the consequences of stress (DeK- though neuroanatomical correlates have loet, Joels, & Holsboer, 2005; McEwen, been identified (e.g., Kanarik et al., 2011). 2007) and brain norepinephrine and sero- Most animal models of depression not only tonin dynamics (from Schildkraut, 1965, to employ very general stressors but also gen- Harro & Oreland, 2001, so to speak). This eral behavioral outcome measures—various excellent work has largely neglected why de- timid behaviors and diminished pleasure re- pression feels so bad. It is becoming harder sponses (e.g., hopeless swimming patterns to believe that general brain serotonergic and diminished sexual eagerness). Rarely and/or noradrenergic changes, which glob- are specific emotional circuits prescribed as ally regulate aspects of forebrain arousal targets of analysis. Because of general stres- and dynamics, will specifically explain the sors and nonspecific outcome measures, such morbid moods of depression (Delgado et al., preclinical studies rarely provide insights 1990). These amines regulate quite general into psychodynamic or interpersonal consid- brain arousal functions that influence all erations of primary concern for clinical prac- emotions. It is no surprise that SSRIs mild- titioners, especially the feelings of social loss ly ameliorate many psychiatric problems, and defeat that promote depressive affects. while having modest overall efficacy, as Modern neuroscience, especially as applied highlighted by disappointing STAR*D find- to animal models, has little room for discus- ings (Rush, 2007; Rush, Trivedi, & Fava, sions of mental phenomena—especially the 2003). Likewise, more recent work on vari- affective experiences—that characterize psy- ous neurotrophic factor depletions (Koziek, chopathologies. Middlemas, & Bylund, 2008), stress-induced In line with Watt & Panksepp (2009) hippocampal shrinkage and CNS inflamma- and Solms & Panksepp (2011), we believe tion (Miller, Maletic, & Raison, 2009), and emotional-systems analyses will promote underlying genetics (Levinson, 2006), albeit better interdisciplinary dialog, yielding bet- of potential causal significance, provide little ter therapeutic interventions, where general understanding of the affective feelings that biogenic amine and cognitive-behavioral reg- characterize depression. Affective neurosci- ulatory strategies are commonly emphasized ence approaches can illuminate the subjec- more than affect-oriented therapies (but see tive manifestations of depressive affect (e.g., Fosha, Siegel, & Solomon, 2009, and Sh- Kroes et al., 2007; Panksepp, 2006; Pank- edler, 2010, for counter-examples). On the sepp et al., 2002.). psychopharmacological front, affective neu- roscience views promote the development AN AFFECTIVE NEUROSCIENTIFIC of new medicinals that target neuropeptide PERSPECTIVE ON WHY systems, such as endogenous opioids (both DEPRESSION FEELS SO BAD mu and kappa varieties) and corticotrophin releasing factor (CRF) dynamics, as well as amino acid transmitters, such as glutamate In extending the original Bowlby ac- and glycine, that substantively control many count of depression to depression neurobiol- affective states, including emotional learn- ogy (Watt & Panksepp, 2009), we here focus ing. On the psychotherapeutic front, affec- on how brain affective networks, altered by tive neuroscience approaches promote better sustained distress, may explain the psycho- and more specific utilization of positive emo- logical pain and dysphoria of depression. tions, such as facilitated SEEKING, CARE, Overactivity of brain separation-distress and PLAY dynamics, which currently remain PANIC/GRIEF and underactivity of SEEK- underdeveloped. ING networks may explain how the biggest Panksepp and Watt 7 FIGURE 1. Human and animal schematics of sadness and separation-distress systems. Animal data comes from localized brain stimulation mapping of separation distress circuits in guinea pigs (Herman & Panksepp, 1981) and human data from Damasio, et al., 2000 (from Panksepp 2003). epidemiological stressor, namely social loss, the last two PANIC facilitators have yielded promotes depression (Bowlby, 1980; Heim promising antidepressant effects (Holsboer, & Nemeroff, 1999). Depression may feel bad 2000; Zarate et al., 2006). Likewise, it is to because brain separation-distress systems be anticipated that opioid and oxytocin fa- create psychological pain. As Bowlby recog- cilitators may alleviate depression. Indeed, nized, separation distress—the “protest” or opioids were widely used as antidepressants “panic” responses that promptly follow so- before the modern era (Tenore, 2008), and cial loss, especially in young animals—feels ultra-lowdoses of buprenorphine are fine an- bad in a unique way. Affective understanding tidepressants for individuals not helped by of such brain processes, garnered by map- traditional medications (Bodkin et al., 1995). ping neuroanatomies and neurochemistries Perhaps oxytocinergic and prolactinergic of separation distress, helps clarify the na- drugs can be harnessed for similar ends. ture of social attachments and loss (Pank- sepp, 1998). These neuroanatomies are sum- BUT SEparaTION DISTRESS marized in Figure 1. IS ONLY THE GATEwaY TO The PANIC/GRIEF circuitry starts in DEPRESSION midbrain central gray regions, commonly called the periaqueductal gray (PAG), and ascends through medial diencephalic struc- The PANIC/GRIEF system prob- tures, especially the dorsomedial thalamus, ably evolved from general pain mechanisms and terminates in various basal forebrain (Panksepp, 1998, 2003) presumably millions nuclei and subcallosal anterior cingulate of years ago (birds possess a homologous forebrain regions, which have been targeted system). This psychic-pain system promotes for deep brain stimulation (DBS) therapy for social cohesion, forges bonds between in- treatment-resistant depressions (Mayberg et fants and caretakers, and fortifies friendships al., 2005). Key neurochemistries that pro- and sexual relationships—in short, promotes mote separation calls (protest) are declining social solidarity among group living
Recommended publications
  • Assessment of Emotional Functioning in Pain Treatment Outcome Research
    Assessment of emotional functioning in pain treatment outcome research Robert D. Kerns, Ph.D. VA Connecticut Healthcare System Yale University Running head: Emotional functioning Correspondence: Robert D. Kerns, Ph.D., Psychology Service (116B), VA Connecticut Healthcare System, 950 Campbell Avenue, West Haven, CT 06516; Phone: 203-937- 3841; Fax: 203-937-4951; Electronic mail: [email protected] Emotional Functioning 2 Assessment of Emotional Functioning in Pain Treatment Outcome Research The measurement of emotional functioning as an important outcome in empirical examinations of pain treatment efficacy and effectiveness has not yet been generally adopted in the field. This observation is puzzling given the large and ever expanding empirical literature on the relationship between the experience of pain and negative mood, symptoms of affective distress, and frank psychiatric disorder. For example, Turk (1996), despite noting the high prevalence of psychiatric disorder, particularly depression, among patients referred to multidisciplinary pain clinics, failed to list the assessment of mood or symptoms of affective distress as one of the commonly cited criteria for evaluating pain outcomes from these programs. In a more recent review, Turk (2002) also failed to identify emotional distress as a key index of clinical effectiveness of chronic pain treatment. A casual review of the published outcome research in the past several years fails to identify the inclusion of measures of emotional distress in most studies of pain treatment outcome, other than those designed to evaluate the efficacy of psychological interventions. In a recent edited volume, The Handbook of Pain Assessment (Turk & Melzack, 2001), several contributors specifically encouraged inclusion of measures of psychological distress in the assessment of pain treatment effects (Bradley & McKendree-Smith, 2001; Dworkin, Nagasako, Hetzel, & Farrar, 2001; Okifuji & Turk, 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • The Psychology of Euthanizing Animals: the Emotional Components
    F. Turner and J. Strak Comment ORIGINAL ARTICLE food. Apart from this, there are likely to be large adjustmentcosts borne by pro­ ducers (at home and abroad) as existing production systems are discarded in The Psychology favor of those advocated by the welfare groups. Furthermore, the adoption of these less intensive forms of farming may result in a completely different pattern of Euthanizing Animals: of labor and capital use in the U.K. farming sector. The subject of animal welfare is undoubtedly one of great public concern. The Emotional Components However, it is also one of great complexity, and if changes in the regulations governing animal production methods are to be made, those changes should take full account of the implications for producers, consumers and society in general. The farming industry should not interpret the interest in animal welfare as a Charles E. Owens, Ricky Davis threat to its livelihood nor should consumers dismiss lightly the likely changes in costs or structure of farming that may result from a revision of the Codes of Prac­ and Bill Hurt Smith* tice relating to animal welfare. The appropriate animal welfare policy for society will be identified only when all the interested parties become fully aware of the consequences of their actions. [Ed. Note: Independent of any proposed changes in the British Codes of Prac­ Abstract tice, the U.K. veal calf industry (Quantock Veal) has taken the initiative of switch­ The emotional effects of euthanizing unwanted animals on professional ani­ ing from individual crate rearing to the use of straw-fi.lled group pens.
    [Show full text]
  • Term Toxic Shame Being Mirrored by One
    Donald Bradshaw Nathanson Coined the The compass of term toxic shame. shame Four universal Mark Epstein, Pema Chodron, Being behaviors to Kevin Griffin Karen Horney mirrored defend against Abiding difficult emotions to observe and learn . Four major by one shame. The Idea of PRACTICE and Right View being wise idea that when aempts of Gershen Kaufman non- or attuned. Led to Present with Self and Present with avoiding shame Find the entrances to shaming the neuro2c Others and Wise-Self you are bigger individual to governing scenes. person than or less than Whenever we are makes all come to others. Says, to soluon able to observe upon the learn from our our experience, we difference shame and "Just immediately detach love yourself." from it. Brene Brown Silvan Thomas Tony Webb Empathy opposite of Scheff/Helen Tomkins Virginia Satir The social aspects of shame; judge in Lewis Block Emotions the compass of Four coping areas most Disrupts bond are shame -- aggression, vulnerable to shame; motivators. stances: depression, isolation, Humiliated Placating, judging numbs-easier Affect and addiction. fury. Blame, Being than loss/grief; pre- Acknowledge theory: Alienation and Super- aggression broader frontal cortex off in shame then Scripts are shame. connection to begun as Reasonable, social results from Perfectionism. others soon as we Being Irrelevant avoiding shame. 'Good' shame as restored. are born. humility. Show deference to others. What does acknowledged shame look like? What is attunement? Shame-anger spirals. Governing Scenes Gershen
    [Show full text]
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • Bereavement Resource Manual 2018 Purpose
    Richmond’s Bereavement Resource Manual 2018 Purpose This manual is designed to serve as an educational resource guide to grieving families and bereavement professionals in the Central Virginia area and to provide a practical list of available national and local support services. It is meant to be a useful reference and is not intended as an exhaustive listing. Grief is not neat and tidy. At Full Circle Grief Center, we realize that each person’s grief journey is unique and personal, based on many factors. Keep in mind that there is no “right” or “wrong” way to cope with grief. After losing a loved one, family members have varying ways of coping and may require different levels of support over time. We hope that some aspect of this manual will be helpful to those grieving in our community and the professionals, friends, and family who support them. Manual created by: Graphic Design by: Copyright © 2010 Allyson England Drake, M.Ed., CT Kali Newlen-Burden Full Circle Grief Center. Founder and Executive Director www.kalinewlen.com Revised January 2018. Full Circle Grief Center All rights reserved. Cover Art Design by: Logan H. Macklin, aged 13 2 Table of Contents Purpose Page 2 Full Circle Grief Center Page 4 Grief and Loss Pages 5 - 9 Children, Teens and Grief Pages 10 - 20 Perinatal Loss and Death of an Infant Pages 21 - 23 Suicide Loss Pages 24 -26 When Additional Support is Needed Pages 27-31 Self-Care Page 32 Rituals and Remembrance Page 33 How to Help and Support Grieving Families Page 34 Community Bereavement Support Services Pages 35-47 Online Grief and Bereavement Services Pages 48-49 Book List for Grief and Loss Pages 50-61 Thoughts from a Grieving Mother Pages 61-63 Affirmations and Aspirations Pages 64-65 3 Full Circle’s mission is to provide comprehensive, professional grief support to children, adults, families, and communities.
    [Show full text]
  • Individual Differences in Empathy Are Associated with Apathy-Motivation
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Individual diferences in empathy are associated with apathy- motivation Received: 1 August 2017 Patricia L. Lockwood 1,2, Yuen-Siang Ang 1,2,3, Masud Husain 1,2,3 & Molly J. Crockett1,4 Accepted: 24 November 2017 Empathy - the capacity to understand and resonate with the experiences of other people - is considered Published: xx xx xxxx an essential aspect of social cognition. However, although empathy is often thought to be automatic, recent theories have argued that there is a key role for motivation in modulating empathic experiences. Here we administered self-report measures of empathy and apathy-motivation to a large sample of healthy people (n = 378) to test whether people who are more empathic are also more motivated. We then sought to replicate our fndings in an independent sample (n = 198) that also completed a behavioural task to measure state afective empathy and emotion recognition. Cognitive empathy was associated with higher levels of motivation generally across behavioural, social and emotional domains. In contrast, afective empathy was associated with lower levels of behavioural motivation, but higher levels of emotional motivation. Factor analyses showed that empathy and apathy are distinct constructs, but that afective empathy and emotional motivation are underpinned by the same latent factor. These results have potentially important clinical applications for disorders associated with reduced empathy and motivation as well as the understanding of these processes in healthy people. Empathy – the capacity to understand and resonate with the experiences of other people – is considered essential for navigating meaningful social interactions and is closely linked to prosocial behaviour1–7.
    [Show full text]
  • Acute Stress Disorder
    Trauma and Stress-Related Disorders: Developments for ICD-11 Andreas Maercker, MD PhD Professor of Psychopathology, University of Zurich and materials prepared and provided by Geoffrey Reed, PhD, WHO Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse Connuing Medical Educaon Commercial Disclosure Requirement • I, Andreas Maercker, have the following commercial relaonships to disclose: – Aardorf Private Psychiatric Hospital, Switzerland, advisory board – Springer, book royales Members of the Working Group • Christopher Brewin (UK) Organizational representatives • Richard Bryant (AU) • Mark van Ommeren (WHO) • Marylene Cloitre (US) • Augusto E. Llosa (Médecins Sans Frontières) • Asma Humayun (PA) • Renato Olivero Souza (ICRC) • Lynne Myfanwy Jones (UK/KE) • Inka Weissbecker (Intern. Medical Corps) • Ashraf Kagee (ZA) • Andreas Maercker (chair) (CH) • Cecile Rousseau (CA) WHO scientists and consultant • Dayanandan Somasundaram (LK) • Geoffrey Reed • Yuriko Suzuki (JP) • Mark van Ommeren • Simon Wessely (UK) • Michael B. First WHO Constuencies 1. Member Countries – Required to report health stascs to WHO according to ICD – ICD categories used as basis for eligibility and payment of health care, social, and disability benefits and services 2. Health Workers – Mulple mental health professions – ICD must be useful for front-line providers of care in idenfying and treang mental disorders 3. Service Users – ‘Nothing about us without us!’ – Must provide opportunies for substanve, early, and connuing input ICD Revision Orienting Principles 1. Highest goal is to help WHO member countries reduce disease burden of mental and behavioural disorders: relevance of ICD to public health 2. Focus on clinical utility: facilitate identification and treatment by global front-line health workers 3. Must be undertaken in collaboration with stakeholders: countries, health professionals, service users/consumers and families 4.
    [Show full text]
  • Social-Emotional Development in the First Three Years Establishing the Foundations
    ISSUE BRIEF Social-Emotional Development in the First Three Years Establishing the Foundations This issue brief, created by The Pennsylvania State University with support from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, is one of a series of briefs that addresses the need for research, practice and policy on social and emotional learning (SEL). SEL is defined as the process through which children and adults acquire and effectively apply the knowledge, attitudes, and skills necessary to understand and manage emotions, set and achieve positive goals, feel and show empathy for others, establish and maintain positive relationships, and make responsible decisions. Learn more at www.rwjf.org/socialemotionallearning. 1 | The Pennsylvania State University © 2018 | April 2018 ISSUE BRIEF Executive Summary In the first three years of life, children achieve remarkable advances in social and emotional development (SED) that establish a foundation for later competencies. Yet even in the first three years, these achievements can be threatened by exposure to elevated stresses of many kinds. Family poverty, marital conflict, parental emotional problems, experiences of trauma, neglect, or abuse and other adversities cause some infants and toddlers to experience anxious fearfulness, overwhelming sadness, disorganized attachment, or serious problems managing behavior and impulses. Programs to strengthen early SED focus on at least two people—including the child and the caregiver—because the development of healthy early SED relies on positive, supportive relationships.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Psychological Pain
    UNDERSTANDING Psychological pain is defined as intense and unbearable emotional pain associated with suffering, guilt, anguish, fear, panic, angst, loneliness, and helplessness. Unresolved, psychological pain can sometimes result in self-harming behaviors, addiction, and death by suicide. SOURCE: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Psychological_pain WHY DO PEOPLE ENGAGE IN SELF-HARMING BEHAVIORS? There are no simple answers. Everyone faces unique life challenges, which may include experiencing intense pain or trauma. For some, the emotions can feel overwhelming, leading them to hurt themselves on purpose to escape or cope. STOPPING OR ESCAPING COPING WITH UNDERLYING TRAUMA INTENSE PSYCHOLOGICAL PAIN To return from numbness: Emotional trauma To gain control: Self-harming can sometimes lead to disassociation (or a lack of behaviors are attempts to regain feelings). As a result, some people may engage subjective control over helplessness in behaviors to generate a strong physical or powerlessness by shifting attention sensation to bring them back to to distract from something more feeling “something” again. overwhelming or painful. To express their feelings: To release tension: Sometimes people have Sometimes self-harming GAIN difficulty acknowledging CONTROL behaviors provide RETURN or articulating their temporary relief and FROM inner emotional turmoil escape for the pain. NUMBNESS in a healthy way and Unfortunately, it the only way to can become an EXPRESS communicate their addictive cycle FEELINGS pain is by expressing when the body through physical and brain adapt self-harm, and develop a aggression, and tolerance. other forms of risky and reckless To experience behaviors. Other euphoria: Some RELEASE times, their actions self-harming TENSION are a silent cry behaviors trigger for help.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: on Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 12-2001 The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: On Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love Marc Bekoff University of Colorado Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_sata Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Comparative Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Bekoff, M. (2001). The evolution of animal play, emotions, and social morality: on science, theology, spirituality, personhood, and love. Zygon®, 36(4), 615-655. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: On Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love Marc Bekoff University of Colorado KEYWORDS animal emotions, animal play, biocentric anthropomorphism, critical anthropomorphism, personhood, social morality, spirituality ABSTRACT My essay first takes me into the arena in which science, spirituality, and theology meet. I comment on the enterprise of science and how scientists could well benefit from reciprocal interactions with theologians and religious leaders. Next, I discuss the evolution of social morality and the ways in which various aspects of social play behavior relate to the notion of “behaving fairly.” The contributions of spiritual and religious perspectives are important in our coming to a fuller understanding of the evolution of morality. I go on to discuss animal emotions, the concept of personhood, and how our special relationships with other animals, especially the companions with whom we share our homes, help us to define our place in nature, our humanness.
    [Show full text]
  • Social and Emotional Skills Well-Being, Connectedness and Success
    Social and Emotional Skills Well-being, connectedness and success ©OECD FOREWORD Contents Foreword Foreword 3 Education systems need to prepare students for continuous effort to create the kind of binding social their future, rather than for our past. In these times, capital through which we can share experiences, ideas Introduction 4 digitalisation is connecting people, cities and continents and innovation and build a shared understanding among to bring together a majority of the world’s population in groups with diverse experiences and interests, thus 01. Measuring Social and Emotional Skills 5 ways that vastly increases our individual and collective increasing our radius of trust to strangers and institutions. potential. But the same forces have made the world also 02. Social and emotional skills drive critical life outcomes 10 more volatile, more complex, and more uncertain. And Over the last years, social and emotional skills have when fast gets really fast, being slow to adapt makes been rising on the education policy agenda and in the 03. The impact of specific social and emotional skills on life outcomes 17 education systems really slow. The rolling processes of public debate. But for the majority of students, their automation, hollowing out jobs, particularly for routine development remains a matter of luck, depending on ○ Conscientiousness – getting things done, as required and in time 17 tasks, have radically altered the nature of work and life whether this is a priority for their teacher and their and thus the skills that are needed for success. For those school. A major barrier is the absence of reliable metrics ○ Openness to experience – exploring the world of things and ideas 20 with the right human capacities, this is liberating and in this field that allow educators and policy-makers to exciting.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychoethological Perspective on Play: Implications for Research and Practice
    Psicologia USP http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0103-656420160122 358 Brincar na perspectiva psicoetológica: implicações para pesquisa e prática Emma Otta* Universidade de São Paulo, Instituto de Psicologia, Departamento de Psicologia Experimental. São Paulo, SP, Brasil Resumo: Este ensaio trata do brincar a partir da perspectiva psicoetológica e examina implicações para a pesquisa e a prática. Ao longo das últimas décadas, crianças vêm ganhando oportunidades de escolarização e atividades dirigidas por adultos, mas perdendo oportunidades de brincadeira livre autogerenciada. Isto é preocupante, considerando as indicações de modelos animais de que a brincadeira social autogerenciada é importante para o desenvolvimento do cérebro social e da capacidade de autorregulação de emoções. Este estudo representa um convite-justificativa para que as crianças recuperem oportunidades de brincadeira natural das quais vêm sendo privadas. Quanto mais conhecermos sobre o brincar, mais adequados seremos nas oportunidades que poderemos oferecer a elas. Precisamos de mais pesquisa sobre este tema na academia, num ambiente intelectual que facilite a colaboração entre etólogos, psicólogos, educadores e neurocientistas, promovendo interação bidirecional entre teoria e prática. Palavras-chave: brincar, cérebro social, desenvolvimento, emoções, natureza. Introdução A razão da etóloga, fascinada pela observação do comportamento Este ensaio trata do brincar a partir da perspectiva psicoetológica e examina as implicações desta Por que convidar a estudar o comportamento de abordagem para pesquisa e prática relativas a um tema brincar dos animais? A resposta mais simples que poderia que considero negligenciado na área acadêmica. O termo dar é o fascínio pela observação do comportamento “abordagem psicoetológica” foi cunhado por Walter espontâneo, livre de limites artificiais.
    [Show full text]